Jointly Improving Parsing and Perception for Natural Language

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Jointly Improving Parsing and Perception for Natural Language Jointly Improving Parsing and Perception for Natural Language Commands through Human-Robot Dialog Jesse Thomason,∗ Aishwarya Padmakumar,y Jivko Sinapov,z Nick Walker,∗ Yuqian Jiang,y Harel Yedidsion,y Justin Hart,y Peter Stone,y and Raymond J. Mooneyy ∗Paul G. Allen School of Computer Science and Engineering, University of Washington yDepartment of Computer Science, University of Texas at Austin zDepartment of Computer Science, Tufts University Abstract In this work, we present methods for parsing natural language to underlying meanings, and using robotic sensors to create multi-modal models of perceptual concepts. We combine these steps to- wards language understanding into a holis- tic agent for jointly improving parsing and perception on a robotic platform through human-robot dialog. We train and eval- uate this agent on Amazon Mechanical Turk, then demonstrate it on a robotic plat- Figure 1: Mechanical Turk web interface. form initialized from that conversational data. Our experiments show that improv- ing both parsing and perception compo- work (Thomason et al., 2015), strengthening its nents from conversations improves com- parsing over time. The agent also uses oppor- munication quality and human ratings of tunistic active learning (Thomason et al., 2017) the agent. to ask questions about nearby objects to refine multi-modal perceptual concept models (Thoma- 1 Introduction son et al., 2016) on-the-fly during command di- alogs. Pre-programming robots with fixed language un- We evaluate this language understanding agent derstanding components limits them, since dif- on Mechanical Turk (Figure1), and implement the ferent speakers and environments use and elicit agent on a physical robot.1 different words. Rather than force humans to use language that robots around them can 2 Conversational Agent understand, robots should dynamically adapt— continually learning new language constructions We implement and evaluate a conversational di- and perceptual concepts as they are used in con- alog agent that uses a semantic parser to translate text. human utterances into semantic meaning represen- Untrained human users providing natural lan- tations, then grounds those meaning representa- guage commands to robots expect world knowl- tions using both a static knowledge base of facts edge, perceptual knowledge, and semantic under- about an office environment and perceptual con- standing from verbal robots. We present a holistic cept models that consider multi-modal representa- 2 system for jointly improving parsing and percep- tions of physical objects. tion on a robotic system for natural language com- We build multi-modal concept models to con- mands through human-robot dialog. This learn- 1The demonstration video can be viewed at https:// ing agent uses clarification questions in a conver- youtu.be/PbOfteZ_CJc sation with a human partner to understand lan- 2The source code for this conversational dialog agent, as well as the experiments described in the follow- guage commands. The agent induces additional ing section, can be found at https://github.com/ training data for a semantic parser, similar to prior thomason-jesse/grounded_dialog_agent tion dialog to refine that belief. When describing objects in the real world, hu- mans can use words the agent has never heard be- fore. In this work, we examine the lexical neigh- bors of unknown words in word-embedding space, and ask the user directly whether the unseen words are perceptual in nature if any neighbors are. We Figure 2: The robot used in our experiment, and then check for synonyms (e.g., users marked tall the objects explored by the robot for grounding as a synonym for long), and add a new perceptual perceptual predicates. concept if no known words are suitable synonyms (e.g., red, a neighbor of yellow, was added in our experiments in this way). nect robot perception to concept labels. We use multi-modal feature representations across various We introduce opportunistic active learning sensorimotor contexts by exploring those objects questions as a sub-dialog routine for the agent, in with a robot arm, performing behaviors such as which it can query about objects local to the hu- grasping, lifting, and dropping objects, in addi- man and the robot to refine its perceptual concept tion to extracting visual information from them models before applying them to the remote test (Figure2)(Sinapov et al., 2016; Thomason et al., object items, similar to previous work (Thomason 2016). et al., 2017). We connect these feature representations of ob- 3 Experiments jects to language labels by learning discriminative classifiers on the feature spaces for each percep- Mechanical Turk users instruct the agent in three tual language concept (e.g., red or heavy)(Sinapov tasks: navigation, delivery, and relocation. We de- et al., 2014; Thomason et al., 2016). ploy the agent in a simulated office environment Since there are multiple possible groundings for populated by rooms, people, and object items. We ambiguous utterances like the office and varied randomly split the set of possible tasks into initial- confidences for perceptual concept models on dif- ization (10%), train (70%), and test sets (20%). ferent objects, we create a confidence distribution Sixteen users (graduate students at the univer- over the possible groundings for a semantic parse. sity across several fields) engaged with a faux- This confidence probability distribution is used as agent on initialization set tasks using the web in- part of an update procedure for helping the agent terface. We used these commands as a scaffold understand the user’s intent during dialog. on which to build an ontology, lexicon, and initial We implement a conversational dialog agent for utterance-semantic parse pairs. Of them, 44 pairs, command understanding similar to that in previ- D0, were used to train an initial parser. ous work (Thomason et al., 2015). The differences We use these initial parsing resources to create between this agent and the previous one are: 1) a baseline agent A1 with a parser P1 trained only grounding semantic parses in both static knowl- on the initialization pairs D0 mentioned above and edge and perceptual knowledge; 2) dynamically concept models for several predicates Pc;1, but adding new ontological predicates for novel per- with no initial object examples against which to ceptual concepts; and 3) leveraging opportunis- train them. tic active learning for refining perceptual concept Three training phases i are carried out by agent models on-the-fly. Ai, after which parser Pi+1 and concept predi- Dialog begins with a human user commanding cates Pc;i+1 are trained to instantiate agent Ai+1. the robot to perform a task. The agent maintains Agent A4 with parser P4 and perception models a belief state modeling the unobserved true task Pc;4 is tested by interacting with users trying to in the user’s mind, and uses the language sig- accomplish tasks from the unseen test set of tasks. ∗ nals from the user to infer it. The command is We also test an ablation agent, A4, with parser ∗ first parsed by the agent’s semantic parser, then P1 and perception models Pc;4 (trained perception grounded against static and perceptual knowledge. with simply initialized parser). These groundings are used to update the agent’s Table1 gives a breakdown of the numbers of belief state, and the agent engages in a clarifica- workers who engaged with our HITs. Condition Number of Workers Submitted Completed Vetted Nav. Del. Rel. HIT Tasks Correct Correct Correct Train (A1;A2;A3) 297 162 113 36 44 18 Untrained (A1) 150 67 44 17 22 10 ∗ ∗ Test (A4) 148 83 50 20 29 10 Test (A4) 143 79 42 16 23 10 Table 1: Breakdown of the number of workers in our experiment. We here count only workers that submitted the HIT with the correct code. Workers that completed all tasks and the survey finished the HIT entirely. Vetted workers’ data was kept for evaluation after basic checks. The Train condition ∗ (A1;A2;A3 agents) draws from the training set of tasks, while the Untrained (A1 untrained agent), Test ∗ (A4 agent with trained perception and untrained parser), and Test (A4 agent with trained parser and perception) conditions draw from the test set of tasks. For our embodied demonstration, we use Learned Concept Model for can the BWIBot (Khandelwal et al., 2014, 2017), equipped with a Kinova MICO arm, an Xtion ASUS Pro camera, a Hokuyo lidar, a Blue Snow- ball microphone, and a speaker. Speech transcripts 0.32 0.22 0.2 0.13 are provided by the Google Speech API3 and speech synthesis is performed with the Festival 4 Speech Synthesis System. Tabletop perception, 0.07 0.03 0.03 0 required for both the dialog interaction and the ex- ecution of the resulting command, is implemented Figure 3: The perceptual concept model learned with RANSAC (Fischler and Bolles, 1981) plane for can after training from conversations with hu- fitting and Euclidean clustering as provided by man users. Point Cloud Library (Rusu and Cousins, 2011). Figure4 gives quantitative and qualitative mea- sures of performance in Mechanical Turk, as well non-visual word rattling. as some snapshots from the embodied demonstra- tion. The agent acquires new perceptual concept Acknowledgements models (25 in total), and synonym words for ex- isting concepts, during the three phases of train- This work was supported by a National Science ing. Table3 shows the learned perceptual concept Foundation Graduate Research Fellowship to the model for can on test objects. first author, an NSF EAGER grant (IIS-1548567), 4 Conclusion and an NSF NRI grant (IIS-1637736). Portions of this work have taken place in the Learning In this article, we presented a holistic system for Agents Research Group (LARG) at UT Austin. jointly improving semantic parsing and grounded LARG research is supported in part by NSF perception on a robotic system for interpreting nat- (CNS-1305287, IIS-1637736, IIS-1651089, IIS- ural language commands during human-robot dia- 1724157), TxDOT, Intel, Raytheon, and Lockheed log.
Recommended publications
  • What the Neurocognitive Study of Inner Language Reveals About Our Inner Space Hélène Loevenbruck
    What the neurocognitive study of inner language reveals about our inner space Hélène Loevenbruck To cite this version: Hélène Loevenbruck. What the neurocognitive study of inner language reveals about our inner space. Epistémocritique, épistémocritique : littérature et savoirs, 2018, Langage intérieur - Espaces intérieurs / Inner Speech - Inner Space, 18. hal-02039667 HAL Id: hal-02039667 https://hal.archives-ouvertes.fr/hal-02039667 Submitted on 20 Sep 2019 HAL is a multi-disciplinary open access L’archive ouverte pluridisciplinaire HAL, est archive for the deposit and dissemination of sci- destinée au dépôt et à la diffusion de documents entific research documents, whether they are pub- scientifiques de niveau recherche, publiés ou non, lished or not. The documents may come from émanant des établissements d’enseignement et de teaching and research institutions in France or recherche français ou étrangers, des laboratoires abroad, or from public or private research centers. publics ou privés. Preliminary version produced by the author. In Lœvenbruck H. (2008). Épistémocritique, n° 18 : Langage intérieur - Espaces intérieurs / Inner Speech - Inner Space, Stéphanie Smadja, Pierre-Louis Patoine (eds.) [http://epistemocritique.org/what-the-neurocognitive- study-of-inner-language-reveals-about-our-inner-space/] - hal-02039667 What the neurocognitive study of inner language reveals about our inner space Hélène Lœvenbruck Université Grenoble Alpes, CNRS, Laboratoire de Psychologie et NeuroCognition (LPNC), UMR 5105, 38000, Grenoble France Abstract Our inner space is furnished, and sometimes even stuffed, with verbal material. The nature of inner language has long been under the careful scrutiny of scholars, philosophers and writers, through the practice of introspection. The use of recent experimental methods in the field of cognitive neuroscience provides a new window of insight into the format, properties, qualities and mechanisms of inner language.
    [Show full text]
  • CNS 2014 Program
    Cognitive Neuroscience Society 21st Annual Meeting, April 5-8, 2014 Marriott Copley Place Hotel, Boston, Massachusetts 2014 Annual Meeting Program Contents 2014 Committees & Staff . 2 Schedule Overview . 3 . Keynotes . 5 2014 George A . Miller Awardee . 6. Distinguished Career Contributions Awardee . 7 . Young Investigator Awardees . 8 . General Information . 10 Exhibitors . 13 . Invited-Symposium Sessions . 14 Mini-Symposium Sessions . 18 Poster Schedule . 32. Poster Session A . 33 Poster Session B . 66 Poster Session C . 98 Poster Session D . 130 Poster Session E . 163 Poster Session F . 195 . Poster Session G . 227 Poster Topic Index . 259. Author Index . 261 . Boston Marriott Copley Place Floorplan . 272. A Supplement of the Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience Cognitive Neuroscience Society c/o Center for the Mind and Brain 267 Cousteau Place, Davis, CA 95616 ISSN 1096-8857 © CNS www.cogneurosociety.org 2014 Committees & Staff Governing Board Mini-Symposium Committee Roberto Cabeza, Ph.D., Duke University David Badre, Ph.D., Brown University (Chair) Marta Kutas, Ph.D., University of California, San Diego Adam Aron, Ph.D., University of California, San Diego Helen Neville, Ph.D., University of Oregon Lila Davachi, Ph.D., New York University Daniel Schacter, Ph.D., Harvard University Elizabeth Kensinger, Ph.D., Boston College Michael S. Gazzaniga, Ph.D., University of California, Gina Kuperberg, Ph.D., Harvard University Santa Barbara (ex officio) Thad Polk, Ph.D., University of Michigan George R. Mangun, Ph.D., University of California,
    [Show full text]
  • Talking to Computers in Natural Language
    feature Talking to Computers in Natural Language Natural language understanding is as old as computing itself, but recent advances in machine learning and the rising demand of natural-language interfaces make it a promising time to once again tackle the long-standing challenge. By Percy Liang DOI: 10.1145/2659831 s you read this sentence, the words on the page are somehow absorbed into your brain and transformed into concepts, which then enter into a rich network of previously-acquired concepts. This process of language understanding has so far been the sole privilege of humans. But the universality of computation, Aas formalized by Alan Turing in the early 1930s—which states that any computation could be done on a Turing machine—offers a tantalizing possibility that a computer could understand language as well. Later, Turing went on in his seminal 1950 article, “Computing Machinery and Intelligence,” to propose the now-famous Turing test—a bold and speculative method to evaluate cial intelligence at the time. Daniel Bo- Figure 1). SHRDLU could both answer whether a computer actually under- brow built a system for his Ph.D. thesis questions and execute actions, for ex- stands language (or more broadly, is at MIT to solve algebra word problems ample: “Find a block that is taller than “intelligent”). While this test has led to found in high-school algebra books, the one you are holding and put it into the development of amusing chatbots for example: “If the number of custom- the box.” In this case, SHRDLU would that attempt to fool human judges by ers Tom gets is twice the square of 20% first have to understand that the blue engaging in light-hearted banter, the of the number of advertisements he runs, block is the referent and then perform grand challenge of developing serious and the number of advertisements is 45, the action by moving the small green programs that can truly understand then what is the numbers of customers block out of the way and then lifting the language in useful ways remains wide Tom gets?” [1].
    [Show full text]
  • Revealing the Language of Thought Brent Silby 1
    Revealing the Language of Thought Brent Silby 1 Revealing the Language of Thought An e-book by BRENT SILBY This paper was produced at the Department of Philosophy, University of Canterbury, New Zealand Copyright © Brent Silby 2000 Revealing the Language of Thought Brent Silby 2 Contents Abstract Chapter 1: Introduction Chapter 2: Thinking Sentences 1. Preliminary Thoughts 2. The Language of Thought Hypothesis 3. The Map Alternative 4. Problems with Mentalese Chapter 3: Installing New Technology: Natural Language and the Mind 1. Introduction 2. Language... what's it for? 3. Natural Language as the Language of Thought 4. What can we make of the evidence? Chapter 4: The Last Stand... Don't Replace The Old Code Yet 1. The Fight for Mentalese 2. Pinker's Resistance 3. Pinker's Continued Resistance 4. A Concluding Thought about Thought Chapter 5: A Direction for Future Thought 1. The Review 2. The Conclusion 3. Expanding the mind beyond the confines of the biological brain References / Acknowledgments Revealing the Language of Thought Brent Silby 3 Abstract Language of thought theories fall primarily into two views. The first view sees the language of thought as an innate language known as mentalese, which is hypothesized to operate at a level below conscious awareness while at the same time operating at a higher level than the neural events in the brain. The second view supposes that the language of thought is not innate. Rather, the language of thought is natural language. So, as an English speaker, my language of thought would be English. My goal is to defend the second view.
    [Show full text]
  • Natural Language Processing (NLP) for Requirements Engineering: a Systematic Mapping Study
    Natural Language Processing (NLP) for Requirements Engineering: A Systematic Mapping Study Liping Zhao† Department of Computer Science, The University of Manchester, Manchester, United Kingdom, [email protected] Waad Alhoshan Department of Computer Science, The University of Manchester, Manchester, United Kingdom, [email protected] Alessio Ferrari Consiglio Nazionale delle Ricerche, Istituto di Scienza e Tecnologie dell'Informazione "A. Faedo" (CNR-ISTI), Pisa, Italy, [email protected] Keletso J. Letsholo Faculty of Computer Information Science, Higher Colleges of Technology, Abu Dhabi, United Arab Emirates, [email protected] Muideen A. Ajagbe Department of Computer Science, The University of Manchester, Manchester, United Kingdom, [email protected] Erol-Valeriu Chioasca Exgence Ltd., CORE, Manchester, United Kingdom, [email protected] Riza T. Batista-Navarro Department of Computer Science, The University of Manchester, Manchester, United Kingdom, [email protected] † Corresponding author. Context: NLP4RE – Natural language processing (NLP) supported requirements engineering (RE) – is an area of research and development that seeks to apply NLP techniques, tools and resources to a variety of requirements documents or artifacts to support a range of linguistic analysis tasks performed at various RE phases. Such tasks include detecting language issues, identifying key domain concepts and establishing traceability links between requirements. Objective: This article surveys the landscape of NLP4RE research to understand the state of the art and identify open problems. Method: The systematic mapping study approach is used to conduct this survey, which identified 404 relevant primary studies and reviewed them according to five research questions, cutting across five aspects of NLP4RE research, concerning the state of the literature, the state of empirical research, the research focus, the state of the practice, and the NLP technologies used.
    [Show full text]
  • Detecting Politeness in Natural Language by Michael Yeomans, Alejandro Kantor, Dustin Tingley
    CONTRIBUTED RESEARCH ARTICLES 489 The politeness Package: Detecting Politeness in Natural Language by Michael Yeomans, Alejandro Kantor, Dustin Tingley Abstract This package provides tools to extract politeness markers in English natural language. It also allows researchers to easily visualize and quantify politeness between groups of documents. This package combines and extends prior research on the linguistic markers of politeness (Brown and Levinson, 1987; Danescu-Niculescu-Mizil et al., 2013; Voigt et al., 2017). We demonstrate two applications for detecting politeness in natural language during consequential social interactions— distributive negotiations, and speed dating. Introduction Politeness is a universal dimension of human communication (Goffman, 1967; Lakoff, 1973; Brown and Levinson, 1987). In practically all settings, a speaker can choose to be more or less polite to their audience, and this can have consequences for the speakers’ social goals. Politeness is encoded in a discrete and rich set of linguistic markers that modify the information content of an utterance. Sometimes politeness is an act of commission (for example, saying “please” and “thank you”) <and sometimes it is an act of omission (for example, declining to be contradictory). Furthermore, the mapping of politeness markers often varies by culture, by context (work vs. family vs. friends), by a speaker’s characteristics (male vs. female), or the goals (buyer vs. seller). Many branches of social science might be interested in how (and when) people express politeness to one another, as one mechanism by which social co-ordination is achieved. The politeness package is designed to make it easier to detect politeness in English natural language, by quantifying relevant characteristics of polite speech, and comparing them to other data about the speaker.
    [Show full text]
  • Natural Language Processing
    Natural Language Processing Liz Liddy (lead), Eduard Hovy, Jimmy Lin, John Prager, Dragomir Radev, Lucy Vanderwende, Ralph Weischedel This report is one of five reports that were based on the MINDS workshops, led by Donna Harman (NIST) and sponsored by Heather McCallum-Bayliss of the Disruptive Technology Office of the Office of the Director of National Intelligence's Office of Science and Technology (ODNI/ADDNI/S&T/DTO). To find the rest of the reports, and an executive overview, please see http://www.itl.nist.gov/iaui/894.02/minds.html. Historic Paradigm Shifts or Shift-Enablers A series of discoveries and developments over the past years has resulted in major shifts in the discipline of Natural Language Processing. Some have been more influential than others, but each is recognizable today as having had major impact, although this might not have been seen at the time. Some have been shift-enablers, rather than actual shifts in methods or approaches, but these have caused as major a change in how the discipline accomplishes its work. Very early on in the emergence of the field of NLP, there were demonstrations that NLP could develop operational systems with real levels of linguistic processing, in truly end-to-end (although toy) systems. These included SHRDLU by Winograd (1971) and LUNAR by Woods (1970). Each could accomplish a specific task — manipulating blocks in the blocks world or answering questions about samples from the moon. They were able to accomplish their limited goals because they included all the levels of language processing in their interactions with humans, including morphological, lexical, syntactic, semantic, discourse and pragmatic.
    [Show full text]
  • “Linguistic Analysis” As a Misnomer, Or, Why Linguistics Is in a State of Permanent Crisis
    Alexander Kravchenko Russia, Baikal National University of Economics and Law, Irkutsk “Linguistic Analysis” as a Misnomer, or, Why Linguistics is in a State of Permanent Crisis Key words: methodology, written-language bias, biology of language, consensual domain, language function Ключевые слова: методология, письменноязыковая предвзятость, биология языка, консенсуальная область, функция языка Аннотация Показывается ошибочность общепринятого понятия лингвистического анализа как анализа естественного языка. Следствием соссюровского структурализма и фило- софии объективного реализма является взгляд на язык как на вещь (код) – систему знаков, используемых как инструмент для коммуникации, под которой понимается обмен информацией. При таком подходе анализ в традиционной лингвистике пред- ставляет собой анализ вещей – письменных слов, предложений, текстов; все они являются культурными артефактами и не принадлежат сфере естественного языка как видоспецифичного социального поведения, биологическая функция которого состоит в ориентировании себя и других в когнитивной области взаимодействий. До тех пор, пока наукой не будет отвергнута кодовая модель языка, лингвистический анализ будет продолжать обслуживать языковой миф, удерживая лингвистику в со- стоянии перманентного кризиса. 1. Th e problem with linguistics as a science There is a growing feeling of uneasiness in the global community of linguists about the current state of linguistics as a science. Despite a lot of academic activity in the fi eld, and massive research refl ected in the ever growing number of publications related to the study of the various aspects of language, there hasn’t been a major breakthrough in the explorations of language as a unique endowment of humans. 26 Alexander Kravchenko Numerous competing theoretical frameworks and approaches in linguistic re- search continue to obscure the obvious truth that there is very little understanding of language as a phenomenon which sets humans apart from all other known biological species.
    [Show full text]
  • Natural Language Processing and Language Learning
    To appear 2012 in Encyclopedia of Applied Linguistics, edited by Carol A. Chapelle. Wiley Blackwell Natural Language Processing and Language Learning Detmar Meurers Universität Tübingen [email protected] As a relatively young field of research and development started by work on cryptanalysis and machine translation around 50 years ago, Natural Language Processing (NLP) is concerned with the automated processing of human language. It addresses the analysis and generation of written and spoken language, though speech processing is often regarded as a separate subfield. NLP emphasizes processing and applications and as such can be seen as the applied side of Computational Linguistics, the interdisciplinary field of research concerned with for- mal analysis and modeling of language and its applications at the intersection of Linguistics, Computer Science, and Psychology. In terms of the language aspects dealt with in NLP, traditionally lexical, morphological and syntactic aspects of language were at the center of attention, but aspects of meaning, discourse, and the relation to the extra-linguistic context have become increasingly prominent in the last decade. A good introduction and overview of the field is provided in Jurafsky & Martin (2009). This article explores the relevance and uses of NLP in the context of language learning, focusing on written language. As a concise characterization of this emergent subfield, the discussion will focus on motivating the relevance, characterizing the techniques, and delin- eating the uses of NLP; more historical background and discussion can be found in Nerbonne (2003) and Heift & Schulze (2007). One can distinguish two broad uses of NLP in this context: On the one hand, NLP can be used to analyze learner language, i.e., words, sentences, or texts produced by language learners.
    [Show full text]
  • LDL-2014 3Rd Workshop on Linked Data in Linguistics
    3rd Workshop on Linked Data in Linguistics: Multilingual Knowledge Resources and Natural Language Processing Workshop Programme 08:30 - 09:00 – Opening and Introduction by Workshop Chair(s) 09:00 – 10:00 – Invited Talk Piek Vossen, The Collaborative Inter-Lingual-Index for harmonizing wordnets 10:00 – 10:30 – Session 1: Modeling Lexical-Semantic Resources with lemon Andon Tchechmedjiev, Gilles Sérasset, Jérôme Goulian and Didier Schwab, Attaching Translations to Proper Lexical Senses in DBnary 10:30 – 11:00 Coffee break 11:00-11:20– Session 1: Modeling Lexical-Semantic Resources with lemon John Philip McCrae, Christiane Fellbaum and Philipp Cimiano, Publishing and Linking WordNet using lemon and RDF 11:20-11:40– Session 1: Modeling Lexical-Semantic Resources with lemon Andrey Kutuzov and Maxim Ionov, Releasing genre keywords of Russian movie descriptions as Linguistic Linked Open Data: an experience report 11:40-12:00– Session 2: Metadata Matej Durco and Menzo Windhouwer, From CLARIN Component Metadata to Linked Open Data 12:00-12:20– Session 2: Metadata Gary Lefman, David Lewis and Felix Sasaki, A Brief Survey of Multimedia Annotation Localisation on the Web of Linked Data 12:20-12:50– Session 2: Metadata Daniel Jettka, Karim Kuropka, Cristina Vertan and Heike Zinsmeister, Towards a Linked Open Data Representation of a Grammar Terms Index 12:50-13:00 – Poster slam – Data Challenge 13:00 – 14:00 Lunch break 14:00 – 15:00 – Invited Talk Gerard de Mello, From Linked Data to Tightly Integrated Data 15:00 – 15:30 – Section 3: Crosslinguistic
    [Show full text]
  • A Joint Model of Language and Perception for Grounded Attribute Learning
    A Joint Model of Language and Perception for Grounded Attribute Learning Cynthia Matuszek [email protected] Nicholas FitzGerald [email protected] Luke Zettlemoyer [email protected] Liefeng Bo [email protected] Dieter Fox [email protected] Computer Science and Engineering, Box 352350, University of Washington, Seattle, WA 98195-2350 Abstract ical workspace that contains a number of objects that As robots become more ubiquitous and ca- vary in shape and color. We assume that a robot can pable, it becomes ever more important for understand sentences like this if it can solve the as- untrained users to easily interact with them. sociated grounded object selection task. Specifically, it Recently, this has led to study of the lan- must realize that words such as \yellow" and \blocks" guage grounding problem, where the goal refer to object attributes, and ground the meaning of is to extract representations of the mean- such words by mapping them to a perceptual system ings of natural language tied to the physi- that will enable it to identify the specific physical ob- cal world. We present an approach for joint jects referred to. To do so robustly, even in cases where learning of language and perception models words or attributes are new, our robot must learn (1) for grounded attribute induction. The per- visual classifiers that identify the appropriate object ception model includes classifiers for phys- properties, (2) representations of the meaning of indi- ical characteristics and a language model vidual words that incorporate these classifiers, and (3) based on a probabilistic categorial grammar a model of compositional semantics used to analyze that enables the construction of composi- complete sentences.
    [Show full text]
  • From Natural Language to Programming Language
    110 Chapter 4 From Natural Language to Programming Language Xiao Liu Pennsylvania State University, USA Dinghao Wu Pennsylvania State University, USA ABSTRACT Programming remains a dark art for beginners or even professional programmers. Experience indicates that one of the first barriers for learning a new programming language is the rigid and unnatural syntax and semantics. After analysis of research on the language features used by non-programmers in describing problem solving, the authors propose a new program synthesis framework, dialog-based programming, which interprets natural language descriptions into computer programs without forcing the input formats. In this chapter, they describe three case studies that demonstrate the functionalities of this program synthesis framework and show how natural language alleviates challenges for novice programmers to conduct software development, scripting, and verification. INTRODUCTION Programming languages are formal languages with precise instructions for different software development purposes such as software implementation and verification. Due to its conciseness, the absence of redundancy causes less ambiguity in describing problems but on the other hand, reduces the expressiveness. Since the early days of automatic computing, researchers have considered the shortcomings DOI: 10.4018/978-1-5225-5969-6.ch004 Copyright © 2018, IGI Global. Copying or distributing in print or electronic forms without written permission of IGI Global is prohibited. From Natural Language to Programming Language that programming requires to accommodate the precision with the adoption of formal symbolism (Myers, Pane, & Ko, 2004). They have been exploring techniques that could help untrained and lightly trained users to write programming code in a more natural way, and natural programming is then proposed (Biermann, 1983; Pollock, Vijay-Shanker, Hill, Sridhara, & Shepherd, 2013).
    [Show full text]