Lexical Tone Perception in Chinese Mandarin Anna Björklund

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Lexical Tone Perception in Chinese Mandarin Anna Björklund Lexical Tone Perception in Chinese Mandarin Anna Björklund University of Florida 1 ABSTRACT Perceptual asymmetry, where Stimulus A is more often confused for B than B is for A or vice versa, has been observed in multiple lexical contexts, such as vowels (Polka & Bohn, 2003) and consonants (Dar, Mariam & Keren-Portnoy, 2018). Because historically perceptual space was assumed to be Euclidean, perception of stimuli was in turn assumed to be symmetrical, and observations of asymmetry were explained as simply response bias (Polka & Bohn, 2003). However, further examination of such biases has suggested that they are a much more fundamental occurrence. This study examines the effects of memory load and native language on perceptual bias in lexical tone perception of Mandarin Chinese for native speakers of Mandarin and English. Native speakers of both languages were given an AX categorical discrimination task with each combination of the four lexical Mandarin tones. To examine the effects of training on this bias, they were then trained on one of two tones (1 and 4) and given the same sequence of discrimination tasks again. Each pre- and post-training discrimination task featured both 250 ms and 1000 ms ISI intervals. 2 0. INTRODUCTION TO PERCEPTUAL ASYMMETRY It is often assumed that perception is symmetrical: that is, when presented with two stimuli, A and B, it is equally as easy to discriminate going from stimuli A to B as going from B to A. Although there had long been data that conflicted with such a model, the aberrant data was explained as response bias (Polka and Bohn, 2003), and subsequently ignored. It was not until the mid-1970s, when research began challenging the assumption of perception as ‘Euclidean’ (Polka and Bohn, 2003)—that is, straightforwardly one-to-one— that new models of perception were considered and previously anomalous data re-contextualized within this new framework. Whereas the ‘Euclidean’ model assumed perception to be symmetrical, this new model of perception posited that perception was asymmetrical. In this paradigm, given stimuli A and B, it is not assumed that distinguishing stimulus A from B is equally as easy as distinguishing B from A. That is, participants may more accurately categorize stimuli when A is the target stimulus, rather than when B is. Although this paper is concerned with asymmetrical perception in the auditory domain (specifically, within speech perception), perceptual asymmetry has been reported across many domains, with the best documented being perhaps the visual one. Biases in the visual system have been noted as far back as the 1960s, when Campbell and Kulikowski observed that the visual system is better at horizontal and vertical discrimination than it is at oblique discrimination—that is, objects are more easily discriminable at vertical and horizontal angles than they are at angles, of, for example, 45 degrees (Campell and Kulikowski, 1966). Likewise, perceptual asymmetries have been noted for phenomena such as a preference for foveal (frontal) stimuli over peripheral stimuli (Karim and Kojima, 2010). Even within the strict domain of 3 horizontal/vertical visual perception, studies have shown that contrast sensitivity and spatial resolution are better along the horizontal midline (Carrasco, Talgar, and Cameron, 2001). Visual asymmetries have also been demonstrated in the perception of texture --it is easier to find a ‘broken’ circle in an array of closed circles versus discriminate a closed circle in an array of ‘broken’ circles (Williams and Julesz 1992)-- as well as in the recognition of faces. In a 1973 study by Gilbert and Bakan, multiple volunteers’ faces were photographed, and then ‘reassembled’ into two new photographs, such that each new photo was comprised only of one half of the original face (a ‘left’ face would, for instance, be comprised of the normal left half of the original photograph and a reversed left half to serve as the ‘right’ side of the face). When participants were asked to compare these new photos to the original photo and rank which one best resembled the original, participants overwhelmingly chose the photo made up of the right half of the volunteers’ faces (Gilbert and Bakan, 1973). The robustness of the literature in regards to visual asymmetrical perception also provides important speculation as to the reason for why such asymmetry exists in the first place. Notably, the above literature primarily cites biology as the driving force. For example, foveal vision is believed to be favored over peripheral vision due to the lower presence of cones in the periphery of the cornea (Karim and Kojima, 2010). Perhaps even more helpful to the present project is Williams and Julesz (1992)’s discussion of the asymmetrical perception of visual texture, in which they state that their “results suggest that visual processing of textures is not limited to simply the collective responses of isolated elements. Instead, it appears that an active visual process encodes the stimulus without necessarily keeping the individual elements intact. The completion of local boundaries into global structures (subjective contours) is fundamental to the process of texture perception” (emphasis mine, pp. 6533). That is, rather than encoding every feature of every stimulus, the visual system ‘sorts’ stimuli into broader super-perceptual categories. 4 1. PROTOYPE THEORY AND THE PERCEPTUAL MAGNET EFFECT Before prototype theory was introduced in the 1970s, the traditional definition of a category was essentially an item that matched a certain set of definitions. An item was either a member of a category or it was not, with little room for gradation (Rosch, 1973). However, beginning in the 1970s, Eleanor Rosch and other researchers began to suggest a new categorization paradigmː that of prototypicality. In this paradigm, there is not necessarily a strict in-/out-of-category binary. Rather, “categories [are] typically composed of distributions of attributes, and some instances of categories [are] designed to be more ‘typical’ members of the category than others” (Rosch, 1973, pp. 329). In such a paradigm, category members are not defined as merely having a certain list of features. Rather, they may individually possess those features to a greater or lesser degree. Therefore, some category members may be considered more “prototypical” than others, even though they all are de facto members of the category. One of the most well-known theories of perceptual categorization and application of prototype theory is the perceptual magnet effect, introduced by Patricia Kuhl in a 2001 study. The study consisted of four experiments. In Experiment 1, Kuhl asked adult English-speaking participants to “rank” a series of vowels from “best” to “worst” in a given category. She discovered that these rankings were not random, but rather that the “best” example of a category was consistently accorded to a specific area of the vowel space. Experiment 2 used the results of this survey to create an AX study where participants were presented with a once-per-second stream of sounds that alternated randomly between a “referent” speech sound and a “comparison” speech sound. Participants were then asked to press a button when they heard a change in the “referent” sound—that is, when the sound had changed to the other, “comparison” 5 sound. The study found that participants were more willing to overgeneralize (and thus give inaccurate button-pushes) when the “prototype” sounds from Experiment 1 were used as the “referent” sound. Experiment 3 replicated the conditions of Experiment 2, except with infants conditioned for the head-turn procedure. These infants were presented with the same “prototype” and “non-prototype” vowels as the adults, and their results were highly correlated with the adults’ as well. In light of this data, Kuhl 2001 proposes the presence of a “perceptual magnet effect”: not only are certain stimuli within a category considered “better” or “more prototypical” than others, but that these prototypical stimuli have a tendency to “assimilate” the other, less- prototypical stimuli. As Kuhl 2001 puts itː “Surrounding members of the category are perceptually assimilated to it to a greater degree than would be expected on the basis of real psychophysical distance. Relative to a non-prototype of the category, the distance between the prototype and surrounding members is effectively decreased; in other words, the perceptual space appears to be ‘warped,’ effectively shrunk around the prototype” (pp. 99). In other words, prototypes hold greater perceptual weight than non-prototypes in a speaker’s mind. Thus, although two vowels may differ equally with respect to a given stimuli, they may not be perceived by the listener as being equally different if one of these vowels is a prototype. Listeners are more willing to over-generalize non-prototypes and categorize them as variants of the prototypical category than they are willing to sort prototypes into non- prototypical categories. Prototypes could then be said to exhibit a sort of masking effect on non-prototypes, making them harder to distinguish when compared to the prototype. This is a fundamental theory used in explaining perceptual asymmetry, which will be explored later in this paper. 6 Another fundamental observation noted in Kuhl 2001 is that prototypes do not merely exist within one person’s perception. Rather, they appear to have a measure of universality. The adults in Experiment 1 consistently and collectively grouped certain stimuli as “better” than others, and these “better,” more prototypical prototypes were then demonstrated to be easier to distinguish against non-prototypes than the reverse, across multiple adults and infants. 2. PERCEPTUAL ASSYMMETRY OF VOWELS The study of symmetrical vowel perception began ostensibly with Polka and Werker 1994’s study of infant vowel perception across native/non-native vowel contrasts. The study’s initial purpose was not related to asymmetrical perception at all, but rather examining if and when infants’ phonetic perception of vowels and consonants began to be affected by native language sound inventories.
Recommended publications
  • A Contrastive Study of the Ibibio and Igbo Sound Systems
    A CONTRASTIVE STUDY OF THE IBIBIO AND IGBO SOUND SYSTEMS GOD’SPOWER ETIM Department Of Languages And Communication Abia State Polytechnic P.M.B. 7166, Aba, Abia State, Nigeria. [email protected] ABSTRACT This research strives to contrast the consonant phonemes, vowel phonemes and tones of Ibibio and Igbo in order to describe their similarities and differences. The researcher adopted the descriptive method, and relevant data on the phonology of the two languages were gathered and analyzed within the framework of CA before making predictions and conclusions. Ibibio consists of ten vowels and fourteen consonant phonemes, while Igbo is made up of eight vowels and twenty-eight consonants. The results of contrastive analysis of the two languages showed that there are similarities as well as differences in the sound systems of the languages. There are some sounds in Ibibio which are not present in Igbo. Also many sounds are in Igbo which do not exist in Ibibio. Both languages share the phonemes /e, a, i, o, ɔ, u, p, b, t, d, k, kp, m, n, ɲ, j, ŋ, f, s, j, w/. All the phonemes in Ibibio are present in Igbo except /ɨ/, /ʉ/, and /ʌ/. Igbo has two vowel segments /ɪ/ and /ʊ/ and also fourteen consonant phonemes /g, gb, kw, gw, ŋw, v, z, ʃ, h, ɣ, ʧ, ʤ, l, r/ which Ibibio lacks. Both languages have high, low and downstepped tones but Ibibio further has contour or gliding tones which are not tone types in Igbo. Also, the downstepped tone in Ibibio is conventionally marked with exclamation point, while in Igbo, it is conventionally marked with a raised macron over the segments bearing it.
    [Show full text]
  • List of Phonetic Symbols
    LIST OF PHONETIC SYMBOLS a open front unrounded vowel—modern RP man, bath æ front vowel between open and open-mid—traditional RP man ɐ near open central unrounded vowel—traditional RP gear [gɪɐ] ɑ: open back unrounded vowel—RP harsh ɒ open back rounded vowel—RP dog b voiced bilabial plosive—RP bet ɔ: open mid-back rounded vowel—RP caught d voiced alveolar plosive—RP daddy dʒ voiced palato-alveolar fricative—RP John ð voiced dental fricative—RP other e close-mid front unrounded vowel—traditional RP bed ɛ open-mid front unrounded vowel—modern RP bed ə(:) central unrounded vowel—RP initial vowel in another; modern RP nurse ɜ: open-mid central unrounded vowel—traditional RP bird f voiceless labiodental fricative—RP four g voiced velar plosive—RP go h voiceless glottal fricative—RP home i(:) close front unrounded vowel—RP fleece; modern RP final vowel in happy ɪ close-mid centralised unrounded vowel—RP sit j palatal approximant—RP you 241 List of phonetic symbols ɹ voiced alveolar approximant—RP row k voiceless velar plosive—RP car l voiced alveolar lateral approximant—RP lie ɫ voiced alveolar lateral approximant with velarisation—RP still m voiced bilabial nasal—RP man n voiced alveolar nasal—RP no ŋ voiced velar nasal—RP bring θ voiceless dental fricative—RP think p voiceless bilabial plosive—RP post s voiceless alveolar fricative—RP some ʃ voiceless palato-alveolar fricative—RP shoe t voiceless alveolar plosive—RP toe tʃ voiceless palato-alveolar affricate—RP choose u: close back rounded vowel—RP sue ʊ close-mid centralised rounded vowel—RP
    [Show full text]
  • Equivalences Between Different Phonetic Alphabets
    Equivalences between different phonetic alphabets by Carlos Daniel Hern´andezMena Description IPA Mexbet X-SAMPA IPA Symbol in LATEX Voiceless bilabial plosive p p p p Voiceless dental plosive” t t t d ntextsubbridgeftg Voiceless velar plosive k k k k Voiceless palatalized plosive kj k j k j kntextsuperscriptfjg Voiced bilabial plosive b b b b Voiced bilabial approximant B VB o ntextloweringfntextbetag fl Voiced dental plosive d” d d d ntextsubbridgefdg Voiced dental fricative flD DD o ntextloweringfntextipafn;Dgg Voiced velar plosive g g g g Voiced velar fricative Èfl GG o ntextloweringfntextbabygammag Voiceless palato-alveolar affricate t“S tS tS ntextroundcapftntexteshg Voiceless labiodental fricative f f f f Voiceless alveolar fricative s s s s Voiced alveolar fricative z z z z Voiceless dental fricative” s s [ s d ntextsubbridgefsg Voiced dental fricative” z z [ z d ntextsubbridgefzg Voiceless postalveolar fricative S SS ntextesh Voiceless velar fricative x x x x Voiced palatal fricative J Z jn ntextctj Voiced postalveolar affricate d“Z dZ dZ ntextroundcapfdntextyoghg Voiced bilabial nasal m m m m Voiced alveolar nasal n n n n Voiced labiodental nasal M MF ntextltailm Voiced dental nasal n” n [ n d ntextsubbridgefng Voiced palatalized nasal nj n j n j nntextsuperscriptfjg Voiced velarized nasal nÈ N n G nntextsuperscript fntextbabygammag Voiced palatal nasal ñ n∼ J ntextltailn Voiced alveolar lateral approximant l l l l Voiced dental lateral” l l [ l d ntextsubbridgeflg Voiced palatalized lateral lj l j l j lntextsuperscriptfjg Lowered
    [Show full text]
  • N. V. Tatsenko INTRODUCTION to THEORETICAL PHONETICS OF
    Ministry of Education and Science of Ukraine Sumy State University N. V. Tatsenko INTRODUCTION TO THEORETICAL PHONETICS OF ENGLISH Study guide Recommended by the Academic Council of Sumy State University Sumy Sumy State University 2020 1 УДК 811.111’342(075.8) T 23 Reviewers: V. A. Ushchyna – DSc. (Philology), professor, head of Department of English Philology, Lesya Ukrainka Eastern European National University; V. H. Nikonova – DSc. (Philology), professor, head of Department of English and German Philology and Translation, Kyiv National Linguistic University Recommended for publication by the Academic Council of Sumy State University as a study guide (minutes № 2 of 07.09.2020) Tatsenko N. V. T 23 Introduction to Theoretical Phonetics of English : study guide / N. V. Tatsenko. – Sumy : Sumy State University, 2020. – 199 p. ISBN 987-966-657-835-1 The study guide contains educational material on the main topics of the course of theoretical phonetics of English: the sound structure of the language and the ways of its description and analysis; features of the modern pronunciation norm of English as a polyethnic formation and its national and regional variants; sounds of English as articulatory and functional units; syllable as a phonetic and phonological unit, word emphasis; prosodic arrangement of English language. Questions and practical tasks for each unit provide an opportunity for self-study of educational material. Meant for students, graduate students, teachers, and all interested in learning English. УДК 811.111’342(075.8) © Tatsenko N. V., 2020 ISBN 987-966-657-835-1 © Sumy State University, 2020 2 TABLE OF CONTENTS P. UNIT 1.
    [Show full text]
  • Unit – I Language and Linguistics – Shs5006
    SCHOOL OF SCIENCE AND HUMANITIES DEPARTMENT OF ENGLISH UNIT – I LANGUAGE AND LINGUISTICS – SHS5006 UNIT-1 ORIGIN AND DEVELOPMENT OF ENGLISH LANGUAGE Indo-European: Indo-European is just one of the language families, or proto- languages, from which the world's modern languages are descended, and there are many other families including Sino- Tibetan, North Caucasian, Afro-Asiatic, Altaic, Niger- Congo, Dravidian etc. The English language, and indeed most European languages, traces it original roots back to a Neolithic (late Stone Age) people known as the Indo- Europeans or Proto-Indo-Europeans, who lived in Eastern Europe and Central Asia Spread of Indo-European Languages Between 3500 BC and 2500 BC, the Indo-Europeans began to fan out across Europe and Asia, in search of new pastures and hunting grounds, and their languages developed - and diverged - in isolation. By around 1000 BC, the original Indo-European language had split into a dozen or more major language groups or families, the main groups being: Hellenic Italic Indo-Iranian Celtic Germanic Armenian Balto-Slavic Albanian GERMANIC The Germanic, or Proto-Germanic, language group can be traced back to the region between the Elbe river in modern Germany and southern Sweden some 3,000 years ago. The early Germanic languages themselves borrowed some words from the aboriginal (non-Indo-European) tribes which preceded them, particularly words for the natural environment (e.g. sea, land, strand, seal, herring); for technologies connected with sea travel (e.g. ship, keel, sail, oar); for new social practices (e.g. wife, bride, groom); and for farming or animal husbandry practices (e.g.
    [Show full text]
  • Organised Phonology Data
    id11101609 pdfMachine by Broadgun Software - a great PDF writer! - a great PDF creator! - http://www.pdfmachine.com http://www.broadgun.com ára OPD Risto Sarsa W Version 3.0, 3 December 2001 Organised Phonology Data ára Language [TCI] W Western Province Linguistic Classification (according to Wurm): Tonda Sub-Family, Morehead and Upper Maro Rivers Family, Trans-Fly Stock, Trans-New Guinea Phylum. Note: In the Tonda Sub-Family there is a dialect chain situation. Therefore, it is difficult ára language (as defined by to establish precise language boundaries. The W ’s Upper Peremka (Rouku) Language the present writer) comprises Wurm and, in part, Tonda Language Population estimate: 800 ékwa, Tékwa, Réku, Wámnefér, Ufaruwa Major Villages: Y Linguistic work done by: S.A. Wurm; SIL Data checked by: Risto Sarsa, March 2001 Data is based on 7 years of fieldwork. PHONEMIC AND ORTHOGRAPHIC INVENTORY (In parentheses: only in loan words) z ŋ ŋɡ۽ɑ æ (b) (d) e ə f (ɡ) i k (l) m mb n nd? nG / < a á (b) (d) e é f (g) i,y k (l) m mb n nd,nt nj, nts ng nḡ < A Á (B) (D) E É F (G) I,Y K (L) M Mb N Nd Nj Ng Nḡ / s u ʉ w j۽o ʌ œ (p) r s t? ð W o ó ô (p) r s t th ts u,w ú w y > O Ó Ô (P) R S T Th Ts U,W Ú W Y > Page 1 of 12 File: Wara (Convert'd)2 ára OPD Risto Sarsa W Version 3.0, 3 December 2001 CONSONANTS Simple Consonants Bilab LabDen Dent Alv PsAl Retr Pala Velr Uvlr Phar Glot Plosive W ݽ k Nasal m n ŋ Trill r Fricat f ð s j Approx Complex Consonants mb (prenasalised voiced bilabial stop) nd? (prenasalised voiced dental stop) ŋɡ (prenasalised voiced velar stop) (s (voiceless alveolar grooved affricate۽W (z (prenasalised voiced alveolar grooved affricate۽nG w (voiced labio-velar approximant) The phonemes / b / , / d / , / g / , / l / and / p / only occur in loan words.
    [Show full text]
  • Using Phonetic Knowledge in Tools and Resources for Natural Language Processing and Pronunciation Evaluation
    Using phonetic knowledge in tools and resources for Natural Language Processing and Pronunciation Evaluation Gustavo Augusto de Mendonça Almeida Gustavo Augusto de Mendonça Almeida Using phonetic knowledge in tools and resources for Natural Language Processing and Pronunciation Evaluation Master dissertation submitted to the Instituto de Ciências Matemáticas e de Computação – ICMC- USP, in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of the Master Program in Computer Science and Computational Mathematics. FINAL VERSION Concentration Area: Computer Science and Computational Mathematics Advisor: Profa. Dra. Sandra Maria Aluisio USP – São Carlos May 2016 Ficha catalográfica elaborada pela Biblioteca Prof. Achille Bassi e Seção Técnica de Informática, ICMC/USP, com os dados fornecidos pelo(a) autor(a) Almeida, Gustavo Augusto de Mendonça A539u Using phonetic knowledge in tools and resources for Natural Language Processing and Pronunciation Evaluation / Gustavo Augusto de Mendonça Almeida; orientadora Sandra Maria Aluisio. – São Carlos – SP, 2016. 87 p. Dissertação (Mestrado - Programa de Pós-Graduação em Ciências de Computação e Matemática Computacional) – Instituto de Ciências Matemáticas e de Computação, Universidade de São Paulo, 2016. 1. Template. 2. Qualification monograph. 3. Dissertation. 4. Thesis. 5. Latex. I. Aluisio, Sandra Maria, orient. II. Título. SERVIÇO DE PÓS-GRADUAÇÃO DO ICMC-USP Data de Depósito: Assinatura: ______________________ Gustavo Augusto de Mendonça Almeida Utilizando conhecimento fonético em ferramentas e recursos de Processamento de Língua Natural e Treino de Pronúncia Dissertação apresentada ao Instituto de Ciências Matemáticas e de Computação – ICMC-USP, como parte dos requisitos para obtenção do título de Mestre em Ciências – Ciências de Computação e Matemática Computacional. VERSÃO REVISADA Área de Concentração: Ciências de Computação e Matemática Computacional Orientadora: Profa.
    [Show full text]
  • 1/2 SAMPA Symbol IPA Equivalent Description # Pause { [Æ] Open Lax
    SAMPA-IPA equivalences University of Toronto Romance Phonetics Database SAMPA Symbol IPA Equivalent Description # Pause { [æ] Open lax front vowel @ [ə] Mid central unrounded vowel 1 [ɨ] Close central unrounded vowel 2 [ø] Close-mid front rounded vowel 3 [ɜ] Open-mid central unrounded vowel 6 [ɐ] Open central vowel 6~ [ɐ̃] Open central nasal vowel 9 [œ] Open-mid front rounded vowel 9~ [œ̃] Open-mid front rounded nasal vowel a [a] Open front unrounded vowel a~ [ã] Open front unrounded nasal vowel aj [aj] Open front diphthong aw [aw] Open front diphthong A [ɑ] Open back unrounded vowel b [b] Voiced bilabial stop b: [bː] Geminate voiced bilabial stop d [d] Voiced dental/alveolar stop d: [dː] Geminate voiced dental/alveolar stop D [ð] Voiced interdental fricative dz [dz] Voiced alveolar affricate dz: [dzː] Geminated voiced alveolar affricate dZ [dʒ] Voiced post-alveolar affricate dZ: [dʒː] Geminate Voiced post-alveolar affricate e [e] Close-mid front unrounded vowel e~ [ẽ] Close-mid front nasal unrounded vowel e_X [e̯] Non-syllabic tense mid front vowel ej [ej] Mid front diphthong E [ɛ] Open-mid front unrounded vowel E~ [ɛ̃] Open-mid front unrounded nasal vowel f [f] Voiceless labio-dental fricative g [ɡ] Voiced velar stop g: [ɡː] Geminate voiced velar stop h [h] Voiceless glottal fricative H [ɥ] Rounded palatal glide i [i] Close front unrounded vowel i~ [i]̃ Unrounded tense high front nasal vowel I [ɪ] Close front lax vowel j [j] Unrounded palatal glide jj [ʝ] Voiced palatal fricative 1/2 SAMPA-IPA equivalences University of Toronto Romance
    [Show full text]
  • Computer-Coding the IPA: a Proposed Extension of SAMPA J.C.Wells, University College London
    Computer-coding the IPA: a proposed extension of SAMPA J.C.Wells, University College London 1. Computer coding. When an ASCII file (a DOS text file) is sent as an e-mail message, the only characters that are sure to be properly transmitted are those with ASCII/ANSI numbers between 32 and 126. These comprise upper-case A..Z, lower-case a..z, numerals 0..9, punctuation marks ! " ' ( ) , - . / : ; ? [ ] { }, other marks # $ % & * + < = > @ \ ^ _ ` | ~, and space. If we want to transmit phonetic symbols, we must therefore recode them using only these characters. It is not even the case that all the 'other marks' mentioned will necessarily reappear correctly at the receiving end: on my own British screen, for example, an incoming character originally transmitted as a hash mark (#) appears as pound sterling (£). But at least there is a consistent one- for-one substitution, so that information is not lost. On the other hand an outgoing character falling outside the range 32..126 is very likely to be converted into something else: a pound sterling sign (£, ASCII 156, ANSI 0163) transmitted from a British keyboard may be received (even in the UK) as an exclamation mark (!), hash (#), or other substitute. 2. The SAM Phonetic Alphabet (SAMPA) conventions were drawn up in 1988-1991 (with subsequent minor revisions and extensions) by the SAM (Speech Assessment Methods) consortium, comprising speech scientists from nine countries of the European Community (Wells et al, 1992). The purpose of SAMPA was to form the basis of an international standard machine-readable phonetic alphabet for purposes of international collaboration in speech research.
    [Show full text]
  • Prosodic Analysis and Asian Linguistics: to Honour R.K
    PACIFIC LINGIDSTICS Series C - No.l04 PROSODIC ANALYSIS AND ASIAN LINGUISTICS: TO HONOUR R.K. SPRIGG David Bradley, Eugenie 1.A. Henderson and Martine Mazaudon eds Department of Linguistics Research School of Pacific Studies THE AUSTRALIAN NATIONAL UNIVERSITY PACIFIC LIN GUISTICS is issued through the Linguistic Circle of Canberra and consists of four series: SERIES A: Occasional Papers SERIES C: Books SERIESB: Monographs SERIES D: Special Publications FOUNDING EDITOR: S.A. Wurm EDITORIAL BOARD: D.T. Tryon, T.E. Dutton, M.D. Ross EDITORIAL ADVISERS: B.W. Bender H. P. McKaughan University of Hawaii University of Hawaii David Bradley_ P. Miihlhl1usler LaTrobe University Bond University Michael G. Clyne G.N. O' Grady Monash University University of Victoria, B.C. S.H. Elbert A.K. Pawley University of Hawaii University of Auckland K.J. Franklin KL. Pike Summer Institute of Linguistics Summer Instituteof Linguistics W.W. Glover E.C. Polome Summer Institute of Linguistics University of Texas G.W. Grace GillianSan koff Universi� of Hawaii University of Pennsylvania M.A.K. Halliday W.A.L. Stokhof University of Sydney University of Leiden E. Haugen B.K. T'sou HarvardUniversity CityPolytechnic of Hong Kong A. Healey E.M. Uhlenbeck Summer Institute of Linguistics University of Leiden L.A. Hercus J .W.M. Verhaar AustralianNational University Divine Word Institute, Madang John Lynch CL. Voorhoeve Umversity of PapuaNew Guinea University of Leiden K.A. McElhanon Summer Institute of Linguistics All correspondence concerningPACIFIC LIN GUISTICS, including orders and subscriptions, should be addressed to: PACIFIC LIN GUISTICS Department of Linguistics Research School of Pacific Studies The AustralianNational University Canberra, A.C.T.
    [Show full text]
  • 2. Non-Native Speech Perception
    PERCEPTION OF ENGLISH VOWELS BY ADVANCED FINNISH LEARNERS Master’s Thesis Riina Kivikangas University of Jyväskylä Department of Languages and Communication Studies English May 2019 1 JYVÄSKYLÄN YLIOPISTO Tiedekunta – Faculty Laitos – Department Humanistis-yhteiskuntatieteellinen Kieli-ja viestintätieteet Tekijä – Author Riina Kivikangas Työn nimi – Title Perception of English Vowels by Advanced Finnish Learners Oppiaine – Subject Työn laji – Level Englannin kieli Maisterintutkielma Aika – Month and year Sivumäärä – Number of pages Toukokuu 2019 69 + liitteet 3 sivua Tiivistelmä – Abstract Vieraan kielen oppija kohtaa usein äänteitä, jotka eivät sisälly äidinkielen inventaarioon, ja joiden havainnointi saattaa tästä syystä olla vaikeaa. Tämän tutkimuksen tavoitteena oli selvittää, miten edistyneet suomalaiset oppijat havaitsevat englannin kielen vokaaleja. Lisäksi tutkimus pyrki selvittämään onko konsonanttikontekstilla vaikutusta vokaalien havainnointiin ja esiintyykö oppijoiden havainnoinnissa vokaalien periferaalisuudesta johtuvia direktionaalisia epäsymmetrioita. Tutkimukseen osallistui kaksikymmentä englantia yliopistossa opiskelevaa oppijaa. Tutkimus koostui kolmesta kuuntelutestistä, jotka selvittivät kuuden englannin kielen vokaalin identifikaatiota, assimilaatiota ja diskriminaatiota. Konsonanttikontekstin vaikutuksen selvittämiseksi vokaalit esitettiin sekä bilabiaalisessa että alveolaarisessa kontekstissa, ja direktionaalisten epäsymmetrioiden selvittämiseksi vokaaliparit esitettiin kummassakin suunnassa. Suomalaiset opiskelijat
    [Show full text]
  • CATALOGUE of SOUNDS for REVIVED TRADITIONAL CORNISH (Base 1500-1800)
    CATALOGUE OF SOUNDS FOR REVIVED TRADITIONAL CORNISH (base 1500-1800) We have evidence1 for 722 different sounds. Symbols according to the International Phonetic Alphabet Pure vowel sounds (23) DKSG 3 Symbol IPA No. Unicode Descriptive Name KS Spelling Example 1 [i:] 301 U+0069 Long close front unrounded vowel i, î, u, y, ÿ mis, cîder, tus, chy, prÿs 2 [i] 4 301 U+0069 Short close front unrounded vowel î lînednor 3 [e:] 5 302 U+0065 Long close-mid front unrounded vowel a, â, ai, e, ê, pras, prais, les, êsy, ë, eu bës, leur 4 [e] 6 302 U+0065 Short close-mid front unrounded vowel ê êtegves 5 [ɛ] 303 U+025B Short open-mid front unrounded vowel e, è, ê, eu pell, mès, yêhes, deuthons 6 [ɔ] 306 U+0254 Short open-mid back rounded vowel o, ò cot, gròn 7 [o:] 7 307 U+006F Long close-mid back rounded vowel o, ô, oo noth, hôk, coos 8 [o] 307 U+006F Short close-mid back rounded vowel ô, oo nôtednow, boostiow 9 [u:] 308 U+0075 Long close back rounded vowel oo, ou, u, û coos, toul, a-ugh, gûn 10 [u] 308 U+0075 Short close back rounded vowel oo, ow, û boostiow, Kernowek, ûsadow 11 [y:] 309 U+0079 Long close front rounded vowel u lus Version 10 May 2021 Ian Jackson, Desky Kernowek Support Group 12 [ø:] 310 U+00F8 Long close-mid front rounded vowel eu leur This vowel is often realized as intermediate between [ø:] and [e:] 13 [œ] 311 U+0153 Short open-mid front rounded vowel eu deuthons This vowel is often realized as intermediate between [œ] and [ɛ] 14 [ɒ:] 313 U+0252 Long open back rounded vowel â or au brâs, dauns 15 [ɒ] 313 U+0252 Short open back rounded
    [Show full text]