Between Both of These and the Process of Short Time, and He

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Between Both of These and the Process of Short Time, and He 1 DOCUMENT RESUME AL 001 322 ED 021 218 48 By- McNeill, David THE DEVELOPMENT OF LANGUAGE Behavior. Michigan Univ., Ann Arbor. Centerfcr Research on Language and Language Spons Agency- Office of Education(DHEW), Washington, D.C. Bureauof Research Bureau No- BR- 6-1784 Pub Date 1 Feb 68 Con trac t OEC- 3- 6-061784- 0508 Behavior, Progress Report No.VI. Note-226p.; Supplementary Report toStudies in Language and Language EDRS Price MF-S1.00 HC-$9.12 DEVELOPMENT, PHONOLOGY, Descriptors-*CHILD DEVELOPMENT, *CHILD LANGUAGE *LANGUAGE SYNTAX, TRANSFORMATIONS (LANGUAGE), *PSYCHOLINGUISTICS,SEMANTICS SURFACE STRUCTURE *VERBAL DEVELOPMENT This chapter, to be included in"Carmichael's Manual of ChildPsychology," edited by P.A. Mussen, deals with the connectionbetween the acquisitionof language and the growth of intellect, and the connectionbetween both of these andthe process of account for maturation. The authorfeels that various theoriesof development cannot the child's acquisition of grammarin a relativelyshort time, and hediscusses the bulk of the chapter reasons aswell as the lines which theexplanation must follow. The is a survey oflanguage acquisition itself.It is organized underthree major headings, of a grammar: syntax,phonology, and one for eachof the three main components of semantics. A descriptionof the methods typically used instudying the development The emergence of the componentsthemselves is traced each component is given. light of (insofar as it is known), and adiscussion of varioustheoretical issues in the of children the empirical findings ispresented. Wherever possible,mention is made exposed to languages otherthan English, with thechief contrast languagesbeing Russian and Japanese. (Seerelated document ED 017921.) (DO) -05C-.3-6-06/7047-1).5-08 e-r44111)1e.2146/F 'THEUNIVERSITY OFMICHIGAII i3X 6- 17P/ ?1,s1 011 Studies in Language and Intot - .7. Language - ;1...... 40.4,` Behavior vf 14144 , ," /I 4.01' 4.4 a.. 4. 44040apata, U.S. DEPARTMENT OF HEALTH,EDUCATION & WELFARE ,4 OFFICE OF EDUCATION FROM THE THIS DOCUMENT HAS BEENREPRODUCED EXACTLY AS RECEIVED POINTS Of VIEW OR OPINIONS PERSON OR ORGANIZATIONORIGINATING IT. EDUCATION STATED DO NOT NECESSARILYREPRESENT OFFICIAL OFFICE OF :011,014`.. POSITION OR POLICY. VI SUPPLEMENT TOPROGRESS REPORT .11 The Developmentof Language DavlidgeNel.u, -arr. 4,4. "4.11.4;1104... a S a*M4.4144, ,146,1111... A... Ite! at. *' ENTER FOR RESEARCHON LANGUAGE,ANDLANGUAGE BEHAVIOR kt la001 322 CITY CENTER BUILDING 220 EAST HURON STREET ANN ARBOR, MICHIGAN 48108 AC 313 - 764-0510 THE DEVELOPMENT OFLANGUAGE* David McNeill The University of Michigan and The Center for CognitiveStudies Harvard University A chapter prepared for P.A. Mussen(Ed.), Carmichael's Manual aLchilc_Usychology. The work was supported by Grant # 5P01 HD 01368-03 from the National Institutefor Child Health and Human Development, and by Contract# OEC-3-6-061784-0508 from the U.S. Offite of Education, both withThe University of Michigarriand bythe Advanced Research Project Agency# SD-187, and by the National ScienceFoundation 111 # GS1153, both with HarvardUniversity. Miss Meredith Fink of TheUniversity of Michigan performed stalwartly in preparingbibliographic information; Mr. J.W. MacDonald of Harvard University washelpful on phonological matters; Mrs. Nobuko B. McNeill was helpful onall other matters; and Mrs. Mavis Atamian provided excellent secretarialservice. The Development of Language David McNeill The University of Michigan Page PLAN OF THE CHAPTER 2 A caveat 3 SYNTAX 3 Nativism and empiricism in developmental psycholinguistics.... 4 The problem of cognition and language 5 Methodology and methodological issues 7 Distributional vs. particular analyses 8 Studies of comprehension 16 The Berkeley Cross-Cultural Project 20 Theories of language acquisition and early speech 21 3 Telegraphic speech 25 Holophrastic speech 26 Holophrastic speech as predication OP 29 Pivot and Open classes 31 Early grammatical rules 34 Contextual generalization 42 Differentiation and generic classification 46 Linguistic capacities, linguistic 'universals, and the problem of abstraction ..... 47 Predication and the basic grammatical relations ...., 52 Intrinsic and extrinsic predication 57 The differentiation of grammatical categories 62 Summary 72 The acquisition of veansformations 75 The emergence of inflections 76 Overgeneralization, simplification, and the question of overt practice 78 Imitation 83 Negation in the acquisition of English 87 The development of questions 101 Imitation 108 Expansions and "prompts." 109 474) A general condition for learning transformations 114 Universal transformational relations 115 PaSe 120 SEMANTICS ,. 121 Semantic features , 124 Semantic anomaly 19.9 Why is semanticdevelopment so slow? 130 discovering semanticfeatures A method for 131 Semantic influences onsyntax 136 The association ofsemantics and actioa , . 140 The exchange ofinformation among children v.142 PHONOLOGY 11L2 The relation ofbabbling to speech of Pre-speech and neurologicalmaturation in the first year 146 The differentiationof distinctive features The development of aphonemic system and thelaws of "irreversible solidarity" 153 153 Phonological rules 155 LINGUISTIC APPENDIX 157 Competence and performance ,, 159 Finite-state grammars 163 Recursiveness and linguisticabstraction w.f.,. 165 Phrase-structure rules 169 Transformations and thenotions of deep andsurface structure 183 REFERENCES The Development of Language David McNeill The University of Michigan Like the humors of the mind,the development of a child mayconveniently be divided into four parts. One part is physicalmaturation; another is personality development, including the process of socialization; a third isintellectual development; useful, and a fourth is languagedevelopment. The division is artificial but tolerated because of it, advantagesfor orderly inquiry. However, it should not be allowed to obscure thefact that the four partsintertwine in complex ways to kingdom -- of human make up the process --absolutely unique in the animal growth. It is clear, for example,that socialization depends onthe acquisition sociali- of language. Yet it is equally clearthat language bears the marks of The develop- zation, as the linguisticdifferences among social classes attest. of intellect, as ment of personality both acts onand reacts to the development evidenced by the cases where bothfail as in schizophrenia(Inhelder, 1967). Indeed, although the interaction israrely examined, thenetwork of character- child's istics we call personality could notdevelop at all were it not for a forms the subject capacity to represent his worldin the particular way that differences in cognitive matter of cognitive psychology. Yet there are also style, in the characteristic modesof thought that accompanyparticular types of personality (Kagan,et'. al., 1963). This chapter is concerned with tv.such interactions. One is the connection between the acquisitionof language and the growthof intellect. maturation. The other is the connection betweenboth of these and the process of The parts so intertwined maystrike the reader as anarbitrary selection. However, the selection is a sensible one; and the interaction among them provides considerable insight into the process whereby achild grows to become an adult. But this is the substance of the chapter. PLAN OF THE CHAPTER In order to understand the acquisition oflanguage, it is essential first to understand something of what is acquired. There is, therefore, aLinguistic Appendix. Readers unfamiliar with developments in modern linguistics are urged to begin with it; others may want only to refer to the Appendix as the occasion reqeres. The phenomenon of language poses achallenge for psychologists. A grammar is a system ofknowledge. It is everywhere complex and at many points abstract. Yet very young children acquire grammars and they do so in asurpris- ingly short period of time. For reasons to be discussed in the first section of the chapter, various theories of development cannot account for thisachievement. Explanation must follow other lines. One view is that the acquisition of language rests on certain cognitive capacities (the presence ofwhich are reflected in language as linguistic universals). These capacities may be innate and may mature with time. The bulk of the chapter is a survey of language acquisitionitself. It is organized under three major headings, one for eachof the three main components of a grammar: syntax, phonology, and semantics. A description will be given of the methods typically used in studying thedevelopment of each component; then the emergence of the components themselves will be traced, insofar as this is known; and finally, there will be a discussion of various theoretical issues in the light of the empiricalfindings presented. Where- ever possible, mention will be made ofchildren exposed to languages other than English, with the chief contrast languages being Russianand Japanese. 3 A caveat. There has been no serious attempt to survey the literature on language acquisition in acomprehensive way. The chapter is organized on principles other than inclusion. For one thing, most of the references are recent, since 1954, whenMcCarthy's chapter on language development appeared in the second edition of The manual of child,psychologyi. Her review should be consulted for the earlier work. For another thing, recent developments in linguistics pose crucial issues for psychology, and examination of them takes priority within the limits of space
Recommended publications
  • A Contrastive Analysis of Arabic and English Noun Plural Markers
    Aliyatul Himmah & Ribut Wahyudi - A Contrastive Analysis of Arabic and English Noun Plural Markers A CONTRASTIVE ANALYSIS OF ARABIC AND ENGLISH NOUN PLURAL MARKERS Aliyatul Himmah Maulana Malik Ibrahim State Islamic University, Malang [email protected] Ribut Wahyudi Victoria University of Wellington, New Zealand & Maulana Malik Ibrahim State Islamic University, Malang [email protected] Abstract This paper is attempting to explore the plural markers in both Arabic and English. The data collected qualitatively are sorted to meet the scope of this paper. Through contrastive analysis, it is discovered that there are numerous significant differences rather than similarities in terms of syllable count start, patterns of plural nouns in relation to gender, regularity, regular vs irregular plural and internal vowel change. Moreover, Arabic has some uniqueness in its plural marking system. Being well informed on all of these might pave the way for second or foreign language learners to comprehensively understand the plural marking system in Arabic and English. Tulisan ini mencoba untuk mengeksplorasi penanda jamak dalam bahasa Arab dan bahasa Inggris. Data yang dikumpulkan secara kualitatif diurutkan untuk memenuhi cakupan makalah ini. Melalui analisis kontrastif, ditemukan banyak perbedaan yang signifikan daripada kesamaan dalam segi jumlah awal suku kata, pola kata benda jamak dalam kaitannya dengan gender, keteraturan dan ketidakteraturan jamak, serta perubahan vokal. Selain itu, bahasa Arab memiliki beberapa keunikan dalam sistem menandai jamaknya. Memahami informasi hal tersebut dengan baik mungkin memudahkan pembelajar bahasa kedua atau asing untuk memahami secara komprehensif sistem penanda jamak dalam bahasa Arab dan Inggris. Key words: Arabic, English, plural marker, suffix INTRODUCTION Understanding what contrastive linguistics and contrastive analysis is a paramount important prior to the discussion and analysis of plural markers in Arabic and English.
    [Show full text]
  • Contrastive Analysis for Translation Students
    See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.net/publication/268274970 Contrastive Analysis for Translation Students Article CITATIONS READS 0 284 1 author: Reima Al-Jarf None 223 PUBLICATIONS 191 CITATIONS SEE PROFILE Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects: Translation Students' Difficulties with Color Metaphors View project The Interchange of Personal Names in Muslim Communities View project All content following this page was uploaded by Reima Al-Jarf on 06 May 2015. The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file. Contrastive Analysis for Translation Students Reima Al-Jarf, Ph.D. King Saud University 1 All rights reserved AL-Obeikkan Printing Press Riyadh, Saudi Arabia i 2 Table of Contents PREFACE ................................................................................................................ vi Introduction .............................................................................................................. 1 Aims of the Present Chapter ................................................................................ 4 Definition of Morphemes ..................................................................................... 2 Definition of Grammar ......................................................................................... 2 Definition of Morphology ..................................................................................... 2 Inflection ...................................................................................................................
    [Show full text]
  • Chapter 1 Negation in a Cross-Linguistic Perspective
    Chapter 1 Negation in a cross-linguistic perspective 0. Chapter summary This chapter introduces the empirical scope of our study on the expression and interpretation of negation in natural language. We start with some background notions on negation in logic and language, and continue with a discussion of more linguistic issues concerning negation at the syntax-semantics interface. We zoom in on cross- linguistic variation, both in a synchronic perspective (typology) and in a diachronic perspective (language change). Besides expressions of propositional negation, this book analyzes the form and interpretation of indefinites in the scope of negation. This raises the issue of negative polarity and its relation to negative concord. We present the main facts, criteria, and proposals developed in the literature on this topic. The chapter closes with an overview of the book. We use Optimality Theory to account for the syntax and semantics of negation in a cross-linguistic perspective. This theoretical framework is introduced in Chapter 2. 1 Negation in logic and language The main aim of this book is to provide an account of the patterns of negation we find in natural language. The expression and interpretation of negation in natural language has long fascinated philosophers, logicians, and linguists. Horn’s (1989) Natural history of negation opens with the following statement: “All human systems of communication contain a representation of negation. No animal communication system includes negative utterances, and consequently, none possesses a means for assigning truth value, for lying, for irony, or for coping with false or contradictory statements.” A bit further on the first page, Horn states: “Despite the simplicity of the one-place connective of propositional logic ( ¬p is true if and only if p is not true) and of the laws of inference in which it participate (e.g.
    [Show full text]
  • Verbs in Relation to Verb Complementation 11-69
    1168 Complementation of verbs and adjectives Verbs in relation to verb complementation 11-69 They may be either copular (clause pattern SVC), or complex transitive verbs, or monotransitive verbs with a noun phrase object), we can give only (clause pattern SVOC): a sample of common verbs. In any case, it should be borne in mind that the list of verbs conforming to a given pattern is difficult to specífy exactly: there SVC: break even, plead guilty, Iie 101V are many differences between one variety of English and another in respect SVOC: cut N short, work N loose, rub N dry of individual verbs, and many cases of marginal acceptability. Sometimes the idiom contains additional elements, such as an infinitive (play hard to gel) or a preposition (ride roughshod over ...). Note The term 'valency' (or 'valencc') is sometimes used, instead of complementation, ror the way in (The 'N' aboye indicates a direct object in the case oftransitive examples.) which a verb determines the kinds and number of elements that can accompany it in the clause. Valency, however, incIudes the subject 01' the clause, which is excluded (unless extraposed) from (b) VERB-VERB COMBINATIONS complementation. In these idiomatic constructions (ef 3.49-51, 16.52), the second verb is nonfinite, and may be either an infinitive: Verbs in intransitive function 16.19 Where no eomplementation oecurs, the verb is said to have an INTRANSITIVE make do with, make (N) do, let (N) go, let (N) be use. Three types of verb may be mentioned in this category: or a participle, with or without a following preposition: (l) 'PURE' INTRANSITIVE VERas, which do not take an object at aH (or at put paid to, get rid oJ, have done with least do so only very rarely): leave N standing, send N paeking, knock N fiying, get going John has arrived.
    [Show full text]
  • The Syntax of Answers to Negative Yes/No-Questions in English Anders Holmberg Newcastle University
    The syntax of answers to negative yes/no-questions in English Anders Holmberg Newcastle University 1. Introduction This paper will argue that answers to polar questions or yes/no-questions (YNQs) in English are elliptical expressions with basically the structure (1), where IP is identical to the LF of the IP of the question, containing a polarity variable with two possible values, affirmative or negative, which is assigned a value by the focused polarity expression. (1) yes/no Foc [IP ...x... ] The crucial data come from answers to negative questions. English turns out to have a fairly complicated system, with variation depending on which negation is used. The meaning of the answer yes in (2) is straightforward, affirming that John is coming. (2) Q(uestion): Isn’t John coming, too? A(nswer): Yes. (‘John is coming.’) In (3) (for speakers who accept this question as well formed), 1 the meaning of yes alone is indeterminate, and it is therefore not a felicitous answer in this context. The longer version is fine, affirming that John is coming. (3) Q: Isn’t John coming, either? A: a. #Yes. b. Yes, he is. In (4), there is variation regarding the interpretation of yes. Depending on the context it can be a confirmation of the negation in the question, meaning ‘John is not coming’. In other contexts it will be an infelicitous answer, as in (3). (4) Q: Is John not coming? A: a. Yes. (‘John is not coming.’) b. #Yes. In all three cases the (bare) answer no is unambiguous, meaning that John is not coming.
    [Show full text]
  • An Analysis of I-Umlaut in Old English
    An Analysis of I-Umlaut in Old English Meizi Piao (Seoul National University) Meizi Piao. 2012. An Analysis of I-Umlaut in Old English. SNU Working Papers in English Linguistics and Language X, XX-XX Lass (1994) calls the period from Proto-Germanic to historical Old English ‘The Age of Harmony’. Among the harmony processes in this period, i-umlaut has been considered as ‘one of the most far-reaching and important sound changes’ (Hogg 1992, Lass 1994) or as ‘one of the least controversial sound changes’ (Colman 2005). This paper tries to analyze i-umlaut in Old English within the framework of the Autosegmental theory and the Optimality theory, and explain how suffix i or j in the unstressed syllable cause the stem vowels in the stressed syllable to be fronted or raised. (Seoul National University) Keywords: I-umlaut, Old English, Autosegmental theory, vowel harmony Optimality theory 1. Introduction Old English is an early form of the English language that was spoken and written by the Anglo-Saxons and their descendants in the area now known as England between at least the mid-5th century to the mid-12th century. It is a West Germanic language closely related to Old Frisian. During the period of Old English, one of the most important phonological processes is umlaut, which especially affects vowels, and become the reason for the superficially irregular and unrelated Modern English phenomenon. I-Umlaut is the conditioned sound change that the vowel either moves directly forward in the mouth [u>y, o>e, A>&] or forward and up [A>&>e].
    [Show full text]
  • English Grammar
    This file is to be used only for a purpose specified by Palgrave Macmillan, such as checking proofs, preparing an index, reviewing, endorsing or planning coursework/other institutional needs. You may store and print the file and share it with others helping you with the specified purpose, but under no circumstances may the file be distributed or otherwise made accessible to any other third parties without the express prior permission of Palgrave Macmillan. Please contact [email protected] if you have any queries regarding use of the file. NOT FOR DISTRIBUTION English Grammar 9781137507396_01_prexiv_1pp.indd 1 25/07/16 11:43 AM NOT FOR DISTRIBUTION 9781137507396_01_prexiv_1pp.indd 2 25/07/16 11:43 AM NOT FOR DISTRIBUTION English Grammar An Introduction Third edition PETER COLLINS AND CARMELLA HOLLO 9781137507396_01_prexiv_1pp.indd 3 25/07/16 11:43 AM NOT FOR DISTRIBUTION © Peter Collins and Carmella Hollo 2000, 2009, 2017 All rights reserved. No reproduction, copy or transmission of this publication may be made without written permission. No portion of this publication may be reproduced, copied or transmitted save with written permission or in accordance with the provisions of the Copyright, Designs and Patents Act 1988, or under the terms of any licence permitting limited copying issued by the Copyright Licensing Agency, Saffron House, 6–10 Kirby Street, London EC1N 8TS. Any person who does any unauthorized act in relation to this publication may be liable to criminal prosecution and civil claims for damages. The authors have asserted their rights to be identified as the authors of this work in accordance with the Copyright, Designs and Patents Act 1988.
    [Show full text]
  • Affirmative and Negated Modality*
    Quaderns de Filologia. Estudis lingüístics. Vol. XIV (2009) 169-192 AFFIRMATIVE AND NEGATED MODALITY* Günter Radden Hamburg 0. INTR O DUCTI O N 0.1. Stating the problem Descriptions of modality in English often give the impression that the behaviour of modal verbs is erratic when they occur with negation. A particularly intriguing case is the behaviour of the modal verb must under negation. In its deontic sense, must is used both in affirmative and negated sentences, expressing obligation (1a) and prohibition (1b), respectively. In its epistemic sense, however, must is only used in affirmative sentences expressing necessity (2a). The modal verb that expresses the corresponding negated epistemic modality, i.e. impossibility, is can’t (2b). (1) a. You must switch off your mobile phone. [obligation] b. You mustn’t switch off your mobile phone. [prohibition] (2) a. Your mobile phone must be switched off. [necessity] b. Your mobile phone can’t be switched off. [impossibility] Explanations that have been offered for the use of epistemic can’t are not very helpful. Palmer (1990: 61) argues that mustn’t is not used for the negation of epistemic necessity because can’t is supplied, and Coates (1983: 20) suggests that can’t is used because mustn’t is unavailable. Both “explanations” beg the question: why should can’t be used to denote negated necessity and why should mustn’t be unavailable? Such questions have apparently not been asked for two interrelated reasons: first, the study of modality and negation has been dominated by logic and, secondly, since the modal verbs apparently do * An earlier version of this paper was published in Władysław Chłopicki, Andrzej Pawelec, and Agnieszka Pokojska (eds.), 2007, Cognition in Language: Volume in Honour of Professor Elżbieta Tabakowska, 224-254.
    [Show full text]
  • Angleški Glagol 1
    FILOZOFSKA FAKULTETA Angleški glagol 1 Oddelek za anglistiko 2009/2010 Zapiski s predavanj prof. dr. Ilca in izpiski iz predpisane študijske literature. The VERB Typically refers to actions, to processes (events that have a span of time between the initial and final state), states. Morphological characteristics of verbs: LEXICAL/ROOT MORPHEME – carries the lexical meaning of the verb walk,.. DERIVATIONAL MORPHEME – used to derive words from other parts of speech -fy, -en, -ise/-ize, .. INFLECTIONAL MORPHEME – carries the grammatical meaning -ed, -ing, -s,.. VERBAL PHRASE Words are joined into phrases. Structure of a phrase is always the same: premodifiers (0-3) headword postmodifiers (0-2) auxiliary verbs: lexical verb adverbial, prepositional tense particles voice aspect modality The verbal phrase (VP) normally follows the nominal phrase functioning as subject in a sentence. There are two kinds of verbal phrases: SIMPLE VERBAL PHRASE consists only of one verb, which is the headword. H H ׀ ׀ Birds fly. COMPLEX VERBAL PHRASE consists of a lexical verb as headword which is preceded by a modal auxiliary and/or by one or more primary auxiliaries as premodifiers (PreM) and/or followed by one or more postmodifiers (PostM) H PreM H ׀ ׀ ׀ Birds can fly. VP She |must have given up| smoking. PreM1 PreM2 headword (H) PostM (modality) (tense) (adverbial particle) 2 IRREGULAR VERBS Use an irregular form in the function of the past tense and/or past participle. go + [+past] = went SUPPLETION: PARTIAL Resemblance between the original and the new form build – built COMPLETE No resemblance between the original and the new form be –was SPELLING CHANGES Basic form + morphemes -s/-ed/-ing spelling change 1.
    [Show full text]
  • *65 EØ RS Mitipse1e9tticaasa.56 9P
    1100&*430 ERIC RE PeRT .RESUNE IEO 010 -t-60 1W,29Hib 24 (REV) USSGE IIANUAL:44LANGUAGE CUUICIMUK. I AND !I TEACHER V ER&Iet4, ZHAER4ALURTAWR RaR610280 UNC.VERSITY OE ORE?, -EUGENE Cit9+1*114114411 1111409,40116.C431 . *65 EØ RS MiTipse1e9tticaasa.56 9P. *GRAMM-0..111GhTH- IIRAINEV.- sevens .'GRADEs. vitiCIINUCAILOCSUIDESS *TIEAZINING- SUIDESL 11-YEAL4IVIL 61/10Ett. TiENGLISK EWEN/v; MESONV4-PROJECIr'ENG11314-- RE* GRAM* A IIANUSL, GRINAR- WAGE WAS PRIEN*.f.a *OR- -TEACHINC,1semoffspoi, AND EIGHTIWERMSE- MOMS .CARINECULUNS.- THEP-MANUAt 11111S44R,-TEAT;NERWAIND CONTAINS1 96 ' GRIMMAR_ USAGEWITEett -*WOE 4-1111111), 4ItykiPROF1TSO1Y,-:TRENNIED: .4N SENENTWAID: E IGHTW GRAvegq: THE7COIITEInt-41ave-litst ,i4MIANGEO- ALAINACITICALLY-41101 A GAUt MOM '41F- -CROSSMIEFERENEU,-311EIMINUAL -414--STIODENT 4111.11NADVE_':OP-.4=UNISSORMATI.-4.14AL'IMAPSIAL--Te MEGRIM: -47 NTH SUER- -ALIPECTS 1iI E 1INGUSH 7CURRICARAIMIAN ACCOMPANYING MANUAL WAS ?NEPA-RED FOR -STUOEUT USE :4110,-010 :ant; IMO el OREGON etlittfAcilLatIM 5.xuuY CINTER **0 014 VELEM IL SiDEPARTMENT OFHEMplti: MICATION v-4 Office cif Echkgati On d**filthy has beenreproduced exactlyslitectile Co This document Points Of VieW OrOPielone organizationoriginating It. v-1 person or represent OMNIOff Ice etEducation stated donot necessarily CI position or policy. C) I USAGE ISAIWATe !ammo Curriculum I Mid II Teacher I/onion The project reported hereinwas -supported through the Cooperative ResearchPrograxa of- the Office of Education, U, S. Departgle4totilealt4s Education, and Welfare USAGE MANUAL TABLE OFCONTENTS Etas Abbreviations 1 Bust for burst Accent- Except 3. CapitalCapitol a Adjective 1 Capitalization 9 Adverb 2 Case 10 I Advice- Advise 2 clothsClothes 11 Affect- Effect 3 Colon 11 Agreement 3 Comma 11 Ain't 5 Almost-Most' 5 ConjUnction 11 Already-All ready 5 Contractions 12 All right 5 tOundil- Counsel 12 Altogether - All together 5 toUrse-Coarse 12 Among- Between 6 DetertDessert 12 ft.n-And 6 Determiner 13 Antecedent 6 ed -bove 13 t Apostrophe 6 Done foi-' did 18 Appositive 7 Negative 14 s.
    [Show full text]
  • Department of English and American Studies English
    Masaryk University Faculty of Arts Department of English and American Studies English Language and Literature Tatiana Bareková English Verbs Followed by an –ing Form and an Infinitive Bachelor's Diploma Thesis Supervisor: doc. PhDr. Naděžda Kudrnáčová, CSc. 2014 I declare that I have worked on this thesis independently, using only the primary and secondary sources listed in the bibliography. …………………………………………….. Tatiana Bareková I would like to thank doc. PhDr. Naděžda Kudrnáčová, CSc. for her patient guidance. I would also like to thank my parents and friends for their encouragement. Table of Contents 1. Introduction ............................................................................................................ 5 2. Overview of the Literature .................................................................................... 7 2.1 Catenative Verbs ................................................................................................ 7 2.2 To-infinitive or -ing Form ................................................................................ 14 2.2.1 No Change of meaning ......................................................................... 16 2.2.2 Little change of meaning ..................................................................... 16 2.2.3 Fundamental change of meaning ....................................................... 17 2.3 Catenative Classes ........................................................................................... 17 2.3.1 Begin, Start ...........................................................................................
    [Show full text]
  • Existential There and Catenative Concord. Evidence from the British National Corpus
    Existential there and catenative concord. Evidence from the British National Corpus Arne Olofsson, University of Gothenburg 1. Introductory1 1.1 Existential sentences Like many other languages, English has a presentative construction that mainly serves to create end-focus. A subject that contains new information (normally expressed by an indefinite noun phrase) is given a position after the verb and the typical subject slot is filled by there.2 The result is, for instance, There is a teacher in the classroom in preference to A teacher is in the classroom. Syntactically, there behaves like a subject, for instance occurring in postverbal position in a direct question (Is there ...?). 1.2 Subject–verb concord There is general agreement that in the existential construction, the predicate verb (prototypically a form of be) shows concord with the notional (postponed) subject, at least in formal Standard English: (1) There is a book on the table (2) There are some books on the table 1 A similar analysis based on CobuildDirect and limited to seem is to be found in Olofsson (2007). For an analysis of the concord patterns of the basic there + be construction in contemporary spoken British English, see Martinez Insua & Palacios Martinez (2003) and Breivik & Martinez-Insua (2008). Spoken Canadian English has been similarly analysed by Meechan & Foley (1994). 2 Often in the past, this there has been regarded as an empty place-holder. However, as noted by e.g. Breivik & Martinez-Insua (2008:356) it can also be described as a presentative signal ―to tell the addressees that they must be prepared to direct their attention towards an item of new information.‖ 30 Arne Olofsson In less formal English, many speakers use and accept the singular verb, but only if it is contracted with there: There’s some books on the table.
    [Show full text]