5206-001 International Conference on Salish Languages Records

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

5206-001 International Conference on Salish Languages Records UNlVERSllY U BRARIJES w UNIVERSITY of WASHI NGTON Spe ial Colle tions 5844 International Conference on Salish Languages records Inventory Accession No: 5206-001 Special Collections Division University of Washington Libraries Box 352900 Seattle, Washington, 98195-2900 USA (206) 543-1929 This document forms part of the Preliminary Guide to the International Conference on Salish Languages Records. To find out more about the history, context, arrangement, availability and restrictions on this collection, click on the following link: http://digital.lib.washington.edu/findingaids/permalink/InternationalConferenceonSalishLanguages5206/ Special Collections home page: http://www.lib.washington.edu/specialcollections/ Search Collection Guides: http://digital.lib.washington.edu/findingaids/search INTERNATIONAL CONFERENCE ON SALISH LANGUAGES. Accession No. 5206-001 Approximate List of Contents Aoki, Haruo. ''Nez Perce Vowel Harmony and Proto-Sahaptian Vowels." -----. ''North Plateau Linguistic Diffusion Area." Author Unknown. "A Concordance to the Translations in Reichard's Coeur d'Alene." Boas, Franz and Herman Haeberlin. "Sound Shifts in. Salishan Dialects." Carlson, Barry F. ''Unstressed Root Vowels in Spokane." Davis, John H. "Four Forms of the Verb in Sliammon." ·Davis, Philip W. and Ross Saunders. "Bella Coola Deictic Roots." Elmendorf, William W. "Geographic Ordering, Subgrouping, and Olympic Salish." -----. ''Relations of Oregon Salish as Evidenced in Numerical Stems." ----. "Word Tahu and Change Rates: Tests of a Hypothesis." -----. and Wayne Suttles. "Pattern and Change in Halkomelem Salish Dialects." Fraenkel, Gerd. "Yaqui Phonemics." Galloway, Brent D. "Some Singularities Between Semology and Phonology (With Illustrations from Chilliwack Halkomelem)." Gudschinsky, Sarah C. "A Discourse Analysis of a Mazatee Text." Haas, Mary R. "Stem Extenders in Nootka-Nitinat." Haeberlin, Herman K. "Types of Reduplication in the Salish Dialects." Hamp, Eric P. "Another Look at Tillamook Phonology." -----. "Some Phonetic Rules for Mainland Comox Vowels." Hess, Thom. "Prefix Constitutents with [ ]." -----. ''Secondary Suffixation in Puget Salish." Hilbert, Vi and Thom Hess. "A Note on [ ] Constructions in Lushootseed." Hoard, James E. ''Problems in Proto-Salish Pronoun Reconstruction." -----. "Remarks on Pronominalization in Quileute and English-A Work Paper." Hukari, Thomas E. "A Skeletal Syntax of Puget Salish." Hymes, Dell. "From Space to Tile in Tenses in Kiksht." Jacobsen, William H., Jr. ''Labialization in Nootkan." -----. "Makah Vowel Insertion and Loss." ----. "Origin of the Nootka Pharyngeals.'~ -----. ''Traces ofGlottalized Resonants in Makah." Kinkade, M. Dale and Clarence Sloat. "Proto-Interior Salish Vowels." -----. "Deictics in Columbian: A Work Paper." ----. ''Third Person Possessives in Cowlitz." Klokeid, Terry J. "Linguistic Acculturation in Nitinat." ----. ''Notes on the Comparison ofWakashan and Salish." Kuipers, Aert H. ''Preserve Your Language: A Shuswap Alphabet." ----. ''On Divergence, Interaction and Merging of Salish Language-Communities." -----. "Shuswap T rans1t1ve.. Vb"er s. ----. ''The Categories Verb-Noun and Transitive-Intransitive in English and Squamish." -----. ''Towards a Salish Etymological Dictionary." Mattina, Anthony. ''The Narrative of Peter J. Seymour Blue Jay and his Brother-in-Law Wolf." Mattina/retold by Douglas Purl. "The Narrative of Peter J. Seymour Blue Jay and Wolf." Newman, Stanley. "A Comparative Study of Salish Lexical Suffixes." O'Brien, Michael. "A Phonology ofMethow." Rigsby, Bruce J. "Tsimshian Comparative Vocabularies with Notes on Nass-Gitksan Systematic Phonology." Silverstein, Michael. "Chinook Jargon: Language Contact and the Problem of Multi- Level Generative Systems." Sloat, Clarence. "A Plea for Conformity and Some Amendments to Reichard." St. Clair, Robert, ed "Special Issue: Lektos: Interdisciplinary Working Papers in Language Sciences." Suttles, Wayne and William W. Elmendorf. "Linguistic Evidence for Salish Prehistory." Swadesh, Morris. "Salish Internal Relationships." ----. "Salish Phonologic Geography." , Thompson, Laurence C. 'The Northwest." ·----.and M. Terry. ''Metathesis as a Grammatical Process in Clallam." Voegelin, C. F. and F. M. "Passive Transformations from Non-Transitive Bases in Hopi." Miscellaneous: Handouts, Announcements, Flyers ''International Conference on Salish Languages" (Dec 1970) "Second International Conference on Salish Languages" ''Invitation to Third International Conference on Salish Languages" "Third International Salish Conference" "Third International Conference on Salish Languages" "Minutes: Fourth International Salish Conference" "Invitation to the Fifth International Conference of Salish Languages" "Pre-Registration Form: Fifth International Conference on Salish Languages" "Invitation to the Sixth International Conference of Salish Languages" "Invitation to the Seventh International Conference on Salish Languages" ''Eighth International Conference on Salish Language: Announcement and Call for Papers" ''Ninth International Conference on Salishan Languages: Minutes" "(I) Preliminary Organization of Symposia for the Xth Conference on American Indian Languages... " . f-1.p,~ "Report on the Xth !CSL by Robert St. Clair, Rosalie Bassett, Lena Owens" --_ /..,4'- ~{,I./, ::_- !:" f !f 7:.; "Xlth International Conference on Salishan Languages, August 12-14, 197 6" A\ Conference Schedule Conference Participants "XII International Conference on Salishan Languages" "XIIIth International Conference on Salishan Languages" Miscellanea Selica, No .....; 19th Century Kalispel Vocabularies ''Vocabularies"; "Indian Catechism" "Choco Language Family" "Algonkian" (M.R Haas, July '63) "Certain Menomini Morphophemic Rules by Bloomfield" (Thomas Bever, MIT) "Diagrammatic View ofBloomfield's Morphology as Presented in ... " "Quileute Prosodics" (E.P. Hamp, July 1963) ''The Word for 'Ear' in Hokan and Penutian Languages" (M.R. Haas, 8-8-64) "Quecha" (W. Wolck, March 2, 1964) "Arapaho" (May 6, 1963) "Announcing the Final Examination of Mr. Joseph Gilbert Jorgensen for the Degree of Doctor of Philosophy ... " "Conference on American Indian Languages" "Announcing the Final Examination of Rayna Green for the Degree of Doctor of Philosophy ... " Letter, 30 Nov 1966, from John Krueger to "Larry" Indiana University interoffice memo, 30 Nov 1966, Florence Vogelin to John Krueger Indiana University interoffice memo, from Vogelin, Chafe, Werner, Teeter to Conference on American Indian Languages Letter, 13 Dec 1966, Laurence C. Thompson to John Krueger Reprint Announcement: "American Indian Languages by James Constantine Pilling" "Invitation to Second Annual Conference on Salish Languages" Mimeograph of cover letter to conference, 23 May 1967, from A. H. Kuipers "Corrections and Additions to A.H. Kuipers, The Squamish Language." Handout: "The Morph [ ] in Snohomish" (7 June 67) Pamphlet: "Pacific Northwest Indian Center 1970 Completion Campaign" / Pamphlet: "Welcome to Gonzaga" Letter, 14 Sep 1970, Larry Thompson to John Krueger "Research Relating to British Columbia Indians Newsletter" "Application to the National Science Foundation" by M. Dale Kinkade ''National Science Foundation Proposal Rating Sheet" filled by J. R. Krueger Letter, 21 May 1971, John B. Cornell ofNational Science Foundation to John Krueger Letter, Karl V. Teeter of Harvard University Dept. of Linguistics to "Mr. Krueger" "Indian Language Activities" (July 1971) Map: British Columbia and University of Victoria Announcement of publication: "Southern Puget Sound Salish: Phonology and Morphology'' by Warren A. Snyder University of Kansas Interoffice Memo from Vogelin, Kinkade, Chafe, to Conference on American Indian Languages < ' "Conference on American Indian Languages Clearinghouse Newsletter" (April 1974), (June 1975), (Sept 1975), (May 1976), (Jan 1977) "Conference on American Indian Languages Newletter'' (June 1972) "Letter of Transmittal" Letter, 10 Sep 1970, M. Dale Kinkade to John Krueger "Letter of Transmittal" (John R. Krueger) .
Recommended publications
  • Native American Languages, Indigenous Languages of the Native Peoples of North, Middle, and South America
    Native American Languages, indigenous languages of the native peoples of North, Middle, and South America. The precise number of languages originally spoken cannot be known, since many disappeared before they were documented. In North America, around 300 distinct, mutually unintelligible languages were spoken when Europeans arrived. Of those, 187 survive today, but few will continue far into the 21st century, since children are no longer learning the vast majority of these. In Middle America (Mexico and Central America) about 300 languages have been identified, of which about 140 are still spoken. South American languages have been the least studied. Around 1500 languages are known to have been spoken, but only about 350 are still in use. These, too are disappearing rapidly. Classification A major task facing scholars of Native American languages is their classification into language families. (A language family consists of all languages that have evolved from a single ancestral language, as English, German, French, Russian, Greek, Armenian, Hindi, and others have all evolved from Proto-Indo-European.) Because of the vast number of languages spoken in the Americas, and the gaps in our information about many of them, the task of classifying these languages is a challenging one. In 1891, Major John Wesley Powell proposed that the languages of North America constituted 58 independent families, mainly on the basis of superficial vocabulary resemblances. At the same time Daniel Brinton posited 80 families for South America. These two schemes form the basis of subsequent classifications. In 1929 Edward Sapir tentatively proposed grouping these families into superstocks, 6 in North America and 15 in Middle America.
    [Show full text]
  • Information to Users
    INFORMATION TO USERS This manuscript has been reproduced from the microfihn master. UMI films the text directly from the original or copy submitted. Thus, some thesis and dissertation copies are in typewriter fece, while others may be from any type o f computer printer. The quality of this reproduction is dependent upon the quality of the copy submitted. Broken or indistinct print, colored or poor quality illustrations and photographs, print bleedthrough, substandard margins, and improper alignment can adversely afreet reproduction. In the unlikely event that the author did not send UMI a complete manuscript and there are missing pages, these will be noted. Also, if unauthorized copyright material had to be removed, a note will indicate the deletion. Oversize materials (e.g., maps, drawings, charts) are reproduced by sectioning the original, beginning at the upper left-hand comer and continuing from left to right in equal sections with small overlaps. Each original is also photographed in one exposure and is included in reduced form at the back o f the book. Photographs included in the original manuscript have been reproduced xerographically in this copy. Higher quality 6” x 9” black and white photographic prints are available for any photographs or illustrations appearing in this copy for an additional charge. Contact UMI directly to order. UMI A Bell & Howell Information Company 300 North Zed) Road, Ann Arbor MI 48106-1346 USA 313/761-4700 800/521-0600 A WORD-AND-PARADIGM APPROACH TO REDUPUCATION DISSERTATION Presented in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree Doctor of Philosophy in the Graduate School of The Ohio State University by Andrew D.
    [Show full text]
  • 33 Contact and North American Languages
    9781405175807_4_033 1/15/10 5:37 PM Page 673 33 Contact and North American Languages MARIANNE MITHUN Languages indigenous to the Americas offer some good opportunities for inves- tigating effects of contact in shaping grammar. Well over 2000 languages are known to have been spoken at the time of first contacts with Europeans. They are not a monolithic group: they fall into nearly 200 distinct genetic units. Yet against this backdrop of genetic diversity, waves of typological similarities suggest pervasive, longstanding multilingualism. Of particular interest are similarities of a type that might seem unborrowable, patterns of abstract structure without shared substance. The Americas do show the kinds of contact effects common elsewhere in the world. There are some strong linguistic areas, on the Northwest Coast, in California, in the Southeast, and in the Pueblo Southwest of North America; in Mesoamerica; and in Amazonia in South America (Bright 1973; Sherzer 1973; Haas 1976; Campbell, Kaufman, & Stark 1986; Thompson & Kinkade 1990; Silverstein 1996; Campbell 1997; Mithun 1999; Beck 2000; Aikhenvald 2002; Jany 2007). Numerous additional linguistic areas and subareas of varying sizes and strengths have also been identified. In some cases all domains of language have been affected by contact. In some, effects are primarily lexical. But in many, there is surprisingly little shared vocabulary in contrast with pervasive structural parallelism. The focus here will be on some especially deeply entrenched structures. It has often been noted that morphological structure is highly resistant to the influence of contact. Morphological similarities have even been proposed as better indicators of deep genetic relationship than the traditional comparative method.
    [Show full text]
  • Lessons from the Pacific Northwest University of British Arizona State
    PERSPECTIVES How to investigate linguistic diversity: Lessons from the Pacific Northwest HENRY DAVIS CARRIE GILLON LISA MATTHEWSON University of British Arizona State University University of British Columbia Columbia On the basis of five case studies from languages of the American Pacific Northwest, we argue that, at least in the areas of syntax and semantics, a scientific approach to the study of linguistic di - versity must be empirically grounded in theoretically informed, hypothesis-driven fieldwork on in - dividual languages. This runs counter to recent high-profile claims that large-scale typology based on the sampling of descriptive grammars yields superior results. We show that only a hypothesis- driven approach makes falsifiable predictions, and only a methodology that yields negative as well as positive evidence can effectively test those predictions. Targeted elicitation is particularly im - portant for languages with a small number of speakers, where statistical analysis of large-scale cor - pora is impossible. Given that a large proportion of the world’s linguistic diversity is found in such languages, we conclude that formal, hypothesis-driven fieldwork constitutes the best way rapidly and efficiently to document the world’s remaining syntactic and semantic diversity.* Keywords : fieldwork, methodology, syntax, semantics, Salish, Wakashan, Tsimshianic 1. Introduction . At least half of the world’s nearly 7,000 languages will no longer be spoken by the end of this century (Harrison 2007, among many others). This immi - nent large-scale language extinction has alarming consequences for the investigation of linguistic diversity. If we wish to understand the scope and limits of crosslinguistic vari - ation, it is imperative that in the near future we gather as much information about endan - gered languages as we can, in a form that allows systematic and accurate crosslinguistic comparison.
    [Show full text]
  • Language Index
    Cambridge University Press 978-0-521-86573-9 - Endangered Languages: An Introduction Sarah G. Thomason Index More information Language index ||Gana, 106, 110 Bininj, 31, 40, 78 Bitterroot Salish, see Salish-Pend d’Oreille. Abenaki, Eastern, 96, 176; Western, 96, 176 Blackfoot, 74, 78, 90, 96, 176 Aboriginal English (Australia), 121 Bosnian, 87 Aboriginal languages (Australia), 9, 17, 31, 56, Brahui, 63 58, 62, 106, 110, 132, 133 Breton, 25, 39, 179 Afro-Asiatic languages, 49, 175, 180, 194, 198 Bulgarian, 194 Ainu, 10 Buryat, 19 Akkadian, 1, 42, 43, 177, 194 Bushman, see San. Albanian, 28, 40, 66, 185; Arbëresh Albanian, 28, 40; Arvanitika Albanian, 28, 40, 66, 72 Cacaopera, 45 Aleut, 50–52, 104, 155, 183, 188; see also Bering Carrier, 31, 41, 170, 174 Aleut, Mednyj Aleut Catalan, 192 Algic languages, 97, 109, 176; see also Caucasian languages, 148 Algonquian languages, Ritwan languages. Celtic languages, 25, 46, 179, 183, Algonquian languages, 57, 62, 95–97, 101, 104, 185 108, 109, 162, 166, 176, 187, 191 Central Torres Strait, 9 Ambonese Malay, see Malay. Chadic languages, 175 Anatolian languages, 43 Chantyal, 31, 40 Apache, 177 Chatino, Zenzontepec, 192 Apachean languages, 177 Chehalis, Lower, see Lower Chehalis. Arabic, 1, 19, 22, 37, 43, 49, 63, 65, 101, 194; Chehalis, Upper, see Upper Chehalis. Classical Arabic, 8, 103 Cheyenne, 96, 176 Arapaho, 78, 96, 176; Northern Arapaho, 73, 90, Chinese languages (“dialects”), 22, 35, 37, 41, 92 48, 69, 101, 196; Mandarin, 35; Arawakan languages, 81 Wu, 118; see also Putonghua, Arbëresh, see Albanian. Chinook, Clackamas, see Clackamas Armenian, 185 Chinook.
    [Show full text]
  • Language Contact and the Dynamics of Language: Theory and Implications 10-13 May 2007 Leipzig
    Paper for the Symposium Language Contact and the Dynamics of Language: Theory and Implications 10-13 May 2007 Leipzig Rethinking structural diffusion Peter Bakker First draft. Very preliminary version. Please do not quote. May 2007. Abstract Certain types of syntactic and morphological features can be diffused, as in convergence and metatypy, where languages take over such properties from neighboring languages. In my paper I would like to explore the idea that typological similarities in the realm of morphology can be used for establishing historical, perhaps even genetic, connections between language families with complex morphological systems. I will exemplify this with North America, in particular the possible connections between the language families of the Northwest coast/ Plateau and those of the eastern part of the Americas. I will argue on the basis on typological similarities, that (ancestor languages of ) some families (notably Algonquian) spoken today east of the continental divide, were once spoken west of the continental divide. This will lead to a more theoretical discussion about diffusion: is it really possible that more abstract morphological features such as morpheme ordering can be diffused? As this appears to be virtually undocumented, we have to be very skeptical about such diffusion. This means that mere morphological similarities between morphologically complex languages should be taken as evidence for inheritance rather than the result of contact. ++++++++++++++++++++++++++++++++++++ 0. Preface Not too long ago, many colleagues in linguistics were skeptical that many were actually due to contact. Now that language contact research has become much more mainstream and central than when I started to work in that field, it is time to take the opposite position.
    [Show full text]
  • Morphological Complexity and Language Contact in Languages Indigenous to North America Marianne Mithun University of California, Santa Barbara
    Morphological Complexity and Language Contact in Languages Indigenous to North America Marianne Mithun University of California, Santa Barbara It has long been noticed that morphological complexity is not distributed evenly across the world. There are also well-known geographical pockets of morphological elaboration in areas of Northern Australia, Papua New Guinea, Siberia, and the Caucasus. During the 19th century, Duponceau (1819), Brinton (1886), and Hewitt (1893) pointed to the special complexity of languages of the Americas. But the idea that complex morphological structure should show areal distributions seems counterintuitive: traditional scales of borrowability have typically begun with vocabulary, then moved through phonetics and phonology, and on to syntax, with morphology at the end or not mentioned at all. Such scales would seem to be in line with what we know about domains of structure: morphological structure is distinguished from syntax by its routinization: most of it is below the level of consciousness. Bilinguals should be less likely to transfer individual morphemes or morphological patterns from one of their languages to the other than to transfer whole words or word orders. One factor underlying the distributions of complexity might be the social circumstances under which the languages are used and transmitted, as proposed by Dahl (2004, to appear) and (Trudgill 2011, to appear). In small, closed communities, where speakers interact with a constant group of native interlocutors who share a common set of presuppositions about the world, culture, and interests, certain expressions are likely to occur more frequently in speech than in larger, more diffuse communities. The frequency can lead to routinization of the recurring sequences and the development of complex morphological structures, with large inventories of lexicalized expressions.
    [Show full text]
  • Paper Algonquian, Wiyot, and Yurok: Proving a Distant Genetic Relationship
    Algonquian-Ritwan, (Kutenai) and Salish: Proving a distant genetic relationship * Peter Bakker Aarhus University A connection between Algonquian and Salishan was first suggested almost a century ago, and several Americanists have mentioned it or briefly discussed it (Boas, Haas, Sapir, ).i, ;, Swadesh, Thompson, Denny). However, nobody has tried to provide proof for the matter beyond a few suggestive lexical correspondences (Haas, Denny) and typological similarities (Sapir). In my paper I follow the method used by Goddard (1975) and focus on morphology to try and prove a genetic relationship between the two families. The morphological organization of the Salishan and Algonquian verbs are highly similar. Moreover, some of the pertinent grammatical morphemes also show striking formal similarities. In addition, a number of shared quirks between Salishan and Algonquian point to a genetic connection. There is archaeological evidence linking both Algonquian and Ritwan languages with the Columbia Plateau, where Salishan languages dominate, suggesting a shared history of the three groupings in the Columbia River area. Introduction The title of this paper is a clear allusion to Ives Goddard's famous 1975 paper Algonquian, Wiyot, and Yurok: Proving a distant genetic relationship. The genetic relationship between Algonquian and the two Californian languages (together the Ritwan family) was controversial, but is now generally accepted, and Ritwan and Algonquian families are now conceived of as members of the Algic stock. I use, to the extent that this is possible, the same method as Goddard used for Algic. Whereas Goddard compared Proto-Algonquian (hereafter P A) * I am not a Salishanist, neither do I have knowledge of Proto-Algonquian This is a preliminary paper and I hope to receive feedback.
    [Show full text]
  • PART I: NAME SEQUENCE Name Sequence
    Name Sequence PART I: NAME SEQUENCE A-ch‘ang Abor USE Achang Assigned collective code [sit] Aba (Sino-Tibetan (Other)) USE Chiriguano UF Adi Abaknon Miri Assigned collective code [phi] Miśing (Philippine (Other)) Aborlan Tagbanwa UF Capul USE Tagbanua Inabaknon Abua Kapul Assigned collective code [nic] Sama Abaknon (Niger-Kordofanian (Other)) Abau Abujhmaria Assigned collective code [paa] Assigned collective code [dra] (Papuan (Other)) (Dravidian (Other)) UF Green River Abulas Abaw Assigned collective code [paa] USE Abo (Cameroon) (Papuan (Other)) Abazin UF Ambulas Assigned collective code [cau] Maprik (Caucasian (Other)) Acadian (Louisiana) Abenaki USE Cajun French Assigned collective code [alg] Acateco (Algonquian (Other)) USE Akatek UF Abnaki Achangua Abia Assigned collective code [sai] USE Aneme Wake (South American (Other)) Abidji Achang Assigned collective code [nic] Assigned collective code [sit] (Niger-Kordofanian (Other)) (Sino-Tibetan (Other)) UF Adidji UF A-ch‘ang Ari (Côte d'Ivoire) Atsang Abigar Ache USE Nuer USE Guayaki Abkhaz [abk] Achi Abnaki Assigned collective code [myn] USE Abenaki (Mayan languages) Abo (Cameroon) UF Cubulco Achi Assigned collective code [bnt] Rabinal Achi (Bantu (Other)) Achinese [ace] UF Abaw UF Atjeh Bo Cameroon Acholi Bon (Cameroon) USE Acoli Abo (Sudan) Achuale USE Toposa USE Achuar MARC Code List for Languages October 2007 page 11 Name Sequence Achuar Afar [aar] Assigned collective code [sai] UF Adaiel (South American Indian Danakil (Other)) Afenmai UF Achuale USE Etsako Achuara Jivaro Afghan
    [Show full text]
  • Languages and Dialects in Straits Salishan Section I Discusses the History of the Terminological Confusion
    249 not be correct for another; and 3) discuss the problem of the four varieties of Samish that appear in the literature. Languages and Dialects in Straits Salishan Section I discusses the history of the terminological confusion. Section 2 discusses the level of mutual intelligibility between Klallam and Northern Straits. Section 3 summarizes the lexical Timothy Montier differences among the dialects, while sections 4 and 5 discuss the phonological and grammatical differences. Section 6 describes the problem of the four recorded varieties of Samish. Section 7 University of North Texas briefly discusses dialect differences within Saanich and within Klallam. Section 8, points out some other complicating factors in sorting out the varieties including generational differences, male/female differences, and the new, revitalized varieties of the Straits languages and dialects. O. Introduction. Straits Salishan is a group oflanguages and dialects spoken in Washington, Section 9 summarizes the conclusions. Vancouver Island, and the small islands around the Juan de Fuca, Haro, and Rosario Straits north and west of Puget Sound. It is a subgroup of the Central Coast Salishan language family bounded 1. History of the terminological confusion. The earliest confusing reference found was that of to the south, east, and north by other Salishan languages, Quinault, Twana, Lushootseed, George Gibbs 1863. Gibbs, an adventurer and amateur anthropologist and linguist, grouped Nooksack, and Halkomelem, and to the west by the non-Salishan Nitinaht, Makah, and Quilleute. Klallam with Sooke and Songish as dialects of one language; Lummi, Saanich, and Semiahmoo as Straits Salishan is composed of two languages: Klallam and Northern Straits.
    [Show full text]
  • DESCRIPTORS American Indian Languages in the United States And
    DOCUMENT RESUME MD 104 168 FL 006 830 AUTHOR Martin, Jeanette TITLE A Survey of the Current Study andTeaching of North American Indian Languages in the United States and Canada. CAL - ERIC /CLL Series on Languages and Linguistics, No. 17. INSTITUTION ERIC Clearinghouse on Languages andLinguistics, Arlington, Va. PUB DATE May 75 NOTE 97p. AVAILABLE FROMCenter for Applied Linguistics, 1611 North sent Street, Arlington, Virginia 22209 ($4.00) EDRS PRICE MR -50.76 RC -$4.43 PLUS POSTAGE DESCRIPTORS *American Indian Languages; *Bilingual Education; Bilingualism; *College Language Programs; Higher Education; Instructional Materials; *Language Instruction; Reference Materials; *Surveys ABSTRACT This survey attempts to bring together as such information as possible on the current study andteaching of North American Indian languages in the United States andCanada. The primary source of data for this survey was aquestionnaire distributed in the spring of 1973 to 61 universitiesand colleges in the U.S. and Canada. Other sources werepublications, conferences, and correspondence with individualsworking with these languages. An overview of the study of North American Indianlanguages is presented first, outlining the contributions of severalgenerations of linguists and leading up to a discussion ofthe present situation. Some current trends are identifiedthrough discussion of a representative group of recently instituted programs.Three appendices present the collected data. Appendix Alists American Indian language courses and other types of programsof 101 universities and colleges in the U.S. and Canada. InAppendix Of materials useful for the study of Amerindianlanguages are cited. Appendix C indicates persons who are makingsignificant contributions to tbe study of American Indianlanguages. Also included are statistics relating to the present neither ofspeakers in the major language groups and the recommendationsof the Conference on Priorities in American Indian Language Work,held in Eugene, Oregon, in August 1973* (Author/PMP) Aliamaarir.
    [Show full text]
  • Code Sequence
    Code Sequence PART II: CODE SEQUENCE aar Afar bej Beja abk Abkhaz bel Belarusian ace Achinese bem Bemba ach Acoli ben Bengali ada Adangme ber Berber (Other) ady Adygei bho Bhojpuri afa Afroasiatic (Other) bih Bihari afh Afrihili (Artificial language) bik Bikol afr Afrikaans bin Edo ain Ainu bis Bislama - ajm Aljamía bla Siksika aka Akan bnt Bantu (Other) akk Akkadian bos Bosnian alb Albanian bra Braj ale Aleut bre Breton alg Algonquian (Other) btk Batak alt Altai bua Buriat amh Amharic bug Bugis ang English, Old (ca. 450-1100) bul Bulgarian anp Angika bur Burmese apa Apache languages byn Bilin ara Arabic cad Caddo arc Aramaic cai Central American Indian (Other) arg Aragonese Spanish - cam Khmer arm Armenian car Carib arn Mapuche cat Catalan arp Arapaho cau Caucasian (Other) art Artificial (Other) ceb Cebuano arw Arawak cel Celtic (Other) asm Assamese cha Chamorro ast Bable chb Chibcha ath Athapascan (Other) che Chechen aus Australian languages chg Chagatai ava Avaric chi Chinese ave Avestan chk Chuukese awa Awadhi chm Mari aym Aymara chn Chinook jargon aze Azerbaijani cho Choctaw bad Banda languages chp Chipewyan bai Bamileke languages chr Cherokee bak Bashkir chu Church Slavic bal Baluchi chv Chuvash bam Bambara chy Cheyenne ban Balinese cmc Chamic languages baq Basque cop Coptic bas Basa cor Cornish bat Baltic (Other) cos Corsican MARC Code List for Languages October 2007 page 161 Code Sequence cpe Creoles and Pidgins, English-based fre French (Other) - fri Frisian cpf Creoles and Pidgins, French-based frm French, Middle (ca. 1300-1600)
    [Show full text]