<<

Educational Psychologist

ISSN: 0046-1520 (Print) 1532-6985 (Online) Journal homepage: http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/hedp20

Beyond : The Case for Teaching Children the Logic of the English System

Jeffrey S. Bowers & Peter N. Bowers

To cite this article: Jeffrey S. Bowers & Peter N. Bowers (2017) Beyond Phonics: The Case for Teaching Children the Logic of the English Spelling System, Educational Psychologist, 52:2, 124-141, DOI: 10.1080/00461520.2017.1288571 To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00461520.2017.1288571

Published online: 07 Mar 2017.

Submit your article to this journal

Article views: 814

View related articles

View Crossmark data

Full Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at http://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journalCode=hedp20

Download by: [University of Bristol] Date: 29 September 2017, At: 07:35 EDUCATIONAL PSYCHOLOGIST, 52(2), 124–141, 2017 Copyright Ó Division 15, American Psychological Association ISSN: 0046-1520 print / 1532-6985 online DOI: 10.1080/00461520.2017.1288571

Beyond Phonics: The Case for Teaching Children the Logic of the English Spelling System

Jeffrey S. Bowers1 and Peter N. Bowers2 1School of Experimental Psychology, University of Bristol, 2WordWorks Centre, Kingston, Ontario, Canada

A large body of research supports the conclusion that early instruction in English should emphasize phonics, that is, the teaching of correspondences. By contrast, we argue that instruction should be designed to make sense of by teaching children that spellings are organized around the interrelation of , etymology, and . In this way, literacy can be taught as a scientific subject, where children form and test hypotheses about how their spelling system works. First, we review arguments put forward in support of phonics and then highlight significant problems with both theory and data. Second, we review the linguistics of English spellings and show that spellings are highly logical once all the relevant sublexical constraints are considered. Third, we provide theoretical and empirical arguments in support of the hypothesis that instruction should target all the cognitive skills necessary to understand the logic of the English spelling system.

The phrase “reading wars” refers to a long-standing debate have motivated the search for new methods (Compton regarding literacy instruction. At issue is whether early et al., 2014; Snowling & Hulme, 2014). instruction should focus on sublexical grapheme–phoneme Here we make a theoretical case for a very different correspondences (phonics) or target meaning at the form of instruction—what P. N. Bowers and Kirby (2010) level (whole instruction). This controversy con- called (SWI)—that takes key tinues in some settings, but both theoretical analyses and insights from both phonics and but goes far empirical findings have led to the widespread view that beyond either approach. SWI is motivated by a fundamen- phonics is the better approach. Indeed, phonics is now tal insight from linguistics, namely, the English spelling required in all UK state schools and is a standard teaching system makes sense when the sublexical constraints of mor- method in the United States and Canada. phology, etymology, and phonology are considered in com- Nevertheless, there is a growing realization that phonics bination (see The English Spelling System section for a

Downloaded by [University of Bristol] at 07:35 29 September 2017 instruction is not working for all children. It is estimated brief tutorial of the English system that includes a that 10%–15% of children who complete intense remedial table with linguistic terms and definitions). Consistent with phonological instruction continue to struggle (Compton, phonics, SWI agrees that it is important to teach sublexical Miller, Elleman, & Steacy, 2014), and an unacceptably grapheme–phoneme correspondences, but it emphasizes high number of children enter secondary school with very that English spellings are organized around the interrelation low levels of literacy skill (Higgins, Katsipataki, & Cole- of morphology, etymology, and phonology and that it is not man, 2014). Indeed, individual differences in reading skills possible to accurately characterize grapheme–phoneme cor- and knowledge are enormous: In the United respondences in isolation of these other sublexical con- Kingdom, for example, the performance of children straints. Consistent with whole language, SWI emphasizes between 11 and 16 years of age ranges from floor to ceiling the importance of meaning in literacy instruction from the (age equivalence of 6–17 years; Stothard, Hulme, Clarke, very start. Indeed, SWI exploits that fact that English spell- Barmby, & Snowling, 2010). These disappointing outcomes ings represent meaning in a consistent fashion, as reflected in morphology. This approach is different from phonics, whole lan- Correspondence should be addressed to Jeffrey S. Bowers, School of guage, and all other approaches because SWI teaches Experimental Psychology, University of Bristol, 12a Priory Road, Bristol, word-level literacy knowledge much like other scientific BS8-1TU, United Kingdom. E-mail: [email protected] disciplines (e.g., biology, physics). That is, children are LITERACY INSTRUCTION 125

engaged in generating and testing hypotheses about how PHONICS INSTRUCTION the system works (thus the word inquiry in “structured word inquiry”). The goal is to get children to understand Phonics instruction refers to a method of teaching reading whywordsarespelledthewaytheyareinorderto that focuses on sublexical grapheme–phoneme correspond- improve literacy, measured in terms of reading ences with little or no reference to other constraints on aloud, spelling words, improving vocabulary, and spelling. Children are taught the most common ways that improving . single letters or groups of letters () map onto Our article is organized in three main sections. First, in (e.g., the single letter grapheme can be pro- the Phonics Instruction section, we describe the theory and nounced /k/ and the phoneme /k/ can be represented by the practice of phonics, as well as summarize the evidence that graphemes , , , ,or) and are supports phonics for children in general and struggling instructed to blend the sounds associated with the letters readers in particular. We then challenge these conclusions, together to produce approximate pronunciations of words. in terms of both theory and practice: Too many readers For example, when taught the sounds for the letters , struggle with phonology-focused instruction. Second, in the , , and , the child can sound out the familiar The English Spelling System section we explain the ratio- word and less familiar or unknown words such as nale for SWI by reviewing the linguistics of the English . spelling system. We show that English is a Phonics instruction has several versions. Practitioners of morphophonemic system in which spellings have evolved , for example, teach children the pronunci- to represent sound (phonemes), meaning (), and ations associated with graphemes in isolation and then history (etymology) in an orderly way. coach students to blend the sounds together. A child might Third, the Teaching Children the Logic of the English be taught to break up the written word into its com- Spelling System section makes the theoretical and empiri- ponent letters, pronounce each letter in turn—/d/, /ɔ/, /g/— cal case that literacy instruction should focus on teaching then blend them together to form the spoken word dog this logic. Theoretically, SWI ensures that children can (Bowey, 2006). By contrast, in , the pho- exploit the well-established finding that memory is best nemes of a given word are not read in isolation. Rather, when the to-be-learned information is organized in a mean- children identify (analyze) words looking for a common ingful and logical manner and when the learning context is target phoneme across a set of words. For instance, children characterized by generating and testing hypotheses. Empiri- are taught that , , and share the letter cally, we review the growing that suggests that , which is pronounced /d/ (Moustafa & Maldonado- morphological instruction is beneficial, and then summarize Colon, 1998). In other words, synthetic goes from “parts to three studies that provide direct support of SWI. Table 1 wholes,” starting with letters and phonemes to build up summarizes the overall motivation of SWI. We hope these words, whereas analytic phonics goes from “wholes to arguments will inspire teachers and researchers interested parts,” starting with words and breaking them into their in literacy instruction to start paying more attention to the component parts, including onsets and rimes, as well as logic of the English spelling system when designing and phonemes. Some researchers conclude that synthetic pho- testing new forms of instruction. nics is more effective (e.g., Rose, 2006), although often it is claimed that the two approaches are equally effective TABLE 1 (, 2000; Torgerson, Brooks, & Hall, Downloaded by [University of Bristol] at 07:35 29 September 2017 Summary of SWI Motivation 2006). On these as well as other versions, including analogy 1. Linguistic analyses show that English spellings are well ordered and phonics and embedded phonics, the focus is on the associa- understandable once the sublexical constraints of phonology, tions between graphemes and phonemes. morphology, and etymology are jointly considered. Of course, children also need practice in reading mean- 2. When designing literacy instruction, the default assumption should be ingful text, and advocates of phonics emphasize the need to that instruction is informed by correct understanding of the writing go beyond grapheme–phoneme correspondences. Indeed, system. 3. There is a strong theoretical justification for SWI. This includes the the evidence strongly suggests that phonics is most effec- findings that (a) learning is best when information is studies in a tive in the context of a broader literacy curriculum (Camilli, meaningful and organized manner, (b) learning is best when learners are Vargus, & Yurecko, 2003). But for advocates of phonics, able to reason and hypothesize about a structured system, and (c) this broader context rarely includes sublexical constraints children who struggle with phonics often have phonological deficits. other than grapheme–phoneme correspondences. As an They may especially benefit by methods that focus more heavily on their alternative skills, including the skills that are known to improve learning example, the Independent Review of the Teaching of Early more generally. Reading (Rose, 2006) recommended that phonics should be 4. Preliminary empirical investigations support SWI. This is especially true the primary instructional approach but embedded within a of struggling readers who benefit the least from phonics. But more broader language and literacy curriculum (p. 70). However, empirical work is needed. no consideration is given to the morphological or etymolog- Note. SWI D Structured Word Inquiry. ical constraints on spelling. And again, no mention of 126 BOWERS AND BOWERS

morphology was made in Rose (2009). Similarly, the report into three distinct phonemes, /k/, /æ/, and /t/), has the stron- of the National Reading Panel (2000) in the United States, gest association with reading. This has led to the hypothesis entitled “Teaching Children to Read,” emphasizes that pho- that remedial instruction should target these skills to restore nics should be integrated with other forms of instruction, them (see Duff & Clarke, 2011; Fletcher, Lyon, Fuchs, & including , fluency, and comprehen- Barnes, 2007; Hulme & Snowling, 2009; Snowling & sion strategies, but the document fails to address the con- Hulme, 2011). Phonics targets relevant phonological skills straints imposed by morphology and etymology. So, by teaching children the mappings between graphemes and although we find widespread acceptance that phonics needs phonemes. to be supplemented by additional instruction, including In addition to theory, a large empirical literature has reading words in meaningful text, phonics instruction is assessed the efficacy of various methods on all aspects of often the main or only way children are taught to identify literacy achievement. This includes accuracy in naming individual words based on their spellings. familiar and unfamiliar words, reading fluency, spelling, Two theoretical arguments are commonly put forward in vocabulary knowledge, and reading comprehension. A key support of phonics, both for children in general and for finding is that phonics is often more effective at teaching struggling readers in particular. First, English spellings are these skills than alternative methods that do not focus on claimed to be based on an alphabetic system in which the grapheme–phoneme correspondences, for both children in primary purpose of letters is to represent sounds (e.g., general and struggling readers. For example, with regard to Byrne, 1998). The strong version of this hypothesis is that children in general, Ehri, Nunes, Stahl, and Willows (2001) spellings are “parasitic on ” (Mattingly, 1972), that compared systematic phonics instruction to unsystematic or is, the claim that spellings are entirely dependent on pro- no-phonics instruction on learning to read across 66 treat- nunciation of words. More recently, the analogy has been ment-control comparisons derived from 38 experiments. modified to the claim that spellings are “parasitic on Benefits from phonics were obtained for word reading, text language.” This later phrase recognizes the fact that letters comprehension, and spelling; effects persisted after instruc- represent more than phonemes (Perfetti, 2003; Seidenberg, tion ended. Similarly, Torgerson et al. (2006) identified 20 2011); nevertheless, the role of these other factors is gener- randomized control trials concerned with the initial teach- ally minimized or ignored when it comes to instruction. For ing of reading. Systematic phonics teaching was associated instance, Snowling and Hulme (2005) claimed that Mat- with better progress in reading accuracy across all ability tingly (1972) was “more or less right” (p. 397) and that levels compared to alternative teaching methods. With recoding (translating orthography to phonology) is indeed regards to remedial instruction, the effectiveness of differ- parasitic on speech. In their view, additional features of lan- ent treatment approaches to was recently guage are more relevant to sentence comprehension than to assessed in a meta-analysis of 22 randomized controlled tri- single-word reading. als (Galuschka, Ise, Krick, & Schulte-Koerne, 2014). Pho- The common conclusion from this characterization of nics was the only approach that had a significant effect on the English is that all children should first word reading (g’ D .32) and spelling (g’ D .34). be taught grapheme–phoneme correspondences. In this way children can learn to name aloud both familiar and unfamil- Some Problems With Phonics iar written words and use their spoken vocabulary to iden- tify and understand the words. As an example, although a The preceding findings support the conclusion that phonics Downloaded by [University of Bristol] at 07:35 29 September 2017 child may not have read the word before, he or she is better than many alternative approaches to literacy will likely be familiar with its spoken form, and accord- instruction. However, theoretical and empirical evidence ingly, he or she will be able to identify after decod- suggest that other forms of instruction may be better still. ing it. Once the familiar and unfamiliar written words can With regards to theory, the claim that English is an alpha- be decoded, then children can exploit the grammatical and betic system in which the primary purpose of letters is to semantic knowledge that support their language compre- represent sounds is incorrect. Rather, English is a morpho- hension in general. In this view, phonics is the key for phonemic system in which spellings have evolved to repre- unlocking the child’s existing verbal language skills. sent an interrelation of morphology, etymology, and The second theoretical argument in favor of phonics is phonology (see details that follow). As Venezky (1999) specifically concerned with remedial instruction. A large wrote, “ is not a failed phonetic tran- number of studies provide evidence for a phonological the- scription system, invented out of madness or perversity. ory of . On this view, deficits in phonological skills Instead, it is a more complex system that preserves bits of (e.g., phonemic awareness, rime awareness, and verbal history (i.e., etymology), facilitates understanding, and also short-term memory) play a causal role in many reading def- translates into sound” (p. 4). icits (Melby-Lervag, Lyster, & Hulme, 2012). Phonemic This has implications for literacy instruction if we accept awareness, the meta-linguistic ability to identify and that instruction should be guided by the logic of the ortho- manipulate phonemes (e.g., separating the spoken word cat graphic system, or as Rayner, Foorman, Perfetti, Pesetsky, LITERACY INSTRUCTION 127

and Seidenberg (2001), put it, “The child learning how to group, such as training in mathematics) with struggling read needs to learn how his or her writing system works” readers. They identified 11 studies that met their criterion (p. 34). Indeed, if this claim is taken seriously, the proper and reported a significant (moderate) effect on word and conclusion is that literacy instruction should target the way nonword reading accuracy, but no significant effect on orthography represents the interrelation of morphology, word or nonword reading fluency and no significant overall etymology, and phonology rather than selectively focus on effects on spelling or reading comprehension. Similarly, orthography to phonology mappings. Torgerson et al. (2006) identified three randomized control In addition, the theoretical assertion that the most effec- trials (Lovett, Ransby, Hardwick, Johns, & Donaldson, tive remedial instruction should target the weak phonologi- 1989; Martinussen & Kirby, 1998; O’Connor & Padeliadu, cal processes of struggling readers is an untested 2000) that assessed the impact of phonics on spelling (the hypothesis. An equally plausible hypothesis is that remedial three studies in this analysis were all different from the two instruction should target the strongest skills of struggling relevant studies included in the McArthur et al., 2012, readers that are relevant to reading. That is, remedial review). The pooled estimate for these three trials was a instruction may be best served by interventions that adopt a nonsignificant effect size of 0.09. These findings regarding “compensatory” as opposed to a “restitutive” strategy of spelling and comprehension contrast with the Ehri et al. (J. S. Bowers, 2016).1 Indeed, this is the case (2001) and Galuschka et al. (2014) studies. Finally, with with acquired disorders across a range of domains (Cice- regard to vocabulary development, we are not aware of any rone et al., 2005). Given the logical and meaningful struc- evidence that phonics is effective. Indeed, vocabulary ture of English spellings and given that dyslexia is often instruction typically takes a different form altogether (Mar- associated with a selective phonological deficit, a promis- ulis & Neuman, 2010; Moore, Hammond, & Fetherston, ing compensatory approach to instruction would target the 2014). semantic and logical skills of struggling readers that are left Even the most sophisticated phonological interventions untapped by phonics. The finding that memory is best when show mixed results. For example, a variety of research sug- information is encoded in a meaningful and structured man- gests that interventions should go beyond standard system- ner (Bower, Clark, Lesgold, & Winzenz, 1969) makes this atic phonics instruction to include training in phonological approach highly plausible, as detailed next. awareness and reinforcing letter-sound knowledge in the It is currently unclear whether remedial literacy instruc- context of reading text (e.g., Hatcher et al., 2006). Signifi- tion should target compensatory or restitutive mechanisms. cant overall effects on single word reading were reported in It may be that some reading difficulties are more effectively these interventions; nevertheless, a substantial minority addressed with a compensatory approach and others more children fail to respond. Hatcher et al. (2006) found that effectively addressed with a restitutive strategy. A potential failure to respond to the intervention (in terms of word important advantage of SWI is that it targets both phono- naming as well as letter knowledge and phoneme aware- logical and semantic skills of children, and accordingly this ness) was predicted by poor initial literacy skills. That is, approach may take advantage of both compensatory and the phonological intervention was least successful for those restitutive mechanisms. Indeed, as we detail next, the best children who need the most help. way to improve weak phonological skills may be to practice Furthermore, the two most recent intervention studies these skills in a meaningful context. Structural and meaning that directly compared phonics and “” training constraints clarify whether a letter sequence is analyzed as failed to show an advantage for phonics. In sight word Downloaded by [University of Bristol] at 07:35 29 September 2017 one set of graphemes or another (e.g., the sequence training condition, children carried out a variety of tasks cannot be a digraph in as these graphemes must designed to improve their ability to identify whole words be separate graphemes in the distinct morphemes rather than convert letters to sounds. In a quasi-randomized and ). trial (McArthur et al., 2013) and a randomized controlled With regard to the empirical evidence in support of pho- trial (McArthur et al., 2015), phonics and sight training pro- nics, it is important to emphasize that the effect sizes on lit- duced similar outcomes across a range of reading outcomes. eracy outcomes are generally small, and in many cases These sorts of results have led many supporters of pho- mixed. For example, McArthur et al. (2012) carried out a nics to highlight the need to explore additional methods. meta-analysis of all studies that compared a phonics inter- However, the hypothesis that instruction should be vention (phonics alone, phonics and phoneme awareness designed to teach children the logic of their writing system training, or phonics and irregular word reading training) to is rarely considered. For example, Al Otaiba and Fuchs a control condition (untrained or an alternative training (2006) wrote that “a different method or combination of methods” (p. 428) is needed for these children, but the authors do not consider the possibility that these children 1The term “restitutive” is used in the context of acquired disorders in which a person has lost a previously acquired skill. In the case of develop- should be taught about morphology, let alone about the mental disorders, perhaps a better term is “ameliorative” as the instruction interrelation of morphology, etymology, and phonology in is attempting to improve the impaired skill. explaining spellings. 128 BOWERS AND BOWERS

More recently, Compton et al. (2014) highlighted the morphology, and etymology. Rather than a perverse system limited success of phonics in remedial instruction and sug- that needs reform, some linguists call English spelling gested some possible ways to improve phonics, including “near optimal” (Chomsky & Halle, 1968). Whether or not teaching children that letter-sound correspondences depend English spellings are near optimal, the key claim we would on the position of letters within words as well as the sur- make is that English spellings are logical and can be inves- rounding letters (“context-dependent” decoding relation- tigated like other scientific subjects given that the English ships). They hypothesized that this might be achieved by spelling system is systematic. carefully designing training corpora that capture these posi- How can it be argued that English spelling system is sen- tional dependences so that children can implicitly learn sible and systematic? In this section we provide a brief tuto- these mappings with practice. This is worth considering, rial that highlights the role that meaning plays in but we would emphasize that morphological constraints are constraining spelling, the consistent way that bases are again ignored despite the fact that morphology plays a key spelled across a morphological family, the regular manner role in constraining grapheme–phoneme correspondences. in which affixes are added to bases, and the way in which Indeed, unlike the position constraints, morphological con- almost all English spellings can be understood. We also straints can easily be taught explicitly (e.g., instruction can highlight how phonics fails to explain all these aspects of highlight the fact that multiletter graphemes cannot straddle the English spelling system, and indeed does a poor job in a border; see next for more details). explaining how words are pronounced (the task for which it Similarly, Snowling and Hulme (2014) highlighted the was designed). See Table 2 for a list of key linguistic terms disappointing results of some phonology-based intervention that are needed for this tutorial. This sets the stage for the studies and considered what should be done. In addition to final section, Teaching Children the Logic of the English suggesting more intensive phonics, they emphasized the Spelling System, where we make the case that literacy need to improve pupil motivation. But again, the authors instruction should be informed by the logical structure of did not suggest that instruction should highlight the role the English spelling system. that morphology and etymology play in constraining spell- As a first illustration of the role that meaning plays in ing. We agree that it is important to make literacy instruc- constraining spelling, consider the principle tion more enjoyable, and we expect that teaching children (Venezky, 1999). English is rich with homophonic words— to generate and test hypotheses in order to make sense of those that share a pronunciation but differ in meaning. If the English spelling system will be more engaging than the prime purpose of spellings is to encode sounds, we additional remedial phonics. Indeed, the SWI approach fits should expect homophonic words to be spelled the same. It well with Dewey’s (1913, p. 14) characterization of the turns out that the is true. As Venezky (1999) conditions that generate “genuine interest,” as well as noted, when two words can share a pronunciation, they reports from teachers and children regarding the importance tend to differ in spelling to signal a difference in meaning. of experiments, demonstrations, and explanations for gen- The different spellings of , , and is not erating interest in a subject (Freeman, McPhail, & Berndt, an example of irregular spellings, but rather is an example 2002; Shulman, 1986; Zahorik, 1996). of how English spellings code for distinctions in meaning. To summarize, there is growing consensus that phonics The fact that we have many ways of writing the same instruction is not working for all children and little or no phonemes is usually identified as a burden to learning to evidence that phonics is effective for spelling, vocabulary, read and write. However, given that English has many Downloaded by [University of Bristol] at 07:35 29 September 2017 and reading comprehension. Even some of the strongest , it is necessary to have multiple graphemes for proponents of phonics are now acknowledging that new a given phoneme in order to mark meaning distinctions. methods are needed. In the next section we describe how That is, having many ways to write the same sound is a fea- the English orthographic system is organized. This provides ture—not a bug of the writing system. The grapheme that is the context for a possible alternative approach to literacy best suited for each member of a homophone pair can often instruction. be understood by considering words related in meaning and spellings that evolved at the same time—synchronic ety- mology. For example, the spellings , , and THE ENGLISH SPELLING SYSTEM all signal information about location, and that similar meaning is marked with the common letter It is widely assumed that the primary role of letters in sequence . This group of spellings distinguishes English is to represent sounds, and the many “exception” from its homophone , the latter of which words are generally taken to reflect a poorly designed spell- uses a spelling linking it to the word . Similarly, the ing system. However, this reflects a misunderstanding. In spelling serves to separate it from its homophones fact, the English spelling system is designed to encode both and links it to related words and .Ina pronunciation and meaning of words, and as a consequence, phonics approach, * would be a more “regular” English word spellings are constrained by phonology, spelling than . But, the digraph in * LITERACY INSTRUCTION 129

TABLE 2 Linguistic Terms and Definitions

Term Definition

Orthography Orthography is the writing system of that evolves to represent the meaning of a language to those who already speak that language. Phonology Phonology is the system by which speech sounds of a language represent meaning to those who speak that language. Phoneme and grapheme A phoneme is a minimal distinctive unit of speech than affects meaning. A grapheme is a single letter or combination of letters that represent a phoneme. Most graphemes can represent more than one phoneme, and most phonemes can be represented by more than one grapheme. Graphemes occur within morphemes. Morpheme and Morphemes are the meaning-bearing structures of words. Morphemes are bases or affixes (prefixes, suffixes and connecting vowel morphology letters). Morphology is the system by which morphemes combine to represent the meaning of words. Every word is either a base or a base with another morpheme fixed to it. Morphophonemic The morphophonemic principle refers to the fact that morphemes can vary widely in their phonological representation across related principle words. Relevance for literacy: English orthography has evolved to favor consistent representation of morphology over phonology to mark connections in meaning across words (e.g. consider the pronunciation of the base in the words , ,or). Etymology Etymology refers to the origins of words and the way in which their meanings and oral and written forms have changed over time. Diachronic etymology Diachronic etymology refers to the influence roots of words (e.g. Latin, Greek, Old English) have on the meanings and spellings of current words. Synchronic etymology Synchronic Etymology refers to ways in which letters mark connections (, , ) or distinctions of meaning (, , ) between spellings of words that evolved at the same time. Orthographic marker An orthographic marker is a letter or combination of letters that may or may not play a phonological role (may not be a grapheme); for example, the plural cancelling of and the etymological marker letter in . Relevance for literacy: Many common spellings can only be investigated and understood with the recognition that letters and letter combinations can have meaningful jobs without playing a phonological role. Base and root A base is the morpheme that carries the main kernel of meaning in a word. The base is what is left when all affixes are removed. A root by contrast is the historical origin of a word (diachronic etymology). For example, the base of is whereas the root is the Old English word helpan. We restrict the term “base” to morphology and “root” to etymology.

would destroy the spelling link between the meaningfully or and being classified as related words them, their, and they. Further, although “irregular words.” Once children understand that there is is not as common as for the /eɪ/ phoneme, it is reason behind these spellings, it is hard to imagine a ped- one of many available spellings for this phoneme as demon- agogically sound rationale for describing these words as strated by (linking to ) and exceptions that just have to be memorized by rote. (linking to and distinguishing from the homo- Morphology plays a more central role in constraining phone ). grapheme choice given that written morphemes in English In addition, and in contrast with the use consistent spellings across words with varied pronuncia- assumed in phonics, letters often have functions that have tions. Take for example the words sign, signal, design,and nothing to do with representing the sounds of words. For signature. In oral language English speakers are exposed to example, one of the jobs of the single, silent (i.e., nonsyl- varied pronunciations of the base :/sɪɡn/ (in < > < > < > ɪ < > Downloaded by [University of Bristol] at 07:35 29 September 2017 labic) e is to serve as an orthographic marker letter for signal and signature ), /za n/ (in design ), and the plural cancelling function in words like or /saɪn/ (in ). But the spelling is consistent. (Venezky, 1999), that is, can be used in The morphological relation between these spoken words is order to avoid words falsely appearing plural. Letters can opaque phonologically but transparent orthographically. also serve as etymological markers that link words related Thefactthatthe in and maps in meaning. The in the word is an etymologi- onto different pronunciations is not evidence of a poor cal marker linking it to words having to do with two things, spelling system; rather this is evidence that a key organiz- such as twice, twin, twenty, between. This marker also dis- ing principle of English spelling is to mark connections in tinguishes it from its homophones and . Simi- meaning. larly, the in serves as a cue to the link to The consistent spelling of morphemes can be highlighted words related in meaning like or . using the word matrix and word sums depicted in Figure 1. If these spelling markers were investigated in the early years The morphemic elements in the matrix correspond to the mor- of schooling, it would help make children aware that there is phemic elements in the word sum. These written morphemes almost always a meaningful explanation for a word’s spell- are abstract representations because they show the underly- ing. By contrast, instruction that fails to highlight that letters ing lexical spelling that may or may not surface in a complete have numerous jobs, including jobs that have nothing to do word. For example, in Figure 1 the full written form of the with pronunciation, results in words like and <-ate> suffix is found in the matrix and the relevant word 130 BOWERS AND BOWERS

FIGURE 1 Word matrix and word sums for the base .

sums. However, only the surface realization of It is important to emphasize that morphology, etymol- includes the full <-ate> suffix. In , the forward ogy, and phonology interact. For example, morphology slash in the word sum marks the fact that the final, nonsyllabic constrains grapheme–phoneme correspondences because of the <-ate> suffix is replaced by the vowel suffix graphemes cannot straddle morphemic boundaries. Con- <-ure> in conformity with the reliable convention of adding sider the letter sequence in the words and vowel suffixes. The matrix and the word sum reveal the . In the word the falls within a underlying abstract spellings of members of a morphological morpheme so it is a digraph, which in this case represents family and show how they resolve into the surface ortho- the /iː/ phoneme. However, the same sequence strad- graphic representation. Henry (2003) was the first to point to dles a morpheme in , and accordingly the the matrix in the instructional research literature. P. N. and must belong to different graphemes that map onto Bowers and Cooke (2012) argued that these tools provide a separate phonemes. Because phonics fails to consider the concrete representation of Carol Chomsky’s (1970) concept role of morphology in constraining grapheme–phoneme of “lexical spelling.” correspondences, many words are considered irregular that Investigating the and word families with in fact have a perfectly good explanation. the matrices in Figure 2 provides another good illustration Another important interaction is found between mor- of how lexical spelling can explain the spelling of the word phology and diachronic etymology. Diachronic etymology , which is typically described as an irregular refers to the historical influences of word origins on the “sight” word. The grapheme is pronounced /u:/ in the spelling and meaning of current words, with each word hav- stand-alone word , whereas it is pronounced /ʌ/ when ing the same origin (a common root). To conclude that two the <-es> suffix is added to form the word . words are part of the same morphological family (e.g., as is Instruction based on the alphabetic principle can describe the case with and ), two criteria must be only as an example of the perversity of the English established. An analysis with a word sum must show that writing system (on phonics, a more appropriate spelling the two words have an underlying morphological structure would be *). However, the parallel morphological that reveals both words share a base of identical spelling structure of the and families shows the logic (e.g., de C sign ! design). In addition, there must be evi- Downloaded by [University of Bristol] at 07:35 29 September 2017 of these spellings. It makes no sense to claim that dence that the two words share the same root—the same is a “regular” word whereas is “irregular” when diachronic etymology (e.g., the root of both and instruction reflects the actual interrelation of morphology is from Latin root signare for “mark, token”). and phonology in English. Discovering that the underlying meaning of “mark, token” is echoed in both of these words can deepen the understand- ing of both words and their spellings. In some cases, words that might appear to be members of the same morphological family (e.g., and ) are not relatives because they do not share a root (e.g., derives from the Latin root plicare “to fold,” whereas the free base goes back to the unrelated Old English root plegan, plegian for “frolic, mock, perform”). The fact that all words in a morphological family share a root is important because it means that words in a morpho- logical family share some meaning. In many cases, the < > FIGURE 2 Word matrices and word sums for the bases and meaning relations are transparent (e.g., sign and . ) and in other cases, the relations are less obvious LITERACY INSTRUCTION 131

(e.g., and ), but in most cases it is possi- Treiman, 2001), but additional sources of irregularity arise ble to see how morphological relatives are related in mean- when considering multisyllabic words (that constitute more ing to the root meaning (e.g., in the case of you than 90% of word types in English; Baayen, Piepenbrock, sign a “letter of resignation”). Of course, it is often possible & van Rijn, 1993). For instance, how is a child to know that to propose meaningful relations between words (e.g., stress should be placed on the second syllable of , and both involve action). The only , and despite the fact that most way to ensure that two words share a common root is to English words are pronounced with first syllable stress? consult an etymological . As detailed next, dia- Furthermore, it is not the case that children first learn chronic etymology and etymological play a words for which the most frequent grapheme–phoneme cor- central role in SWI. respondences apply. In fact the high-frequency words In sum, the English spelling system is designed to repre- included in children’s text are more likely to be irregular sent both pronunciation and meaning, and the spelling of (according to phonics) compared to the low-frequency most if not all English words can be explained once the words in adult text; the majority of the words in The Child- constraints of phonology, morphology, and etymology are ren’s Printed Word Database are multisyllabic (77%), and jointly considered. The claim that English is full of excep- the majority of words from third-grade text are morphologi- tion words that just have to be remembered (so-called sight cally complex (60%–80%; Anglin, 1993). Accordingly, words) reflects a fundamental misunderstanding of the writ- children are being taught phonics in a context in which per- ing system, and as we detail next, this misunderstanding fect performance will lead to many errors. And given that precludes forms of instruction that emphasize reasoning struggling readers have phonological deficits, they will pre- and hypothesis testing in order to teach children that their sumably not be able to fully exploit these sublexical writing system is logical. regularities. Sound-to-spelling (as opposed to spelling-to-sound) cor- Is the Theory Underpinning Phonics a Close Enough respondences are even more irregular when instruction is Approximation to the English Spelling System? based on the alphabetic principle. For example, Hanna, Hanna, Hodges, and Rudorf (1966) developed a complex A possible response to the preceding analysis is to agree set of phoneme–grapheme correspondences in an attempt that English spellings are organized by phonology, mor- to provide an estimate of the best possible sublexical spell- phology, and etymology but at the same time maintain that ing performance. They found that their algorithm could cor- the overwhelming constraint on English spellings is pho- rectly spell only 49.9% of the words taken from a database nology (e.g., Byrne, 1998). In such a case, it might be of 17,000 words: 36.4% of the words were spelled with one tempting to conclude that morphology and etymology can error, 11.4% with two errors, and 2.3% with three spelling be safely ignored (at least at the start of instruction) so that errors. More recently, Kreiner (1992) estimated that 60% of children can focus on the critical factor, namely, graph- all words are irregular for spelling, and Crystal (2003) esti- eme–phoneme correspondences. mated that 56% of words can be predicted by phonological But this greatly underestimates the role of morphology rules. and etymology in shaping spellings. In fact, grapheme–pho- Given the limitations of sublexical grapheme-to-pho- neme correspondences by themselves fail to explain the neme and phoneme-to-grapheme correspondences, it is pronunciation of a relatively high percentage of words. clear that children need to learn many lexical-orthographic Downloaded by [University of Bristol] at 07:35 29 September 2017 Consider the Dual Route Cascaded model of word naming representations early on. How is this best achieved? that includes sublexical grapheme–phoneme correspond- Because it is widely assumed that there is little or no logic ences (Coltheart, Rastle, Perry, Langdon, & Ziegler, 2001). to these spellings, children are typically expected to The sublexical route mispronounces (i.e., generates the remember them by rote, often on a case-by-case basis (e.g., wrong phonological transcription for) 16% of the 8,000 learning to spell words through the “Look, say, cover, monosyllabic words in the model’s vocabulary (i.e., only write, check” method). However, given that most words’ 84% of these monosyllabic words are regular). Similarly, spellings can be understood when morphology, etymology, 16% of the monosyllabic words included in the The Child- and phonology are jointly considered, it is possible that lex- ren’s Printed Word Database are mispronounced by the ical-orthographic forms might be learned through more sublexical route.2 It is possible to explain the pronunciation meaningful instruction. We consider this possibility next. of a higher percentage of monosyllable words if larger sub- lexical letter-sound correspondences are included, such as Teaching Children the Logic of the English Spelling rimes (the vowel and following consonants; Kessler & System

2These percentages are based on the DRC 2.0.0-beta.3511’s vocabulary We are advancing the hypothesis that children should be and GPC rules. I thank Max Coltheart and Steven Saunders for providing taught the logic of the English writing system. In this way, us this information. literacy instruction can be designed much like instruction in 132 BOWERS AND BOWERS

other systematic domains that are studied scientifically pattern (Konkle, Brady, Alvarez, & Oliva, 2010; Wiseman (e.g., biology, physics). That is, children can be engaged in & Neisser, 1974). generating and testing hypotheses about how the system These findings are relevant given that morphological works. instruction requires children to focus on the meaning and In this final section we support this hypothesis on both interrelations between words. Consider the word matrices theoretical and empirical grounds. Our main theoretical in Figures 1 and 2 that highlight how word spellings are argument is that the learning processes encouraged by SWI organized by meaning. There is every reason to think that are the very same learning processes that are known to sup- studying a set of words in a matrix will improve memory port effective learning more generally. We then review because it highlights the meaningful relations between empirical evidence that supports SWI. Although only a few words, much as the method used by Bower et al. (1969). studies have directly tested SWI, the results are promising, Indeed, given the role that meaning has in visual memory especially in the context of a growing literature highlighting (e.g., Konkle et al., 2010), it should be expected that the the successes of morphological interventions, and most meaningful study of written words will help children important in the context of the theoretical motivation of remember the visual forms of words (i.e., their spellings). SWI. By contrast, in standard phonics instruction many words in a morphological family will be encountered separately Theoretical Motivations for Structured Word Inquiry from one another, with little or no consideration of how words relate to one another, much like the random condi- A key insight from psychology that helps motivate struc- tion in Bower et al. (1969) memory study. tured word inquiry is that information is better remembered A second insight from psychology that motivates struc- if it is encoded in an elaborative and organized manner. For tured word inquiry is that memory and learning benefit example, Craik and Tulving (1975) manipulated the extent from a strategy labeled “explanatory questioning.” Roe- to which participants encoded the meanings of words dur- diger and Pyc (2012) introduced this term to describe two ing the study phase of a memory experiment. Memory was related techniques, namely, elaborative interrogation and best for words encoded in a meaningful manner. Further- self-explanation. Elaborative interrogation involves gener- more, memory is better when the to-be-learned words are ating plausible explanations as to why some stated fact is presented in a format that highlights the relations between true. Roediger and Pyc gave the example of a student being items. Bower et al. (1969) carried out a memory experi- told that it takes longer for Neptune to revolve around the ment in which a set of words were displayed within a hier- sun than it takes Mars. Students should ask themselves why archy that highlighted the meaning and the interrelations this is the case. By answering the question why, they are between words, as depicted in Figure 3, or alternatively, better able to remember this information. Self-explanation words were presented in an unstructured manner by placing involves students monitoring their learning and reflecting them randomly within the hierarchy. Memory was approxi- on some features of their learning. For example, while read- mately 3 times better when the words were presented in an ing a new page of text, a child might ask him- or herself, organized manner. Visual memory is also dramatically bet- What facts do I already know? What facts are new? Both ter when meaning can be attached to the to-be-remembered strategies encourage students to be active learners, and both Downloaded by [University of Bristol] at 07:35 29 September 2017

FIGURE 3 To-be-remembered words displayed in hierarchy. LITERACY INSTRUCTION 133

are effective (Dunlosky, Rawson, Marsh, Nathan, & Will- (separate from mappings linking orthography to phonol- ingham, 2013). ogy). These models are all consistent with the strong evi- Structured word inquiry is well suited to inspiring dence that orthographic word knowledge can be accessed explanatory questioning. Rich classroom discussions can independently of first computing grapheme–phoneme cor- grow from questions about the structure–meaning connec- respondences and that orthographic word knowledge can tions of complex words and their morphological relatives. directly access semantic knowledge (J. S. Bowers, Davis, For example, children can be presented with words such as & Hanley, 2005; Hino, Lupker, & Taylor, 2012; Taft, & play, playful, replay, plays, plane, playmate, and say and van Graan, 1998), even in children (Nation & Cocksey, investigate the structure and meaning of these words with 2009). word sums and matrices to develop and test hypotheses This raises the question of how to best learn ortho- about which words are from the same morphological family graphic word forms (in addition to grapheme–phoneme cor- and which are not. respondences). We have no doubt that learning grapheme– Consider instruction that explores the question of phoneme correspondences is essential, with phonological whether and belong to the same mor- codes serving as a “teacher” to the orthographic system phological family. A word sum shows that there is a possi- (Share, 1995; Ziegler, Perry, & Zorzi, 2014). But there is ble structural connection given . every reason to believe that instruction directed at But is the base part of ? How can we highlighting the meaning relations between morphological know? Here children can be introduced to the idea that forms of words would be helpful in learning lexical ortho- words that share the same base must also share the same graphic forms as well. Indeed, given that almost everyone root. Using an , the teacher can help agrees that skilled reading involves both converting graphe- children see that derives from the Latin root pli- mes to phonemes and accessing lexical-orthographic repre- care “to fold.” So to “display” something is to “unfold” it. sentations directly, it makes good theoretical sense to However, the free base goes back to the unrelated develop instruction that targets both processes. Whereas Old English root plegan, plegian for “frolic, mock, per- phonics targets only the former processes, structured word form.” In this way, the hypothesis that and inquiry targets both. In line with this view, Share (2011) belong to a common morphological family can recently proposed that the self-teaching hypothesis that be rejected. A lesson organized this way underscores the originally emphasized phonological recoding as the central relationship between morphology and etymology. means by which readers acquire lexical-orthographic repre- Kirby and Bowers (in press) used the phrase “structure sentations should also incorporate morphological knowl- and meaning test” to describe the scientific process of using edge in the process. word sums and etymological references to test a hypothesis Consistent with this analysis, Kirby and Bowers (in of whether any two words share a base element. Teachers press) proposed a theoretical framework that motivates and students learn that in order to draw a conclusion about instruction designed to strengthen the links between mor- whether two words like and are related, phological, phonological, orthographic, and semantic repre- they need evidence from both structural analysis with a sentations. They described morphology as a binding agent word sum (morphology) and historical (etymology) connec- given that it is the one representation that is directly linked tions. In this way, the structure and meaning test provides a to semantics, orthography, and phonology. Their binding method to draw a principled conclusion based on objective agent theory draws explicitly from Perfetti’s (2007) lexical Downloaded by [University of Bristol] at 07:35 29 September 2017 criteria. We would like to emphasize that studying words quality hypothesis. In this view, the key to literacy includes through a linguistic analysis exploits explanatory question- not only the quality of separate language representations ing that is well documented to support excellent memory but also the quality of the interrelation of the representa- and learning. tions. There are many advantages to well-integrated mental A third motivation for structured word inquiry comes representations (Perfetti, 2007). Kirby and Bowers pro- from theories of reading. Almost all theories and models of posed that morphology offers an agent for better integration word identification and naming assume that skilled reading of the mental representations of words by facilitating clues involves identifying words lexically (unmediated by letter- to meaning from both orthography and phonology. sound correspondences). This includes the dual route cas- Structured word inquiry is promising for a fourth reason: caded model (Coltheart et al., 2001), the connectionist dual In principle, it can support robust and widespread generali- process model (Perry, Ziegler, & Zorzi, 2007), as well as zation, such that learning one word provides insights into the interactive activation model (McClelland & Rumelhart, how to spell and understand many related words. Surpris- 1981) and the spatial coding model (Davis, 2010), among ingly little evidence addresses what types of learning condi- many others. Similarly, parallel distributed processing tions engender useful transfer (Roediger, Finn, & models of word naming (Seidenberg & McClelland, 1989) Weinstein, 2012), but generalization is possible only in are typically developed within the “triangle model” frame- conditions in which information is organized and system- work that includes mappings from orthography to meaning atic such that previous learning is relevant to new 134 BOWERS AND BOWERS

situations. In a completely arbitrary world, no generaliza- Henry, 1989, 2003/2010; Nunes, Bryant, & Olsson, 2003; tion is possible. Thus it is highly relevant that the structured Stephens & Hudson, 1984). In addition, it has become clear word inquiry approach highlights how the English writing that young children draw on untaught morphological knowl- system is structured for meaning and details the spelling edge during the process of becoming literate. For example, rules that explain how affixes are added to bases. Thus untaught morphological awareness explains unique variance structured word inquiry is well suited to support Kirby and for reading (Carlisle, 2000; Deacon & Kirby, 2004), vocabu- Lawson’s (2012) definition of high-quality learning: lary learning (Nagy, Berninger, & Abbot, 2006; Stahl & “learning that results in knowledge that is extensive, inte- Nagy, 2006), and spelling (Deacon & Bryant, 2006a, 2006b; grative and generative so that it supports transfer” (p. 3). Kemp, 2006; Perry, Ziegler, & Coltheart, 2002). But only By contrast, phonics instruction fails to represent (and mis- recently have a sufficient number of morphological interven- characterizes) many of these key rules, including graph- tion studies been carried out to support reviews and meta- eme–phoneme correspondences, and as a consequence, less analyses (P. N. Bowers, Kirby, & Deacon, 2010; Carlisle, generalization is possible. 2010; Goodwin & Ahn, 2010, 2013; Reed, 2008). Consistent with these analyses, preliminary evidence Here we focus on two meta-analyses of Goodwin and suggests that morphological instruction supports generali- Ahn (2010, 2013) who assessed the statistical significance zation in children. P. N. Bowers and Kirby (2010) used the of effect sizes. First, Goodwin and Ahn (2010) looked spe- structured word inquiry approach in Grade 4/5 classrooms cifically at studies focusing on populations with learning with matrices and word sums to investigate the morphologi- disabilities. The authors searched for both published and cal structure of word families. They found the experimental unpublished literature from 1980 to 2010 relying on a vari- group was better than controls at defining untaught words ety of databases, with keywords including morphology, that shared a base with taught words. The results of this morphemes, morphological awareness, intervention, study are described in more detail in the next section. instruction, learning disabilities, student characteristics, Finally, we would like to make a more general point. language impairments, reading difficulties, reading ability, Because structured word inquiry teaches literacy as a scien- vocabulary development, reading, structural analysis,and tific subject, the complexity of study—as in any other scien- language processing. They synthesized 79 standardized tific subject—can vary from rudimentary to advanced. mean-change differences between control and treatment Accordingly, this form of instruction is appropriate for all groups from 17 independent studies. These studies varied in age ranges, from young children (as early as Grade 1; Dev- a number of ways, including the nature of the intervention onshire, Morris, & Fluck, 2013, as discussed next) to adults (some interventions included only morphological instruc- interested in understanding the linguistics of the English tion, others included morphological instruction as part of writing system. By contrast, phonics is intended only as a larger intervention), the nature of the control condition (con- short-term form of instruction that, if all goes well, is aban- trol groups included participants who received other types doned in Grade 3. With older “treatment resistors” who are of instruction with no morphological components, e.g., con- struggling, relatively little is offered at present other than textual instruction or phonological instruction), the design more phonics and more practice reading text. Structured of the study (experimental designs with random assignment, word inquiry opens up a wide range of possibilities for quasi-experimental design, or nonexperimental design), the older children, with meaningful scientific investigations number of students in an instructional group, the length of that have the potential to be far more interesting. the intervention, and the nature of the language difficulty Downloaded by [University of Bristol] at 07:35 29 September 2017 (such as speech and language delay or poor spelling). This Empirical Evidence in Support of SWI makes it difficult to make any strong conclusions, but never- theless a number of statistically significant results did Our main goal has been to advance a theoretical argument emerge that highlight the promise of morphological in support of SWI with the hope that it will motivate more instruction. empirical research. But there is also growing empirical evi- Overall morphological instruction showed a moderate dence for this approach, both indirectly from morphological and significant improvement on literacy achievement intervention studies and directly through a small number of (d D 0.33). Breaking down the outcome measures, they studies that have carried out SWI interventions. We con- reported the largest effects on the metalinguistic tasks of sider these two data sets in turn. (d D 0.49) and morphological awareness (d D 0.40) but also reported significant effects Morphological intervention studies: Review of the across a range of literacy measures, namely, vocabulary research. Researchers in psychology have noted for quite (d D 0.40), reading comprehension (d D 0.24), and spelling some time that English spellings are constrained by mor- (d D 0.20). There were no significant advantages on word phology and argued that morphology should play a more decoding (d D 0.23) or fluency (d D –.28). Important to prominent role in literacy instruction (Bryant & Nunes, note, morphological instruction was effective for children 2004; Henderson, 1984; Henderson, & Templeton, 1986; categorized with a wide range of language disorders. LITERACY INSTRUCTION 135

Specially, children with speech and language delay showed emphasis on phonological training is less than optimal, the largest improvement averaging overall all outcome especially for those children who have poor phonologi- measures (d D 0.77), followed by children learning English cal-processing skills. By contrast, morphological instruc- as a second language (d D 0.62), followed by struggling tion emphasizes the role that meaning plays in readers (d D 0.46), poor spellers (d D 0.24), children with organizing spellings, and accordingly, morphological learning disabilities (d D 0.22), and children with reading interventions for strugglingreadersmaytakeadvantage disabilities (d D 0.17). of compensatory, as opposed to restitutive, processes. In addition, Goodwin and Ahn (2013) carried out a Second, morphological instruction was effective, and meta-analysis of morphological studies carried out on stu- often most effective, with younger children (this was also dents from the general population. Again, the authors found by P. N. Bowers et al., 2010; Carlisle, 2010). This searched for both published and unpublished literature rely- directly contradicts the common claim that morphology ing on a variety of databases, with keywords including should be taught only, if at all, following phonics (Adams, morph, intervention, instruction, vocab, read, spell, 1990; Ehri & McCormick, 1998; Larkin & Snowling, decod, fluency, structural analysis, and language process- 2008). Again, this may reflect the fact that learning and ing. They synthesized 92 standardized mean differences (d) memory are best when information is encoded in a mean- from 30 independent studies that again varied in many ways ingful and organized manner. As far as we are aware, this is (e.g., nature of the intervention, control group, design of the true for everyone. study, age of student population, etc.). Once again the authors reported a moderate overall effect of morphological Structured word inquiry: Initial research. The pre- instruction (d D 0.32). Breaking down performance, the ceding studies provide some evidence that teaching chil- analyses revealed significant intervention effects on mor- dren about morphology, especially young and struggling phological knowledge (d D 0.44), phonological awareness readers, is beneficial. This lends some indirect support to (d D 0.48), as well as the literacy outcome measures of SWI given that SWI emphasizes the importance of mor- decoding (d D 0.59; that was not significant in the previous phology for instruction. Nevertheless, these morphological meta-analysis), vocabulary (d D 0.34), and spelling (d D interventions differ from the SWI approach in a number of 0.30). However, in this meta-analysis (unlike the pervious important ways. meta-analysis), no overall effect on reading comprehension First, most morphological studies are not designed to (d D 0.09) was observed. Nevertheless, it is worth noting teach children the logic of the writing system. For example, that a moderate effect on reading comprehension was P. N. Bowers et al. (2010) reported that of the 22 studies in obtained for interventions of more than 20 hr (d D 0.40), their meta-analysis, only four (excluding SWI studies) for small-group interventions (d D 0.38), and for interven- explicitly targeted the fact that the spellings of a base tions with preschool/early elementary students (d D 0.40). within a morphological family are consistent despite pro- Important to note, the greatest overall effect was nunciation changes (as in and ). obtained for children in preschool/early elementary Although morphology has evolved to represent meaning grades (.68) compared to upper elementary (.29) and with consistent spelling, the failure to teach this founda- middle school (.34), suggesting that morphological inter- tional fact of the English spelling system is widespread ventions should start early. In addition, and broadly con- even when morphology is included in instructions. For sistent with the earlier Goodwin and Ahn (2010) study, example, the document “English Programmes of Study: Downloaded by [University of Bristol] at 07:35 29 September 2017 children learning English as a second language showed Key Stages 1 and 2. National Curriculum in England” the largest intervention effect (d D 0.54), followed by (Department for Education, 2013) includes a list of affixes children with learning disabilities (d D 0.37), then poor that legally must be taught (e.g., –ment, –ness, –ful, –less, readers/spellers (d D 0.35), with typical achievers etc.) to Year 2 students (pp. 46–47). Then on page 48, a list benefiting the least (d D 0.29). of common “exception” words is provided, including the We would like to highlight two findings from these words “because,” “Christmas,” and “children.” But in fact, meta-analyses. First, morphological instruction consis- these words highlight the regular nature of spellings: be C tently produced greatest benefits with struggling readers cause ! because; Christ C mas ! Christmas; child C ren (not only in Goodwin and Ahn, 2010, 2013, but also in ! children. This list of irregular words reflects a failure to the P. N. Bowers et al., 2010, as well as the Reed, understand the logic of orthographic morphology, as the 2008, analyses). Why might struggling readers tend to following quote at the bottom of the exception word list benefit most from morphological instruction (whereas shows: “Note: ‘children’ is not an exception to what has struggling readers tend to benefit least with phonics; been taught so far but is included because of its relationship Hatcher et al., 2006)? One obvious point to note is that with ‘child’” (p. 48). most struggling readers are typically failing in the con- Second, as far as we can tell, none of the morphological text of a curriculum that already emphasizes grapheme– studies (apart from the SWI studies) taught words in the phoneme correspondences. It may be that the continued context of their morphological families (e.g., teaching the 136 BOWERS AND BOWERS

stem alongside a set of its morphological relatives, Spelling Scheme (Jackman, 1997). This requires children to such as , , , ). complete different written activities. Critically, it employs Rather, interventions tended to include pairs of words (e.g., phonic strategies and does not explicitly teach morphemic ), often focusing on the fact that a segmentation or identification. The emphasis is on teaching given affix can be added to many different words (e.g., children spelling patterns that represent a particular sound , , – (e.g., that tion makes the sound /ʃɘn/). ). This is far from ideal because it fails to high- On a preintervention test, children were matched on a light the meaning relations between words in a morphologi- customized spelling test that included morphologically cal family. In addition, selecting morphological pairs is less complex words as well as the standardized Schonell Spell- likely to highlight phonological shifts in the pronunciation ing Test (Schonell & Goodacre, 1971). Both groups of the base than when multiple affixes are studied in associ- improved at postintervention, but the children in the SWI ation with one base (explaining why there is a in condition outperformed the control group in both tests with ). As just noted, memory is best when materials can the interaction between time of testing and intervention be studied in a meaningful and organized manner (Bower group significant for both tests, F(1, 66) D 4.56, p D .036, et al., 1969), and nevertheless, most morphological study partial h2 D .06; F(1, 66) D 25.99, p < .001, partial h2 D fails to exploit this fact. .28, respectively. Third, many morphological intervention studies teach More recently, Devonshire et al. (2013) assessed the morphology in isolation, separate from phonological and ety- impact of SWI on spelling and naming aloud words in a mological constraints. For example, the P. N. Bowers and fully randomized control study with 120 children. In this Kirby (2010) meta-analysis of morphological interventions case Year 1 and Year 2 mixed ability children in the United found 11 of 22 studies selectively focused on morphology, Kingdom ranging in age from 5 to 7 were assigned to the with no consideration of other sublexical constraints on spell- intervention first condition (first exposed to SWI and then ing, let alone how morphology, etymology, and phonology standard classroom instruction) or the intervention second interact. A central tenet of SWI is that children need to study condition (first given standard classroom instruction fol- the interrelation between morphology, etymology, and pho- lowed by SWI). The intervention was composed of daily nology in order to understand the writing system. 15- to 25-min lessons over the 6 weeks. In these sessions Fourth, because most morphological studies fail to explain children were taught the logic of English spelling, with the logic of the writing system, it is not possible for children explicit instruction on morphology, etymology, and phonol- to carry out word investigations, making generating and test- ogy, and the key rules that guide spellings. Children learned ing hypotheses about what words belong to a morphological how the spelling of a base word is retained in derived and family. That is, these morphological interventions all failed inflected words even when the pronunciation changes, as in to exploit explanatory questioning that is another robust way and . By contrast, in the standard to improve learning (Roediger & Pyc, 2012). It is encourag- classroom instruction condition children were taught pho- ing that the existing morphological intervention studies show nics (“business as usual” condition). promising results, but for the reasons just stated, we predict Testing took place prior to the intervention, halfway that SWI will prove to be far more effective. through the intervention (after the “intervention first” group In fact, we know of only three SWI interventions. First, completed 6 weeks of instruction), and at the end of the Devonshire and Fluck (2010) assessed the impact of SWI on intervention. Before the intervention the two groups per- Downloaded by [University of Bristol] at 07:35 29 September 2017 spelling in groups of Year 3 and Year 4 children in the United formed similarly on a customized spelling test that included Kingdom ranging 7–9 years (M age D 8years5months). morphologically complex words as well as the standardized The study employed a quasi-randomized design, with 38 chil- Schonell spelling test and the standardized Schonell reading dren from a Year 3 and Year 4 class completing the SWI test. After 6 weeks, children in the intervention first condi- intervention, and 34 children taken from another Year 3 and tion outperformed the children in the intervention second Year 4 class carrying out an alternative reading intervention group on all three measures; in the final test (after both described next. The school contained mixed ability children groups had completed the SWI intervention), both groups from a predominantly middle-class background. The inter- performed equally well. Once again, robust interactions vention involved 9 £ 35-min lessons. In the SWI condition were found between time of testing and intervention group children were taught to identify base words and affixes and to for all tests. Most important, on the second test (when only combine morphemes (e.g., drop the final from a base the intervention first group had received morphological word when adding a vocalic suffix: love/ C ing ! loving). instruction), the intervention first group performed better Children were also taught about basic etymology, such as the than the intervention second group on all measures: Scho- fact that certain silent letters are etymological markers link- nell Spelling effect size d D .63, customized spelling effect ing words that share a root (e.g., the in serves size d D .84, and Schonell Reading effect size d D .52. to link the words , , ,and Finally, P. N. Bowers and Kirby (2010) carried out an ). The control group worked through the Nelson intervention that assessed the impact of structured word LITERACY INSTRUCTION 137

inquiry on vocabulary acquisition in a quasi-randomized pointed out that in studying a matrix, the teacher can intervention study in which two Grade 4 and two Grade 5 offer deep and rich instruction for a few words in a matrix classes (81 children) were randomly assigned to the struc- and, at the same time, show how their spelling and meaning tured word inquiry and control condition, with the con- relate to other words (and potential words) in the matrix, straint that each condition included one Grade 4 and one providing instruction that is wide (and that supports Grade 5 class. The control group classes continued with generalization). typical instruction while the structured word inquiry group Together, these structured word inquiry intervention classes participated in three or four 50-min lessons each studies not only show that children can learn about the logi- week until 20 sessions were completed. cal structure of words but also provide some preliminary The structured word inquiry group investigated word evidence that learning about the phonological, morphologi- families with word sums and matrices. Participants learned cal, and etymological constraints on English spelling to develop and test hypotheses about spelling–meaning improves decoding (Devonshire et al., 2013), spelling connections between words. Among the topics covered, the (Devonshire & Fluck, 2010), and vocabulary knowledge lessons explained three suffixing changes (replacing final, (P. N. Bowers & Kirby, 2010). single, silent s; doubling final, single consonants; and / changes), explained the difference between Possible Objection to Structured Word Inquiry bound and free base elements, explained the difference between etymological families (share a root) and morpho- One possible objection to SWI is that young children in logical families (share a root and a base element), and Grade 1 who are just starting to read do not have the cogni- highlighted that the underlying spelling of morphemes as tive skills necessary to explore the interrelations between shown by the matrix and the word sum are consistent orthography, phonology, morphology, and etymology. despite pronunciation shifts across related words. Indeed, it is often claimed that morphological instruction After the intervention, children were tested on defini- should occur only after phonics (Adams, 1990; Ehri & tions of three categories of words: (a) “Word Taught” McCormick, 1998; Larkin & Snowling, 2008). For exam- words that children may (but not necessarily) have been ple, Ehri’s influential developmental model (Ehri & exposed to over the course of the 20 lessons, (b) “Base McCormick, 1998) posited five phases of learning to read: Taught” words that were never presented in lessons but that prealphabetic, partial alphabetic, full alphabetic, consoli- shared a base with a “Word Taught” word, and (c) “Affix dated alphabetic, and automatic. Instructional recommenda- Taught” words that included trained affixes but with bases tions were offered for each stage. Morphology does not that had been carefully avoided. Regression analysis appear until the fourth phase. Similarly, Larkin and Snowl- controlling for initial vocabulary showed that not only was ing (2008) cited the stage models of reading by Frith (1985) the experimental group better at defining the words they and Ehri (1995) to support the conclusion that teaching may have been exposed to, F(1, 79) D 10.4, p < .01, should have an initial focus on teaching grapheme–pho- partial h2 D .12, but they were better at defining “Base neme mappings rather than morphology. Consistent with Taught” words, F(1, 79) D 6.01, p < .05, partial h2 D .07. these conclusions, a number of authors have noted that For example, they were better at defining a word like some morphological skills emerge long after children mas- that was never addressed in the instruction ter phonics. For example, Nunes, Bryant, and Bindman but was related to the family that was addressed. (1997) suggested that 7- and 8-year-old children are enter- Downloaded by [University of Bristol] at 07:35 29 September 2017 No advantage was obtained in the Affix taught condition. ing the morphemic stage in spelling (children in Grades 2 The generalization afforded by the structured word and 3). Henderson and Templeton (1986) suggested that inquiry relates to a controversy concerning vocabulary children do not start applying morphological principles to instruction. Some researchers (Beck, McKeown, & Kucan, derivational spelling until around age 10 or 11. These find- 2002; Beck, Perfetti, & McKeown, 1982) have argued for ings might also be taken as evidence that SWI instruction is vocabulary instruction that is deep and rich. They have not appropriate in beginning readers in Grade 1. argued that instruction should provide opportunities for However, these claims and findings provide no evidence deeper processing in order for the to be against the claim that SWI should be adopted from the very effective. Others (Biemiller & Boote, 2006) have argued beginning. First, the claim that morphological instruction for instruction that is shallow and wide. They have provided should be introduced only in older children (e.g., Adams, evidence that if children were exposed briefly to many 1990) is challenged by the multiple meta-analyses showing words in the context of meaningful experiences, children that morphological interventions are more effective with with impoverished could start to catch up to younger children, including preschool and elementary stu- more advantaged peers. However, P. N. Bowers and Kirby dents (P. N. Bowers et al., 2010; Goodwin & Ahn, 2010, (2010) argued that morphological instruction investigating 2013). As noted earlier, a successful SWI intervention has word families with the matrix provides a way to meet both recently been carried out with children between ages 5 and 7 of these seemingly contradictory goals and more. They (Devonshire et al., 2013). It is also worth noting that children 138 BOWERS AND BOWERS

from Grade 1 on are successfully completing these investiga- SWI intervention studies in total and only one carried out tions in a growing number of public and private schools. For with young children (ages 5–7; Devonshire et al., 2013). In example, see http://tinyurl.com/ox4c68b for a video of a fact, we agree that caution is warranted at present. But given Grade 1 class investigating using an ety- the strong theoretical motivation for SWI that we have just mological dictionary and http://tinyurl.com/pv2qroq for a detailed, the growing evidence that morphological instruc- video of 7-year-olds working with an etymological dictio- tion is useful, especially for young and struggling readers, nary. Also see http://tinyurl.com/zlr27pn for a video of a and the promising initial evidence for SWI, we think it is more basic lesson in public school Grade 1 classroom that time to carry out more empirical research on SWI. This is illustrates how morphology and graphemes interact using our goal: to motivate future empirical studies of SWI in word sums. We fully acknowledge that these illustrations order to assess whether indeed this method is more effective are only anecdotal, but the videos show that structured than phonics that is currently failing too many children. word inquiry is possible in younger children and, most important, give the reader a more concrete idea of how these lessons can be implemented in practice. SUMMARY Second, the finding that morphology does not constrain how Grade 1 students spell (e.g., Henderson, 1984; Nunes In contrast with the vast amount of empirical research on et al., 1997) provides no evidence that SWI is too challeng- phonics, the research on SWI is only beginning. Neverthe- ing for these students given that they were not taught about less, we would argue that the theoretical motivation for SWI morphology. The relevant question is not when children is extremely strong (see Table 1). Furthermore, the empiri- acquire morphological knowledge implicitly, but rather cal evidence is highly promising. Morphological instruction when children should be given explicit instruction regard- is a central feature of SWI, and the evidence from the three ing morphology, etymology, and phonology. The Devon- meta-analyses of morphological instruction (P. N. Bowers shire et al. (2013) study provides some preliminary et al., 2010; Goodwin and Ahn, 2010, 2013) show that mor- evidence that SWI instruction is effective for beginning phological instruction benefits all students, but it is particu- readers between ages 5 and 7. larly beneficial for less able and younger students. In Another possible objection is that structured word inquiry addition, the three existing SWI studies report improve- will be difficult to implement with struggling readers who ments in decoding (Devonshire et al., 2013), spelling (Dev- exhibit more general learning difficulties. For example, spe- onshire & Fluck, 2010), and vocabulary knowledge (P. N. cific language impairment is manifest as a difficulty in Bowers & Kirby, 2010), with morphological instruction acquiring language despite otherwise normal IQ, normal directed at children as young as 5 years of age (Devonshire hearing, and an adequate learning environment. The cogni- et al., 2013). tive deficits extend beyond phonology to include deficits in Although there is now growing evidence that literacy semantics, syntax, and discourse. Important for present pur- instruction should be designed to make sense of the English poses, approximately one third of children with specific lan- spelling system, little evidence is available concerning how guage impairment in kindergarten are dyslexic in later grades to best teach these facts. Nevertheless, we are now in a posi- (Catts, Adlof, Hogan, & Weismer, 2005), with some esti- tion to see a promising path forward. Psychological theories mates higher still (McArthur, Hogben, Edwards, Heath, & of learning highlight the importance of organizing informa- Mengler, 2000; Snowling, Bishop, & Stothard, 2000). Other tion in meaningful ways, as well as the importance of explan- Downloaded by [University of Bristol] at 07:35 29 September 2017 cases of reading disorders will be attributed to even broader atory questioning in which students generate plausible deficits, including low IQs. This raises the question as to explanations as to why some stated fact is true, as well as whether SWI is appropriate for these populations of children. monitoring and reflecting on their learning. These findings We agree that future research is needed to answer this motivate the use of tools such as morphological matrices and question, but there is every reason to believe our theoretical word sums that highlight the meaningful relations between arguments for structured word inquiry apply to these popu- words and etymological dictionaries that allow hypotheses to lations as well. Indeed, as far as we are aware, memory and be tested. Clearly more research is needed to assess the effi- learning is best when information is encoded in a meaning- cacy of these specific tools, and the approach more generally. ful and organized manner for all individuals. In general sup- Our main goal here is to help inspire more research into port of this claim, meta-analyses show that morphological teaching children the logic of their spelling system (also see intervention are more effective for struggling readers (P. N. Crystal, 2013; Henderson, 1984; Venezky, 1999). Bowers et al., 2010; Goodwin & Ahn, 2010, 2013). In our view the default assumption should be that all subgroups of learners will benefit from instruction that correctly repre- DISCLOSURE sents the writing system. Finally, a skeptic might note that the empirical evidence Peter N. Bowers runs the company WordWorks, where he in direct support of SWI is limited, with only three published uses Structured Word Inquiry to teach children literacy. LITERACY INSTRUCTION 139

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS Carlisle, J. F. (2010). Effects of instruction in morphological awareness on literacy achievement: An integrative review. Reading Research Quar- We thank Jacqueline Aldridge, Patricia Bowers, Gina terly, 45, 464–487. http://dx.doi.org/10.1598/RRQ.45.4.5 Catts, H. W., Adlof, S. M., Hogan, T. P., & Weismer, S. E. (2005). Are Cook, Nina Kazanina, John Kirby, Rebecca Marsh, and specific language impairment and dyslexia distinct disorders?. Journal Gail Venable for comments on previous drafts. of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 48, 1378–1396. http://dx. doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388 Chomsky, C. (1970). Reading, writing, and phonology. Harvard Educa- REFERENCES tional Review, 40, 287–309. 10.17763/haer.40.2.y7u0242x76w05624 Chomsky, N., & Halle, M. (1968). The sound pattern of English. New Adams, M. J. (1990). Beginning to read: Thinking and learning about York, NY: Harper & Row. print. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Cicerone, K. D., Dahlberg, C., Malec, J. F., Langenbahn, D. M., Felicetti, Al Otaiba, S., & Fuchs, D. (2006). Who are the young children for whom T., Kneipp, S., ... Catanese, J. (2005). Evidence-based cognitive reha- best practices in reading are ineffective? An experimental and longitudi- bilitation: Updated review of the literature from 1998 through 2002. nal study. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 39, 414–431. http://dx.doi. Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, 86, 1681–1692. org/10.1177/00222194060390050401 10.1016/j.apmr.2005.03.024 Anglin, J. M. (1993). Vocabulary development: A morphological analysis. Coltheart, M., Rastle, K., Perry, C., Langdon, R., & Ziegler, J. (2001). Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 58(10, DRC: A dual route cascaded model of visual and read- Serial No. 238). ing aloud. Psychological Review, 108, 204–256. 10.1037/0033- Baayen, R. H., Piepenbrock, R., & van Rijn, H. (1993). The CELEX lexical 295X.108.1.204 database [CD-ROM]. Philadelphia, PA: Linguistic Data Consortium, Compton, D. L., Miller, A. C., Elleman, A. M., & Steacy, L. M. (2014). University of Pennsylvania. Have we forsaken reading theory in the name of “quick fix” interven- Beck, I., McKeown, M. G., & Kucan, L. (2002). Bringing words to life: tions for children with reading disability?. Scientific Studies of Reading, Robust vocabulary instruction. New York, NY: Guilford. 18, 55–73. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10888438.2013.836200 Beck, I. L., Perfetti, C. A., & McKeown, M. G. (1982). Effects of long- Craik, F., & Tulving, E. (1975). Depth of processing and retention of words term vocabulary instruction on lexical access and reading comprehen- in episodic memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 104, sion. Journal of , 74, 506–521. http://dx.doi.org/ 268–294. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.104.3.268 10.1037/0022-0663.74.4.506 Crystal, D. (2003). The Cambridge encyclopedia of the Biemiller, A., & Boote, C. (2006). An effective method for building vocab- (2nd Edition). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. ulary inprimary grades. Journal of Educational Psychology, 98, 44–62. Crystal, D. (2013). Spell it out: The curious, enthralling and extraordinary http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.98.1.44 story of english spelling. London, UK: Macmillan. Bower, G. H., Clark, M. C., Lesgold, A. M., & Winzenz, D. (1969). Hierar- Davis, C. J. (2010). The spatial coding model of visual word identification. chical retrieval schemes in recall of categorized word lists. Journal of Psychological Review, 117, 713–758. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0019738 Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 8, 323–343. http://dx.doi.org/ Deacon, S. H., & Bryant, P. (2006a). Getting to the root: Young writers’ 10.1016/S0022-5371(69)80124-6 sensitivity to the role of root morphemes in the spelling of inflected and Bowers, J. S. (2016). The practical and principled problems with educa- derived words. Journal of Child Language, 33, 401–417. http://dx.doi. tional neuroscience. Psychological Review, 123, 600–612. http://dx.doi. org/10.1017/S0305000906007409 org/10.1037/rev0000025 Deacon, S. H., & Bryant, P. (2006b). This turnip’s not for turning: Child- Bowers, J. S., Davis, C. J., & Hanley, D. A. (2005). Automatic semantic ren’s morphological awareness and their use of root morphemes in spell- activation of embedded words: Is there a “hat” in “that”? Journal of ing. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 24, 567–575. http:// Memory and Language, 52, 131–143. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j. dx.doi.org/10.1348/026151005X50834 jml.2004.09.003 Deacon, S. H., & Kirby, J. R. (2004). Morphological awareness: Just “more Bowers, P. N., & Cooke, G. (2012). Morphology and the common core: phonological”? The roles of morphological and phonological awareness Building students’ understanding of the written word. Perspectives on in reading development. Applied Psycholinguistics, 25, 223–238. http:// Language and Literacy, Fall, 4, 31–35. dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0142716404001110

Downloaded by [University of Bristol] at 07:35 29 September 2017 Bowers, P. N., & Kirby, J. R. (2010). Effects of morphological instruction Department for Education. (2013). English programmes of study: Key Stages on vocabulary acquisition. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary 1and2.National curriculum in England. London, UK: Crown Copyright. Journal, 23, 515–537. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11145-009-9172-z Devonshire, V., & Fluck, M. (2010). Spelling development: Fine-tuning Bowers, P. N., Kirby, J. R., & Deacon, S. H. (2010). The effects of mor- strategy-use and capitalising on the connections between words. Learn- phological instruction on literacy skills: A systematic review of the liter- ing and Instruction, 20, 361–371. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j. ature. Review of Educational Research, 80, 144–179. http://dx.doi.org/ learninstruc.2009.02.025 10.3102/0034654309359353 Devonshire, V., Morris, P., & Fluck, M. (2013). Spelling and reading Bowey, J. A. (2006). Need for systematic synthetic phonics teaching development: The effect of teaching children multiple levels of repre- within the early reading curriculum. Australian Psychologist, 41, 79–84. sentation in their orthography. Learning and Instruction, 25, 85–94. Bryant, P., & Nunes, T. (2004). Morphology and spelling. In Handbook of 10.1016/j.learninstruc.2012.11.007 children’s literacy (pp. 91–118). Amsterdam, The Netherlands: Springer. Dewey, J. (1913). Interest and effort in education. Cambridge, MA: Rive. Byrne, B. J. (1998). The foundation of literacy: The child’s acquisition of Duff, F. J., & Clarke, P. J. (2011). Practitioner review: Reading disorders: the alphabetic principle. Philadelphia, PA: Psychology Press. What are the effective interventions and how should they be imple- Camilli, G., Vargas, S., & Yurecko, M. (2003). Teaching children to read: mented and evaluated?. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, The fragile link between science & federal education policy. Education 52, 3–12. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2010.02310.x Policy Analysis Archives, 11, 15. Dunlosky, J., Rawson, K. A., Marsh, E. J., Nathan, M. J., & Willingham, Carlisle, J. F. (2000). Awareness of the structure and meaning of morpho- D. T. (2013). Improving students’ learning with effective learning tech- logically complex words: Impact on reading. Reading and Writing: An niques: Promising directions from cognitive and educational psychol- Interdisciplinary Journal, 12, 169–190. http://dx.doi.org/10.1023/ ogy. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 14, 4–58. http://dx. A:1008131926604 doi.org/10.1177/1529100612453266 140 BOWERS AND BOWERS

Ehri, L. C., & McCormick, S. (1998). Phases of word learning: Implica- Kirby, J. R., & Bowers, P. N. (in press). Morphological instruction and lit- tions for instruction with delayed and disabled readers. Reading and eracy: Binding phonological, orthographic, and semantic features of Writing Quarterly, 14, 135–163. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/ words. In K. Cain, D. Compton, & R. Parrila, (Eds.), Theories of reading 1057356980140202 development. Amsterdam, The Netherlands: John Benjamins. Ehri, L. C., Nunes, S. R., Stahl, S. A., & Willows, D. M. (2001). System- Kirby, J. R., & Lawson, M. (2012). Enhancing the quality of learning: Dis- atic phonics instruction helps students learn to read: Evidence from the positions, instruction and learning processes. New York, NY: Cam- National Reading Panel’s meta-analysis. Review of Educational bridge University Press. Research, 71, 393–447. http://dx.doi.org/10.3102/00346543071003393 Konkle, T., Brady, T. F., Alvarez, G. A., & Oliva, A. (2010). Conceptual Fletcher, J. M., Lyon, G. R., Fuchs, L. S., & Barnes, M. A. (2007). Learn- distinctiveness supports detailed visual long-term memory for real- ing disabilities: From identification to intervention. New York, NY: world objects. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 139, 558. Guilford Press. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0019165 Freeman, J. G., McPhail, J. C., & Berndt, J. A. (2002). Sixth graders’ views Kreiner, D. S. (1992). Reaction time measures of spelling: Testing a two- of activities that do and do not help them learn. The Elementary School strategy model of skilled spelling. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Journal, 335–347. Learning, Memory and Cognition, 18, 765–776. http://dx.doi.org/ Frith, U. (1985). Beneath the surface of dyslexia. In K. E. Patterson, J. C. 10.1037/0278-7393.18.4.765 Marshall, & M. Coltheart (Eds.), : Neuropsychological Larkin, R. F., & Snowling, M. J. (2008). Morphological spelling develop- and cognitive studies of phonological reading (pp. 301–330). Hillsdale, ment. Reading & Writing Quarterly, 24, 363–376. http://dx.doi.org/ NJ: Erlbaum. 10.1080/10573560802004449 Galuschka, K., Ise, E., Krick, K., & Schulte-Koerne, G. (2014). Effective- Lovett, M. W., Ransby, M. R., Hardwick, N., Johns, M. S., & Donaldson, S. ness of treatment approaches for children and adolescents with reading A. (1989). Can dyslexia be treated? Treatment-specific and generalized disabilities: A meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials. Plos One, treatment effects in dyslexic children’s response to remediation. Brain and 9(8), e105843. http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0105843 Language, 37, 90–121. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0093-934X(89)90103-X Goodwin, A. P., & Ahn, S. (2010). A meta-analysis of morphological inter- Martinussen, R. L., & Kirby, J. R. (1998). Instruction in successive phono- ventions: Effects on literacy achievement of children with literacy diffi- logical processing to improve the reading acquisition skills of at-risk culties. Annals of Dyslexia, 60, 183–208. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/ kindergarten children. Developmental Disabilities Bulletin, 26, 19–39. s11881-010-0041-x Marulis, L. M., & Neuman, S. B. (2010). The effects of vocabulary inter- Goodwin, A. P., & Ahn, S. (2013). A meta-analysis of morphological inter- vention on young children’s word learning a meta-analysis. Review of ventions in English: Effects on literacy outcomes for school-age chil- Educational Research, 80, 300–335. http://dx.doi.org/10.3102/ dren. Scientific Studies of Reading, 17, 257–285. http://dx.doi.org/ 0034654310377087 10.1080/10888438.2012.689791 Mattingly, I. G. (1972). Reading, the linguistic process and linguistic Hanna, P. R., Hanna, J. S., Hodges, R. E., & Rudorf, E. H. (1966). Pho- awareness. In J. Kavanagh & I. Mattingly (Eds.), Language by ear and neme–grapheme correspondences as cues to spelling improvement. by eye (pp. 133–147). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office. McArthur, G., Castles, A., Kohnen, S., Larsen, L., Jones, K., Anandaku- Hatcher, P. J., Hulme, C., Miles, J. N. V., Carroll, J. M., Hatcher, J., Gibbs, mar, T., & Banales, E. (2013). Sight word and phonics training in chil- S., &, et al. (2006). Efficacy of small group reading intervention for dren with dyslexia. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 48, 391–407. beginning readers with reading-delay: A randomised controlled trial. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0022219413504996 Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 47, 820–827. http://dx.doi. McArthur, G., Eve, P. M., Jones, K., Banales, E., Kohnen, S., Anandaku- org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2005.01559.x mar, T., &, et al. (2012, December 12). Phonics training for English- Henderson, E. (1984). Teaching children to spell English. Boston, MA: speaking poor readers. Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews, Houghton Mifflin. CD009115. Henderson, E. H., & Templeton, S. (1986). A developmental perspective of McArthur, G. M., Hogben, J. H., Edwards, V. T., Heath, S. M., & Mengler, formal spelling instruction through , pattern, and meaning. The E. D. (2000). On the 00specifics00 of specific reading disability and spe- Elementary School Journal, 86, 305–316. cific language impairment. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry Henry, M. K. (1989). Children’s word structure knowledge: Implications and Allied Disciplines, 41, 869–874. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1469- for decoding and spelling instruction. Reading and Writing: An Interdis- 7610.00674

Downloaded by [University of Bristol] at 07:35 29 September 2017 ciplinary Journal, 2, 135–152. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF00377467 McArthur, G., Kohnen, S., Jones, K., Eve, P., Banales, E., Larsen, L., & Henry, M. K. (2010). Unlocking literacy: Effective decoding & spelling Castles, A. (2015). Replicability of sight word training and phonics instruction. Baltimore, MD: Brookes. *Original work published 2003) training in poor readers: A randomised controlled trial. PeerJ, 3, e922. Higgins, S., Katsipataki, M., & Coleman, R. (2014). Reading at the Transi- McClelland, J. L., & Rumelhart, D. E. (1981). An interactive activation tion. Interim evidence brief. London, UK: Education Endowment model of context effects in letter perception: I. An account of basic find- Foundation. ings. Psychological Review, 88, 375–407. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/ Hino, Y., Lupker, S. J., & Taylor, T. E. (2012). The role of orthography in 0033-295X.88.5.375 the semantic activation of neighbors. Journal of Experimental Psychol- Melby-Lervag, M., Lyster, S. A. H., & Hulme, C. (2012). Phonological ogy: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 38, 1259–1273. http://dx.doi.org/ skills and their role in learning to read: A meta-analytic review. Psycho- 10.1037/a0028150 logical Bulletin, 138, 322–352. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0026744 Hulme, C., & Snowling, M. J. (2009). Developmental disorders of lan- Moore, W., Hammond, L., & Fetherston, T. (2014). Strengthening vocabu- guage, learning and cognition. Oxford, UK: Wiley-Blackwell. lary for literacy: An analysis of the use of explicit instruction techniques Jackman, J. (1997). Nelson Spelling. London, UK: Nelson. to improve word learning from story book read-alouds. Australian Jour- Kemp, N. (2006). Children’s spelling of base, inflected and derived words: nal of Learning Difficulties, 19, 153–172. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/ Links with morphological awareness. Reading and Writing: An Interdis- 19404158.2014.964992 ciplinary Journal, 19, 737–765. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11145-006- Moustafa, M., & Maldonado-Colon, E. (1998). Whole-to-parts phonics 9001-6 instruction: Building on what children know to help them know more. Kessler, B., & Treiman, R. (2001). Relationships between sounds and let- The Reading Teacher, 52, 448–458. ters in English monosyllables. Journal of memory and Language, 44, Nagy, W. E., Berninger, V. W., & Abbott, R. (2006). Contributions of mor- 592–617. http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/jmla.2000.2745 phology beyond phonology to literacy outcomes of upper elementary LITERACY INSTRUCTION 141

and middle-school students. Journal of Educational Psychology, 98, Seidenberg, M. S. (2011). Reading in different writing systems: One archi- 134–147. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.98.1.134 tecture, multiple solutions. In P. McCardle, J. Ren, & O. Tzeng (Eds.), Nation, K., & Cocksey, J. (2009). Beginning readers activate semantics Dyslexia across : Orthography and the gene-brain behavior from sub-word orthography. Cognition, 110, 273–278. http://dx.doi.org/ link (pp. 151–174). Baltimore, MD: Brookes. 10.1016/j.cognition.2008.11.004 Seidenberg, M. S., & McClelland, J. L. (1989). A distributed, developmen- National Reading Panel. (2000). Teaching children to read: An evidence- tal model of word recognition and naming. Psychological Review, 96, based assessment of the scientific research literature on reading and its 523–568. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.96.4.523 implications for reading instruction. Bethesda, MD: National Institute Share, D. L. (1995). Phonological recoding and self-teaching: Sine qua non of Child Health and Human Development. of reading acquisition. Cognition, 55, 151–218. Nunes, T., Bryant, P., & Bindman, M. (1997). Children’s understanding of Share, D. L. (2011). On the role of phonology in reading acquisition: The the connection between grammar and spelling. In B. Blachman (Ed.), self-teaching hypothesis. In S. Brady, D. Braze, & C. A. Fowler (Eds.), Foundations of reading acquisition and dyslexia: Implications for early Explaining individual differences in reading: Theory and evidence (pp. intervention (pp. 219–240). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. 45–68). New York, NY: Psychology Press. Nunes, T., Bryant, P., & Olsson, J. (2003). Learning morphological and Shulman, L. S. (1986). Those who understand: Knowledge growth in phonological spelling rules: An intervention study. Scientific Studies of teaching. Educational Researcher, 15, 4–14. Reading, 7, 289–307. http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/S1532799XSSR0703_6 Snowling, M. J., Bishop, D. V. M., & Stothard, S. E. (2000). Is preschool O’Connor, R. E., & Padeliadu, S. (2000). Blending versus whole word language impairment a risk factor for dyslexia in adolescence? Jour- approaches in first grade remedial reading: Short-term and delayed effects nal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 41, on reading and spelling words. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary 587–600. Journal, 13, 159–182. http://dx.doi.org/10.1023/A:1008134818771 Snowling, M., & Hulme, C. (2005). Learning to read with a language Perfetti, C. A. (2003). The universal grammar of reading. Scientific Studies impairment. In M. Snowling & C. Hulme (Eds.), The science of reading: of Reading, 7, 3–24. http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/S1532799XSSR0701_02 A handbook (pp. 397–412). Oxford, UK: Blackwell. Perfetti, C. A. (2007). Reading ability: Lexical quality to comprehension. Snowling, M. J., & Hulme, C. (2011). Evidence-based interventions for Scientific Studies of Reading, 11, 357–383. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/ reading and language difficulties: Creating a virtuous circle. British 10888430701530730 Journal of Educational Psychology, 81, 1–23. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/ Perry, C., Ziegler, J. C., & Coltheart, M. (2002). How predictable is spell- j.2044-8279.2010.02014.x ing? Developing and testing metrics of phoneme-grapheme contingency. Snowling, M. J., & Hulme, C. (2014). Closing a virtuous circle: Reciprocal The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology: Section A, 55, 897– influences between theory and practice in studies of reading interven- 915. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/02724980143000640 tion. Journal of Research on Educational Effectiveness, 7, 300–306. Perry, C., Ziegler, J. C., & Zorzi, M. (2007). Nested incremental modelling http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/19345747.2014.925307 in the development of computational theories: The CDPC model of Stahl, S. A., & Nagy, W. E. (2006). Teaching word meanings. Mahwah, reading aloud. Psychological Review, 114, 273. http://dx.doi.org/ NJ: Erlbaum. 10.1037/0033-295X.114.2.273 Stephens, M., & Hudson, A. (1984). A comparison of the effects of direct Rayner, K., Foorman, B. R., Perfetti, C. A., Pesetsky, D., & Seidenberg, M. instruction and remedial English classes on the spelling skills of second- S. (2001). How psychological science informs the teaching of reading. ary students. Educational Psychology, 4, 261–267. http://dx.doi.org/ Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 2, 31–74. http://dx.doi.org/ 10.1080/0144341840040401 10.1111/1529-1006.00004 Stothard, S. E., Hulme, C., Clarke, P., Barmby, P., & Snowling, M. J. Reed, D. K. (2008). A synthesis of morphology interventions and effects (2010). York Assessment of Reading for Comprehension. Secondary on reading outcomes for students in Grades K–12. Learning Disabilities Test. Brentford, UK: GL Assessment. Research and Practice, 23, 36–49. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1540- Taft, M., & van Graan, F. (1998). Lack of phonological mediation in a 5826.2007.00261.x semantic categorization task. Journal of Memory and Language, 38, Roediger, H. L., Finn, B., & Weinstein, Y. (2012). Applications of cogni- 203–224. tive science to education. In S. Della Salla & M. Anderson (Eds.), Neu- Torgerson, C. J., Brooks, G., & Hall, J. (2006). A systematic review of the roscience in education: The good, the bad and the ugly (pp. 128–151). research literature on the use of phonics in the teaching of reading and Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. spelling (DfES Research Rep. 711). London: Department for Education

Downloaded by [University of Bristol] at 07:35 29 September 2017 Roediger, H. L., & Pyc, M. A. (2012). Inexpensive techniques to improve and Skills, University of Sheffield. education: Applying cognitive psychology to enhance educational prac- Venezky, R. (1999). The American way of spelling. New York: Guilford. tice. Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition, 1, 242– Wiseman, S., & Neisser, U. (1974). Perceptual organization as a determi- 248. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jarmac.2012.09.002 nant of visual recognition memory. The American Journal of Psychol- Rose, J. (2006). Independent review of the teaching of early reading: ogy, 87, 675–681. Interim report. Literacy Today, 45, 18. Zahorik, J. A. (1996). Elementary and secondary teachers’ reports of how Rose, J. (2009). Identifying and teaching children and young people with they make learning interesting. The Elementary School Journal, 96, dyslexia and literacy difficulties. London, UK: Department for Children, 551–564. Schools and Families. Retrieved from http://www.teachernet.gov.uk/ Ziegler, J. C., Perry, C., & Zorzi, M. (2014). Modelling reading develop- wholeschool/sen/ ment through phonological decoding and self-teaching: Implications for Schonell, F., & Goodacre, E. (1971). The psychology and teaching of read- dyslexia. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological ing. London, UK: Oliver & Boyd. Sciences, 369(1634). http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2012.0397