1 Vowel Height Allophony and Dorsal Place Contrsats in Cochabamba

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

1 Vowel Height Allophony and Dorsal Place Contrsats in Cochabamba Vowel height allophony and dorsal place contrsats in Cochabamba Quechua Gillian Gallagher, New York University August, 2015 Abstract This paper reports on the results of two studies investigating vowel height allophony triggered by uvular stops in Cochabamba Quechua. An acoustic study documents the lowering effect of a preceding tautomorphemic or a following heteromorphemic uvular on the high vowels /i u/. A discrimination study finds that vowel height is a significant cue to the velar-uvular place contrast. 1. Introduction This paper has two goals. The first is to document vowel height allophony in Cochabamba Quechua and the second is to explore how allophonic vowel quality is used in the perception of dorsal consonant place. An acoustic study finds a strong, word-level effect of a uvular consonant on surrounding vowels and a perception study finds vowel height to be a strong cue to the uvular-velar contrast. This introduction begins by describing the allophonic pattern, and then moves on to motivating the two studies. 1.1 Vowel allophony Cochabamba Quechua is a variety of South Bolivian Quechua (Lewis et al. 2014), classified in group IIC of the Quechua language family (Torero 1964) along with Cuzco Quechua and many other varieties of southern Quechua. The description of the phonological effects of uvular consonants on vowels holds across this subgroup of the language family, though the results of the acoustic study below may be specific to the Cochabamba variety. The phonemic consonantal inventory is given in Table 1 (Rowe 1950; Cusihuamán 1976; Bills et al. 1969); of particular interest are the three uvular consonants [q qh q’] which contrast with their velar counterparts [k kh k’]. In Cochabamba Quechua, the plain uvular stop /q/ is often realized as a voiced sonorant [ʁ] (Bills et al. 1969). labial dental postalveolar velar uvular glottal plain p t tʃ k q aspirate ph th tʃh kh qh ejective p’ t’ tʃ’ k’ q’ fricative s h nasal m n ɲ liquid l ɾ ʎ glide w j Table 1: Quechua consonant inventory. 1 Cochabamba Quechua has three underlying phonemic vowels /i u ɑ/, which correspond to the five surface vowels [i u e o ɑ].1 Mid vowels are found in the vicinity of a uvular consonant, either immediately preceding or following a uvular (1a,b) or preceding a consonant cluster with a uvular in C2 (1c). Impressionistic descriptions of this system are supported by the acoustic studies of Cochabamba Quechua in Holliday (2014) and of Cuzco Quechua in Pasquale (2001) Fabian (2011) and Molina Vital (2011). (1) a. [q’epij] ‘to carry’ h [q eʎu] ‘lazy’ [qosɑ] ‘husband’ [q’osɲi] ‘garbage’ b. [leq’e] ‘hat’ [seq’oj] ‘to smack’ [hoq’o] ‘damp’ [moq’ej] ‘to love’ c. [erqe] ‘son’ [p’esqo] ‘bird’ [soNqo] ‘heart’ [orqo] ‘mountain’ The high and mid vowels are in an allophonic relationship: the mid vowels appear in the vicinity of a uvular consonant and high vowels appear elsewhere (Bills et al. 1969; Adelaar with Muysken 2004; Hoggarth 2004; Laime Ajacopa 2007). The low vowel may appear in any consonantal context. This distribution is illustrated in (2) where the quality of vowels in words with uvulars, velars and non-dorsal consonants is shown. Similar patterns are seen in many languages with uvulars or other post-velar consonants, including Eskimo-Aleut languages (Rischel 1972; Dorais 1986), Interior Salish languages (Bessell 1998), Nuu-chah-nulth (Wakashan) (Wilson 2007), Chilcotin (Athabaskan) (Cook 1983, 1993; Bird 2014), Aymara (de Lucca 1987; Adelaar with Muysken 2004) and many varieties of Arabic (McCarthy 1994; Shahin 2002; Zawaydeh 1998; Al-Ani 1970; Butcher and Ahmad 1987). (2) a. no dorsals: [i u ɑ] *[e o] misi ‘cat’ *mese t’uru ‘mud’ *t’oro wɑsɑ ‘back’ b. velars: [i u ɑ] *[e o] siki ‘behind’ *seke ruku ‘old’ *roko pɑkɑ ‘to cover’ c. uvulars: [e o ɑ] *[i u] 1 All vowels are also described as tense in open syllables and lax in closed syllables (Laime Ajacopa 2007). For simplicity, this distinction is not transcribed throughout the paper. 2 leq’e ‘hat’ *liq’i hoq’o ‘damp’ *huq’u ɑqhɑ ‘corn beer’ Because of allophonic lowering, the contrast between velar and uvular consonants is often accompanied by a distinction in a surrounding vowel. Minimal pairs contrasting just for place are found with surrounding low vowels (3a), but in many cases minimal pairs contrast for both place and vowel height (3b,c). (3) a. wɑkɑj ‘my cow’ wɑqɑj ‘to cry’ b. kiru ‘tooth’ qeru ‘vase’ c. kusɑ ‘good’ qosɑ ‘husband’ 1.2 Motivating the acoustic study The current study contributes to the description of vowel lowering by documenting lowering across morpheme boundaries for the first time and by comparing the lowering effects of preceding and following uvular consonants. The available literature on Southern Quechua explicitly states that vowels are lowered immediately preceding and following a uvular, as in [leq’e] ‘hat’, and preceding a cluster that contains a uvular, as in [senqa] ‘nose’ (Bills et al. 1969; Cerrón Palomino 1994). The existence of lowering in these contexts, as well as its categorical nature, has been confirmed in two acoustic studies. Holliday (2014) looks at Cochabamba Quechua vowels following word-initial uvular and velar consonants, e.g., [qosɑ] ‘husband’ vs. [kusɑ] ‘good’, and finds that vowels following uvulars have a significantly higher F1 (indicating a lower vowel) than vowels following velars, and that this difference is present for the entire duration of the vowel. Molina Vital (2011) similarly found a categorical lowering effect by uvular consonants in Cuzco Quechua. Molina Vital’s study included uvular consonants in a variety of contexts (word initial, intervocalic and in a cluster), but did not systematically compare these different contexts. The current study focuses on vowel height at morpheme boundaries, comparing the height of root final vowels preceded by a non-dorsal consonant (4a) or a velar (4b) to those preceded by a uvular consonant (4c). The effects of a suffixal uvular on a root final vowel are also compared across roots with different consonantal content (5). (4) a. /hɑp’i-ni/ ‘have, 1 sg’ /tɑpu-ni/ ‘ask, 1 sg’ b. /tɑki-ni/ ‘sing, 1sg’ /hɑk’u-ni/ ‘grind, 1sg’ c. /sɑqi-ni/ ‘leave, 1 sg’ 3 /siq’u-ni/ ‘slap, 1 sg’ (5) a. /hɑp’i-rqɑ/ ‘have, 3sg past’ /tɑpu-rqɑ/ ‘ask, 3sg past’ b. /tɑki-rqɑ/ ‘sing, 3sg past’ /hɑk’u-rqɑ/ ‘grind, 3sg past’ c. /sɑqi-rqɑ/ ‘leave, 3sg past’ /siq’u-rqɑ/ ‘slap, 3sg past’ Based on the previous literature, a significant difference in height is expected between the vowels in the forms in (4a,b) vs. (4c): e.g., [hɑp’ini] ‘have, 1sg’, [takini] ‘sing, 1sg’ but [sɑqeni] ‘leave, 1sg’. While Molina Vital (2011) reports that lowering of a stem vowel is not triggered by a suffix, to my knowledge no phonetic study has ever examined vowels in this position. The current study explicitly addresses the question of whether suffixes trigger lowering across a morpheme boundary and finds that such lowering does occur, regardless of whether the root contains a velar, e.g., [hɑp’erqɑ] ‘have, 3sg past’ and [takerqɑ] ‘sing, 3sg past’. Because lowering is found across morpheme boundaries, a comparison of the lowering effects of a preceding and following consonantal trigger is possible, e.g., [sɑqeni] ‘leave, 1 sg’ vs. [hɑp’erqɑ] ‘have, 3sg past’. Gick & Wilson (2006) report that, in some languages, uvulars have a stronger impact on preceding vowels, while in other languages following vowels are more strongly affected. In Nuu-chah-nulth (Wakashan), high front vowels preceding uvulars have a schwa-like offglide, but are not fully lowered /iq/ ! [iəq]. High front vowels following uvulars, however, are more fully lowered to a mid vowel /qi/ ! [qe] or [qɛ] (Wilson 2007). In Chilcotin (Athabaskan), the reverse pattern is found (Cook 1983, 1993; Bird 2014). The high front vowel is slightly lowered when it precedes a uvular /iq/ ! [ɪq], but following a uvular only a schwa- like offglide is found /qi/ ! [qəi]. Directional asymmetries in the effects of post-velar consonants on surrounding segments are also found in studies of several Arabic dialects (Shahin 2002; Zawaydeh 1998; Al-Ani 1970; Butcher and Ahmad 1987), where effects on preceding segments are typically found to be stronger than effects on following segments. The interest of comparing roots with and without velar consonants lies in understanding the importance of height allophony to the dorsal place contrast. Comparing lowering by a suffixal uvular consonant in stems with and without a velar consonant, e.g., [hɑp’erqɑ] ‘have, 3sg past’ vs. [takerqɑ] ‘sing, 3sg past’, assesses whether the contrast enhancing properties of vowel allophony may block or alter the lowering effect of a uvular. To summarize, the acoustic study first asks whether vowel lowering applies across a morpheme boundary. Given the positive result indicating such lowering, two further questions are posed. First, do preceding and following uvular consonants have the same effect on vowel height? And second, does vowel lowering triggered by a suffix have the same effect on roots with and without velar consonants? 4 1.3 Motivating the perception study The perception study investigates the role of allophonic vowel lowering in the perception of the uvular-velar place contrast. As mentioned above, the identity of a given stop as uvular or velar is often accompanied by differences in surrounding vowel height as well as in the cues internal to the consonant. This is a common situation. Across languages, many contrasts are cued by phonetic properties internal to the contrasting segments as well as on surrounding segments, and listeners are often very sensitive to the cues in surrounding segments. In Korean, for example, the ternary laryngeal contrast in stops is largely cued by VOT and F0 distinctions in the following vowel and Korean listeners use both as strong cues to the laryngeal category of a stop (Cho 1996; Cho et al.
Recommended publications
  • Anatolian Evidence Suggests That the Indo- European Laryngeals * H2 And
    Indo-European Linguistics 6 (2018) 69–94 brill.com/ieul Anatolian evidence suggests that the Indo- European laryngeals *h2 and *h3 were uvular stops Alwin Kloekhorst Leiden University [email protected] Abstract In this article it will be argued that the Indo-European laryngeals *h2 and *h3, which recently have been identified as uvular fricatives, were in fact uvular stops in Proto- Indo-Anatolian. Also in the Proto-Anatolian and Proto-Luwic stages these sounds prob- ably were stops, not fricatives. Keywords Indo-European – laryngeals – phonological change – Indo-Anatolian 1 Background It is well-known that the Indo-European laryngeals *h2 and *h3 have in some environments survived in Hittite and Luwian as consonants that are spelled with the graphemes ḫ (in the cuneiform script) and h (in the hieroglyphic script).1 Although in handbooks it was usually stated that the exact phonetic interpretation of these graphemes is unclear,2 in recent years a consensus seems to have formed that they represent uvular fricatives (Kümmel 2007: 1 Although there is no full consensus on the question exactly in which environments *h2 and *h3 were retained as ḫ and h: especially the outcome of *h3 in Anatolian is debated (e.g. Kloekhorst 2006). Nevertheless, for the remainder of this article it is not crucial in which environments *h2 and *h3 yielded ḫ and h, only that they sometimes did. 2 E.g. Melchert 1994: 22; Hoffner & Melchert 2008: 38. © alwin kloekhorst, 2018 | doi:10.1163/22125892-00601003 This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the prevailing CC-BY-NC license at the time of publication.
    [Show full text]
  • Vowel Quality and Phonological Projection
    i Vowel Quality and Phonological Pro jection Marc van Oostendorp PhD Thesis Tilburg University September Acknowledgements The following p eople have help ed me prepare and write this dissertation John Alderete Elena Anagnostop oulou Sjef Barbiers Outi BatEl Dorothee Beermann Clemens Bennink Adams Bo domo Geert Bo oij Hans Bro ekhuis Norb ert Corver Martine Dhondt Ruud and Henny Dhondt Jo e Emonds Dicky Gilb ers Janet Grijzenhout Carlos Gussenhoven Gert jan Hakkenb erg Marco Haverkort Lars Hellan Ben Hermans Bart Holle brandse Hannekevan Ho of Angeliek van Hout Ro eland van Hout Harry van der Hulst Riny Huybregts Rene Kager HansPeter Kolb Emiel Krah mer David Leblanc Winnie Lechner Klarien van der Linde John Mc Carthy Dominique Nouveau Rolf Noyer Jaap and Hannyvan Oosten dorp Paola Monachesi Krisztina Polgardi Alan Prince Curt Rice Henk van Riemsdijk Iggy Ro ca Sam Rosenthall Grazyna Rowicka Lisa Selkirk Chris Sijtsma Craig Thiersch MiekeTrommelen Rub en van der Vijver Janneke Visser Riet Vos Jero en van de Weijer Wim Zonneveld Iwant to thank them all They have made the past four years for what it was the most interesting and happiest p erio d in mylife until now ii Contents Intro duction The Headedness of Syllables The Headedness Hyp othesis HH Theoretical Background Syllable Structure Feature geometry Sp ecication and Undersp ecicati on Skeletal tier Mo del of the grammar Optimality Theory Data Organisation of the thesis Chapter Chapter
    [Show full text]
  • What Is and What Is Not an Articulatory Gesture in Speech Production: the Case of Lateral, Rhotic and (Alveolo)Palatal Consonants
    What is and what is not an articulatory gesture in speech production: The case of lateral, rhotic and (alveolo)palatal consonants Daniel Recasens [email protected] Universitát Autònoma de Barcelona Gradus Revista Brasileira de Fonologia de Laboratório Vol. 1, nº 1 Dezembro de 2016 https://gradusjournal.com Bibtex: @article{recasens2016what, author = {Daniel Recasens}, journal = {Gradus}, month = {dec}, number = {1}, pages = {23–42}, title = {What is and what is not an articulatory gesture in speech production: The case of lateral, rhotic and (alveolo)palatal consonants}, volume = {1}, year = {2016}} Este texto pode ser livremente copiado, sob os termos da licença Creative Commons Atribuição-NãoComercial 4.0 Interna- https://creativecommons.org/ cional (CC BY-NC 4.0). licenses/by-nc/4.0/deed.pt_BR Gradus 1 (1) 24 Abstract Articulatory data are provided showing that, in lan- guages in which they have phonemic status, (alve- olo)palatal consonants, dark /l/ and the trill /r/ are articulated with a single lingual gesture instead of two independent tongue front and tongue body gestures. They are therefore simple, not complex segments. It is argued that tongue body lowering and retraction for dark /l/ and the trill /r/ is associated with manner of articulation re- quirements and with requirements on the implementation of the darkness percept in the case of dark /l/, and that tongue body raising and fronting for (alveolo)palatals results naturally from the contraction of the genioglossus muscle. These consonant units resemble truly complex palatalized and velarized or pharyngealized dentoalveo- lars regarding lingual coniguration and kinematics, as well as coarticulatory efects and phonological and sound change processes.
    [Show full text]
  • LINGUISTICS 221 LECTURE #3 the BASIC SOUNDS of ENGLISH 1. STOPS a Stop Consonant Is Produced with a Complete Closure of Airflow
    LINGUISTICS 221 LECTURE #3 Introduction to Phonetics and Phonology THE BASIC SOUNDS OF ENGLISH 1. STOPS A stop consonant is produced with a complete closure of airflow in the vocal tract; the air pressure has built up behind the closure; the air rushes out with an explosive sound when released. The term plosive is also used for oral stops. ORAL STOPS: e.g., [b] [t] (= plosives) NASAL STOPS: e.g., [m] [n] (= nasals) There are three phases of stop articulation: i. CLOSING PHASE (approach or shutting phase) The articulators are moving from an open state to a closed state; ii. CLOSURE PHASE (= occlusion) Blockage of the airflow in the oral tract; iii. RELEASE PHASE Sudden reopening; it may be accompanied by a burst of air. ORAL STOPS IN ENGLISH a. BILABIAL STOPS: The blockage is made with the two lips. spot [p] voiceless baby [b] voiced 1 b. ALVEOLAR STOPS: The blade (or the tip) of the tongue makes a closure with the alveolar ridge; the sides of the tongue are along the upper teeth. lamino-alveolar stops or Check your apico-alveolar stops pronunciation! stake [t] voiceless deep [d] voiced c. VELAR STOPS: The closure is between the back of the tongue (= dorsum) and the velum. dorso-velar stops scar [k] voiceless goose [g] voiced 2. NASALS (= nasal stops) The air is stopped in the oral tract, but the velum is lowered so that the airflow can go through the nasal tract. All nasals are voiced. NASALS IN ENGLISH a. BILABIAL NASAL: made [m] b. ALVEOLAR NASAL: need [n] c.
    [Show full text]
  • The Violability of Backness in Retroflex Consonants
    The violability of backness in retroflex consonants Paul Boersma University of Amsterdam Silke Hamann ZAS Berlin February 11, 2005 Abstract This paper addresses remarks made by Flemming (2003) to the effect that his analysis of the interaction between retroflexion and vowel backness is superior to that of Hamann (2003b). While Hamann maintained that retroflex articulations are always back, Flemming adduces phonological as well as phonetic evidence to prove that retroflex consonants can be non-back and even front (i.e. palatalised). The present paper, however, shows that the phonetic evidence fails under closer scrutiny. A closer consideration of the phonological evidence shows, by making a principled distinction between articulatory and perceptual drives, that a reanalysis of Flemming’s data in terms of unviolated retroflex backness is not only possible but also simpler with respect to the number of language-specific stipulations. 1 Introduction This paper is a reply to Flemming’s article “The relationship between coronal place and vowel backness” in Phonology 20.3 (2003). In a footnote (p. 342), Flemming states that “a key difference from the present proposal is that Hamann (2003b) employs inviolable articulatory constraints, whereas it is a central thesis of this paper that the constraints relating coronal place to tongue-body backness are violable”. The only such constraint that is violable for Flemming but inviolable for Hamann is the constraint that requires retroflex coronals to be articulated with a back tongue body. Flemming expresses this as the violable constraint RETRO!BACK, or RETRO!BACKCLO if it only requires that the closing phase of a retroflex consonant be articulated with a back tongue body.
    [Show full text]
  • Sociophonetic Variation in Bolivian Quechua Uvular Stops
    Title Page Sociophonetic Variation in Bolivian Quechua Uvular Stops by Eva Bacas University of Pittsburgh, 2019 Submitted to the Graduate Faculty of the Dietrich School of Arts and Sciences in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Bachelor of Philosophy University of Pittsburgh 2019 Committee Page UNIVERSITY OF PITTSBURGH DIETRICH SCHOOL OF ARTS AND SCIENCES This thesis was presented by Eva Bacas It was defended on November 8, 2019 and approved by Alana DeLoge, Quechua Instructor, Department of Linguistics, University of Pittsburgh Melinda Fricke, Assistant Professor, Department of Linguistics, University of Pittsburgh Gillian Gallagher, Associate Professor, Department of Linguistics, New York University Thesis Advisor/Dissertation Director: Claude Mauk, Senior Lecturer, Department of Linguistics, University of Pittsburgh ii Copyright © by Eva Bacas 2019 iii Abstract Sociophonetic Variation in Bolivian Quechua Uvular Stops Eva Bacas, BPhil University of Pittsburgh, 2019 Quechua is an indigenous language of the Andes region of South America. In Cochabamba, Bolivia, Quechua and Spanish have been in contact for over 500 years. In this thesis, I explore sociolinguistic variation among bilingual speakers of Cochabamba Quechua (CQ) and Spanish by investigating the relationship between the production of the voiceless uvular stop /q/ and speakers’ sociolinguistic backgrounds. I conducted a speech production study and sociolinguistic interview with seven bilingual CQ-Spanish speakers. I analyzed manner of articulation and place of articulation variation. Results indicate that manner of articulation varies primarily due to phonological factors, and place of articulation varies according to sociolinguistic factors. This reveals that among bilingual CQ-Spanish speakers, production of voiceless uvular stop /q/ does vary sociolinguistically.
    [Show full text]
  • A Comparative Analysis of Consonants in Chinese and Arabic Phonetics
    Journal of Literature and Art Studies, November 2019, Vol. 9, No. 11, 1188-1193 doi: 10.17265/2159-5836/2019.11.010 D DAVID PUBLISHING A Comparative Analysis of Consonants in Chinese and Arabic Phonetics FAYROUZ ABDELSAMIE ELSAYED MOHAMED School of International Chinese Studies (SICS) of East China Normal University (ECNU), Shanghai , China Arabic and Chinese are one of the most difficult languages to learn in the world. The mark of Arabic is Pinyin characters plus pinyin symbols. On the other hand, the mark of Chinese is ideographic square shaped characters; each pronunciation of a Chinese character is generally a syllable. There are great differences between Arabic and Chinese consonants, so this paper mainly discusses the similarities and differences between the two languages and their respective characteristics through the comparative analysis of Chinese and Arabic consonants. Keywords: Chinese, Arabic, pronunciation, consonant, comparison A Comparative Analysis of Chinese and Arabic Consonants In order to make a comparative analysis of Chinese and Arabic consonants, first of all, we need to have a quite comprehensive and clear understanding of the features of each consonant pronunciation of the two languages. All consonant phonemes in Chinese and Arabic are included in Table 2 below. The Chinese phonetic symbol is Pinyin, which uses the International Latin alphabets. It has both consonant and vowel letters, while Arabic is phonetic alphabets transcription and the phonetic symbol of consonants is Arabic letters itself. Chinese language has 22 consonant phonemes; among them [ŋ] can only be used as a tail vowel, [n] can be used as both initial and final, and the rest can only be used as initials.
    [Show full text]
  • Variation and Change in Latin American Spanish and Portuguese
    Variation and change in Latin American Spanish and Portuguese Gregory R. Guy New York University Fieldworker:¿Que Ud. considera ‘buen español? New York Puerto Rican Informant: Tiene que pronunciar la ‘s’. Western hemisphere varieties of Spanish and Portuguese show substantial similarity in the patterning of sociolinguistic variation and change. Caribbean and coastal dialects of Latin American Spanish share several variables with Brazilian Portuguese (e.g., deletion of coda –s, –r). These variables also show similar social distribution in Hispanic and Lusophone communities: formal styles and high status speakers are consonantally conservative, while higher deletion is associated with working class speakers and informal styles. The regions that show these sociolinguistic parallels also share common historical demographic characteristics, notably a significant population of African ancestry and the associated history of extensive contact with African languages into the 19th C. But contemporary changes in progress are also active, further differentiating Latin American language varieties. Keywords: Brazilian Portuguese, Latin American Spanish, coda deletion, variation and change. 1. Introduction The Spanish and Portuguese languages have long been the objects of separate tradi- tions of scholarship that treat each of them in isolation. But this traditional separation is more indicative of political distinctions – Spain and Portugal have been separate nation-states for almost a millennium – than of any marked linguistic differences. In fact, these two Iberian siblings exhibit extensive linguistic resemblance, as well as no- tably parallel and intertwined social histories in the Americas. As this volume attests, these languages may very fruitfully be examined together, and such a joint and com- parative approach permits broader generalizations and deeper insights than may be obtained by considering each of them separately.
    [Show full text]
  • Lecture 5 Sound Change
    An articulatory theory of sound change An articulatory theory of sound change Hypothesis: Most common initial motivation for sound change is the automation of production. Tokens reduced online, are perceived as reduced and represented in the exemplar cluster as reduced. Therefore we expect sound changes to reflect a decrease in gestural magnitude and an increase in gestural overlap. What are some ways to test the articulatory model? The theory makes predictions about what is a possible sound change. These predictions could be tested on a cross-linguistic database. Sound changes that take place in the languages of the world are very similar (Blevins 2004, Bateman 2000, Hajek 1997, Greenberg et al. 1978). We should consider both common and rare changes and try to explain both. Common and rare changes might have different characteristics. Among the properties we could look for are types of phonetic motivation, types of lexical diffusion, gradualness, conditioning environment and resulting segments. Common vs. rare sound change? We need a database that allows us to test hypotheses concerning what types of changes are common and what types are not. A database of sound changes? Most sound changes have occurred in undocumented periods so that we have no record of them. Even in cases with written records, the phonetic interpretation may be unclear. Only a small number of languages have historic records. So any sample of known sound changes would be biased towards those languages. A database of sound changes? Sound changes are known only for some languages of the world: Languages with written histories. Sound changes can be reconstructed by comparing related languages.
    [Show full text]
  • A Cinefluorographic Study of the Articulation of Uvular Consonants in Swedish and West Greenlandic
    A cinefluorographic study of the articulation of uvular consonants in Swedish and West Greenlandic Abstract This poster concerns the articulation of uvular consonants with particular reference to quantal aspects of speech production. Data from X-ray motion films are presented. Two speakers of Southern Swedish give examples of [R], and one speaker of West Greenlandic Inuit gives examples of [R] and [q]. Traditionally, uvular consonants are said to be produced by articulating the dorsum against the uvula. Unfortunately, this ought not work, owing to the presence of air passages either side of the uvular: (1) these passages should prevent occlusion for a stop, (2) similarly they should prevent necessary conditions for a Bernoulli force for a tremulant, and (3) they should prevent a Reynolds number sufficiently small for fricatives. The formant transitions to vowels adjacent to these consonants suggest instead that the place of articulation is in the upper pharynx, at the same place as is constricted for [o]-like vowels. The X-ray films confirm that these three subjects constrict the upper pharynx for these consonants. But there was a difference of timing of the uvular consonant gesture for these two languages. The Swedish subjects completed the vowel as usual and then initiated the uvular tongue body gesture. The Inuit subject initiated both vowel and uvular consonant gestures earlier, so that the vocoid segment consisted of the transition to the uvular consonant, reflecting the regular pharyngeal assimilation of vowels preceding uvulars in West Greenlandic. Wood, S. A cinefluorographic study of uvular consonants in Swedish and West Greenlandic. Poster presented at the conference From Sound to Sense, 11-13 June 2004.
    [Show full text]
  • LT3212 Phonetics Assignment 4 Mavis, Wong Chak Yin
    LT3212 Phonetics Assignment 4 Mavis, Wong Chak Yin Essay Title: The sound system of Japanese This essay aims to introduce the sound system of Japanese, including the inventories of consonants, vowels, and diphthongs. The phonological variations of the sound segments in different phonetic environments are also included. For the illustration, word examples are given and they are presented in the following format: [IPA] (Romaji: “meaning”). Consonants In Japanese, there are 14 core consonants, and some of them have a lot of allophonic variations. The various types of consonants classified with respect to their manner of articulation are presented as follows. Stop Japanese has six oral stops or plosives, /p b t d k g/, which are classified into three place categories, bilabial, alveolar, and velar, as listed below. In each place category, there is a pair of plosives with the contrast in voicing. /p/ = a voiceless bilabial plosive [p]: [ippai] (ippai: “A cup of”) /b/ = a voiced bilabial plosive [b]: [baɴ] (ban: “Night”) /t/ = a voiceless alveolar plosive [t]: [oto̞ ːto̞ ] (ototo: “Brother”) /d/ = a voiced alveolar plosive [d]: [to̞ mo̞ datɕi] (tomodachi: “Friend”) /k/ = a voiceless velar plosive [k]: [kaiɰa] (kaiwa: “Conversation”) /g/ = a voiced velar plosive [g]: [ɡakɯβsai] (gakusai: “Student”) Phonetically, Japanese also has a glottal stop [ʔ] which is commonly produced to separate the neighboring vowels occurring in different syllables. This phonological phenomenon is known as ‘glottal stop insertion’. The glottal stop may be realized as a pause, which is used to indicate the beginning or the end of an utterance. For instance, the word “Japanese money” is actually pronounced as [ʔe̞ ɴ], instead of [je̞ ɴ], and the pronunciation of “¥15” is [dʑɯβːɡo̞ ʔe̞ ɴ].
    [Show full text]
  • Part 1: Introduction to The
    PREVIEW OF THE IPA HANDBOOK Handbook of the International Phonetic Association: A guide to the use of the International Phonetic Alphabet PARTI Introduction to the IPA 1. What is the International Phonetic Alphabet? The aim of the International Phonetic Association is to promote the scientific study of phonetics and the various practical applications of that science. For both these it is necessary to have a consistent way of representing the sounds of language in written form. From its foundation in 1886 the Association has been concerned to develop a system of notation which would be convenient to use, but comprehensive enough to cope with the wide variety of sounds found in the languages of the world; and to encourage the use of thjs notation as widely as possible among those concerned with language. The system is generally known as the International Phonetic Alphabet. Both the Association and its Alphabet are widely referred to by the abbreviation IPA, but here 'IPA' will be used only for the Alphabet. The IPA is based on the Roman alphabet, which has the advantage of being widely familiar, but also includes letters and additional symbols from a variety of other sources. These additions are necessary because the variety of sounds in languages is much greater than the number of letters in the Roman alphabet. The use of sequences of phonetic symbols to represent speech is known as transcription. The IPA can be used for many different purposes. For instance, it can be used as a way to show pronunciation in a dictionary, to record a language in linguistic fieldwork, to form the basis of a writing system for a language, or to annotate acoustic and other displays in the analysis of speech.
    [Show full text]