Remarks on the History of the Indo-European Infinitive Dorothy Disterheft University of South Carolina - Columbia, [email protected]

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Remarks on the History of the Indo-European Infinitive Dorothy Disterheft University of South Carolina - Columbia, Ddisterheft@Sc.Rr.Com University of South Carolina Scholar Commons Faculty Publications Linguistics, Program of 1981 Remarks on the History of the Indo-European Infinitive Dorothy Disterheft University of South Carolina - Columbia, [email protected] Follow this and additional works at: https://scholarcommons.sc.edu/ling_facpub Part of the Linguistics Commons Publication Info Published in Folia Linguistica Historica, Volume 2, Issue 1, 1981, pages 3-34. Disterheft, D. (1981). Remarks on the History of the Indo-European Infinitive. Folia Linguistica Historica, 2(1), 3-34. DOI: 10.1515/ flih.1981.2.1.3 © 1981 Societas Linguistica Europaea. This Article is brought to you by the Linguistics, Program of at Scholar Commons. It has been accepted for inclusion in Faculty Publications by an authorized administrator of Scholar Commons. For more information, please contact [email protected]. Foli.lAfl//ui.tfcoFolia Linguistica HfltorlcGHistorica II/III/l 'P'P.pp. 8-U3—Z4 © 800£etruSocietae lAngu,.ticaLinguistica Etlropaea.Evropaea, 1981lt)8l REMARKSBEMARKS ONON THETHE HISTORYHISTOKY OFOF THETHE INDO-EUROPEANINDO-EUROPEAN INFINITIVEINFINITIVE i DOROTHYDOROTHY DISTERHEFTDISTERHEFT I 1.1. INTRODUCTIONINTRODUCTION WithWith thethe exceptfonexception ofof Indo-IranianIndo-Iranian (Ur)(Hr) andand CelticCeltic allall historicalhistorical Indo-EuropeanIndo-European (IE)(IE) subgroupssubgroups havehave a morphologicalIymorphologically distinctdistinct II infinitive.Infinitive. However,However, nono singlesingle proto-formproto-form cancan bebe reconstructedreconstructed foforr themthem -— onlyonly a widewide arrayarray ofof actionaction nouns.nouns. A goodgood dealdeal hashas beenbeen writtenwritten aboutabout thethe developmentdevelopment ofof thethe infinitive,infinitive, almostalmost allall fromfrom a morphologicalmorphological perspective.perspective. ButBut thethe evolutionevolution ofof a singlesingle infinitiveinfinitive fromfrom a largelarge numbernumber ofof Proto-Indo-EuropeanProto-Indo-European (PIE)( ) actionaction nounnounss isis primarilyprimarily a syntactiosyntactic developmentdevelopment involvinginvolving notnot onlyonly anan expan­expan- sionsion inin thethe numbenumberr ofof clauseclause typestypes thatthat useuse infinitiveinfinitive asäs embeddedembedded predicatepredicate bubutt alsoalso changchangee inin ththee struoturestructure ofof thethe infinitiveinfinitive clausclausee itselfitself (i.e.(i.e. expansionexpansion ofof typestypes ooff subject/objecsubject/objectt occurrence),occurrence), changchangee inin objecobjectt casecase,, andand worwordd Ordeorderr shiftsshifts.. MMyy aiaimm herheree isis toto identifyidentify archaicarchaic featuresfeatures,, bothboth morphologicamorphologicall andand syntacticsyntactio,, ofof attesteattestedd IEIE languageslanguages whicwhiohh wilwilll givgivee a clearerclearer pioturepicture ooff whawhatt nonfinitenonfinite subsub­- ordinatingordinating strategiestrategiess werweree useusedd iinn certaioertainn areaareass ofof ththee latlatee PDPIEE languaglanguage,e community.Community. This'discussionThis discussion differdifferss frofromm paspaatt syntaotiosyntactic studiestudiess (e.g(e.g.. thosthosee ooff HahHahnn 1950,1950, MilleMiUerr 1974,1974, ArArdd 1975)1975) whicwhiohh focusfocus oonn embeddeembeddedd structurestructuress ooff a further-developefurther-developedd typtypee (e.g(e.g.. thosethose wherwheree subject-raisinsubject-raisingg iiss involvedinvolved)) ttoo reconstrucreconstruott earlierearlier stagesstages ooff infinitivainfinitivall Subordinationsubordination.. I wilwilll arguarguee herheree thathatt undeunderr oertaincertain conditionconditionss ththee actioactionn nounounn wawass indeterminatelindeterminatelyy a nounounn oorr ththee prepre­- dicatdicatee ooff a subordinatsubordinatee clauseolause.. BeanalysiReanalysiss äass aann infinitivinfinitivee resulteresultedd iinn a completcompletee shifshiftt iinn mosmostt language]an~agess ttoo parpartt ooff ththee verbaverball Systemsystem. *... A pre~preliminary sketchsketch ofof thisthis paperpapor waswas preSentedpresented atat thethe WinterWinter 19719788 LSLSAA meetingmeeting.. I wisWishh ttoo thanthankk CaroCaroll FF.. JustuJustuss anandd TheodorTheodoree MM.. LightneLightnerr foforr theitheirr comrnentcommentss oonn iit t andand alsoalso SandraSandra A.A. ThompsonThompson andand PatrickPatrick K.K. FordFord foforr heljhelp) duiinduringg previoupreviouss stagestagess ooff thithiss researehresearch.. OOff coursecourse,, I taktakee fülfuJIl rere­- sponsibilitsponsibilityy foforr presentatiopresentationn anandd analysianalysiss ooff ththee datadata.. Brought to you by | University of South Carolina Libraries (University of South Carolina School of L) Authenticated | 172.16.1.226 Download Date | 5/31/12 9:18 PM 4 TheThe datadata presentedpresented herehere areare fromfrom thethe twotwo languagelanguage groups,groups, IIrIlr andand Celtic,lCeltic,1 thatthat preservepreserve severalseveral IEIE actionaction nounsnouns asäs infinitives.infinitives. I concludeconclude thatthat OldOld IrishIrish (OIr)(Olr) hashas subordinatesubordinate clausesclauses veryvery closeclose toto whatwhat PIE had,had, whilewhile thethe RigvedicRigvedic (RV)(RV) periodperiod ofof SanskritSanskrit hashas innovatedinnovated fromfrom purelypurely nominalnominal status;Status; AvestanAvestan (Av),(Av) , representingrepresenting OldOld Iranian,Iranian, alsoalso showsshows tracestraces ofof earlierearlier stagesstages eveneven thoughthough itit hashas standardizedstandardized objectobject casecase selectionselection whichwhich waswas variantvariant inin IIr.Ilr. OtherOther topicstopics likelike thethe extentextent toto whichwhich morphologymorphology andand syntaxsyntax areare concon­- nectednected andand whatwhat conditionsconditions werewere necessarynecessary toto allowallow incipientincipient infinitiveinfinitive analysesanalyses willwill bebe explored.explored. I willwill alsoalso sketchsketch thethe morpho­morpho- logicallogical andand syntacticsyntactic consequencesconsequences ofof thisthis reanalysisreanalysis andand comparecompare themthem withwith otherother IEIE languageslanguages whichwhich havehave moremore developeddeveloped infinitiveinfinitive clauses.clauses. 2.2. INDO-IRANIANINDO-IRANIAN AND'AND' CELTICCELTIC TheThe infinitivesinfinitives ofof RVRV andand Av areare obliqueoblique casescases ofof productiveproductive actionaction nounnounss withwith thethe onlyonly exceptionsexceptions beingbeing RRVV -dhyai andand AAvv -dyai-dyäi («Il <IIrr *-dhyai)*-dhyäi),,RV RV -sani and-tava -tavaii (u). *-dhyai*-dhyäi isis morpholog­morpholog- icallyically obscurobscuree andand nonott ttoo bbee derivedderived fromfrom anyany IIEE nominanominall para­para- digm,2digm,2 whilwhilee RRVV -sani iiss ththee locativelocative singularsingular ooff aann extendeextendedd heteroclitheteroclitee stestemm (-sar-/-san-;(-sar-l-san-; foforr morphologicamorphologicall detaildetailss seseee BenBen­- venistvenistee 1935b:1935b: ChapteChapterr 1)1) whichwhich,, thougthoughh attesteattestedd iinn otheotherr IE 1 1 MyMy descriptiondescription ofof CelticCeltic isis basedbased uponupon OldOld IrishIrish whichwhich isis thethe bestbest attestedattested ooff ththee oldeolderr languagelanguagess iinn thathatt groupgroup.. TraditionaTraditionall descriptiondescriptionss ooff thesthesee lan­lan- guageguagess tentendd ttoo taktakee ththee positiopositionn thathatt ththee verbaverball nounounn iiss nonott aann infinitiveinfinitive (e.g(e.g.. ThurneyseThurneysenn 19461946:: 455455,, DilloDillonn 19551955:: 111122 f.)f.);; iinn thithiss discussiodiscussionn I shoshoww thathatt iinn certaicertainn clausclausee typetypess infinitivainfinitivall Statustatuss shoulshouldd bbee assigneassignedd ttoo somsomee OlOldd IrisIrishh structuresstructures.. 2 2 BuButt cfcf.. argumentargumentss liklikee thosthosee bbyy BartholomaBartholomaee 18901890,, SchwyzeSchwyzerr 1953/11953/1:: 808099 whicwhichh trtryy ttoo matcmatchh *-dhyäi*-dhyai witwithh ththee GreeGreekk middlmiddlee infinitivinfinitivee -sthai.-8thai. ThiThiss correspondenccorrespondencee iiss untenabluntenablee oonn phonologicaphonologicall groundsgrounds:: HIIrr *-dhy­*-dhy- correspondcorrespondss ttoo GGkk -ss--88- (Benvenist(Benvenistee 1935b1935b:: 208)208).. I havhavee discussediscussedd elsewherelsewheree (Disterhef(Disterheftt 19801980:: 34—4034-40)) earlieearlierr elaimsclaims,, notablnotablyy bbyy BenvenistBenvenistee 1935a:1935a: ChapteChapterr 22,, thathatt -*dhyäi-*dhyai iiss a medio-passivmedio-passivee infinitiveinfinitive.. HiHiss twtwoo critericriteriaa areare:: 11.. iiff ththee infinitivinfinitivee hahass ththee samsamee meaninmeaningg äass ththee finitfinitee medio-passivemedio-passive,, iitt hahass ththee samsamee voicvoicee äass wellwell;; 22.. iiff ththee finitfinitee paradigmparadigmss araree predominantlpredominantlyy middlmiddlee oorr passiv:p~s~ivee ththee infinitiv~i~tiv~e iiss likewis~ikewisee middlemiddle.. MMyy examinatioexamir~ationn ooff ththee datdataa upouponn whichwhlCn* Benveniste'BenvenISte ss claiclaImm iISs basebasedd ddoo nonott supporsupportt hihISs argumentargument:: whilwhilee ououtt ooff a totatotall ooff 3333 verbaverball Sternss.tems,, 8 araree exclusivelexclusivelyy anandd 44 araree predominantlypredominantly middlemiddle,, 7 appeaappearr equallequallyy ofteoftenn iinn botbothh activactivee anandd middlemiddle,, 1111 araree infrequentlyinfrequently middlemiddle,, anandd 3 araree exclusivelexclusivelyy activeactive.. SincSincee middlmiddlee voicvoicee doedoess nonott changchangee tht~ee .marking.marking
Recommended publications
  • Possessive Constructions in Modern Low Saxon
    POSSESSIVE CONSTRUCTIONS IN MODERN LOW SAXON a thesis submitted to the department of linguistics of stanford university in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of master of arts Jan Strunk June 2004 °c Copyright by Jan Strunk 2004 All Rights Reserved ii I certify that I have read this thesis and that, in my opinion, it is fully adequate in scope and quality as a thesis for the degree of Master of Arts. Joan Bresnan (Principal Adviser) I certify that I have read this thesis and that, in my opinion, it is fully adequate in scope and quality as a thesis for the degree of Master of Arts. Tom Wasow I certify that I have read this thesis and that, in my opinion, it is fully adequate in scope and quality as a thesis for the degree of Master of Arts. Dan Jurafsky iii iv Abstract This thesis is a study of nominal possessive constructions in modern Low Saxon, a West Germanic language which is closely related to Dutch, Frisian, and German. After identifying the possessive constructions in current use in modern Low Saxon, I give a formal syntactic analysis of the four most common possessive constructions within the framework of Lexical Functional Grammar in the ¯rst part of this thesis. The four constructions that I will analyze in detail include a pronominal possessive construction with a possessive pronoun used as a determiner of the head noun, another prenominal construction that resembles the English s-possessive, a linker construction in which a possessive pronoun occurs as a possessive marker in between a prenominal possessor phrase and the head noun, and a postnominal construction that involves the preposition van/von/vun and is largely parallel to the English of -possessive.
    [Show full text]
  • Deixis and Reference Tracing in Tsova-Tush (PDF)
    DEIXIS AND REFERENCE TRACKING IN TSOVA-TUSH A DISSERTATION SUBMITTED TO THE GRADUATE DIVISION OF THE UNIVERSITY OF HAWAIʻI AT MĀNOA IN PARTIAL FULFILLMENT OF THE REQUIREMENTS FOR THE DEGREE OF DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY IN LINGUISTICS MAY 2020 by Bryn Hauk Dissertation committee: Andrea Berez-Kroeker, Chairperson Alice C. Harris Bradley McDonnell James N. Collins Ashley Maynard Acknowledgments I should not have been able to finish this dissertation. In the course of my graduate studies, enough obstacles have sprung up in my path that the odds would have predicted something other than a successful completion of my degree. The fact that I made it to this point is a testament to thekind, supportive, wise, and generous people who have picked me up and dusted me off after every pothole. Forgive me: these thank-yous are going to get very sappy. First and foremost, I would like to thank my Tsova-Tush host family—Rezo Orbetishvili, Nisa Baxtarishvili, and of course Tamar and Lasha—for letting me join your family every summer forthe past four years. Your time, your patience, your expertise, your hospitality, your sense of humor, your lovingly prepared meals and generously poured wine—these were the building blocks that supported all of my research whims. My sincerest gratitude also goes to Dantes Echishvili, Revaz Shankishvili, and to all my hosts and friends in Zemo Alvani. It is possible to translate ‘thank you’ as მადელ შუნ, but you have taught me that gratitude is better expressed with actions than with set phrases, sofor now I will just say, ღაზიშ ხილჰათ, ბედნიერ ხილჰათ, მარშმაკიშ ხილჰათ..
    [Show full text]
  • Expanding the Scope of Control and Raising∗
    1 Expanding the scope of control and raising∗ Maria Polinsky and Eric Potsdam University of California at San Diego and University of Florida 1. Introduction The relationship between linguistic theory and empirical data is a proverbial two-way street, but it is not uncommon for the traffic in that street to move only in one direction. The study of raising and control is one such case, where the empirical lane has been running the risk of becoming too empty, and much theorizing has been done on the basis of English and similar languages. A statement in a recent paper is quite telling in that regard: “Our impression from the literature … is that control behaves cross-linguistically in much the same fashion [as in English]…” (Jackendoff and Culicover 2003: 519). If indeed all languages structure control in ways similar to English, cross-linguistic investigation might not be expected to have much to offer, so there has not been much impetus for pursuing them. This paper offers a new incentive to pursue empirical data on control and raising. For these phenomena, recent results from both theoretical and empirical work have coalesced in a promising way allowing us to expand the boundaries of a familiar concept. This in turn provides a stronger motivation for the development of raising/control typologies. 2. Innovations in linguistic theory and their consequence for control and raising Two main innovations in linguistic theory allow us to predict a greater range of variation in control and raising: the unification of control and raising under a single analysis as movement and the compositional view of movement.
    [Show full text]
  • From Latin to Romance: Case Loss and Preservation in Pronominal Systems
    FLORE Repository istituzionale dell'Università degli Studi di Firenze From Latin to Romance: case loss and preservation in pronominal systems Questa è la Versione finale referata (Post print/Accepted manuscript) della seguente pubblicazione: Original Citation: From Latin to Romance: case loss and preservation in pronominal systems / Manzini, MARIA RITA; Savoia, LEONARDO MARIA. - In: PROBUS. - ISSN 1613-4079. - STAMPA. - 26, 2(2014), pp. 217-248. Availability: This version is available at: 2158/891750 since: 2016-01-20T16:23:29Z Terms of use: Open Access La pubblicazione è resa disponibile sotto le norme e i termini della licenza di deposito, secondo quanto stabilito dalla Policy per l'accesso aperto dell'Università degli Studi di Firenze (https://www.sba.unifi.it/upload/policy-oa-2016-1.pdf) Publisher copyright claim: (Article begins on next page) 27 September 2021 Probus 2014; 26(2): 217 – 248 M. Rita Manzini* and Leonardo M. Savoia From Latin to Romance: case loss and preservation in pronominal systems Abstract: The evolution from Latin into Romance is marked by the loss of case in nominal declensions. In most Romance varieties, however, pronouns, specifi- cally in the 1st/2nd person singular, keep case differentiations. In some varieties 1st/2nd singular pronouns present a three-way case split, essentially the same re- constructed for proto-Romance (De Dardel and Gaeng 1992, Zamboni 1998). We document and analyze the current situation of Romance in the first part of the article (section 1). In the second part of the article we argue that the Dative Shifted distribution of loro in modern Italian, accounted for by means of the category of weak pronoun in Cardinaletti and Starke (1999), is best construed as a survival of oblique case in the 3rd person system (section 2).
    [Show full text]
  • 250 Word List
    Core Vocabulary Therapy Checklist for ____________________ Tested and Heard Used Word Part of Speech Taught Using ... Passed On: Spontaneously: afraid adjective angry adjective another adjective bad adjective big adjective black adjective blue adjective bored adjective brown adjective busy adjective cold adjective cool adjective dark adjective different adjective dirty adjective dry adjective dumb adjective early adjective easy adjective fast adjective favorite adjective first adjective full adjective funny adjective good adjective green adjective grey adjective happy adjective hard adjective hot adjective hungry adjective important adjective last adjective late adjective light adjective little adjective lonely adjective long adjective mad adjective many adjective more adjective naughty adjective new adjective next adjective nice adjective old adjective only adjective orange adjective other adjective ©VanTatenhove, 2001 page 1 Core Vocabulary Therapy Checklist for ____________________ Word Part of Speech Taught Using ... Tested and Heard Used Passed On: Spontaneously: pink adjective purple adjective real adjective red adjective right adjective sad adjective same adjective sick adjective silly adjective sure adjective thirsty adjective tired adjective wet adjective white adjective wrong adjective yellow adjective adjective adjective adjective again adverb all right adverb almost adverb already adverb always adverb away adverb backwards adverb forward adverb here adverb indoors adverb just adverb maybe adverb much adverb never adverb not
    [Show full text]
  • On the Argument Structure of Causatives
    Alex Alsina On the Argument Structure of Causatives Morphologicallyderived causatives are composed of a causative morphemeand a base verb. It is generally accepted that the causative morpheme is a two-place predicate expressing a relation between a causer and a caused event. This has suggested to some researchersthat morphologicalcausatives have essentially the same syntactic represen- tation at D-Structure as English periphrasticcausatives involving verbs like make or cause, which are also semanticallytwo-place predicates. This line of research is most clearly representedby theories that derive causatives through syntactic incorporation, particularlythose of Baker (1988) and Li (1990). In this study I will show that in certainlanguages the causative morphemeis a three- place predicate, involving a patient in addition to a causer and a caused event. This assumption will not only provide a simple account of the basic generalizationsabout causatives, but also explain a whole set of facts that are completely mysterious for the Incorporationtheories. I will argue that the three-place causative predicate yields an argumentthat bears a semantic relation to both the causative predicate and the base predicate. This semanticallycomposite argumentcannot be derived throughmovement in the syntax, since it would violate the 0-Criterion(Chomsky (1981; 1982)).It must be formed elsewhere (i.e., in the argument structure)and assigned fully formed to one syntactic position. A consequence of this is that morphologicalcausatives cannot be formed by a syntactic derivation,contrary to the claim made by the Incorporationthe- ories. 1. The Basic Facts In this section we will establish the basic facts about causatives in Chichewa, a language that has figured centrally in previous theories of causatives (e.g., Baker (1988) and Li (1990)).
    [Show full text]
  • Idioms-And-Expressions.Pdf
    Idioms and Expressions by David Holmes A method for learning and remembering idioms and expressions I wrote this model as a teaching device during the time I was working in Bangkok, Thai- land, as a legal editor and language consultant, with one of the Big Four Legal and Tax companies, KPMG (during my afternoon job) after teaching at the university. When I had no legal documents to edit and no individual advising to do (which was quite frequently) I would sit at my desk, (like some old character out of a Charles Dickens’ novel) and prepare language materials to be used for helping professionals who had learned English as a second language—for even up to fifteen years in school—but who were still unable to follow a movie in English, understand the World News on TV, or converse in a colloquial style, because they’d never had a chance to hear and learn com- mon, everyday expressions such as, “It’s a done deal!” or “Drop whatever you’re doing.” Because misunderstandings of such idioms and expressions frequently caused miscom- munication between our management teams and foreign clients, I was asked to try to as- sist. I am happy to be able to share the materials that follow, such as they are, in the hope that they may be of some use and benefit to others. The simple teaching device I used was three-fold: 1. Make a note of an idiom/expression 2. Define and explain it in understandable words (including synonyms.) 3. Give at least three sample sentences to illustrate how the expression is used in context.
    [Show full text]
  • Possessive Pronouns As Oblique Dps: Linkers and Affix Stacking
    19.3 (2018): 393-425 UDC 81’367.626.2=111 Original scientific article Received on 16.02. 2018 Accepted for publication on 05.11. 2018 M. Rita Manzini Università degli Studi di Firenze Possessive pronouns as oblique DPs: Linkers and affix stacking In many familiar European languages, e.g. German or Italian, possessive pro- nouns agree in φ-features with their head noun. We argue that they are geni- tive pronouns, endowed with an extra φ-features set. As such, they are part of a range of phenomena including case stacking and linkers unified under the historical-typological label of Suffixaufnahme. We express the formal basis for this unification as the Stacking Generalization (Section 1). We then apply our analysis to the narrower domain of facts involving possessive pronouns, specifically in Balkan and Romance languages. We further find that 1/2P pro- nouns present a richer stacking structure than their 3P counterparts (Section 2). We examine this latter fact in the context of a more general phenomenon, whereby the 1/2P vs 3P Person split not only tends to correlate with different case and agreement alignments – but seems to govern the morphological ex- pression of case and agreement itself, in terms of richer vs poorer content (Section 3). Key words: oblique case; genitive; possessives; pronouns; linkers; agree- ment; person. 1. Linkers and affix stacking This section aims at establishing the framework for the discussion of agreeing pos- sessive pronouns to be pursued in later sections. According to a well established historical-typological view (Plank 1995), modifier structures involving both free standing heads (linkers) and stacked affixation (case stacking), are to be unified on the basis of functional considerations.
    [Show full text]
  • Navajo Verb Stem Position and the Bipartite Structure of the Navajo
    678 REMARKSANDREPLIES NavajoVerb Stem Positionand the BipartiteStructure of the NavajoConjunct Sector Ken Hale TheNavajo verb stem appears at the rightmost edge of the verb word. Innumerouscases it forms a lexicalconstituent with a preverb,occur- ringat theleftmost edge of thesurface verb word, much in themanner ofDutchand German verb-particle arrangements in verb-secondfinite clauses.In Navajothe initial and finalpositions are separated by eight morphemeorder ‘ ‘slots’’ recognizedin the Athabaskan literature (and describedin detail for Navajo in Young and Morgan 1987). A phono- logicalsolution to this and a numberof otherdeep-surface disparities isexploredhere, based on theinsights of earlierworks on theNavajo verb,including Speas 1984, 1990, McDonough 1996, 2000, and Rice 1989, 2000. Keywords: verbmorphology, morphosyntactic disparity, spell-out, Navajo,Athabaskan 1Verb Stem Position TheNavajo verb stem forms asinglelexical constituent with the prefixal, particle-like category called preverb inthe Athabaskan linguistic literature (see Rice2000). However, like particle- verbcombinations in Dutchand German verb-second clauses, the parts of thisconstituent in the Navajocounterpart are separated in the surface forms ofverbal projections in syntax. In the Navajoversion of thisarrangement, the preverb occupies the leftmost position in the verb word, andthe verb stem occupies the rightmost position. TheNavajo verb in (1) canbe used to illustrate the phenomenon. The verb is segmented intoits various parts in (2), and the components are identified in slightly greater detail in (3). (1) Sila´o t’o´o´’go´o´ ch’´õ shidinõ´daøzh.(Young and Morgan 1987:283) ‘Thepoliceman jerked me outdoors (to get me out of afight).’ (2) ch’´õ sh d n-´ daøzh PreverbObject Qualifier Mode/ SubjVoice V (stem) Iwishto dedicate thisarticle totheNavajo Language Academy, asmall groupof linguists and teachers devotedto Navajolanguage research andeducation.
    [Show full text]
  • Unity and Diversity in Grammaticalization Scenarios
    Unity and diversity in grammaticalization scenarios Edited by Walter Bisang Andrej Malchukov language Studies in Diversity Linguistics 16 science press Studies in Diversity Linguistics Chief Editor: Martin Haspelmath In this series: 1. Handschuh, Corinna. A typology of marked-S languages. 2. Rießler, Michael. Adjective attribution. 3. Klamer, Marian (ed.). The Alor-Pantar languages: History and typology. 4. Berghäll, Liisa. A grammar of Mauwake (Papua New Guinea). 5. Wilbur, Joshua. A grammar of Pite Saami. 6. Dahl, Östen. Grammaticalization in the North: Noun phrase morphosyntax in Scandinavian vernaculars. 7. Schackow, Diana. A grammar of Yakkha. 8. Liljegren, Henrik. A grammar of Palula. 9. Shimelman, Aviva. A grammar of Yauyos Quechua. 10. Rudin, Catherine & Bryan James Gordon (eds.). Advances in the study of Siouan languages and linguistics. 11. Kluge, Angela. A grammar of Papuan Malay. 12. Kieviet, Paulus. A grammar of Rapa Nui. 13. Michaud, Alexis. Tone in Yongning Na: Lexical tones and morphotonology. 14. Enfield, N. J (ed.). Dependencies in language: On the causal ontology of linguistic systems. 15. Gutman, Ariel. Attributive constructions in North-Eastern Neo-Aramaic. 16. Bisang, Walter & Andrej Malchukov (eds.). Unity and diversity in grammaticalization scenarios. ISSN: 2363-5568 Unity and diversity in grammaticalization scenarios Edited by Walter Bisang Andrej Malchukov language science press Walter Bisang & Andrej Malchukov (eds.). 2017. Unity and diversity in grammaticalization scenarios (Studies in Diversity Linguistics
    [Show full text]
  • Russian Case Morphology and the Syntactic Categories David Pesetsky (MIT)1 • Terminology: I Will Use the Abbreviations NGEN, DNOM, VACC, PDAT, Etc
    Russian case morphology and the syntactic categories David Pesetsky (MIT)1 • Terminology: I will use the abbreviations NGEN, DNOM, VACC, PDAT, etc. to remind us of the traditional names for the cases whose actual nature is simply N, D, V, and 1. Introduction (types of) P. The case-name suffixes to these designations are thus present merely for our convenience. • Case morphology: Russian nouns, adjectives, numerals and demonstratives bear case Genitive: The most unusual aspect of the proposal will be the treatment of genitive as suffixes. The shape of a given case suffix is determined by two factors: N -- with which I will begin the discussion in the next section. 1. its morphological environment (properties of the stem to which the suffix attaches; e.g. declension class, gender, animacy, number) ; and • Syntax: The treatment of case as in (1) will depend on two ideas that are novel in the context of a syntax based on external and internal Merge, but are also revivals of well- 2. its syntactic environment. known older proposals, as well as a third important concept: The traditional cross-classification of case suffixes by declension-class and by case- name (nominative, genitive, etc.) reflects these two factors. 1. Morphology assignment: When [or a projection of ] merges with and assigns an affix, the affix is copied α α β α • Specialness of the standard case names: Traditionally, the cases are called by onto β and realized on the (accessible) lexical items dominated by β. special names (nominative, genitive, etc.) not used outside the description of case- systems. The specialness of case terminology reflects what looks like a complex This proposal revives the notion of case assignment (Vergnaud (2006); Rouveret & relation between syntactic environment and choice of case suffix.
    [Show full text]
  • New Latin Grammar
    NEW LATIN GRAMMAR BY CHARLES E. BENNETT Goldwin Smith Professor of Latin in Cornell University Quicquid praecipies, esto brevis, ut cito dicta Percipiant animi dociles teneantque fideles: Omne supervacuum pleno de pectore manat. —HORACE, Ars Poetica. COPYRIGHT, 1895; 1908; 1918 BY CHARLES E. BENNETT PREFACE. The present work is a revision of that published in 1908. No radical alterations have been introduced, although a number of minor changes will be noted. I have added an Introduction on the origin and development of the Latin language, which it is hoped will prove interesting and instructive to the more ambitious pupil. At the end of the book will be found an Index to the Sources of the Illustrative Examples cited in the Syntax. C.E.B. ITHACA, NEW YORK, May 4, 1918 PREFACE TO THE SECOND EDITION. The present book is a revision of my Latin Grammar originally published in 1895. Wherever greater accuracy or precision of statement seemed possible, I have endeavored to secure this. The rules for syllable division have been changed and made to conform to the prevailing practice of the Romans themselves. In the Perfect Subjunctive Active, the endings -īs, -īmus, -ītis are now marked long. The theory of vowel length before the suffixes -gnus, -gna, -gnum, and also before j, has been discarded. In the Syntax I have recognized a special category of Ablative of Association, and have abandoned the original doctrine as to the force of tenses in the Prohibitive. Apart from the foregoing, only minor and unessential modifications have been introduced. In its main lines the work remains unchanged.
    [Show full text]