Vocabulary and Grammar-1, Reading Skills

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Vocabulary and Grammar-1, Reading Skills Certificate in Communication Skill (CCS) CCS-02 Reading Skills Block – 4 Vocabulary and Grammar-1 UNIT-1 The Verb Phrase-1: Lexical, Auxiliary and Phrasal Verb UNIT-2 The Verb Phrase-2: Tense, Aspect and Modality UNIT-3 Dictionaries This course material is designed and developed by Indira Gandhi National Open University (IGNOU), New Delhi. OSOU has been permitted to use the material. 1 UNIT-1: The Verb Phrase-1: Lexical, Auxiliary And Phrasal Verbs Structure 1.0 Objective 1.1 Introduction 1.2 Lexical Verbs 1.3 Auxiliary Verbs 1.4 Phrasal Verbs 1.5 Let Us Sum Up 1.6 Suggested Reading 1.7 Answers 1.0 OBJECTIVE In this unit we shall identify the elements of the verbal group (or verb phrase) and note their properties. 1.1 INTRODUCTION The verb can be said to be the most important element of a sentence because the structure of a sentence depends largely on the verb. The difference between the sentences He laughed/ and He built a house is mainly that the first sentence has the verb laugh, which needs nothing to complete its meaning, but the second sentence has build, which requires an object to complete its meaning: what did he build?-A house. The difference between laugh and built is lexical which means that the two verbs represent different meanings or activities. Now look at the sentence: He is building a house. We have once again the item build but it is now preceded by is (a form of be). As you can see the meanings of the two sentences. 1) He is building a house. 2) He has built a house. are not the same, though they represent the same activity. Here the difference is a grammatical one, represented by the structure of the verbal group in the two sentences. The difference in the meaning of these two sentences arises from the auxiliaries (is, has) and the forms of the verbs (-ing and -en forms) in them. That they also have a common shared meaning, is because they share the same lexical verb, build. 2 Thus we can distinguish two elements in the verbal group; the auxiliary and the lexical verb. A special class of lexical verbs is seen in 3) We shall back you up in your efforts. Compare it with 4) We shall support you in your efforts. You will notice that the two sentences mean the same; the only difference is that while (4) has the lexical verb support, (3) has the combination back up, which means much the same as support. Combinations like back up (where the verbal meaning is given by a combination of more than one element) are called phrasal verbs. This unit is organized as follows: in Section 1.2 we shall take a brief look at lexical verbs; in 1.3 we shall study the auxiliaries, and in 1.4 look at phrasal verbs. 1.2 LEXICAL-VERBS Lexical verbs are items like build, write, laugh, sleep, etc. They normally indicate various types of activities. A verbal group must have a lexical verb. Without a lexical verb no verbal group is possible: cf. the difference between has been and has been writing/reading/talking, etc. Write, read, and talk are lexical verbs. Notice also that in the verbal group the lexical verb (Also called the full verb in school/traditional grammars) comes last. (Ex. Has written, will/shall have written, has been writing) Some lexical verbs require nothing more to complete their meanings. These are the intransitive verbs (Ex. cry, laugh, sleep, snore, etc. as in 'They cry, She laughs, Everyone sleeps but Gopu snores) Other lexical verbs must be followed by some noun phrase to complete their meanings: He wrote a novel. She told us a story. Write and tell are the transitive verbs. There are literally thousands of transitive verbs. Among transitive verbs, some require two noun phrases to complete their meanings: Give Mary a book. Usually these verbs have an alternative construction where one of the noun phrases appears as a prepositional phrase (Give a book to Mary). The preposition may be to or for, depending on the verb. (She made John a cup of tea/She made a cup of tea for John). Some other verbs are immediately followed by a prepositional phrase: I agree with you; You can depend on John; Congratulations on your success. Some of the verbs which are followed by a noun phrase can also be followed by a clause: I know John. I know that he is coming. I am expecting Mary. I expect that Mary will come. The most important (at least the most frequent) lexical verb in the language (it is also an auxiliary, as we shall see) is be with its different forms - are , was, were ....Strictly 3 speaking be is neither transitive nor intransitive. Unlike intransitive verbs it can be followed by a noun phrase but there is a difference. A sentence with a transitive verb as in Tagore wrote Gitanjali. has an alternative construction as in Gitanjali was written by Tagore. (This alternative construction is said to be a passive.) But no passive is possible with a sentence containing be. He is a surgeon. * A surgeon is been by him. There are other properties of be which make it quite different from all other verbs. Similar is the case with have (which is also both a lexical and an auxiliary verb). Raju has a computer. * A computer is had by Raju. The properties distinguished above are properties in respect of what can follow a verb. As for the verb itself, the lexical verb in English has six forms. Ex. The six forms for the verb wait are. 4) 1) Stem wait 2) Stem + s waits 3) Stem + ing waiting 4) Stem + ed waited 5) Stem + en waited 6) To + stem to wait The stem form is also called 'base' or 'dictionary' form. (It is the form in which the verb is entered in the dictionary). The stem + s form is the present tense form (in the third person singular). The stem + ing form is also known as the 'present participle' form. The stem + ed is the form of the verb in the past tense.(A majority of the verbs have this form in the past tense.) Hence '-ed' is used as a symbol for past tense. Actually a few verbs have the base form for the past also (e.g. cut, put, cost) or show a different pattern of change ( write-wrote , go-went, speak-spoke, etc). The stem + en form is also called the 'past participle' form. (Here again verbs like cut and put show no change; wait itself shows the same form as in the past.) But again the majority of verbs do have this form: speak- spoken, write-written . (On verb morphology, see also Unit 15 in Block 3. The last form (to + stem) is the infinitival form (You will find more on this below). Here again, sometimes the base form is used for the infinitive. (e.g. We can say either I helped her to wash the clothes/ or 4 without using to: I helped her wash the clothes. Note, however that this optional use of to is possible with only a few lexical verbs). Lexical verbs generally but not always have these six forms. However, phrasal verbs (being a sub-class of lexical verbs) also generally have all the six forms. Finite and Non-finite Of the forms of the verb, some are called finite and some non-finite. Finite forms are those which make predication: that is, a statement about a subject e.g. Wait here/Time waits for none/I waited for you at the station. The forms waiting, to wait cannot make predication: to wait for the bus/waiting for the bus. These expressions require some other finite verb to make predication. I have to wait for Mary/We were waiting outside the hall. Have, were are finite forms and help make the sentences complete in meaning. Forms which cannot, in themselves, make predication are called non-finite forms. The non-finite forms are: the infinitive (e.g. to wait), the present participle (e.g. waiting) and the past participle (e.g. waited). Since the past form of wait is the same as its past participle, we can take a verb like break to see that the past participle is a non-finite form and cannot make predication. He broke the chair, but not *He broken the chair. The finite form of the verb can make predication because it contains tense: either present or past. In contrast the non-finite forms dc not contain tense. (See the example above.) Finally, the finite for111 shows ‘agreement’ with the subject. This can be seen in I am learning French. We are learning Spanish. He is learning English. learning (a non-finite form) remains the same in all the three sentences. But the forms of be are different in each sentence, depending on the subject. Only the forms am, are, is show 'agreement' with the subject. The finite form always comes first in the verbal group. The rest ofthe verbal elements, if any, are all non-finite. 5) He has been working on a research project. (Has is the only finite verb here: it is the third person singular form in the present tense of the verb have; been = be + en (past participle); working = work + ing (present participle); both are non-finite forms. Check Your Progress 1 5 1) In the following passage list all the finite lexical verbs.
Recommended publications
  • A Minimalist Study of Complex Verb Formation: Cross-Linguistic Paerns and Variation
    A Minimalist Study of Complex Verb Formation: Cross-linguistic Paerns and Variation Chenchen Julio Song, [email protected] PhD First Year Report, June 2016 Abstract is report investigates the cross-linguistic paerns and structural variation in com- plex verbs within a Minimalist and Distributed Morphology framework. Based on data from English, German, Hungarian, Chinese, and Japanese, three general mechanisms are proposed for complex verb formation, including Akt-licensing, “two-peaked” adjunction, and trans-workspace recategorization. e interaction of these mechanisms yields three levels of complex verb formation, i.e. Root level, verbalizer level, and beyond verbalizer level. In particular, the verbalizer (together with its Akt extension) is identified as the boundary between the word-internal and word-external domains of complex verbs. With these techniques, a unified analysis for the cohesion level, separability, component cate- gory, and semantic nature of complex verbs is tentatively presented. 1 Introduction1 Complex verbs may be complex in form or meaning (or both). For example, break (an Accom- plishment verb) is simple in form but complex in meaning (with two subevents), understand (a Stative verb) is complex in form but simple in meaning, and get up is complex in both form and meaning. is report is primarily based on formal complexity2 but tries to fit meaning into the picture as well. Complex verbs are cross-linguistically common. e above-mentioned understand and get up represent just two types: prefixed verb and phrasal verb. ere are still other types of complex verb, such as compound verb (e.g. stir-fry). ese are just descriptive terms, which I use for expository convenience.
    [Show full text]
  • Identification of Zero Copulas in Hungarian Using
    Proceedings of the 12th Conference on Language Resources and Evaluation (LREC 2020), pages 4802–4810 Marseille, 11–16 May 2020 c European Language Resources Association (ELRA), licensed under CC-BY-NC Much Ado About Nothing Identification of Zero Copulas in Hungarian Using an NMT Model Andrea Dömötör1;2, Zijian Gyoz˝ o˝ Yang1, Attila Novák1 1MTA-PPKE Hungarian Language Technology Research Group, Pázmány Péter Catholic University, Faculty of Information Technology and Bionics Práter u. 50/a, 1083 Budapest, Hungary 2Pázmány Péter Catholic University, Faculty of Humanities and Social Sciences Egyetem u. 1, 2087 Piliscsaba, Hungary {surname.firstname}@itk.ppke.hu Abstract The research presented in this paper concerns zero copulas in Hungarian, i.e. the phenomenon that nominal predicates lack an explicit verbal copula in the default present tense 3rd person indicative case. We created a tool based on the state-of-the-art transformer architecture implemented in Marian NMT framework that can identify and mark the location of zero copulas, i.e. the position where an overt copula would appear in the non-default cases. Our primary aim was to support quantitative corpus-based linguistic research by creating a tool that can be used to compile a corpus of significant size containing examples of nominal predicates including the location of the zero copulas. We created the training corpus for our system transforming sentences containing overt copulas into ones containing zero copula labels. However, we first needed to disambiguate occurrences of the massively ambiguous verb van ‘exist/be/have’. We performed this using a rule-base classifier relying on English translations in the English-Hungarian parallel subcorpus of the OpenSubtitles corpus.
    [Show full text]
  • Glossary of Linguistic Terms
    Glossary of Linguistic Terms accent Often used to refer to distinctive pronuncia­ tions which differ from that of Received Pronunciation It differs from dialect which includes syn­ tax and vocabulary as well acronym A word formed from the initial letters of the words which make up a name, e.g. NATO (from North Atlantic Treaty Organisation) active A clause in which the subject is the actor of the verb; in a passive clause the actor is not the grammatical subject; seep. 14 addressee The person being addressed or spoken to in any form of discourse adjective In traditional grammar a word which de­ scribes a noun, as happy in 'the happy man'; an adjective phrase is a group of one or more words fulfilling the function of an adjective; seep. 11 adverb In t:r:aditional grammar a word which de­ scribes a verb; in 'he ran slowly', slowly describes how he ran An adverb phrase is a group of one or more words fulfilling the function of an adverb; see p. 11 affix A morpheme which is attached to another word as an inflection or for derivation Affixes include prefixes at the beginning of a word and suffixes at the end of a word, e.g. un-god-ly with prefix un- and suffix -ly A derivational affix is used to form a new word, e.g. the suffix -less with hope gives the new word hopeless; an inflectional affix marks grammatical relations, in comes, the -s marks third person singular present indicative 159 160 Glossary alliteration The repetition of the same sound at the beginning of two or more words in close proximity, e.g.
    [Show full text]
  • Preposition Stranding Vs. Pied-Piping—The Role of Cognitive Complexity in Grammatical Variation
    languages Article Preposition Stranding vs. Pied-Piping—The Role of Cognitive Complexity in Grammatical Variation Christine Günther Faculty of Arts and Humanities, Universität Siegen, 57076 Siegen, Germany; [email protected] Abstract: Grammatical variation has often been said to be determined by cognitive complexity. Whenever they have the choice between two variants, speakers will use that form that is associated with less processing effort on the hearer’s side. The majority of studies putting forth this or similar analyses of grammatical variation are based on corpus data. Analyzing preposition stranding vs. pied-piping in English, this paper sets out to put the processing-based hypotheses to the test. It focuses on discontinuous prepositional phrases as opposed to their continuous counterparts in an online and an offline experiment. While pied-piping, the variant with a continuous PP, facilitates reading at the wh-element in restrictive relative clauses, a stranded preposition facilitates reading at the right boundary of the relative clause. Stranding is the preferred option in the same contexts. The heterogenous results underline the need for research on grammatical variation from various perspectives. Keywords: grammatical variation; complexity; preposition stranding; discontinuous constituents Citation: Günther, Christine. 2021. Preposition Stranding vs. Pied- 1. Introduction Piping—The Role of Cognitive Grammatical variation refers to phenomena where speakers have the choice between Complexity in Grammatical Variation. two (or more) semantically equivalent structural options. Even in English, a language with Languages 6: 89. https://doi.org/ rather rigid word order, some constructions allow for variation, such as the position of a 10.3390/languages6020089 particle, the ordering of post-verbal constituents or the position of a preposition.
    [Show full text]
  • Interrogating Possessive Have: a Case Study Argumentum 9 (2013), 99-107 Debreceni Egyetemi Kiadó
    99 József Andor: Interrogating possessive have: a case study Argumentum 9 (2013), 99-107 Debreceni Egyetemi Kiadó József Andor Interrogating possessive have: a case study Abstract Major, standard grammars of English give an account and interpret interrogatively used possessive have as a unique specialty of genres and text types of British English. Reviewing descriptions offered by some of these grammars and presenting empirically based evidence on acceptability of usage and function, the present paper offers results revealing the occurrence of inverted possessive have in other regional varieties, specifically in American English. It is suggested that have, retaining its possessive lexical meaning behaves as a semi-auxiliary in such constructions. Keywords: possessive, inversion, do-support, corpus-based, semi-auxiliary, notionally and morpho-syntactically based categorization 1 Introduction What made me start researching the functional-semantic and pragmatic-contextual force of interrogative sentences with the possessive lexical status of have was finding the example Have you a pen? on page 88 of the recently published Oxford Modern English Grammar authored by Bas Aarts (2011). The sentence was given under section 4.1.1.6. titled “Subjects invert positions with verbs in interrogative main clauses”, which section, due to its scope, did not address discussing syntactic variation concerning possessive usage of have, contrasting syntactic as well as cognitive-semantic and pragmatic, usage based issues of formally pure cases of inversion with the co-occurrence of have and do-support (also called do-periphrasis) or the have got construction. This came to me as a surprise, as types of have-based possession could have been discussed in a dictionary based on one of the most valuable corpora of British English, the British component of the International Corpus of English (ICE-GB).
    [Show full text]
  • *65 EØ RS Mitipse1e9tticaasa.56 9P
    1100&*430 ERIC RE PeRT .RESUNE IEO 010 -t-60 1W,29Hib 24 (REV) USSGE IIANUAL:44LANGUAGE CUUICIMUK. I AND !I TEACHER V ER&Iet4, ZHAER4ALURTAWR RaR610280 UNC.VERSITY OE ORE?, -EUGENE Cit9+1*114114411 1111409,40116.C431 . *65 EØ RS MiTipse1e9tticaasa.56 9P. *GRAMM-0..111GhTH- IIRAINEV.- sevens .'GRADEs. vitiCIINUCAILOCSUIDESS *TIEAZINING- SUIDESL 11-YEAL4IVIL 61/10Ett. TiENGLISK EWEN/v; MESONV4-PROJECIr'ENG11314-- RE* GRAM* A IIANUSL, GRINAR- WAGE WAS PRIEN*.f.a *OR- -TEACHINC,1semoffspoi, AND EIGHTIWERMSE- MOMS .CARINECULUNS.- THEP-MANUAt 11111S44R,-TEAT;NERWAIND CONTAINS1 96 ' GRIMMAR_ USAGEWITEett -*WOE 4-1111111), 4ItykiPROF1TSO1Y,-:TRENNIED: .4N SENENTWAID: E IGHTW GRAvegq: THE7COIITEInt-41ave-litst ,i4MIANGEO- ALAINACITICALLY-41101 A GAUt MOM '41F- -CROSSMIEFERENEU,-311EIMINUAL -414--STIODENT 4111.11NADVE_':OP-.4=UNISSORMATI.-4.14AL'IMAPSIAL--Te MEGRIM: -47 NTH SUER- -ALIPECTS 1iI E 1INGUSH 7CURRICARAIMIAN ACCOMPANYING MANUAL WAS ?NEPA-RED FOR -STUOEUT USE :4110,-010 :ant; IMO el OREGON etlittfAcilLatIM 5.xuuY CINTER **0 014 VELEM IL SiDEPARTMENT OFHEMplti: MICATION v-4 Office cif Echkgati On d**filthy has beenreproduced exactlyslitectile Co This document Points Of VieW OrOPielone organizationoriginating It. v-1 person or represent OMNIOff Ice etEducation stated donot necessarily CI position or policy. C) I USAGE ISAIWATe !ammo Curriculum I Mid II Teacher I/onion The project reported hereinwas -supported through the Cooperative ResearchPrograxa of- the Office of Education, U, S. Departgle4totilealt4s Education, and Welfare USAGE MANUAL TABLE OFCONTENTS Etas Abbreviations 1 Bust for burst Accent- Except 3. CapitalCapitol a Adjective 1 Capitalization 9 Adverb 2 Case 10 I Advice- Advise 2 clothsClothes 11 Affect- Effect 3 Colon 11 Agreement 3 Comma 11 Ain't 5 Almost-Most' 5 ConjUnction 11 Already-All ready 5 Contractions 12 All right 5 tOundil- Counsel 12 Altogether - All together 5 toUrse-Coarse 12 Among- Between 6 DetertDessert 12 ft.n-And 6 Determiner 13 Antecedent 6 ed -bove 13 t Apostrophe 6 Done foi-' did 18 Appositive 7 Negative 14 s.
    [Show full text]
  • Intelligent Tutoring Systems
    Se Design Recommendations for Intelligent Tutoring Systems Volume 2 Instructional Management Edited by: Robert A. Sottilare, Arthur C. Graesser, Xiangen Hu, and Benjamin S. Goldberg A Book in the Adaptive Tutoring Series Copyright © 2014 by the U.S. Army Research Laboratory. Copyright not claimed on material written by an employee of the U.S. Government. All rights reserved. No part of this book may be reproduced in any manner, print or electronic, without written permission of the copyright holder. The views expressed herein are those of the authors and do not necessarily reflect the views of the U.S. Army Research Laboratory. Use of trade names or names of commercial sources is for information only and does not imply endorsement by the U.S. Army Research Laboratory. This publication is intended to provide accurate information regarding the subject matter addressed herein. The information in this publication is subject to change at any time without notice. The U.S. Army Research Laboratory, nor the authors of the publication, makes any guarantees or warranties concerning the information contained herein. Printed in the United States of America First Printing, June 2014 First Printing (errata addressed), July 2014 U.S. Army Research Laboratory Human Research & Engineering Directorate SFC Paul Ray Smith Simulation & Training Technology Center Orlando, Florida International Standard Book Number: 978-0-9893923-2-7 We wish to acknowledge the editing and formatting contributions of Carol Johnson, ARL Dedicated to current and future scientists and developers of adaptive learning technologies CONTENTS Preface ..................................................................................................... i Prologue ............................................................................................... xv Section I: Affect, Engagement and Grit in Instructional Management (Sottilare) 1 Chapter 1 ‒ Thoughts on the Instructional Management of Affect, Engagement, and Grit ..................................................
    [Show full text]
  • Features of Lexical Verbs in the Discussion Section of Masters' Dissertations
    FEATURES OF LEXICAL VERBS IN THE DISCUSSION SECTION OF MASTERS' DISSERTATIONS FAIZAH BINTI MOHAMAD NUSRI FACULTY OF LANGUAGES AND LINGUISTICS UNIVERSITY OF MALAYA KUALA LUMPUR 2018 FEATURES OF LEXICAL VERBS IN THE DISCUSSION SECTION OF MASTERS' DISSERTATIONS FAIZAH BINTI MOHAMAD NUSRI DISSERTATION SUBMITTED IN PARTIAL FULFILMENT OF THE REQUIREMENTS FOR THE DEGREE OF MASTER OF ENGLISH AS SECOND LANGUAGE FACULTY OF LANGUAGE AND LINGUISTICS UNIVERSITY OF MALAYA KUALA LUMPUR 2018 UNIVERSITY OF MALAYA ORIGINAL LITERARY WORK DECLARATION Name of Candidate: Faizah binti Mohamad Nusri I.C/Passport No: 860610-38-6394 Matric No: TGB 120045 Name of Degree: Master of English as Second Language Title of Project Paper/Research Report/Dissertation/Thesis (“this Work”): Features of Lexical Verbs in the Discussion Section of Masters’ Dissertations Field of Study: Corpus Linguistics I do solemnly and sincerely declare that: (1) I am the sole author/writer of this Work; (2) This Work is original; (3) Any use of any work in which copyright exists was done by way of fair dealing and for permitted purposes and any excerpt or extract from, or reference to or reproduction of any copyright work has been disclosed expressly and sufficiently and the title of the Work and its authorship have been acknowledged in this Work; (4) I do not have any actual knowledge nor do I ought reasonably to know that the making of this work constitutes an infringement of any copyright work; (5) I hereby assign all and every rights in the copyright to this Work to the University of Malaya (“UM”), who henceforth shall be owner of the copyright in this Work and that any reproduction or use in any form or by any means whatsoever is prohibited without the written consent of UM having been first had and obtained; (6) I am fully aware that if in the course of making this Work I have infringed any copyright whether intentionally or otherwise, I may be subject to legal action or any other action as may be determined by UM.
    [Show full text]
  • Department of English and American Studies Phrasal Verbs in the British National Corpus and ELT Textbooks 2018
    Masaryk University Faculty of Arts Department of English and American Studies Teaching English Language and Literature for Secondary Schools Jan Štěrba Phrasal Verbs in the British National Corpus and ELT Textbooks Master’s Diploma Thesis Supervisor: Mgr. František Tůma, Ph. D. 2018 I declare that I have worked on this thesis independently, using only the primary and secondary sources listed in the bibliography. …………………………………………….. Jan Štěrba I would like to thank my supervisor Mgr. František Tůma, Ph.D., for his time, kindness, willingness to help and the excellent feedback he provided me with. I would also like to thank my original supervisor, doc, PhDr. Naděžda Kudrnáčová, Csc., who approached Mr. Tůma after my topic had to be changed and thus helped me to finish the thesis successfully. I cannot forget to thank James for proofreading the whole thesis as well as my grandfather who proofread the Czech parts. I am also very grateful for the support I have received from my family throughout my studies and finally, I have to thank my girlfriend Janča for simply being out there for me. Table of Contents Introduction ....................................................................................................................... 9 1. Multi-word Verbs ....................................................................................................... 13 1.1. Phrasal Verbs ....................................................................................................... 14 1.1.1. Definition ......................................................................................................
    [Show full text]
  • Fundamentals of English Syntax 1
    English Syntax Fundamentals of English Syntax 1. Introductory Concepts (Version 4; 06/11/2014) Andrew McIntyre 1.1. Syntactic categories This text is a brief introduction to syntax, the study of the structure of sentences. It introduces All words in a sentence belong to a particular part of speech or, in more modern parlance, the most important basic concepts, aiming to give readers an idea of syntactic phenomena category or syntactic category, like those in (1). and argumentation. Some analyses given below are simplified in order to make this text (1) Category Abbreviation Example accessible to beginners. Readers wishing to know about the more sophisticated analyses are a. noun N computer, city, stupidity, event, John, London directed to the book-length introductions to syntax listed in the bibliography. b. verb V hear, think, disagree, shorten, eavesdrop, exist Readers are welcome to send any questions and suggestions for improving the manuscript c. adjective A good, obscene, demented, lovely, schoolmasterly to [email protected]. d. preposition P off, by, in, with, from, to, at, inside, despite Contents e. adverb Adv slowly, often, now, mostly 1. Introductory Concepts ..................................................................................................... 2 f. determiner D (or Det) a, the, this, those1 1.1. Syntactic categories ................................................................................................. 2 Here we will not try to give a set of completely failsafe criteria for determining the category 1.2. Constituents ............................................................................................................. 2 a word, but will describe some issues arising in defining the various categories. One kind of 1.3. Tests for identifying constituents ............................................................................. 3 criterion is semantic, i.e. based on meaning. Such criteria take the form of statements such as 1.4.
    [Show full text]
  • Chapter 36. Grammatical Change
    Chapter 36. Grammatical change Willem B. Hollmann 36.1 Preliminaries Grammatical change is an extremely interesting area but does presuppose some knowledge of grammar. For this reason, if you do not have this knowledge yet, this chapter will work best if you have first familiarised yourself with the terminology discussed in the chapters on grammar, i.e. especially chapters and 5–8. Chapters on grammatical change in traditional textbooks on the history of English or historical linguistics by and large focus on change in morphology (the structure of words) and/or syntax (the structure of phrases and clauses). Some of the most important changes in the history of English can indeed be described in more or less purely structural terms, i.e. without considering meaning very much. Sentences (1–2), below, illustrate object-verb (or OV) constituent order,1 which was quite frequent in Old English but became less and less common during Middle English, until it was completely lost in the sixteenth century. (1) Gregorius [hine object] [afligde verb]. (Homilies of Ælfric 22.624) Gregory him put-to-flight ‘Gregory made him flee’ (2) Ne sceal he [naht unaliefedes object] [don verb]. (Pastoral Care 10.61.14) not shall he nothing unlawful do ‘He will do nothing unlawful’ Example (2) shows that we must be careful when discussing OV order in Old and Middle English: the finite verb (here: sceal) actually often occurred before the object, taking up the second position in the clause (the initial position here being occupied by the negator ne). Thus, it is often only the lexical verb (here: don) that follows the object.
    [Show full text]
  • Generating Anagrams from Multiple Core Strings Employing User-Defined Vocabularies and Orthographic Parameters
    Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers 2003, 35 (1), 129-135 Generating anagrams from multiple core strings employing user-defined vocabularies and orthographic parameters TIMOTHY R. JORDAN and AXEL MONTEIRO University of Nottingham, Nottingham, England Anagrams are used widely in psychological research. However, generatinga range of strings with the same lettercontent is an inherently difficult and time-consuming task for humans, and current computer- based anagram generatorsdo not provide the controls necessaryfor psychological research.In this ar- ticle,we present a computational algorithm that overcomes these problems. Specifically,the algorithm processes automatically each word in a user-defined source vocabulary and outputs, for each word, all possible anagrams that exist as words (or as nonwords, if required) as defined by the same source vo- cabulary. Moreover, we show how the output of the algorithm can be filtered to produce anagrams within specificuser-definedorthographic parameters.For example, the anagramsproduced can be filtered to produce words that share, with each other or with other words in the source vocabulary, letters in only certain positions. Finally, we provide free access to the complete Windows-based program and source code containing these facilitiesfor anagram generation. Anagrams play an important and pervasive role in psy- ences of linguistic category (e.g., frequency, imageabil- chological research. For example, anagrams provide a ity, concreteness, orthographic structure, lexicality) on measure
    [Show full text]