i

Vowel Quality and Phonological Pro jection

Marc van Oostendorp

PhD Thesis

Tilburg University

September

Acknowledgements

The following p eople have help ed me prepare and write this dissertation

John Alderete Elena Anagnostop oulou Sjef Barbiers Outi BatEl

Dorothee Beermann Clemens Bennink Adams Bo domo Geert Bo oij Hans

Bro ekhuis Norb ert Corver Martine Dhondt Ruud and Henny Dhondt

Jo Emonds Dicky Gilb ers Janet Grijzenhout Carlos Gussenhoven Gert

jan Hakkenb erg Marco Haverkort Lars Hellan Ben Hermans Bart Holle

brandse Hannekevan Ho of Angeliek van Hout Ro eland van Hout Harry

van der Hulst Riny Huybregts Rene Kager HansPeter Kolb Emiel Krah

mer David Leblanc Winnie Lechner Klarien van der Linde John Mc

Carthy Dominique Nouveau Rolf Noyer Jaap and Hannyvan Oosten

dorp Paola Monachesi Krisztina Polgardi Alan Prince Curt Rice Henk

van Riemsdijk Iggy Ro ca Sam Rosenthall Grazyna Rowicka Lisa Selkirk

Chris Sijtsma Craig Thiersch MiekeTrommelen Rub en van der Vijver

Janneke Visser Riet Vos Jero en van de Weijer Wim Zonneveld

Iwant to thank them all They have made the past four years for what

it was the most interesting and happiest p erio d in mylife until now ii

Contents

Intro duction

The Headedness of

The Headedness Hyp othesis HH

Theoretical Background

Structure

Feature geometry

Sp ecication and Undersp ecicati on

Skeletal tier

Mo del of the grammar

Data

Organisation of the thesis

Chapter

Chapter

Chapter

Chapter

Chapter

Chapter

Chapter

App endices

Quality and Rhyme Structure in Dutch

Intro duction

The Dutchvowel system

versus The Case of Dutch

The argument for length

The arguments against length

A theory based on the feature lax

lax and syllable structure in

Tenseness and branchingness in Dutch iii

CONTENTS iv

Formalization in Optimality theory

Some more arguments for the length of A

Tenseness cannot b e dened in a satisfactory way

phonetically

Minimality requires branching

Avowels form the domain of tonal contour in Lim

burg Dutch

Conclusion

App endix Historical overview

Dutch structuralism

Pregenerative literature

Early generative grammar

Bisegmental analyses in generative

Tilburg Dutch and Standard DutchVowel Length

Details in the Standard Dutchvowel system

Ambisyllabicity

rlengthening

The phonetic nature of the tensing feature

Extrasyllabicity and catalexis

A dialect with real length Tilburg Dutch

The vowel system

Why only lax vowels can b e long

Vowel shortening

Analysis

Long vowels in other Brabant dialects

The limited distribution of long vowels

Conclusion

Derivation of the Dutchvowel system

Conclusion

Derived schwa in Dutch

Intro duction

Prop erties of rschwa

initial p osition

Wordnal p osition

Vowel quality

Closed syllables

Style registers

Prop erties of eschwa

CONTENTS v

The ep enthetic vowel is schwa

Eschwa do not o ccur at the end of the word

Eschwa only o ccurs in the last syllable of the word

Wordinternal contexts in whicheschwa do es not o ccur

Style registers

Summary and conclusion

Dutch Uschwa

Intro duction

Prop erties of uschwa

Syllable weight

Uschwa do es not o ccur wordinitially

Some other segmental eects

The onset of schwasyllables

The co da of schwaheaded syllables

Degenerate and sc hwaheaded syllables

Obligatory versus optional ep enthesis

Again on complex onsets

Schwa surrounded by identical

Schwa after ng

Uschwa and stress

Adjacency b etween schwa and full vowels

Complementary distribution of uschwa and eschwa

Schwadeletion

Conclusion

Previous analyses of uschwa

Reduction Theory

Ep enthesis Theory

NoSyllable Theory

Remaining problems

Final Devoicing

Sup erheavy syllables b efore schwa

Postlexical uschwa

Conclusion

Table of Prop erties

SchwainFrench and Norwegian

Intro duction

French

Eschwaistheepenthetic vowel

Eschwa do es not o ccur at the end of the word

CONTENTS vi

Uschwamust o ccur in an op en syllable

Laxing in the head of a fo ot

Uschwa do es not o ccur at the b eginning of the word

clusters b efore schwa cannot b e p ossible

complex onsets

Schwa is stressless

Schwa cannot o ccur next to a vowel

Schwadeletion

A parameter

Conclusion plus a note on learnability

Norwegian

Schwa is the ep enthetic vowel

Ep enthetic schwadoesnotoccurattheendoftheword

Schwamust o ccur in an op en syllable

Schwa do es not o ccur at the b eginning of the word

Consonant clusters b efore schwa cannot b e p ossible

complex onsets

Alternation with degenerate syllables

Conclusion and another note on learnability

Conclusion

AVowelGlide Alternation in Rotterdam Dutch

Intro duction

The second p erson clitic

Hiatus after high vowels

After coronal stops

Third p erson singular clitic

The diminutivesux

Sieverss Law

Other issues

Clitics and the diminutive in Standard Dutch

Lexical forms

The underlying form of S is not i

High vowel followed byschwa

st p erson plural clitic

Conclusion

The Pro jection ConstraintFamily

Intro duction

Pro jection and weakness

The fo ot level

CONTENTS vii

N level The

Nlevel

Nuclear level

Features

Constraints conicting with pro jection and weakness

Conclusion

A Constraints and Families of Constraints

A Proso dic wellformedness

A Syllable wellformedness

A Foot wellformedness

A Word wellformedness

A Autosegmental Representations

A Feature Co o ccurrence and Licensing

A Parsing

A Constraints against unnecessary structure

A Ad Ho c Constraint

B Arguments for ranking

B Top ology of the Dutch lexicon

B Top ology of the Dutch p ostlexical phonology

C Ranking schemes

C Top ology of the Standard Dutch lexicon

C Top ology of the Standard Dutch p ostlexical phonology

C Top ology of the

C Top ology of the Rotterdam

References

Summary in Dutch

Summary in English

Intro duction

The Headedness of Syllables

Almost all phonological theories which recognise the syllable as a con

stituent also p ostulate that this constituent has a head The notions head

and constituent are formalised in dierentways in dierent theories but

the underlying intuition seems to b e the same in all formalisms In this

thesis I explore some consequences of this headedness hyp othesis whichI

think have not suciently b een worked out

The criteria for headedness are quite diverse but fortunately they con

verge most of the time I will mention the most imp ortant ones here

One criterion is sonority As is wellknown universally the syllable

consists of a monotonic sonority rise optionally followed by a monotonic

sonority fall see Basbll Clements for a few recent discussions

Therefore every syllable has exactly one sonority p eak We could call this

p eak the head

Another criterion is prominence Some phonological features like stress

or seem phonologically not to b e assigned to individual segments but

rather to syllables or other proso dic constituents When these features get

realized they generally do so on one sp ecic segment Again we could

call this segmentthe head of the syllable Clements Hermans a

Odden

Inversely it has sometimes b een argued that the fact that vowel fea

tures can spread skipping consonants in vowel harmony pro cesses whereas

owels is an consonantal features seem to never spread over intervening v

argument for headedness Kaye Lowenstamm and Vergnaud Van Lit

Van der Hulst and Van de Weijer Vowels b eing typical syl

lable heads pro ject to a level for instance the nucleus where consonants

are no longer visible Whereas there is a level at which these vowels are

adjacent to one another there is no similar level for consonants

A third criterion is dispensabilityWhenwehave a syllable like tap in

a we can strip o the t andor the p still retaining a complete

syllable ta ap a But in most we cannot do the same

The Headedness of Syllables

with the a tp We could call this indisp ensi ble segmentthehead

Unfortunately this criterion is not very strong as it stands b ecause it gives

undesirable results in the case of diphthongs pai and in languages with

syllabic sonorants pan In the rst case b oth a and i would b e

assigned the status of head b ecause the language would presumably allow

b oth pa and pi syllables in the second case b oth a and n would

get the same status b ecause the language would allow b oth pa and pn

On the other hand we will see that some syllable heads in some languages

can only o ccur in closed syllables Also the onset seems to b e an obligatory

sub constituent of syllables in many languages without it b eing a head In

those cases this criterion would say the syllable has no head at all Still the

intuition b ehind this criterion seems clear the head is the segmentthat

determines to a considerable extent the nature of the syllable

A last p ossible criterion amply discussed in Dresher and Van der Hulst

is complexity According to these scholars heads in proso dic struc

ture in general allow more complexity than their dep endents For instance

the typical head of a fo ot is a heavy syllable whereas the typical dep endent

is light Although Dresher and Van der Hulst do not explicitly discuss the

internal structure of syllables we might assume that segments have some

internal complexity measured for instance bythenumb er of marked fea

tures they contain we then exp ect every syllable to contain exactly one

most complex segment whichwewould call the head Some phonologists

haverecently argued for a complexity metric in the sense discussed here

Harris Rice Dogil Interestingly this notion of complex

ity seems to coincide with sonority The most sonorous segmen ts are shown

to b e the most complex ones by the latter two authors but the rst author

gives exactly the opp osite interpretation This criterion is the only one that

do es not seem to b e directly compatible with the framework prop osed in

this thesis where some kinds of complexity cause segments to b e syllable

heads whereas other kinds of complexity cause segments to b e dep endents

in the syllable



Although the Headedness Hyp othesis is accepted bymany phonologists

Icontend that it has more consequences than is usually acknowledged

In other areas of grammatical theory it is often assumed that features of

the head can determine the structure of the pro jection which is built on that

This situation would b ecome even worse if we consider languages suchasBerberDell

and Elmedlaoui or Bella Co ola Bagemihl in such languages anysegment

can form a syllable of its own so that the criterion would collapse



The most wellknown current theory that do es not accept the syllable as a constituent

at all is Government Phonology Kaye Lowenstamm and Vergnaud Kaye

The most wellknown theory that recognizes the syllable but no headedness is mora

theory under some of its guises Hyman Hayes b but cf also Hayes

The Headedness of Syllables

head For instance in the socalled minimalist view of syntactic structure

Chomsky a lexical head which p ossesses a strong feature F requires

a functional head to b e present higher up in the pro jection which can license

F cf Muysken and Van Riemsdijk for an older collection of articles

dealing with features and pro jection in syntax Similarily in morphology

it is often assumed that morphological heads can have sub categorization

frames which determine the structure of the word ie the presence vs

absence of other morphemes

In phonological theory the head is not often assumed to play this role

In this thesis I will derive and defend a more sp ecic hyp othesis on the role

of the syllable head in proso dic structure whichsays that the head of a

syllable denes the internal structure of that syllable

Headedness The structure of a syllable is determined by the

feature structure of its head

In particular I will argue that the feature structure of heads determines

to a large extent the licensing of onset and co da p ositions in the syllable

For instance it is wellknown that reduced vowels often show a severely

simplied syllable structure and do not allow co das or complex onsets I

will argue that there are constraints of the following typ es i if a segment

dominates a feature F it should head a branching constituent of typeTand

ii if a segment heads a branching constituent of type T it should dominate

afeatureF I call these constraints pro jection constraints I will show

that there are such constraints for every level of the metrical hierarchyupto

and including the fo ot level and for several typ es of features most notably

those features referring to vowel height This is the central h yp othesis of

this thesis

Below I will briey intro duce the meaning of this hyp othesis Subse

quently I will oer a division into chapters of the material which has to b e

discussed

The Headedness Hyp othesis HH Icho ose to adopt the

following template for a maximal syllable cf section on several details

of this structure like the status of the onset graphically representing the

headedness prop erty in the most straightforward way

The Headedness of Syllables

N

N

N



N

b r k

The Headedness Hyp othesis claims that the variable prop erties of the struc

ture in are dep endent on the feature material in the head Sp ecically

I will prop ose certain conditions relating the presence of phonological fea

tures in the head and the branchingness of X no des of some pro jection

level

It is a direct consequence of the Headedness Hyp othesis that no part of

syllable structure should b e underlying Underlying representations only

contain phonological features and p erhaps asso ciation lines I assume that

skeletal p oints X slots or morae cannot b e part of underlying repre

sentations This means that underlying segmental length long vowels or

geminate consonants can only b e represented bywayofatworo ot rep

resentation Selkirk It also has some obvious consequences for the

underlying representation of glides and vowels as will b e discussed in chap

ter

Yet I will argue that also this typ e of representation is not always nec

essary and that phonetic length can in some cases also b e the result of

the presence versus absence of some feature I will show that this is one

instance of the wellknown tenselax distinction which corresp onds to the

op en versus closed structure of syllables

It is usually claimed that Dutch phonology draws a distinction b e

tween vowels which are long and vowels which are short on the basis

of paradigms such as the following

a kim horizon km kip chicken kp krimp shrink krmp

b kiem germ kim keep goalie kip kriemp krimp

Phonetically the dierence is one of tenseness rather than length at least

in the case of the high vowels but this tenseness can b e derived from

the length The phonetic mo dule automatically interprets long vowels as

tense and subsequently shortens tense high vowels under this standard

interpretation

The gap in can b e explained if we accept that i Dutchallows at

most three segments in the rhyme mo dulo coronal stops and ii the vowel

The Headedness of Syllables

i o ccupies two slots in the rhyme whereas the vowel o ccupies only one

slot ie we distinguish b etween the twovowels by representing them as

in where I stands for the segmental material which is supp osed to b e

the same for i and under some analyses Similar distinctions could b e

made for instance b etween a and o and and and

a x

I

b x x

I

A lot of empirical problems arise however if we assume the representations

in All these problems lead me to conclude that Dutch do es not havea

length distinction at all The underlying dierence rather has to b e stated

in terms of lax Furthermore the Dutchrhyme allows two p ositions rather

than three b oth rmp and rip are larger than monosyllabic parsing the

p into a degenerate syllable These assumptions allow me to explain the

paradigm in by the following constraint

Connect N lax



N N dominates lax N branches a Projectlax



b ProjectNlax N branches N dominates lax

This is an instance of what I call a biconditional pro jection constraint

Such constraints will b e generally referred to in this thesis by the name

ConnectX Y In many places it will b e useful to split the biconditional

formulation into two simple conditional formulations These will b e referred

to as ProjectX Y Both parameters X and Y can range over a set of

features or the set of pro jection levels

The constraint Connectlax N can b e shown to b e op erativealsoin

a harmony language like Eastern Javanese in this language all the vowels

in a word are lax i its nal syllable is closed Similar conclusions can b e

reached for some Portuguese Spanish and French dialects and for Tagalog

Chamorro has a constraint relating the op enclosed character of a syllable

to the height of the dominating vowel

All the prop erties of Dutch just mentioned get a natural explanation

under the present assumptions Furthermore by it is also explained

why syllables cannot end in a lax vowel Dutch has C V syllables in all



cases where V is not a lax vowel

The use of a parameterlike notation in ConnectX Y of course sug

gests that there are other p ossibiliti es Ihopetoshowinthefollowing

The Headedness of Syllables

chapters that Dutch has evidence for pro jection constraints at every level



of pro jection up to and including the fo ot ie at the N N N and fo ot

level In most cases we refer to branching constituents for instance the

constraint ProjectFt lax in the analysis of French crucially refers to



branching feet this is indicated by the subscript Yet in the chapters

and I will refer in a few cases to a constraint on pro jection to the head

of a fo ot regardless whether this fo ot is branching or not

The range of features which can b e mentioned in pro jection constraints

is also quite large and includes at least the ma jor class features consonantal

and sonorant the features high low and lax which I will call the

aperture features tone and the feature sets V the set of all vo calic features

and F the set of all features

a V fhigh low lax coronal labial dorsal cons song

v v v

the set of vo calic features

b C fcoronal labial dorsal g the set of consonantal fea

c c c

tures

c F the set of all segmental features

A complete formal discussion of all p ossible and imp ossible pro jection con

straints will b e provided in chapter

It mightbeinteresting that one of the most wellknown constrain ts of

syllabic theory can now b e reformulated as a pro jection constraint

Onset ProjectF NIfanN has any feature F F in its

head N should branch

According to this version of Onset the only onsetless syllables that are

allowed are syllables with a completely empty head This is what makes

it dierent from other formulations of this constraint Yet we could argue

that empyheaded syllables without an onset will b e ruled out by indep en



dent considerations of representational economy As so on as a syllable has

anything in its head it should also have something in its onset also accord

ing to For ease of reference I will stick to the name Onset instead of

ProjectX Y throughout this thesis

For now it is imp ortant to note that I assume that the pro jection con

straints can b e divided into two subsets one b ottom up eg Projectlax

N and one top down eg ProjectN lax It is useful to distinguish

between the twotyp es of constraints b ecause in some languages we nd

evidence for the b ottom up version of a constraint but not for the top down



Or by the constraint Contour p

The Headedness of Syllables

version or vice versa If we assume a principle of constraintinteraction

it can b e shown that b ottom up and top down constraints play dierent

roles in the hierarchyFor instance in some French dialects mid vow

els in closed syllables get always laxed chapter showing an eect of

ProjectN lax Yet the reex of Projectlax N simply cannot b e

observed mid lax vowels can also o ccur in op en syllables This shows that

the former constraint is visibly activeinFrench but the latter is not

Pro jection constraints in this thesis refer either to the sp ecic features

cons son lax or low or to one of the feature sets F the set of all

features or V the set of all vo calic features

It will also b e necessary in a few cases to refer to the logical opp osite of

pro jection constraints These are constraints that sayeitheri if a segment

has a feature F it should beinadependent position at level T or ii if a

segment is in a dependent position at level T it should have a featureFAll

weakness constraints we encounter in this thesis refer to either high or

consonantal Such constraints I call weakness constraints Examples

of such constraints are the constraints whichsaythathighvowels are the

b est candidates for gliding Weak N high and that consonants prefer

to o ccur in the margin of a syllable Weak cons N Constraints like

these will b e intro duced in chapters and

Inow turn to schwa I will analyse this vowel as an empty root node

with just the ma jor class features sp ecied on it I think it is p ossible to

show that the many prop erties of schwa can b e derived from its b eing an

empty root node

The main prop erties can b e stated as follows schwadoesnotallow

complex onsets it cannot b e stressed and it only allows sonorant consonants

in the co da I wish to argue that each of these prop erties follows from the

defective feature contentofthisvowel For this reason it can only pro ject

a minimal structure ie a core CV syllable

We already knowthatschwa cannot head a closed syllable b ecause of

I supp ose syllables where schwa seems to b e followed by a sonorant

actually have this sonorant as the head of the syllable phonologically the

schwa is lled in latersay in the phonetic comp onent

What I need to derive then is that placeless vowels do not allow adjunc

tion even though like all vowels they require an onset Potential complex

onsets always consist of an sonorant sequence in Dutch By the

reasoning just outlined these sequences could as well b e syllables of their

own I will argue it is actually even b etter for them to b e complete syllables

than to b e adjunction structures to an emptyheaded syllable

If these assumptions are worked out in the prop er way they explain at

least the rst two prop erties of schwa immediately Supp ose for instance

The Headedness of Syllables

that a full syllable like drie three dri has the following structure

N

N

N



N

d r i

Supp ose that vo calic place features are necessary in order to license the

higherlevel N Since schwadoesnothave these features it can only license

the lower level N

N

N



N

m

We can also assume that N cannot branch If it did by it would get a

sp ecication as lax Because vowels with only ap erture sp ecications do

not surface in Dutch this would in turn trigger a whole apparatus of rules

lling in also place features for schwa whichwould nally surface as as

in French achter to buy il achte he buys The question now

is why in Dutchwe can nd sonorant consonants after the schwavader

father adem breathe open id vadr adm opn I prop ose

that in these cases the schwa is not underlying but rather a sp ecic form

of phonetic sp ellout Standard Dutch uses for syllabic sonorants This

means that at the end of the word sonorant consonants have the p ossibility

of pro jecting a CV syllable The last syllable of vader can b e represented

as

N

N



N

d r

Theoretical Background

This concludes the outline of my attempt to uphold the Headedness Hy

p othesis in the phonology of Dutch Approximately the same problems

arise in the phonology of French and Norwegian I will show these prob

lems can b e solved in this same framework and with a few minor dierences

in the sp ecication of the grammar of these three languages

Theoretical Background

In this section I will present a discussion of the phonological framework

adopted in this study Unfortunately there is nothing like a uniform Stan

dard theory of phonology to which one can refer at this moment In a study

like this one therefore has to make partly arbitrary c hoices b etween

comp eting phonological theories on topics which are not of direct concern

to the study at hand In this section I present some of the particular the

oretical choices I have made A more complete overview of recent theories

can b e found in works such as Kenstowicz a Ro ca and Gold

smith This section can b e used as a reference while studying the

chapters that follow

Syllable Structure The representation of syllable structure I

X theoretic constituent but one without skeletal p oints Xslots use is an

or mor cf section

N

N

N



N

b r k

Versions of Xbar theoretic representations have b een prop osed in Levin

Uriagereka Michaels Harris and Hermans

a We will see b elow that there is also some evidence for branching

nuclei in the representation of long vowels and diphthongs chapter

The choice for this particular notation is partly arbitraryInVan Oost

endorp I have established equivalence classes of trees Xstructures

bracketed grids dep endency graphs and autosegmental representations

Each of these can imitate imp ortant asp ects of the b ehaviour of anyof

Theoretical Background

the others We can therefore cho ose the notation which suits our purp oses

b est

An advantage of X structures is that they graphically representthehead

relations that form the theme of this dissertation in a fairly straightforward

way With a few adaptations an alternative notation like the mora theory

Hyman Hayes b can also represent headedness but it do es

not seem to b e particularily suitable for this goal The same is true for the

classical onsetrhymenucleus theory The notation used in Government

Phonology Kaye Lowenstamm and Vergnaud is not able to

represent the relation b etween syllable head and onset segments in the way

which is needed here In other I crucially need a syllable no de

for some of my analyses to hold Dep endency trees nally are simply

equivalent to the Xbar structures used here I havechosen for the latter

b ecause they are more familiar b ecause the morphological and syntactic

theory I assume throughout this study is Government and Binding theory

Chomsky rather than Dep endency syntax and b ecause

my assumptions on segmental structure dier from those of Dep endency

Phonology

Furthermore recentdevelopments in syntactic X theory seem to help

solving some problems The classical problem for instance why onsets

o ccur on the left and co das on the right can in Xtheory b e reduced to the

more general problem of why al l Xstructure syntactic or phonological has

a Sp ecierHeadComplement order Kayne and Chomsky

have argued that syntactic Xbar structure universally corresp onds to one

constituent order only This order seems to b e exactly the one prop osed

for the syllable

The representation in would not b e an acceptable Xbar repre

sentation in the sense of Kayne b ecause the sisters to the main

pro jection line are not maximal pro jections themselves and b ecause the N

no de pro jects twice Both of these latter two options are allowed within

Chomskys version of bare phrase structure theory the graphical



asp ects of which I will therefore adopt Another parallelism b etween the

Xbar structure used here and Chomskys bare phrase structures is that

N no des This corresp onds exactly to the number there can b e at most two

of segments in the onset

The most signi problem with the syllable tree in lies in its

relation to stress The dierence b etween heavy versus light syllables cannot

b e captured easily in this framework This is the reason why many theorists

have adopted a moraic mo del of syllable structure Stress is not the main

topic of this studyhowever and furthermore various prop osals have b een



Cf Chomsky for yet another view of syntactic X structure

Theoretical Background

made in the literature to reconcile nonmoraic mo dels of syllable structure

with the stress facts One of these prop osals could b e adopted

A large part of this dissertation will b e devoted to an argumentthat

n

the branching or nonbranching of a given N no de is dep endent on the



presence or absence of phonological features under N and vice versa I will

for instance argue that N can only branch if its head is lax in Dutch The

structure of a syllable therefore is very much dep endent on the head of that

syllable

I will not discuss the structure of the onset in any detail With Levin

McCarthy and Prince Hayes b and many others I as

sume that the onset do es not exist as a constituent Since I also assume

that phonological metrical structure is binary branching Lieb erman and

Prince Kiparsky seems the most plausible structure For

syntax it has b een argued that every phrase contains maximally one sp eci

er and one adjunct ie two elements dominated by N byZwart

and Chomsky Although it is hard to nd criteria distinguishing

between sp eciers and adjuncts in syllable structure it seems to b e gen

erally accepted that in many languages the maximal numb er of segments

inanonsetistwo mo dulo the coronal I will give indep endent

evidence for the assumption that b oth onset segments are dep endenton

the head in chapter

Feature geometry I adopt the following feature tree McCarthy

Clements and Hume



cons son

Laryngeal nasal

Cplace

coronal vo calic dorsal labial

lateral

Ap erture

Vplace high low lax

coronal labial dorsal

Some asp ects of this tree I havechosen simply for the sake of explicitnes s

Theoretical Background

For instance in most chapters I am not concerned with the structure

of the Laryngeal no de Ihave adopted the prop osal byLombardi



concerning this p oint Also the exact p osition of nasal lateral and

esp ecially continuant Selkirk Wetzels Steriade Padgett

is of no direct relevance to anyofmy claims

While I do need to draw a distinction b etween a Cplace and a Vplace

no de I am not sure the dep endency relation established here is crucial

A tree structure like the one prop osed in Lahiri and Koreman can

p ossibly work as well I also assume that the class no des lab eled Cplace

Vplace Laryngeal vo calic and Ap erture are structural nonterminals

they cannot o ccur in a representation unless they dominate some feature

Except for the ma jor class features Clements all features have

only one activevalue in the parts of the phonology that I study I will

assume that these features are monovalent but similar results can of course

b e obtained by theories of undersp ecicati on or of feature value markedness

The activevalue of all features is F

Ihavechosen what seemed to me the most common lab els for eachof

the features except p erhaps for the Ap erture features for which again I

refer to the next chapter I omitted several features to whichInever make

any reference at all such as strident and aspirated

Crucial asp ects of this tree for my analysis are the fact that the ma

jor class features are sp ecied on the ro ot McCarthy and further

more the structure directly under the ro ot Cplace vo calic Vplace and

Ap erture no des Clements a and the identityofvo calic and

consonantal place features Clements and Hume

Finally there is some ongoing debate on the status of the class no des

in The classical interpretation of these no des as real autosegmental

elements present on their own tier has recently b een questioned byHa yes

Van de Weijer and Padgett among others These au

thors have argued for an alternativeinterpretation under which lab els such

as Place or Supralaryngeal refer to sets of features rather than to indep en

dent no des Since these prop osals do not seem to diminish the descriptive

power of the theory in anywaymy prop osals here are neutral with resp ect

to this issue of interpretation Yet I do assume that class no des Vplace

or Ap erture can never b e terminal elements in the representation and this

assumption seems to b e more easily captured under the interpretation of

Hayes Van de Weijer and Padgett than under the classical interpretation

A large part of this thesis deals with Dutch phonologyInchapter I

will amply discuss the feature structure of the Dutchvowels and in chapter



In chapter I will briey consider the p osition of tone in the hierarchy and adopt a

prop osal under which it is dep endent on the Laryngeal no de

Theoretical Background

I discuss the glides j and and some palatalized consonants typical

for Rotterdam Dutch Here I simply list the Standard Dutch consonants



with their feature sp ecications

b t p k d b s f x

consonantal

sonorant

voice

coronal

labial

dorsal

continuant

nasal

r l n m v h

consonantal

sonorant

voice

coronal

labial

dorsal

continuant

nasal

rhotic

This sp ecication is not to o distant from what most Dutch phonologists

assume cf Bo oij

I sometimes refer to the in the discussion that follows al

though I do not assume that it is an underlying segment of Dutch It will

turn out to b e an imp ortant assumption that the glottal stop and h are

almost empty consonants the former consisting of a ro ot no de without

any dep endents and the latter of a ro ot no de which only dominates the fea

ture continuant I will return to the prop er sp ecication of these segments

in chapter I am not very sure ab out the feature sp ecication of r On

the one hand I will showbelow that it cannot have a place sp ecication



such as coronal or dorsal On the other hand it also do es not b ehave

like the really empty consonants b and h I haveprovisionally solved

this problem by assuming a feature rhotic



The features high lax and low have b een omitted since they are only connected

to vowels



Cf also Mester and Ito Steriade ab out the placelessness of r

Theoretical Background

Sp ecication and Undersp ecication As I have already in

dicated in the previous section I need to refer only to one value for every

feature except for the ma jor class features I therefore will assume all these

features are monovalent again except for cons and son The latter

features are supp osed to b e sp ecied throughout the derivation

Undersp ecic ation theory b ecomes less relevant under suchaviewof

features Nevertheless some additional assumptions have b een made First

of all I assume that coronal has the status of the most unmarked place

feature Paradis and Prunet Mohanan McCarthy and

Taub it is the place feature that is most likely to reduce and most

likely to b e inserted in the case of ep enthesis Yet it is still presentinall

coronal consonants and in all frontvowels

Furthermore I have decided to use a version of the theory of Combina

tory Sp ecication Archangeli and Pulleyblank which seemed most

compatible with OptimalityTheory I assume that every geometrically well

formed feature tree can b e input to the grammar Surface lters will then

lter out unwellformed feature combinations For instance Dutchdoesnot

haveanylow rounded vowels as we will see b elow In my view there can

still b e an input segmentwhichislinked to b oth low and labial Yet for

such a segment to surface something has to happ en either the feature low

or the feature labial or b oth has to b e delinked in order to escap e the

surface lter against low labial combinations

In addition to Combinatory Sp ecication I adopt a theory of p ossible

output lters or feature co o ccurrence restrictions All lters I need

t into one of the following twoschemes I assume AB

 

A

a

B

b AB

Constraints with the general form of a forbid the combination of in

compatible features such as low and labial or high and lax

Constraints following the scheme in b state that a certain feature A

should b e accompanied by another feature B If we ll in low for A and

lax for B we get the statement that all lowvowels are lax

The variables A and B can only refer to single features not to feature

bundles This already restricts the numb er of p ossible feature co o ccurrence

restrictions to in the case of features If wewant

to restrict the numb er of lters even more we can assume the socalled

If wewould allow for binary features we could furthermore rewrite b as A B

or a as AB  B A

Theoretical Background

Grounded Phonology Hypothesis of Archangeli and Pulleyblank ev

ery feature co o ccurrence restriction needs to b e grounded in the phonetics

This means for instance that we assume there can b e a constraint against

the combination of low and labial say b ecause it is relatively dicult to

push the tongue to its lowest p osition and op en the mouth and at the same

time round the lips but no similar constraint against high and labial

For more discussion of this issue I refer to section on page

Skeletal tier For the representation of vo calic and consonantal

length I adopt a more radical version of the socallled tworo ot theory of

Selkirk According to this theory long consonants ie geminates

are represented bytwo ro ot no des on the melo dy rather than bytwoskeletal

p oints or by moraic consonants My approach is slightly more radical than

Selkirks b ecause I assume the same typ e of representation also for

vowels

Of course nothing in principle will prevent us from adorning the struc

tures used in this thesis with moras or xslots Yet I have found no real

argument for suchamove I think that one of the main arguments in favour

of a tworo ot analysis is that it do es not need any additions to the theory

If we accept the notion of an autosegmental ro ot no de there is nothing in

the theory which can exclude a representation of the following typ e for long

vowels

 

ro ot ro ot

vocalic

We also predict this representation to contrast with the following

 

ro ot

vo calic

In fact given the fairly rich notion of a ro ot tier that I employIhave found

no particular reason to explicitly distinguish b etween a melo dic ro ot tier

and a skeletal tier consisting of either xslots or moras in anyofmy analyses

and therefore I havechosen to leave the latter tier out of my representations

Theoretical Background

Mo del of the grammar I assume the following minimal orga

nization of the phonological comp onent for Dutch

Underlying representations

Lexical Phonology

Postlexical Phonology

Phonetic forms

The only division is that into lexical and p ostlexical phonology This

division also seems to b e the only residue of derivational theory that I

need The typ e of argument that will b e made for this division b elowruns

as follows Some phenomena can b e derived on the surface but it can b e

shown that they cannot b e present at some deep er level For instance we

will see in chapter that Standard Dutch has b oth an underlying schwa and

an ep enthetic schwawhich are in complementary distribution and which

have dierent prop erties in particular ep enthetic schwa can form a closed

syllable whereas underlying schwa can not I can only explain this dierence

by assuming that ep enthetic schwa is the result of a p ostlexical pro cess

arising at a level where the condition forcing schwa to b e in an op en syllable

is no longer very strong

Another imp ortant assumption I make is that a pro cess of strayera

sure applies b oth after the lexical and after the p ostlexical comp onentIto

This means that in b oth cases nonparsed material is erased and

will not b e input to the p ostlexical comp onent or to the phonetics resp ec

tively This assumption seems to b e in accordance with current practice in

Optimality Theory as it is represented for instance in McCarthy and Prince

a

Optimality Theory The hyp othesis which I defend in this dis

sertation is a hyp othesis on representations there is a relation b etween

features on a segment and the prop erties of the proso dic structure whichis

headed by that segment Derivational issues therefore do not seem to play

a crucial role Y et in some cases the analyst needs to refer to them I have

solved this problem in the following way

Theoretical Background

Ihavechosen to use a framework of constraintbased phonology in which

the linguistic generalisations are expressed in terms of output constraints

More in particular I havechosen to adopt the framework of Optimality

Theory as it is presented in Prince and Smolensky a McCarthy and

Prince and related work In this section I will briey outline

the main prop erties of this theory as used in this thesis

In this framework linguistic theory consists of two functions called Gen

and Eval resp ectively

Gen takes at its input any phonological representation and generates

an innite set of output candidates forms whichhave b een parsed into

proso dic constituents to whichepenthetic material has b een added Fur

thermore under the standard version of OT adopted here Gen is b ounded

by the following restricti ons only

Containment No elementmay b e removed from the input form The

input is thus contained in every candidate form

Consistency of Exp onence No changes in the exp onence of a phonologically

sp ecied morpheme are p ermitted

Freedom of Analysis Any amount of structure may b e p osited

The principle of Containment is abandoned in some more recentversions of

OT McCarthy and Prince b in favour of some other mechanism which

makes the input representation available for output constraintevaluations

Yet I think this development is of no particular consequence to the theory

I presenthere

The function Gen thus generates every p ossible analysis for a given input

form A subsequent function Eval takes this set of output candidates as

its input and selects one of them as the actual optimal output It do es this

by comparing all the candidate outputs in parallel to a linearily ordered

ranked set of constraints The higherranked a constraint the more

forceful it is

Supp ose that a language has the constraints A B C and D ranked in

that order in OT notation ABCD Supp ose furthermore that for

some given input Gen generates the output candidates    and  An

arbitrary constraintevaluation is now pictured in the following way

Following current practice I assume this representation do es not contain any

proso dic structure cf McCarthy and Prince

Theoretical Background

Candidates A B C D





p





An asterisk in a b ox means that the candidate in the horizontal line violates

the constraintinthevertical column The optimal candidate the actual

p

output is marked bya sign This candidate is optimal b ecause all other

candidates violate some higher ranked constraint more often than this one



do es

Candidate  for instance is nonoptimal b ecause it violates the high

est constraint A while there are other forms which do not violate this

constraint Violation of A is therefore fatal for asindicatedby the ex

clamation mark b ehind the violation sign the asterisk Notice that this

candidate is nonoptimal even though in all it has the smallest number of

violations

Similarily candidate  is nonoptimal b ecause it violates constraintB

while there are other candiadtes notably  and  which do not violate B

and also do not have more violations of any more highly ranking constraint

in casu A than 

Now the only two candidates which are left in the candidate set are 

and  Both of these candidates violate constraintCyet  is chosen as

the optimal form b ecause it violates this constraint only once whereas 

violates the same constrainttwice Multiply violated constraints playa

crucial role in many OT analyses also in the present study An instance is

the constraint Onset This constraintsays syllables should have onsets

it can b e violated byanynumb er of syllables in a word If Onset were

b e constraint C in the tableau in  could b e a candidate with one

onsetless syllable whereas  would b e a candidate output with twosuch

oending syllables Candidate  is therefore selected

In my view using Optimality Theory in the analysis has the following

advantages In the rst place manyofmy analyses will b e based on the

family of pro jection constraints This notion of a family of constraints is

central to Optimality Theory

Also the b etter part of the analysis of language variation is based on the

interaction of the pro jection constraints with each other eg in chapter

and with other constraints eg in chapter The most imp ortant



For practical reasons I do not conform to all representational conventions of the OT

literature For instance I do not shade irrelevantboxes and I also do not use dotted lines

to indicated unordered constraints

Theoretical Background

among these are the socalled Faithfulness constraints which demand that

input and ouput structures are maximally similar Almost all Faithfulness

constraints used here refer to features I assume that there are two general

typ es of Faithfulness constraints

a ParseIf is present in the input it should b e present in the

output and parsed into higherorder structure eg Parse

coronal Parselabial

b If is not present in the input it should also not b e present

in the output eg coronal labial

We can also see b as a set of surface constraints prohibiting all o c

currences of a certain feature Parse would then give exactly the

required result and Parse causes a feature to never surface at all

high toneParsehigh tone in a language so that we can saythat

that do es not use tone for linguistic purp oses

Finally I wanttosay a few words ab out mybackground assumptions of

learnability I assume that something like the Subset Principle of Berwick

holds the initial grammar is the most restrictive p ossible and the

child only minimally changes its grammar on the basis of p ositiv e evidence

In Optimality theoretic terms at the initial stage of the acquisition pro

cess approximately the following statement holds all constraints are ranked

very high are assumed to b e unviolated except for the faithulness con

straints This gives approximately as an eect that the child generates only

universally unmarked forms and this in turn has the eect of the Subset

Principle whichmakes language learning easier to comprehend When a

child hears a form it cannot generate she assumes this is the consequence

of some lexical marking on that form and that the violation of the con

straint she observes is due to the higher ranking of one of the relevant

Faithfulness constraints Of course this is not a full worked out mo del of

OT language acquisition For instance it do es not tell us what is assumed

in the case of two p otentially conicting constraints that are b oth member

of the Faithfulness family or that b oth do not b elong to this family and it

also abstracts away from problems of noise in the input but for the cases

at hand it seems to work reasonably well

Initially the child assumes only the word tata or baba Chomsky

to b e wellformed b ecause it satises all relevant constraints for instance

OnsetNow the child hears a word starting with a vowel It knows that

it can only realize this form by ranking some faithfulness constraint for

instance Parsevowelabove Onset

I also abstract away from the partial reranking b etween lexical and p ostlexical phonology that seems to b e needed

Data

This is admittedly a heavily simplied theory of phonological acquisi

tion but it serves its limited purp oses in this thesis rather well

Data

In this section I will briey outline the sources of my data Most of my

data come from Standard Dutchofwhich I consider myself a nativespeaker

The Standard Dutch data come partly from written sources most often the

phonological literature and in a few cases dictionaries and partly from my

own observations I havealways checked all data against myown intuition

but it seems to me that hardly any piece of Standard Dutch data is con

troversial I have tried to indicate the few exceptions All words and parts

of words were considered to b e part of my data with two classes of excep

tions names and interjectives Names sometimes reect facts ab out older

stages of the language and interjectives seem to have a p eculiar phonology

of their own For instance Dutch has an interjective transcrib ed as ttt

indicating disagreement or moral ob jection with a rep eated clickwhich

is not otherwise part of the Dutch segmentinventory The phonology of

the class of names seems not so extremely deviant but still I haveassumed

that there are no forms with underlying schwabetween l and m if the

only counterexamples app ear to b e the names Wil lem wlm and Col lem

klm

ve also used data from Dutch dialects most prominently from Iha

Tilburg Dutch and Rotterdam Dutch For the data of these dialects I

have relied more heavily on written sources for these dialects and on my

own observations I do not havenativespeaker knowledge of either fortu

nately these proved to b e suciently adequate I have furthermore checked

all my data against at least one nativespeaker It turned out that this typ e

of data is p otentially sub ject to more controversy

For French and Norwegian I used approximately the same metho d as

for the nonstandard Dutch dialects ie I checked written sources against

native sp eaker intuitions For all other languages I have relied on written

sources only unless indicated otherwise

In my transciption I followed a system which is closely related to IPA

Furthermore I used x and x to denote input and output representa

tions of the grammar resp ectivel y this means that x always denotes a

phonological representation

Organisation of the thesis

Organisation of the thesis

I will now prop ose a division into chapters of the material to b e discussed

I will briey indicate the topic of every chapter Themainlineofthe

argument is as follows In chapter I give evidence for the constraint

N lax a pro jection constraintwhich is op erativeontheNlevel Connect

and refers to the feature lax and in chapter I discuss the interaction of

this constraint with phenomena of In chapter and I

discuss schwa the emptyvowelinDutch French and Norwegian I will

show that this vowel shows a defective b ehaviour at almost all pro jection

levels ie N N and the Fo ot In chapter I discuss the b ehaviour of high

vowels and argue that in addition to pro jection constraints we also need

weakness constraints which are the logical opp osites of pro jection Weak

ness constraints say thatifasegment dominates a feature F it wants to b e

in a dep endent p osition High vowels are a go o d example of this they are

the typical vowels that o ccur in the weak part of a that alter

nate with glides and that in some languages o ccur in unstressed p osition

Chapter nallygives an overview of all the pro jection constraints

that are intro duced in this thesis Ishow that the constraint set is not

some arbitrary unprincipled set of unrelated constraints but instead can

b e sub divided into a small numb er of classes of tightly related constraints

Chapter This chapter discusses the tense long vowels of

Dutch The analysis based on Connect N lax sketched ab oveisworked

out I argue that a lengthbased theory of the Dutchvowel system encoun

ters to o many problems and that for this reason it should b e replaced by

a theory based on the feature lax I will refute the arguments that have

b een adduced in favour of a lengthbased theory and present all arguments

Ihave found to b e in favour of a theory based on lax This chapter is

concluded by an app endix in which the history of the length vs tenseness

debate of the last years is reviewed

Chapter In this chapter I develop a complete analysis of length

in the Dutchvowel system including diphthongs and loan vowels and its

relation to syllable structure on the basis of the Headedness Hyp othesis

A prominent role is assigned to Brabant dialects of Dutch These dialects

have a complete set of long lax vowels in addition to the system found in

Standard Dutch I will take the Tilburg dialect as an instance The Tilburg

dialect has vowels schwa tense a e i o yulax

y and long lax q q q qq yq It can b e shown that eachofthese

vo wels is phonologically distinct from all the others

Organisation of the thesis

On the one hand Tilburg Dutchgives extra evidence for the tenselax

distinction in Standard Dutch b ecause this distinction is needed in this

dialect in order to describ e the dierence b etween long qontheone

hand and tense o on the other On the other hand Tilburg Dutch seems

to contradict the hyp othesis that Dutch dialects do not have underlying

length distinctions at all b ecause the dierence b etween andqcan

phonetically only b e describ ed as one of length

This chapter also includes sections on the nature of the feature that

is needed to describ e the dierence b etween the two main sets of vowels

tense ATR  op en p eripheral etc on diphthongs and on

extrasyllabicityandonrcolouring

Chapter Chapter is devoted to Dutch derived schwa I ar

gue that there are three typ es of schwainDutch The most wellstudied

of these is underlying and while this o ccurrence has manyinteresting prop

erties in this chapter I discuss twotyp es of derived schwa viz schwaas

a reduction vowel and schwaasanepenthetic vowel In some ways these

b ehave exactly as the underlying sch wainDutch but in others they do

not The reduction pro cess can b e seen as the loss of all vo calic qualities in

a given unstressed nonnal p osition The ep enthetic schwaisadefault

vowel which is inserted for syllabic and sometimes metrical reasons In this

waywe can hop e to establish a unied theory of all o ccurrences of Dutch

schwa I also review the prop erties of p ostlexical schwa the schwaof

clitics and inectional elements

Chapter In this chapter I argue that there is ample evidence

showing that schwa do es not license a complex onset at an underlying level

of representation Also schwa is notoriously invisible for rules of stress

In this chapter I will show that this follows if we assume that schwacan

only pro ject to a minimal syllable as the result of indep endently needed

constraints on syllable structure The defective pro jecting b ehaviour of

schwa itself follows from the fact that it is maximally undersp ecie d and

in particular from the fact that it lacks a place no de It is wellknown that

segments lacking a place no de eg ep enthetic segments do not participate

fully in the syllable structure

Chapter Chapter deals with classical questions with resp ect

to French and Norwegian schwa There is ample literature on b oth the

French and the Dutchschwa as well as the default vow els of some other

languages but hardly anywork has b een done on language variation in this area

Organisation of the thesis

NowtheFrenchschwa clearly is a puzzle for the theory sketched for

Dutchschwa Phonetically the twovowels are very similar and their b e

haviour also has a lot of prop erties in common like the stressless nes s of

schwa On the other hand the b ehaviour of Frenchschwa in the syllable

is dierent from the one of Dutchschwa For Dutchschwawe could sayit

licenses elements in its nucleus tafl but not in its onset ta but

for French it seems to b e exactly the opp osite cf tabl tabl On

the other hand Dutchschwa and Frenchschwa should have the same pho

netic representation b ecause they are indistinguish able at least in some

of their dialects and they share some imp ortant phonological prop erties

b oth cannot b e stressed and b oth cannot o ccur at the b eginning of a word

for instance

Norwegian is very interesting in the comparison b etween French and

Dutch on the surface this language seems to have a Dutchtyp e schwa

allowing a co da tigr but it also has a Frenchtyp e schwaallowing a

complex onset epl This can b e explained bytheinteraction of prin

ciples governing schwa with the principles of stress in Norwegian where

primary stressedwordinitialsyl labl es have to b e bimoraic as a result of

Prokosch Law

Chapter Although chapters through are basically con

cerned with vowels in the head p osition of a syllable we can also wonder

what happ ens if a vowel o ccurs in a dep endent p osition such as the syllable

onset In other words gliding is also of relevance for the theory on vo calic

features and syllable structure

Chapter presents as a case study the ji alternation in monomor

phemic forms diminutives and nd p erson clitics in the Rotterdam dialect

of Dutch

The forms raampje and koningkje are normally realized as rampi and

konki in the Rotterdam dialect with a long high vowel instead of Stan

dard Dutchj whereas voetje and dingetje are always realized as vutj

and dtj In other words the form jshows up after a coronal stop

and the form i shows up in other contexts Similar but sligh tly dierent

alternations can b e found in the case of the second p erson clitic ji and

in some monomorphemic forms

The analysis I want to prop ose for these facts is based on work done

in previous chapters here I intro duce the notion of a weakness constraint

shortly discussed ab ove

In this way I hop e to show the theories develop ed in these chapters can

beputtowork for these complicated facts A comparison of this vowelglide

alternation with the working of Sieverss Law in Gothic will furthermore

Organisation of the thesis

reveal additional arguments against vowel length in Dutch

Chapter In this chapter I review the formalism used in this

thesis and oer a conclusion It will app ear that the constraints which are

used in this thesis can b e organised into several separate families sets of

related constraints which are derived from a common ancestor by the setting

of several parameters Iwork out these families in some formal detail

In particular I concentrate on the pro jection constraints and weakness

constraints mentioned earlier in this intro duction

It app ears that the pro jection constraints always refer either to the

whole set of vo calic features or to the particular features low and lax

and in some cases to tone andor the feature voice whereas the weakness

constraints all refer to the feature high

App endices OT analyses can get rather complicated esp ecially

when we treat a somewhat larger fragment of a grammar as is the case in

this thesis In App endix A I giveanoverview of all constraints used in the

analyses in this dissertation in app endix B I subsequently list all ranking

arguments for Dutchwhich are referred to in this thesis and in app endix C

Ihave graphically represented the top ology of the grammar of Dutchwhich

is studied in this thesis My exp erience is that it is extremely dicult to

rememb er all individual rankings This is the reason whyIhave added the

latter two app endices

Vowel Quality and Rhyme Structure in

Dutch

Intro duction

In many languages there is a phonetic correlation b etween vowel length

and tenseness If this correlation is complete that is to say if all long

vowels in a language are tense and all short vowels are lax it is attractive

to assume that either length or tenseness is absent from the phonological

representations and derived from the other prop ertyby a rule of phonetic

interpretation Once this p oint is established we still have to face the

question of which prop erty is phonological in a given language The aim of

this chapter is to reevaluate some of the evidence that has b een presented

to settle this latter p oint

Awellknown instance of the problem mentioned here arises in the study

of the in particular German and Dutch In those lan

guages the correlation just noted can b e observed with some minor prob

lems to b e discussed b elow Phonologically the prop erty distinguishing the

two sets of vowels seems to b e most relevant for syllable structure long

tense vowels can b e followed by at most one noncoronal consonantword

nally whereas short lax vowels can b e followed bytwo Most analysts

have taken this to b e an argument for phonological length from which tense

ness can b e deriv ed in the phonetic comp onent I will argue that this view

is mistaken and that there are strong arguments for considering tenseness

rather than length relevant for the phonological comp onent The sylla

ble structure facts can b e derived from a constraint relating vowel quality

to the structure of the syllable This typ e of relation gets indep endent

supp ort from languages in which the relevant phonological feature shows

up prop erties of vowel harmony under standard autosegmentalist assump

tions length is not something that can spread Finally I try to address

the question whether also other elements of the proso dic hierarchycanbe

inuenced by phonological features like tenseness and whether there are

phonological features other than tense which can aect proso dic structure

The Dutchvowel system

I pro ceed along the following lines in my argumentation In the next

section I giveanoverview of the Dutchvowel system The main arguments

for and against tenseness theory will b e discussed in section In section

I will reinterpret the main argument for length in suchawaythatit

can b e seen as an argumentinfavour of tenseness In section I discuss

the role of lax in harmony systems In section I then examine whether

there are any arguments in favour of length left and I try to refute these

The prenal section contains a conclusion I have also added an app endix

section to this chapter in which I briey summarize and review some

of the th century literature on the Dutchvowel system

The Dutchvowel system

The tenseness versus length debate most probably is the oldest in Dutch

phonology The issue has received some discussion by almost all scholars

working on Dutch such as De Gro ot Van Ginneken Trub et

zkoy Van Wijk Cohen et al Van Haeringen

Heeroma Moulton De Rijk Brink Zonneveld

Bo oij a Trommelen Van der Hulst

b Kager Lahiri and Koreman Smith et al and

Hermans a an overview of the debate will b e provided in an

app endix to this chapter section Similar partly parallel discussions

have also b een conducted on the German vowel set see Ramers

Wiese Vennemann Hall Kager and references

cited there In the following table I ha ve listed all Dutchvowels

i iq yq uq

ii i y u

iii y

iv

v e o

vi i y u

vii

viii q q q

ix

x a

Some of the vowels I will set apart in this chapter to b e treated in one

of the chapters that follow The vowelsinrows i and viii are usually

Throughout this study I assume that the vowel marked y is phonologically

rounded although phoneticallytherounding of this vowel is very slight See

Fudge and Gilbers for discussion

The Dutchvowel system

considered to b e loan segments b ecause they o ccur almost exclusively in

loan words from French and English see section in the next chapter

the schwa in iv is in manyways a very exceptional vowel in the Dutch

system and will b e discussed in the chapters and and the sounds in vi



are diphthongs not simple segments section

The core system of Dutchvowelsisthus formed by the segments in rows



ii iii v vii ix and x

Of these the segments in rows ii v and x are usually referred to as

tense or long and the segments in rows iii and vii as lax or short

I will follow Moulton and use the terms Avowel and Bvowel

to refer to tense long vowels or lax short vowels resp ectivel ywherever a

theoryneutral term is needed I also assume that the vowels can b e paired

in the following way a o e AB The high

vowels i y u do not have a counterpart in the Bset The lowvowel

do es not have a counterpart in the Aset The reasons why I mak ethese

pairings will hop efully get clear from the discussion that follows The most

controversial assumption probably is that isa midvowel and not high

An empirical reason for making this assumption is that this vowel always

alternates with e and never with i For instance in plural formation we

nd pairs suchasschipschepen ships sxpsxepn sxipn along with

dagdagen days dxdan Spa Noske Also mid vowels



Also the vowels in v are slightly diphthongized phonetically they movetowards

the centre of the vowel triangle but most scholars agree that this is not a phonological

issue See Van de Velde for a recent so ciolinguistic study on the phonetic dier

ences b etween Standard Dutch as sp oken in Flanders and as sp oken in the Netherlands

in this resp ect



The rules of Dutch with resp ect to vowels are somewhat complicated

In closed syllables which notion is supp osed to include wordnal syllables ending in

one consonant tense long vowels are written as doubled vowels zeep soap zep

boos angry b os raam window ram The high vowels i y u are written ie dief

thief dif uu bluut philtrum blyt and oe boef scoundrel buf resp ectively

is written eu deuk dent dk

In op en syllables most tense vowels except i u and are written with a single

letter ramen windows ram ga go a bozer angrier b ozr zie see zi

doe do du keu cue k e is written with a single letter in wordinternal op en

syllables but with a double letter wordnally reden reason red zee sea ze

Single orthographic e at the end of a word is always read as schwa made maggot

mad

Lax vowels are sp elled with single letters zit sit zt zet put zt mug mosquito

myx koppel b elt kpl dapper brave dpr As the last two examples indicate

consonants after wordinternal lax vowels are doubled when they are not in a cluster

This do es not mean that consonants are long but that the precding vowel is lax or

short

u resp ectivelySchwais Diphthongs are sp elled ei or i ui y au or ou

normally sp elled e These are not all rules of Dutchvowel sp elling but they should suce to understand the examples in this thesis

Tenseness versus Length The Case of Dutch

get lax and lengthen b efore an r in the same fo ot koor choir kqr

keur choice kqr keer turn kqr Gussenhoven Again wesee

e and b ehaving as a pair High vowels b efore r only lengthen so that

i alternates with iq rather than with q See Levelt and Bo oij

a for more arguments that islowandmid

Tenseness versus Length The Case of Dutch

In this section I will discuss the most imp ortant argumentinfavour of

length as the characteristic distinguishing Avowels from Bvowels and on

the other hand the arguments for viewing tenseness as that prop ertyThis

argumentation will lead to a paradox on the one hand syllable structure

seems to indicate that Avowels are long but on the other hand all other

tests that we can develop seem to indicate that they are not

The argument for length After Zonneveld and with the

exception of Van der Hulst b all authors on Dutch mentioned ab ove

have assumed that length is a phonological prop erty present at least at the

stage of the deriv ation where syllable structure is assigned The strongest

and as far as I can see the only reason for this is that Avowels pattern

with diphthongs as opp osed to Bvowels with resp ect to syllable structure

as is shown in the following paradigm

ra ra raam ram raap rap raamp ramp

yard window turnip

rah r ram rm rap rp ramp rmp

ram quickly disaster

rui ry ruim rym kruip kryp kruimp krymp

moulting time wide creep

The paradigm gap in can b e lled by just a handful of words For un

known reasons often the vowel is i and the form is an irregular past tense

forms of a verb hielp help ed hilp stierf died stirf twaalf twelve

twalf and a few others Usually one abstracts awayfromthesecounterex

amples but I will explain the high vowel cases in section

twaalf thus is the really problematic case Numerals form an The form



exceptional stratum in the of many languages For instance in

French wordnal consonants are deleted b efore a consonantinitial word

if and only if they are also deleted phrasenally six id sis and dix



The same is true for prop er names of course which also sometimes are counterex

amples Tjeenk tjek Rienk rik etc

Tenseness versus Length The Case of Dutch

ten dis are counterexamples to this generalisation however Their nal

s deletes b efore a consonant but not phrasenally

Furthermore a b efore a sonorant obstruent cluster might b e sub ject

to a sp ecial tensing rule We will see in section that this pro cess is

pro ductive in at least one Dutch dialect It might b e that this form is a

last remnant in Standard Dutch of the same pro cess

At the end of the word Avowels and diphthongs can b e followed by

one noncoronal consonantorby no consonant at all Bvowels on the other

hand can b e followed by one or two noncoronal consonants In the middle

of the word Bvowels are normally only followed by one tautosyllabic con

sonant Avowels and diphthongs in those p ositions are not followed byany

tautosyllabic consonant at all The picture is somewhat blurred b ecause all

of the wellformed structures in can b e followed by one or more voiceless

coronal

raad rat raamt ramt raapt rapt raampt

advice he plans carries

rat rt rand rnt hapt hpt stampt stmpt

rat edge he bites he stamps

ruit ryt ruimt rymt kruipt krypt kruimpt

window he empties he creeps

Forms like rat rand or raad can b e analysed as falling under the template

in This is not true however for forms like stampt or staand Often the

more complex forms are morphologically derived but some phonologically

complex monomorphemic forms suchasherfst autumn hrfst and ernst

earnest rnst do also o ccur Wemay surmise that the coronal obstruents

have some sp ecial status with resp ect to the proso dic word I will refer to

these coronals as extraprosodicFurthermore I assume that coronal ob

struents are only extraproso dic when they havetobethus the t in raad

or rand is not extraproso dic since it can b e incorp orated into the proso dic

word in a normal way Coronal obstruents can o ccur in extraproso dic p o

sitions they do not have to o ccur in those p ositions

Now diphthongs are usually assumed to b e bip ositional in Dutch The

facts in can therefore b e explained in an elegantwayifweassumeA

vowels are phonologically long and if in addition we make the following



assumptions

A Lengthbased Theory of the Dutch Syllable



The analysis presented here comes closest to that presented in Zonneveld

This is only one p ossible theory based on length Actually all assumptions presented

here havebeencontested by some length theorist I do not think however that this

aects the strength of my argumentinanyway

Tenseness versus Length The Case of Dutch

a Avowels are bip ositional Bvowels are monop ositional

b Coronal obstruents can b e extraproso dic ie if necessary they

can get licensed outside of the phonological word

c Syllable rhymes contain exactly two p ositions

d At the end of the phonological word there can b e at most one

noncoronal extrasyllabic consonant

It can b e observed that sup erheavy C VVCorC VCC syllables in most

 

languages also in Dutch are limited to wordnal p ositions while word

internally we nd at most heavy syllables This has led some researchers to

assume this is one instance of a case where the system is more p ermissive

regarding incomplete structure at wordedge than elsewhere In particular

it is assumed that wordnal syllables are only heavy not sup erheavyThe

wordnal consonant is extrasyllabic in one way or another This has to

b e seen under anyb o dys theory as a dierenttyp e of sp ecial p osition than

the one of coronal obstruents discussed earlier after a wordnal syllable

we can nd at most one non but a much larger number

of coronals The dierenttyp es of proso dic p ositions will b e discussed in

section of the next chapter

I will refer to the p ositions at the end of words in whichalltyp es of

consonants can o ccur as extrasyl labic whereas I refer to the p ositions for

coronals only as extraprosodic The p in kruipt is extrasyllabic and the t

ys p eriph is extraproso dic By denition extraproso dic p ositions are alwa

eral to extrasyllabic p ositions Extraproso dic p ositions are outside of the

proso dic word whereas extrasyllabic p ositions are inside of those

Dutch has a class of words like aalmoes alms almus which seem

exceptions to this rule On the basis of their stressb ehaviour wehave

to conclude however that these words are proso dic comp ounds Zonneveld

and Trommelen which consist of two separate proso dic words aal

and moes at some level of the representation If aalmoes were a normal

word wewould exp ect a stresspattern aalmoes Ifaalmoes proso dically is

aal moes we exp ect aalmoes whichiswhatwe actually nd

The idea that the nal consonant of a sup erheavy syllable is outside the

syllable structure can b e worked out in at least two dierentways Wecan

assume the consonant remains completely outside syllable structure and

gets licensed in a nonproso dic way Alternativelywe can assume it forms

the onset of a degenerate syllable with a phonetically emptyvowel as its

head The last metho d rst prop osed in generative phonology by Aoun

McCarthy and Giegerich is esp ecially known from

socalled Gov ernment Phonology Kaye Lowenstamm and Vergnaud

Charette Recently Zonneveld has prop osed a similar

Tenseness versus Length The Case of Dutch

analysis for Dutch outside of the framework of Government Phonology cf

also Langeweg An advantage of this analysis is that it can explain

why sup erheavy syllables b ehaveinmany resp ects as bisyllabic units In

this theory they are bisyllabic

I will p ostp one the formal choice b etween these two analyses to section

Empirically they servemy purp oses equally well Both haveasa

consequence under tenseness theory that Dutch has only twoorthree

typ es of syllable CV CV Candmayb e CV V for diphthongs The

A B i j

nal consonantofziek ill zien see zink sink and zijn b e zin is

outside of syllable structure or forms a syllable with an empty V The nal

consonantin zin or tik on the other hand is tautosyllabic with the vowel

as we will see b elow

According to this lengthbased theory then the form r is ruled out

b ecause it only contains a monomoraic vowel thereby violating the con

straint in c the forms ra ry with a bimoraic vowel and diphthong

resp ectivel y rm with a monomoraic vowel and a moraic consonant

all are p erfectly wellformed syllables and rap kryp and rmp are all

analysed as a bimoraic syllable followed by a degenerate syllable Forms

suchas ramp rymp and rlmp are ruled out b ecause they cannot

b e generated in anyway according to the templates in

The length theory has a quite elegant analysis of the syllable structure

facts The reason for this is that length can b e analysed in terms of a

numb er of syllabic p ositions The distinction b etween and a can b e

describ ed in the following way where the xs stand for skeletal p ositions or

ro ot no des and the F for the relevant feature tree

a

x x x

F F

On the other hand it is not easy to see how a phonological feature such

as tense can b e related to the numb er of p ositions in the syllable or in the

word

The arguments against length Iwillnowshow that despite

its usefulness in the theory of syllable structure length theory suers from

several empirical problems whichwould b e very easy to circumventinathe

ory which uses a feature I will also show that this feature should probably



b e lax rather than tense in the case of Dutch



The rst three arguments have also b een discussed in Van der Hulst b

Tenseness versus Length The Case of Dutch

The Dutch stress system treats Avowels as light

Most theorists agree that Dutch hasaquantitysensitive stresssystem

in which closed syllables and syllables ending in a diphthong countas



heavy but syllables which end in an Avowel count as light Dutch

stress is tro chaic and seems to b e b ound by a threesyllable window

at the right edge of the word One of the facts which p oint at the

quantitysensitivity of Dutch stress is that primary stress can b e on

the antep enultimate syllable only if the p enult has an Avowel not

when it contains a closed syllable Evers and Huybregts Trom

melen and Zonneveld

tremolo id tremolo agenda id axnda axnda

From a typ ological p oint of view this situation is very marked if weas

sume a length theory There do es not seem to b e any nonGermanic

system for which the generalisation holds true that CVV syllables are

light and CVC syllables heavy A generally accepted universal says

that if a language distinguishes b etween heavy and light syllables

CVV syllables always countasheavy The universal holds even par

tially for Dutch but only in the realm of diphthongs

This problem received a lot of attention in the prop osals of those de

fending length theory A large numb er of prop osals can b e collected

from the literature Dutchvowels would havetwo ro ot no des which

are attached to the same proso dic p osition and therefore they would

count as light Lahiri and Koreman they would rather haveone

ro ot no de and two proso dic p ositions and Dutchwould exceptionally

calculate syllable weight at the level of ro ot no des Kager or

they would b e analysed as a short vowel followed byaschwalike

element at the level of stress assignment where schwawould b e invis

ible for stress Smith et al Hermans a It seems therefore

p ossible to technically solve the problem in a length theory

This do es not diminish the imp ortance of the fact that all of these

theories need some very sp ecic stipulations while the whole problem

simply do es not exist for tenseness theory b ecause syllables ending

in an Avowel in the latter theory get a CV structure universally the

characteristic structure of a light syllable



See however Bo oij a for a recent analysis of Dutch stress which do es not dep end

on the notion quantitysensitivity



Duanmu has argued that also Chinese imp oses a bimoraic minimum on its

syllables Yet even if this were true it could b e p ointed out that the Chinese proso dic

system is dierent in the sense that there do es not seem to b e a clearcut distinction

between syllable and fo ot Bimoraicity requirements on feet are of course quite common

Tenseness versus Length The Case of Dutch

Dutchdoesnothave CVsyllables

Another wellknown universal has it that all languages have core syl

lables consisting of exactly one consonantandonevowel Trub etzkoy

Jakobson As wehave seen ab ove DutchBvowels are

always in a checked p osition whichistosay that they always have

to b e followed by a consonant Only Avowels can app ear at the end

of a syllable but if these vowels are long Dutch only has CVV CVC

and mayb e CVVCandCVCC syllables

Again tenseness theory do es not have this problem As wewillsee

b elow there is evidence that all Avowels head a C V syllable Under

tenseness theoryDutch therefore do es have core syllables

There are more Avowels than Bvowels

Trub etzkoy gives a third markedness criterion that causes

problems for length theory the set of AvowelsinDutchandin

owels Germanic languages in general is bigger than the set of Bv

Such a division normally is taken to suggest that the bigger set in

this case the set of Avowels is unmarked Length theory cannot

make this prediction If Avowels are identical to Bvowels except

that they are asso ciated to two rather than to one proso dic p osition

we exp ect that the Avowels are more marked than Bvowels Length

theory also has a problem explaining the exact p osition of the gap

in the vowel triangle It is a strange coincidence that exactly the

high vowels lack a short counterpart while all long high vowels are

phonetically short

Tenseness theory do es not have this problem provided we assume

that lax if we assume this feature is to b e interpreted as ATR

or RTR is the marked value in Dutch The Bvowels can nowbe

seen as formally marked Furthermore the restriction on high vowels

is a natural one The lter against lax high can get a phonetic mo

tivation at least if we assume laxness involves tongue ro ot retraction

Archangeli and Pulleyblank Asymmetries in the realm of high

and lowvowels are not uncommon in ATR vowel systems

The problems for length theory b ecome even harder if we consider

sc hwa foramoment Dutchschwacounts as the asymmetric vowel

of Dutch showing a p eculiar b ehaviour with resp ect to onsets stress

and other phenomena cf chapters and

The most plausible analysis for this segmentisthatitisheavily under

sp ecied b oth phonologically and phonetically Keating Brow

man and Goldstein Ko opmansVan Beinum Further

more phonetically this vowel is extremely short Yet according to

Tenseness versus Length The Case of Dutch

the syllable structure test it should b e classied as phonologically

long

moede tired mud moeder mother mudr adem breath

adm moederm drm

Some length theorists Trommelen Bo oij a have indeed

prop osed that schwa should b e classied as phonologically long in

spite of the fact that there is no indep endent evidence for suchan

assumption Again the additional problem arises that there is no

short counterpart to schwa Furthermore if we adopt the p osition

that Dutch stress is quantity sensitive we get the undesirable result

that Dutchisaquantity senstive system in which some CVV syllables

count as sup erlight

A theory based on the feature lax do es not encounter anysuch prob

lems If schwaisheavily undersp ecie d it also lacks the feature lax

Therefore we simply exp ect it to pattern with the Avowels rather

than with the Bvowels

Morpheme Structure Constraints distinguish b etween tense

and lax vowels

Some constraints on morpheme structure referring to the dierence

betw een Avowels and Bvowels cannot b e expressed naturally in

terms of length For instance Dutch has a constraint against tau

tomorphemic ji This constraint can b e seen as an instance of

the OCP It is absolute for the Avowel i but there are a few p er



fectly normal words with j plus short Bvowel jiddisch jicht

Similarly there is a constraint banning w plus long y or but

there are p erfectly acceptable words with wywurm wurg wulp

Both of these constraints seem to b e related to the OCP The ji

lter esp eciall y can b e analysed as a constraint forbidding twoidenti

cal segments in a rowifwe assume that j is an i in a consonantal

In chapter I will argue that the constraint should b e Contour a constraint some

what similar but not equal to the OCP

The argument based on Morpheme Structure Constraints is due to Van Wijk

I will argue b elow that is b est seen as the lax counterpart of e not i there are a few

words starting with je all Biblical names and words derived from biblical names Jezus

jesuet Jesuit Jeremias jeremieren lament This somewhat weakens the argument

based on ji but do es not aect the parallel argument based on wyw

Again the mid vowels are slightly more p ermissive than the low ones witness

the new acronym WEUWestEuropese Unie w which is the only counterexample I found

Tenseness versus Length The Case of Dutch

p osition But it is unclear why all of a sudden jisallowed if the

feature tree of is attached to only one skeletal p osition

The working of this lter is also observable in the pro cess of homor

ganic glide insertion MiekeTrommelen pc a j is inserted in hiaat

hiatus hijat as well as in hobbyist amateur hbijst but not

in shiiet shiite siit sijit

Secret languages treat Avowelsasaunit

For the last few years secret languages and language gameswhichI

will uniformly call secret languageshave received more attention

as p ossible sources of evidence for phonological structure than was

usual in earlier generative literature In an overview of several such

systems McCarthy a p oints out that English secret languages

suchasGoat Latin and Alfalfa without exception treat long vowels as

unities In Goat Latin each syllable gets an inx Vbw between the

onset and the rhyme where V isacopy of the head of the original

syllable An example of an English sentence and its translation in

Goat Latin McCarthys a example d is given immediately

b elow

a This is Goat Latin

b bws bbwzgowbwowt lbw tbwn

According to McCarthy a it is typically the case in English

secret languages that the long vowel is copied as a whole and never

just the rst half of it in whichcasewewould get gobwowt for

goat At the same time Goat Latin excludes copying of a rhyme

constituentaswe can see in the translation of latin This translation

is lbw tbwn and not lbw tnbwn If Dutchwould

have a secret language like Goat Latin this would again provide us

with a problem for length theory whichwould not necessarily b e a

problem for tenseness theory

Unfortunately the study of Dutch secret languages is heavily under

develop ed Such languages probably exist but I have not b een able to

nd any phonological or nonphonological study dealing with them I

ha ve therefore informally conducted a miniature exp erimentinwhich

I explained Goat Latin to some ie nativespeakers of Dutch

all nonlinguists by telling them they had to nd the simple rule

of a secret language of whichIgave and if necessary rep eated an

examplesentence that only included Bvowels and schwa

Tenseness versus Length The Case of Dutch

After this I gave them a few other Dutchsentences to translate

into the secret language in order to see whether they actually under

sto o d the basic rules These sentences also included Bvowels and

schwa only Finally I gave them a set of sentences whichalsoin

cluded Avowels and diphthongs These sentences are given in

b elow where I rst give the Dutchsentence I read to the informants

subsequently the translation that most of the informants gave and

thirdly variants that were given by a minority it happ ened that for

no form more than dierent translations were given eachtimewith

an indication of the numb er of informants that pro duced the variant

o ccasional apparent mistakes likebubuwuk have b een left out

Dit is erg simp el

Dbwt bbws bbwrx sbwmpbwl

This is very easy

a Ik lees dat boek erg

bbwk lebwes dbwt bubwuk bbwrx

kbwk

graag als het

rabwax bbwls bwt

rax bwax hbwt

regent buiten

rebwebwnt bybwytbwn

n rebwenbwnt bybwytbw

I like to read that b o ok when it rains outside

b Het is ook erg

hbwt bbws bobwok bbwrx

bbwt rbwrx

mo oi

mobwoj

mo jbwo j

Because it is very b eautiful

c Wij allen houden

wibwi bbwl bwn hubwudbwn

bbwlnbwn hobwudbn

van lezen

vbwn lebwezbwn

vnbwn lebweznbwn

We all love

Tenseness versus Length The Case of Dutch

In so far as there is variation at all it do es not directly b ear on the

issue under discussion Some informants had problem with schwa

headed syllables presumably b ecause in secret languages the original

syllables are reanalyzed as proso dic words and proso dic words cannot

only haveschwa syllables Also o ccasionally clusters of Bvowels plus

a following consonant where copied This has to b e seen as a mistake

against the basic rule given that this copying was not attested in the

example sentence not even where this was p ossible Almost all of

these mistakes were made by the same informant The same mistake

also o ccurred once with an Avowel raxbwax

More imp ortantlyaword like lees was translated by all of myinfor

mants as lebwes and never as lbwebws or lbwebws

Crosslinguistical ly underlying long vowels tend to get split in this

typeofword game Vago A complication arises however b e

cause all informants but one treated diphthongs as unities as well wij



we wi was never translated as wbwi Diphthongs however

b ehave like bisegmental unities in other resp ects most signicantly

they make syllables heavy I will try to solve this paradox in the next

chapter

A less sup ercial empirical research could clearly shed more lighton

this p oint but for now the conclusion seems justied that native

sp eakers of Dutch tend to treat Avowels more as a unity than length

theory predicts

There are dialects with a real length contrast Some dialects of

Dutchhave long lax vowels in addition to the short lax and tense ones

of Standard Dutch For instance Tilburg Dutchhasthefollowing

underlying vowel set Boutkan and chapter fi yueo

qgTenseness and length thus are q q q qq

indep endent parameters in this dialect Some dialects also havelong



variants of the high tense vowels Hermans Geert Bo oij pc

Yet in all of the dialects I examined the short tense vowels b ehave

exactly likeinDutch This means that for these dialects wehaveto

invoke a sp ecial mechanism to account for the long b ehaviour of the

tense segments If wehave todothisanywayitisnotclearwhy

we could not use the same mechanism for the analysis of Standard



The one informant who did split the diphthongs seems to have b een inuenced by

sp elling given the phonetic outcome of her variant of Goat Latin Another informant

even copied the long diphthong o j in mooi



As a matter of fact if wetakeinto consideration the loan of rows i and

viii even Standard Dutch has suchlongvowels in its inventory

A theory based on the feature lax



Dutch I will return to Tilburg Dutch in the next chapter

A theory based on the feature lax

Wearethus faced with a paradox On the one hand the syllable structure

facts seem to p oint in the direction of a lengthbased theory On the other

hand there are many other facts which seem to p oint to a theory based on

the feature lax rather than on length Some of the problems noted ab ove

have also b een observed by prop onents of a lengthbased analysis they have

tried to solvetheminvarious ways I refer to Kager Trommelen and

Zonneveld Smith et al for a few fairly recent prop osals Yet

none of these prop osals seem to b e able to deal with all the problems noted

here For this reason I prop ose the following theory based on the feature

lax

A Laxbased Theory of the Dutch Syllable

a Bvowels have a sp ecication for lax Avowels are unsp ecied

for this feature Only diphthongs and truly long vowels are

bip ositional

b Coronal obstruents can b e extraproso dic ie if necessary they

can get licensed outside of the phonological word

c Connect N lax

d At the end of the phonological word there can b e at most one

noncoronal extrasyllabic consonant

The constraint in c is an instance of what I call a projection constraint

N pro jection has a certain feature in this case lax the If the head of an

syllable itself has a certain structure it branches Inversely the head of

a branching rhyme should always carry the feature lax The denition of

ConnectN laxis as follows

def



Connect N lax ProjectNlax Projectlax N N

dominates lax i N branches



English to o has twosetsofvowels which can b e distinguished in terms of b oth

length and tenseness English diers from Dutch and German in that we probably

need both features in its phonology Halle and Mohanan Lab ov but cf

Giegerich Harris for arguments that this is not necessarily true for all

English dialects Still most arguments against length adduced in this chapter for Dutch

cannot b e transferred to English in English long vowels do count for stress for instance

and wealsohave syllables ending in short vowels

A theory based on the feature lax

The formulation in is stated in terms of the Xbar representation used

in this thesis but it seems p ossible to phrase a version of it in other

frameworks of syllabic representation For instance wecouldalsosay

that a syl lable is bimoraic i the head of dominates a featurelax

Connect lax in a moraic theory of syllable structure enriched



with the notion of a head The precise nature of the feature lax will b e

discussed in section in the next chapter

The analysis in is minimally dierent from the length theory pre

sented under It can explain the facts in as follows r is ruled

out b ecause its head vowel contains a sp ecication lax but the rhyme

do es not branch this causes a violation of ConnectN lax ra is an



op en syllable without a lax sp ecication on the head rm and ry each

have branching rhymes with a lax head and rap rmp and kryp are

analysed as sequences of a syllable followed by a degenerate syllable just as

under length theory Notice that tenseness theory also explains without

extra costs why the head of a diphthong is lax The phonetic length of the

nonhigh tense vowels can b e derived by p ostulating a phonetic rule which

lengthens nonhigh vowels in op en syllables

This analysis solves the problems mentioned for length theoryDutch

can b e analysed as having a large numb er of CV syllables viz all the syl

lables of which the head is not lax Furthermore these syllables all count

as light as exp ected Grounded constraints can explain the asymmetries

in the vowel system it is quite common in languages of the world that lax



and high or tense lax and low do not go together That the

MSC works against w and not against w can b e accounted for b ecause

and are dierent segments Finally it is not surprising that secret

languages treat Avowels as a unit b ecause that is what they are at every

level of the analysis

Another interesting prop erty of this theory is that it explains why truly

long vowels ie the loan phonemes and diphthongs as opp osed to the

Avowels b ehave as heavy in the stress system Trommelen and Zonneveld

In the theory presented here they are bisegmental hence heavy just

like closed syllables



The reader maywonder whether a should not b e analysed as low lax The

a pair b ehaves in an exceptional way in that there are exceptions to the general

pattern in whichvery often involve either the high vowels or a In some dialects

for instance in Tilburg Dutch the a also seems to serve as a p erfectly normal vowel in

closed syllables Similar anomalies with the a pair have b een observed for German

Kager and French Dell This pair should therefore b e treated as exceptional

in some way

lax and syllable structure in vowel harmony

lax and syllable structure in vowel harmony

N lax The The crucial assumption in the laxbased theory is Connect

reader mightwonder whether this do es not count as a languagesp ecic

assumption In this section I want to argue however that it is a universal

constraint b elonging also to the grammars of other languages

One dierence b etween a feature like lax or ATR and a prop erty

like length is that under standard autosegmentalist assumptions the for

mer can spread whereas the latter cannot Therefore if we nd a language

in which there is vowel harmony on the relevantproperty while there is

also an interaction b etween the prop erty and the op en vs closed nature of

the syllable wehave an additional argument for a laxbased theory

Now it is true that there are many languages with ATR harmonyin

which there do es not seem to b e a relation with syllable structure at

all I b elieve however that there is sucient evidence that the con

straint Connect N lax is a principle of Universal Grammar whichis

also op erative in other languages some of which are not genetically or ge

ographically related to Dutch Some examples are Tagalog Carrier

EasternJavanese Archangeli and Pulleyblank Andalusian Span

ish Ho op er Lusitian Portuguese Redenb erger and Midi and

Queb ec French Durand Dumas Dechaine

In all these languages we can distinguish b etween Avowels and Bvowels

in a way that is phonetically rather similar to Dutch In eachofthese

languages wehave to assume the dierence is to b e describ ed in terms

of a feature b ecause there is vowel harmony with resp ect to exactly this

prop erty All vowels in a certain domain usually a proso dic word either

b elong to the Aset or to the Bset In an autosegmental mo del this can only

b e explained if wehave a feature for the standard autosegmental analysis

of harmonyinvolves spreading It is much more dicult if not imp ossible

to see howvowel length ie b eing linked to two p ositions can harmonize

The harmonic b ehaviour of lengthtenseness in these languages under

autosegmental assumptions unambiguously shows wehaveafeature in these

languages Yet in each of these languages there is also a relation b etween

vowel quality and syllable structure whic h is b est describ ed as ConnectN

lax

Interestingly in each of these languages there also is a correlation b e

tween tense and the ap erture features high and low which I argue b elow

also holds for the Dutchvowel system In particular it seems that the

combinations high ATR or highlax and low ATR are often

disallowed or at least avoided in many languages

EasternJavanese Schlindwein Archangeli and Pulleyblank is p erhaps the clearest example of this state of aairs In this language

lax and syllable structure in vowel harmony

all vowels in the word are lax ATR according to the authors just

mentioned if the nal syllable is closed otherwise all vowels in the word

are tense

Actually the situation in Javanese is somewhat more complex b ecause

the harmony takes place only b etween vowels that agree in the features

high and low What we could assume is that there are two dierent

harmony pro cesses one aecting the nonhigh vowels and one aecting the

high vowels or alternatively that there is one pro cess which can only apply

between vowels that agree on the feature high cf the discussion of

Andalusian Spanish b elow

a Distribution of lax high vowels

buri back mVrt student

turu sleep plpr edge

bali return adVs bathe

b Distribution of lax mid vowels

ombe drink bbt weight

bodo stupid lrn stop

kere b eggar glb get

rame noisy al dicult

Archangeli and Pulleyblank explain this distribution as follows

tense is underlyingly absent in Eastern Javanese morphemes If the last

vowel o ccurs in a closed syllable it pro jects a tense feature according

to a rule much like Connect N lax which subsequently spreads through

the word Otherwise all vowels get the value tense by default Inter

estinglythelowvowel a do es not participate in the opp osition It o ccurs

b oth in words with lax vowelsasinwords with tense vowels In the next

section I will argue that a plays an ambiguous role in the Dutchvowel

system as well

We might assume that there is a harmony constraint op erativeinthese

languages which cannot b e seen at work in a language likeDutch Since

vowel harmony is not a topic of this dissertation I will provisionally for

mulate this constraintasfollows

HarmonyAvowel V should have the same sp ecication for RTR

as the vowel W that o ccurs on the right of V if V and W are in

the same proso dic word

N lax in Javanese all vowels except If Harmony dominates Connect

the wordnal one prefer to b e harmonic over having the required value

lax and syllable structure in vowel harmony

to satisfy ConnectN lax Harmony is not applicable to the word

nal vowel however so that ConnectN lax is able to select the optimal

candidate here This will give the eect just describ ed

A somewhat more complicated example of the same phenomenon is

seen in Tagalog Carrier asserts that a lmost without exception

closed syl lables do not contain long vowels in native stemsHowever some

suxes such as adjectivizing an and in includeavowel whichislongand

makes all vowels in the stem long Finally all short vowels get laxed at

the end of the derivation while all long vowels stay tense The result is a



pattern very muchlike the one in EasternJavanese

Tagalog adjectivizing suxes

N A

hiqmaqtay fainting hiqmaqtaqyiqn given to fainting

duqgob blood duqguqbaqn covered with blo o d

puqtk mud puqtiq kaqn

In mo dern autosegmental theory it would b e more plausible to say the

feature tense is underlying and length is phonetically derived b ecause

of the spreading pro cess But still there would b e the relation with rhyme

structure the op en syllable eect whichwould have to b e accounted for

by something like ConnectN lax

The same constraint ConnectN lax can b e observed at work in Ro

mance dialects Most scholars do not assume havea

length distinction In some dialects however wendlaxvowels in closed

syllables and tense vowels in op en syllables

Take for instance the following paradigms with mid vowels of Languedo c

French copied from Durand



Notice that in this case it should b e tense rather than lax that spreads I will

not go into the question of whether the features presented here are binary or whether

there are features with opp osing interpretations See chapter and app endix A for some discussion

lax and syllable structure in vowel harmony

e

ses se his sec sk dry

mettra metra will put mettre mtr put

sechait seMe will dry sceptique sptik sceptical

peu p little p eur pr fear

heureux r happy heurter rte hit

aveugler avgle to blind aveugle avgl blind

o

beau b o handsome bord br side

p oser p ose put p oster pste p ost

rosier rozje rosebush rose rz rose

Durand describ es this pro cess as lowering of the mid vowels in closed sylla



bles yet we could just as well see it as laxing of these vowels However

French do es not haveany stress rule whichwould justify a length distinc

tion b etween the dierentvowels

Again the restriction to mid vowels p oints to a sp ecial relation of high

and low to the pro cess involved

Ho op er prop oses the rules a and b to account for a similar

phenomenon in Spanish

lej lj ley law

le le le himyou

lerdo lrdo lerdo slow

piedra p jedra piedra stone

ekuestre ekwstre ecuestre equestrian

a syll tense syll

b syll tense

These rules are exactly the same as the ones prop osed for Dutch That the

rules are really pro ductiv e can b e seen in morphological pro cesses with s

plural formation for nouns second p erson singular formation for verbs

klase klass clase clases class

tnto tnts tonto tontos stupid

korqe korqs corre corres run

Interestingly some Spanish dialects have a pro cess whichisvery muchlike

Eastern Javanese tense harmonyIgive the following Andalusian Ho op er



In Languedo c French the vowels are also sub ject to this rule in the environment

b efore C

lax and syllable structure in vowel harmony

Zubizaretta Lieb er paradigm as an example the authors

just mentioned have shown that the s is deleted phonetically in this dialect

orthography singular plural gloss

p edazo p eaSo pS piece

piso piso ps o or

grup o grup o grVp group

Again this typ e of harmony cannot b e explained if vowel length is at play

b ecause length is not a feature that can spread

In Andalusian as in Javanese the relation with vowel heightisagain

very clear According to Zubizaretta there are twotyp es of rules

laxing the nal vowelofaword The rst rule is obligatory in all Andalusian

dialects it makes the vowel very lax and it feeds spreading This rule applies

to nonhigh vowels only The second rule is not obligatory do es not o ccur

in all Andalusian dialects and makes the vowel only slightly lax This rule

applies to all vowels also the high ones

The lowvowel a also plays a sp ecial role in Andalusian harmonybe

cause it b ehaves as an opaque vowel it do es not undergo laxing itself and

it blo cks harmony to pass through it Although a formalisation of the

harmony pro cesses would lead us to o far aeld I refer to the work of the

scholars cited earlier it should b e clear bynow that constraints on co o ccur

rence of height and lax harmony enforcing constraints and Connect N

lax interact in Andalusian to pro duce a complicated pattern in which the

role of ConnectN lax is nevertheless undeniable

Finally I want to remark that lax is mayb e not the only feature that

has a relation to rhyme structure At least one other ap erture feature

seems to play a similar role as lax viz high This has b een observed for

Chamorro byChung Her formulation of the distribution of high

her ex and some examples her exx followbelow

Nonlowvowels surface as mid in stressed closed syllables and as

high elsewhere

metgut strong poddu to fall

w

manei cold cobg i to do

ettigu short mundogu cows stomach

w w

g esg is to brush kurason heart

lebblu b o ok ucan rain

psaw shing line peskadot sherman

impatcu b ored tired mumu to ght

dispasyu slow gobyenu government governor

w

g to dream cetnut wound

lax and syllable structure in vowel harmony

Since tense and high seem to b e intimately connected feature values in

many languages including Dutch cf section and since Chamorro

do es not seem to have a phonetic tenselax distinction indep endentof

height it is not very clear whether phonologically should b e anal

ysed as simply an instance of ConnectN lax or as a variation of this

constraint in terms of high rather than tense In any case Chamorro

shows more evidence that a relation b etween vowel features and branch

ingness of rhymes is not just a p eculiarity of Dutch but o ccurs in many

other languages as well A more complete overview of all p ossible relations

between ap erture features and syllable structure will b e provided in chapter

It seems that a feature like lax can havetwo eects an eect on

syllable structure and a harmonizing eect We can draw the following

table of languages

Vowel Harmony No Vowel Harmony

Syll Str Javanese Dutch

No Syll Str Dagaare French

Dutch has an eect on syllable structure but no harmonytheWestAfrican

language Dagaare inversely has ATRharmony but no relation b etween the

feature and syllable structure and Javanese is an example of a language

which has b oth I nd it most dicult to come up with a language that has

a tenselax distinction but neither harmony or a syllable structure eect

Paris French sup ercially seems a case in p oint but on the other hand

this language shows some marginal syllable structure eect closed syllable

adjustment of e cf chapter It could b e that the reason whyitis

dicult to nd a language without harmony or closed syllable adjustment

is that in such a language it is dicult to argue for the existence of an

indep endent feature lax

More formallywe mightsay that there are three groups of constraints

at work The rst constraint of course is Connect N lax the second con

straintisHarmony and the third group of constraints is Faithfulness ban

ning all deletion and insertion of the feature lax ie Parselax lax

These constraints together dene four dierenttyp es of language

N laxFaithfull Javanese a Harmony Connect

b Harmony FaithfullConnect N lax Dagaare



For the sake of simplicityI have assumed that nothing can happ en to a representation

except for the insertion or deletion of lax ie that the constraints against deletion and

insertion of consonants are undominated in all examples presented here

lax and syllable structure in vowel harmony

c FaithfullHarmony ConnectN lax

FaithfullConnectN laxHarmony Paris French

N laxHarmony Faithfull d Connect

ConnectN laxFaithfullHarmony Dutch

If the faithfulness constraints are undominated no feature lax can ever b e

deleted or inserted in order for a structure to meet either Connect N lax

or Harmony The relative ordering of the latter two constraints is therefore

irrelevant If ConnectN lax is undominated similarly the ranking of

the other two constraints is irrelevant since the value for lax is dep endent

on the structure of the syllable in every p osition in the word Only when

Harmony is the highestranking constraintcanwe determine what the

relative ranking of the other constraint is since this constraint is dep endent

on an initial value on whichwe can harmonize When this initial value is

N lax determined by the structure of the nal syllable hence Connect

we get Javanese When it is determined by an underlying feature weget

Dagaare

Tenseness and branchingness in Dutch Ihavenow moti

vated the constraint Connect N lax on the basis of some languages not

directly related to Dutch Let us see what it can do for us in the analysis

of Dutch itself In order to explain the Trub etzkoyan asymmetry b etween

the two sets I assume that lax is the marked value I furthermore assume

that lax is absent in order to b e maximally explicit but my prop osal

do es not crucially dep end on any particular version of undersp ecic ation

theory

It mightbeofinterest also that Dutch is a language which has nal

devoicing This could b e seen as the result of a constraint disallowing voice

to o ccur at the end of a syllable Nowwe will see at various places b elow

that there is reason to assume a strong connection b etween the features

voice and lax For instance in Dutch monomorhemic VCV sequences lax

vowels are almost always followed byvoiceless fricatives and tense vowels

are almost always followed byvoiced fricatives cf section on page

It therefore seems quite suggestive that b oth features are disallowed to

o ccur at the end of the syllable

Nowif is alax and these are the only twovowels that have

b een recognized as a tenselax pair by all phonologists wehave logically

sp eaking four p ossible syllables whichareofinterest to us

My prop osals for schwainchapters and will however dep end on the assumption

that lax is absent indeed

lax and syllable structure in vowel harmony

N N

N N

d

d t

lax

lax

N N

N N

d a

d a t

Of these four logical p ossibiliti es only two are actually allowed by Connect N

lax viz a a lax vowel in a closed syllable and d a tense

vowel in an op en syllable b is disallowed b ecause the vowel is lax

but the rhyme is not branching c is disallowed b ecause the rhyme is

branching but the vowel is not lax I once again emphasize that daad is

a p ossible word of Dutch but the nal t falls outside the rst syllable in

my view cf section in the next chapter

Formalization in Optimality theory The observations made

ab ove can b e formalized in Optimality Theory in the following way



N laxParseconsonant ProjectN V Connect

Parseconsonant forces all consonants to b e parsed into syllables ProjectN

V is a constraint against degenerate syllables Since degenerate syllables

are syllables with branching N they have an onset segment but without

a head such a constraint can b e formulated as a pro jection constraintas

follows



ProjectN V If an Nbranches N should dominate some vo calic

feature FV

I assume that the onset consonant is parsed i the vowel is parsed all other

analyses will crash due to indep endent constraints The four underlying



The analysis will b e changed slightly b elow section where Parseconsonant

will fall to a lower rank and another constraint will take its place to ensure dat

Some more arguments for the length of Avowels

forms d dt da and dat will get the following evaluations the

dots represent syllable b oundaries the angled brackets unparsed material



d

Candidates ConnectN lax Parseconsonant

p

d 

d

dt

Candidates ConnectN lax Parseconsonant ProjectN V

dt 

p

dt

d t 

dt

da

Candidates ConnectN lax Parseconsonant

da 

p

da

dat

Candidates ConnectN lax Parseconsonant ProjectN V

dat 

dat

da t 

p

dat

The two prop erties that count as the most serious arguments in favour

of vo calic length can b e explained in this system as well viz b ehaviour

b efore consonant clusters and b ehaviour at the end of the syllable A vowel

b efore a consonant cluster at the end of the word is in a closed syllable and

therefore by necessity lax a vowel at the end of the syllable is in an op en

syllable and therefore by necessityitisnot lax

Some more arguments for the length of Avowels

In section I mentioned one imp ortant argumentwhich sup ercially

seemed to b e in favour of the lengthbased theory This was the argu



One output candidate that has to b e ruled out as well is db with an ep enthetic

consonant This will b e done in section

Some more arguments for the length of Avowels

ment based on syllable structure which has however b een refuted in the

previous sections

As far as I know three other arguments have b een mentioned in the

literature in favour of length theory I think that also these arguments are

not necessarily leading us to a length theoryhowever I will now discuss

each of these arguments in a separate subsection

Tenseness cannot b e dened in a satisfactory way pho

netically The phonetic argument is in fact more an argument against

tenseness than an argumentinfavour of length As I indicated in the intro

duction it is very dicult to giveawatertight p erceptual or articulatory

denition of the contrast b etween tense and lax vowels A strong tradition

in mo dern phonology demands that phonological features always havea

phonetic basis Therefore more abstract features are less desirable

I will discuss the issue of the phonetic basis of lax in more detail in

section I will show there that there is a straightforward articulatory

denition of the feature viz Retracted Tongue Ro ot

For now I note that this argument is the weakest of the ones discussed

here b ecause length theory has the same problems in providing a phonetic

basis There is a number of vowels which b ehave as long phonologically but

which are phonetically just as short as the phonologically unambiguously

short vowels These recalcitrantvowels are i y and u V an Haeringen

Heeroma Moulton No oteb o om Trommelen

Hermans Bo oij Therefore the notion of phonological length is

at least as abstract as the notion of laxness

Minimality requires branching A p otential argumentinfa

vour of length theory comes from Word Minimality McCarthy and Prince

quite convincingly argued that we should recognize a principle of

Universal Grammar requiring all lexical words to have a minimum size

This minimum is one branching Fo ot either bimoraic or bisyllabic sub ject

to languagesp ecic requirements

Now Dutch do es not have lexical words consisting of a CV syllable where

V is a Bvowel It does have a suciently large sto ckofwords of the CV

shap e with V an Avowel mee with P vree made love V thee tea

N wee queer A to mention just one example for every lexical category

This could b e describ ed as a consequence of Minimality with the parameter

setting bimoraic for Dutch under a length theory

It has b een observed that Dutch syllables can never end in a lax vowel

not even when they o ccur in p olysyllabic monomorphemic forms Wedo

not nd words ending in a lax vowel and we also do not nd lax vowels in

Some more arguments for the length of Avowels

front of another vowel Van der Hulst

tks taxi id tksi

kad cadeau gift kado

hat hiaat hiatus hijat

krol creool creole krejol

Since all the unattested forms would still b e bimoraic even bisyllabic

these gaps cannot b e the consequence of Word MinimalityItisthus usually

assumed that Dutch has a minimality requirement on syllables Trommelen

Van der Hulst Kager Syllable rhymes should contain at

least two p ositions in Dutch

Tenseness theory do es not need to p osit such a minimality requirement

but it is still faced with the problem howtoaccount for monomoraic mee

thee vree and wee It seems that wewould have to assume Dutchhasno

minimality requirement

Kiparsky and Kager have argued that in tro chaic stress

systems there is a relationship b etween apparent minimality violations and

wordnal stress a tro chaic system has forms with nal stress only if it has

apparent minimality violations What this suggests is that these systems

have some underlying marking which enables seemingly monomoraic feet

Both p olysyllabic words with nal stress and monomoraic words would have

this exception marking

Kiparsky and Kager argue this marking is catalexis tro chaic systems

whichhave nal stress have a catalectic mora or syllable ie a stress

b earing unit which is not lled by melo dic material We can assume this

catalectic element is also presentinwords which are sup ercially coun

terexamples to the proso dic minimality requirement These forms are then

bimoraic and their second mora is catalectic

Dutchhasatrochaic stress system and it clearly has words with nal

stress menu chocola etc Therefore we could assume that Dutchhas

catalexis in its technical vo cabulary cf Nouveau in whatever way

this formal instrument is expressed I will discuss the representation of

catalexis in section in the next chapter In any case it is not necessary

to assume that the second mora of CV lexical words is lled It might

b e catalectic Therefore we can discard the Proso dic Minimality argument

Minimality is not a problem for a tenseness theory at all

Avowels form the domain of tonal contour in Limburg

Dutch The last argument for length theory I want to discuss is provided

by Hermans a in a discussion on the interaction b et ween syllable

structure and tone in Maasbracht Limburg Dutch a dialect sp oken in the

Some more arguments for the length of Avowels

Dutch province of Limburg as well as in surrounding areas in Germany and

Belgium The facts are pretty complicated and involveseveral factors such

as stress and consonantal feature comp osition not under discussion here I

will therefore not b e able to fully discuss them here and have to conne

myself to a summary

Limburg Dutch phonetically has two distinct tonal contours a falling

tone analyzed as HL and a dragging tone analyzed as HLH A contrast

between the twocontours can only b e established in the syllable b earing

primary stress and only if that syllable has a welldened structure Its

rhyme should contain either a nonhigh Avowel or a Bvowel followed



by one of the sonorant consonants fm n l g No contrast is ever found

in any of the other cases All the following facts have b een copied from

Hermans a I use a to indicate a falling tone and a to indicate a

dragging tone

a nonhigh Avowel

falling tone dragging tone

steeg alley stex stex rose stex

slaag succeed slax slaag blow slax

Koos name kos koos crust kos

weer again wer weer we wer

b Bvowel followed by tautosyllabic sonorant

falling tone dragging tone

kan jug kn kan can kn

klam trap klm klam hardly klm

min minus mn min vile mn

c Other categories

pit kernel pt

kat cat kt

rok skirt rk

Piet name pit

koes quiet kus

Mien name min

Hermans a explanation of this paradigm is as follows The rhyme

includes a smaller constituent the nucleus This nucleus dominates either

just an Avowel or a Bvow el plus a following tautosyllabic sonorantcon

sonant r l m or n Obstruents can by stipulation never b e part of the

nucleus



The sonorants r and by stipulation are not part of the nucleus Limburg

Dutch also has a set of overlong vowels from which I abstract away In the next chapter

I will discuss this typ e of vowels in another dialect of Dutch

Some more arguments for the length of Avowels

N

N



N

 r k

Phonologically the falling tone is an HL contour and the dragging tone

an HH contourThe rst high tone is assigned to the rst mora of a syl

lable automatically Both contours are bitonal According to Hermans it

is attractive to assume the nucleus is bip ositional and tones can only b e

assigned within the domain of the nucleus

If b oth nuclear slots are lled either by a long vowel or by a short

vowel followed by a sonorant the contours can get complete realization If

however we let a short vowel b e followed by an obstruent which will b e a



sister rather than a daughter of N only one nuclear p osition is available

This p osition is lled by the high tone whichissharedbythetwocontours

The low tone in the dragging contour is inserted in the phonetic com

p onent to satisfy the OCP The nucleus will b e phonetically lengthened to

giveway to three rather than to two tones

The phonetic comp onent is also needed in the analysis of high Avowels

which do not allow for a contrast even though they are phonologically long

in the sense that they o ccupytwo p ositions in the nucleus According to

Hermans a the high vowels phonologically take the only two p ositions

which are available in the nucleus If they are followed b y a sonorant this

sonorant therefore can only b e syllabied outside of the nucleus Since it is

extranuclear it cannot b e a tone b earer In the phonetic comp onent the

high vowels are however shortened by stipulation After this op eration they

are to o short to b ear a tonal contour The nal part of the contour will

therefore disapp ear The sonorant gets a low tone at the end of the deriva

tion In this way the sequence Avowelsonorant is predicted to always

surface with a falling tone

In my view these multiple references to the phonetic comp onenttode

rive the right contours weaken the force of Hermans a argumentfor

a phonological role of length to a considerable degree Tenseness theory

also recognizes the phonetic length of nonhigh Avowels A further obser

vation we could make is that the contours are needed only on syllables with

primary stress That vowels should lengthen under such a condition is not

very surprising and indeed lengthening of nonhigh tense vowels under

primary stress is a pro cess that is wellknown from the literature

Conclusion

Hermans a arguments give a strong indication that Avowels are

phonetical ly long but this is not a matter of dispute for either length theory

or tenseness theory

Conclusion

In this chapter I have argued for one sp ecic pro jection constraint ConnectN

lax This constraint can regulate the relation b etween the op en vs closed

nature of a syllable and the tenseness vs laxness of a vowel This con

straintisvery highly ranked in Dutch so that it regulates the structure

of every indep endent syllable In other languages most notably in regu

lar ATR harmony languages the working of the constraint is obscured by

other factors Yet there are also languages in whichvowel harmony and

N lax interact in suchawaythatwe can see the eect of b oth Connect

these are Polynesian languages suchasJavanese or Romance dialects such

as Andalusian Spanish

The assumption of Connect N lax also solves a problem of Dutch

phonology and it seems to indicate that the length of Dutch Avowels is

a phonetic derivative of tenseness tense vowels always are syllabied into

op en syllables and vowels in op en syllables get lengthened esp ecially when

they are stressed

The analysis of wordnal consonants as p otentially b elonging to a de

generate syllable led me to p ositing one other pro jection constraint viz

ProjectN V the constraintthatevery syllable with an onset ie ev

ery syllable with a branching N no de has to have at least some vo calic

feature material in its head something that degenerate syllables violate by

denition This constraintwillbeworked out in more detail in the next

chapter

The other constraints that were prop osed were fairly straightforward for

instance faithfulness constraints blo cking unnecessary consonant deletion

The exact p osition in the hierarchy will b e approximated more exactly in

the course of the chapters that follow App endix B gives a top ology of

the constraint hierarchyofDutch as far as it could b e determined by the

research rep orted on in this thesis

App endix Historical overview

In this app endix I will briey review the phonological literature that has

app eared since the year of the rst article on the phonology of Dutch

written by AW de Gro ot It is not myintention to give a complete and

ob jective historical survey of the literature for an overview of the history

App endix Historical overview

of Dutch linguistics see Bakker and Dibb ets for an overview of the

twentieth century see Anderson In the b o dy of this chapter I present

ideas that go back to the p erio d of early structuralism while hop efully

preserving the insights of more recentwork

It is therefore useful to have at least a sup ercial idea ab out b oth the

structuralist ideas and ab out the insights that were formulated later in

order to reject these This also gives me the p ossibility to discuss some

classical problems from the p ointofviewdevelop ed here I havechosen

to skip most of the literature already discussed in previous sections but it

is dicult to avoid all overlap

Dutch structuralism The Europ ean structuralist tradition had

a signicant impactonDutch phonological research One of the most im

p ortant founding conferences of this new branch of linguistics was organized

by Dutchscholars in The Hague In the decade after this confer

ence Dutch phonology came to prosp erity The problem of distinguishing

Avowels and Bvowels was among the very rst to b e attacked

Traditional linguists likeVan Helten Van Wijk Verdam

who were often working on earlier stages of the language or on pho

netics had referred to the distinction as one of short versus long vowels

Most phoneticians in this century havekept to this distinction as the only

real one This p osition was rejected by all phonological authors of the

early structuralist p erio d however

De Gro ot prop osed to distinguish b etween vowels whichwere

bright helder and those whichwere dull dof Vowel length was

a concomitant qualityofbrightvowels not a distinctiveproperty itself

However De Gro ots own terminology was based on phonetic introsp ection

that most subsequent authors have found hard to replicate It has b een

rejected as b eing to o much based on intuition In the words of Moulton

this proposal does not advance matters It merely tel ls us that the

two classes exist Aswehave seen it is still dicult to nd a term that is

more insightful

Other phonologists used a terminology that was more directly related

to Sieverss stark geschnitten versus schwach geschnitten strongly

cut versus weakly cut and Trub etzkoys Silbenschnittkorrelation

Syllable Cut

Es ist klar dass auch die Silb enschnittkorre lati on ein proso dis

cher Anschlussgegensatz ist Sie ist ja eigentlichnichts an

deres als eine Opp osition zwischen dem sogenannten festen

und dem sogenannten losen Anschluss eines vokalischen Sil

agers an einen folgenden Konsonanten Wenn dab ei der bentr

App endix Historical overview

Vokal mit festem Anschluss kurzer als der Vokal mit losem An

schluss ist so ist dies nur eine phonetische Folgeerscheinung

 Die Silb enschnitts korrel ation b eruht somit auf einer

privativen Opp osition deren merkmallosen Glied der unge

schnittene voll ablaufende Vokal ohne festen Anschluss an einen

folgenden Konsonanten ist Dadurcherklaren sich auchdie

Ergebnisse der Aufhebung dieser Korrelation sie wird im Aus

laute o der vor Vokalen aufgehob en wob ei in der Aufhebungsstel

lung naturlic herweise nur die phonetisch langen b ezw halb

langen voll ablaufenden Vokalphoneme stehen so im Englis

chen Hollandischen Deutschen Norwegischen Schwedischen

im SchottischGaelischen im Hopi usw

Idevoted a relatively large amount of space to this quotation b ecause it ex

presses the structuralists p oints in a very concise way First vowel length

is describ ed as an epiphenomenon of the real privative opp osition namely

the one b etween a tight and a lo ose connection b etween consonants and vow

els But most imp ortantly it gives one of the most comp elling arguments

in fa vour of the Silb enschnittkorre lati on instead of the length hyp othesis

we can explain why it is the memb ers of group A that count as unmarked

There are two reasons for this In the rst place these are the vowels

that o ccur at the end of the word as wehave seen In a Trub etzkoyan way

of thinking it often happ ens that a privative opp osition is lifted at the edge

the end of a domain we only nd the unmarked form there Therefore

we can see set A as the unmarked set

Another way not mentioned in the quotation but immediately b elow

it in whichwecanseesetAasunmarked is by counting the number of

memb ers set B is smaller than set A byoneortwo dep ending on whether

we recognise the distinction memb ers This means that there are

afewvowels in set A whichdonothave a corresp onding vowel in set B

This is an indication that set A really is the unmarked set If B would

b e unmarked there would b e vowels which can only exist if they b ear

the marked feature This line of reasoning seems to have b een accepted

by all Dutch structuralists A lot of discussion subsequently wentinto

the exact nature of the phonological feature corresp onding to Trub etzkoys

correlation As noted De Gro ots prop osal to distinguish b etween brigh t

and dull vowels has not b een accepted Among the other terms that have

b een prop osed I mention scherp gesneden versus zwak gesneden Van

Wijk and gedekt checked versus ongedekt free Van Ginneken

The connection to Trub etzkoys line of thinking is obviouseven

though it is not clear there has b een a oneway relation of inuence only

Yet the phonetic arbitrariness remained a problem For instance Van

App endix Historical overview

Haeringen remarked with resp ect to Van Wijks division that

there is no a priori way to tell that the syllable b oundary which follows the

zwak gesneden vowels of gieten futen boeken is in anyway dierentfrom

the scherp gesneden vowels of pitten putten pokken The Syllable Cut

did not seem to corresp ond to anything in the phonetic reality

Pregenerative literature The next few steps were taken at

the end of the s and the b eginning of the s I call this p erio d

pregenerativist not b ecause the scholars in this p erio d could b e seen as

a group with a sp ecic goal in mind but b ecause the ideas suggested in

this p erio d seem to have had the largest impact in subsequent generativist

work

Two dierent lines of reasoning were set up First came Cohen et al

who can b e set in the structuralist tradition as far as the analysis

ofsetAversus set B is concerned b e it that these authors intro duced the

terms tense gespannen versus lax ongespannen in the Dutch literature

in order to describ e the phonetic dierence They set up the following

corresp ondences

lax y

tense i e a o u y

At the same time some scholars b ecame aware of the problems that were

inherent in this approach I already mentioned the criticism against the

earlier structuralist work and indeed the same questions were raised with

resp ect to Cohen et al by Moulton the division into two groups

was not wellmotivated phonetically and esp eciall y the corresp ondence pairs

were arbitrary in some resp ects Other divisions were p ossible whichwould

do the phonological job of distinguishing pairs of phonemes equally well

e eey o

i y u a

The distinction b etween e and i phonetically seems to b e more or less

the same as that b etween i and so that it is not very clear what is

exactly the reason for lab eling the one pair as tenselax and describing the

other in some other way

An imp ortant problem for the theory was discussed byVan Haeringen

Heeroma and Moulton the classication of the high

vowels fiyug Phonetically these vowels pattern with the short vowels

rather than with the long ones cf No oteb o om but phonologically

they are more ambiguous

The most imp ortant reasons to distinguish phonologically b etween group

A and group B are distributional In this p erio d the following citeria were recognized

App endix Historical overview

ExtensibilityAt least Van Haeringen claimed that vowels

in group A are extensible they can b e delib erately pronounced in a

longer p erio d of time whereas the vowels in group B are not extensible

Now i y and u could not b e extended in the sp eechofVan Haeringen

and therefore this author argued they were memb ers of group

B The nonextensibility of i y and u as well as that of the lax

vowels was immediately questioned by Heeroma and nowadays

it do es no longer count as a criterion for the distinction b etween A

and B altogether As a matter of fact the extensibilityoflaxvowels is

sometimes used as an argument against length theory A sp eaker can

freely extend the lax ofbom b omb bm to bqqqqqm which still

do es not sound like boom tree b om In a theory of grammatical

comp etence the extensibili ty criterion is however very dicult to

evaluate in either direction

Intervo calic fricatives In the context VFV a vowel F a frica

tive F is voiceless if and only if V is a memb er of group B Heeroma

OK OK OK

Compare lavn refresh levn live lovn

OK OK OK

praise lvr hank liv dear luvn lu lvn

OK OK

lvn lvn ly vn lvn blfn bark bsn

OK OK OK

b erries rfn ris hsn jig and jog ky sn do o dd

jobs blafn besn rifn husn kysn According to

this criterion the high vowels should clearly b e in group A There are

however some pretty normal words Pasn Eastern mzl luck

pyzl puzzle that count as exceptions to the generalization which

therefore is not watertight I have no explanation for the interaction

between voiced fricatives and tenseness in vowels I will return to this



issue in the next chapter

Occurrence b efore consonant clustersVowels from group B

o ccur b efore wordnal consonant clusters C C where C is a non

i j j

coronal The dorsal nasal b ehaves like a consonant cluster in this

OK OK OK

resp ect For instance b afraid kr carrion j

OK

kre jo di du d young d thing ba

OK OK OK

blk b eam balk wlp whelp welp krmp shrink

krimp This criterion seems to indicate that i y and u are in group

A A substantivenumber of counterexamples however involves exactly

OK OK

these vowels esp ecially i hilp help ed wirp throwed etc

This criterion has b een extensively discussed ab ove The reason why

high vowels count as exceptions might b e exactly that these vowels



Henk van Riemsdijk pc notes that there is some variation in the pronunciation

of puzzel some sp eakers saypyzl

App endix Historical overview

do not have a lax variant I will return to this b elow

The criterion is also not watertight however b ecause there are again

OK

some counterexamples like twalf on the one hand On the other

hand for instance the combination C C is also very rare while

i j

is in group B according to all other criteria The only words where

OK OK OK

it shows up are wlx willow zwlk oilcloth schlf er

OK

chip zlver silver

Occurrence at end of a wordVowels of group B cannot o ccur at

the end of the word Van Haeringen questioned this criterion

b ecause schwa phonetically the shortest vowel can o ccur at the end

of the word as well Yet Heeroma argued that this cannot count

as an argument b ecause schwa is an exceptional vowel in many other

resp ects Therefore this remains to b e one of the most imp ortant

are clearly in group A according criteria The high vowels i y and u

OK OK OK

to this criterion zi see du do ny now

Diminutives the diminutiveshows up as tj after V Swhere

B

V isavowel in group B and S a sonorant consonantdtj

B

mntj bmtj hltj krtjandasCj C some stop

after V S where V is a vowel in group A According to Van Haerin

A A

gen the high vowels b ehave sometimes as if they were in

group A and sometimes as if they were in group B wiltj wiltj

blump j blumtj rimp j strimtj Heeroma ho wever

observes that also this criterion is weak b ecause there are to o many

counterexamples also in the other classes Trommelen notes

that Van Haeringens arguments mighthavebeentypical for his Rot

terdam Dutch background

The discussion was brought to a conclusion byavery inuential article

by Moulton who prop osed to distinguish b etween a set of long

and a set of short vowels The long vowels were e a o the short

vowels y and the vowels i y u did not participate in the

opp osition This explained the ambiguous b ehaviour of the latter group

Notice however that this typ e of analysis can only b e parsed if long is

a feature not under an autosegmental approach to length

Early generative grammar In spite of Moultons argu

ments against it early workers on the generative phonology of DutchDe

Rijk Spa Brink used the feature tense This mightbe

traced back to the inuence of Chomsky and Halle who used the

same feature for a similar distinction in English The interpretation of the

feature varied however from author to author

App endix Historical overview

For instance De Rijk argued that we may regarda tense

vowel as a sequence of lax ones The reasons that were given for this

version of length theory were somewhat dierent from the ones hitherto

discussed This pap er is full of original observations most of which are

unaccounted for in most of the following literature on the distribution of

vowels Following SPE the author furthermore allowed himself to givea

highly abstract analysis of these data

One of the observations was that y o ccurs in all contexts except

b efore w and r y on the other hand is very frequent b efore w and r

but hardly o ccurs in any other context according to De Rijk For

instance in wordnal p osition it o ccurs only in u you y and nu now



ny He therefore prop osed a diphthongization rule deriving y from

y in all contexts except b efore glides the y b efore r was assumed to

b e the result of a tensing rule applying after diphthongization He tried

to extend this approachtoderivealsoi and u from underlying i and

u resp ectivel y but as the author admitted the p oint there turned out not

very strong

Spa remarked that an approach along the lines of SPE was able

to solve a problem of Dutch morphophonemics It could describ e plural

formation in en in a uniform way In earlier accounts examples like the

following were problematic

a dx dan

b vlx vln

c wp wbn

d bvl bveln

Spa prop osed to solve this problem by assuming the following un

derlying representations for these forms dvlwbb bvl Plural

formation now simply is the addition of n Subsequently there is a rule

of vowel tensing in op en syllables the rule version of Connect N lax



 

syll

C tense tense

cons

Finally a rule degeminates the obstruent clusters at the end of the word Of

course this is not really an argumentinfavour of tense nor is it intended

to b e We could just as well assume that the vowel is lengthened in op en

syllables The other analysts of this p erio d also do not give convincing



We mightaddmenu menu mny individu individualndividy paraplu um

brella paraply and some other words admittedly all originally loans from Frenchbut

I think accepted by most sp eakers as normal Dutchforms

App endix Historical overview

arguments in favour of any p osition De Rijk remarks it is very

dicult to cho ose b etween long and tense by phonological criteria he

cho oses the latter b ecause unfortunately we cannot postpone our decision

until the happy times when phonological theory wil l have a rm grasp on its

problems He therefore cho oses to follow Cohen et al

Bisegmental analyses in generative phonology AsfarasI

know the rst to level substantive criticism against the use of the feature

tense within the generative framework is Zonneveld Interestingly

Zonneveld bases himself partly on an attackby Lass on the

use of tense for English in SPE Lasss main argument sounds

familiar bynow

Even the few apparently empirical tests for recognizing the fea

ture fail either through b eing hop elessly sub jective or through

logical incoherence 

Zonnevelds own main motivation was that he wanted to put the diphthongs

i y u in one class together with the Avowels The diphthongs b e

have like Avowels with resp ect to many of the criteria mentioned ab ove

they can o ccur at the end of a word but not b efore a consonantclus

ter they cho ose the same allomorph of the diminutive Zonneveld

also mentioned that diphthongs and Avowels alternate in ablaut rijdreed

ritret buigboog byxb ox etc that in sp eech errors long vowels

were often substituted for diphthongs and vice versa No oteb o om

and that precisel y the Avowels and the diphthongs formed the lefthand

context for a rule of Dutch Zonneveld was interested in Weakening

Zonneveld argued that the simplest assumption would b e that

b oth diphthongs and Avowels are bisegmental the diphthongs would con

sist of two dierentvowels following one another and the Avowels would b e

diphthongs of which the two memb ers happ ened to b e identical Because

otherwise he was following SPE he prop osed that a set of morpheme struc

ture constraints would take care of the dierences b etween Bvowels on the

one hand and diphthongs and Avowels on the other These constraints

now could all refer to the dierence b etween cons and conscons

The account presented ab ove can explain these data without assuming

length Ablaut is nowadays regarded as a typically very early pro cess in the

Lexical Phonology something applying to stems if it is considered to b e

of interest for phonological theory at all Substitution may therefore take

place at a moment where diphthongs are still regarded as monosegmental

just like Goat Latin Weakening may b e sensitive to preceding syllables

ending in a vowel etc

App endix Historical overview

This prop osal was accepted and recast in a nonlinear framework by

Trommelen Van der Hulst and Bo oij a among others

In these theories the vowels and a are represented as follows

a

x x x

A A

A diphthong is a sequence of two short vowels each linked to its own

xslot The most salient prop erties of the Dutchvowel system can nowbe



explained in an elegantway the o ccurrence b efore consonant clusters

and at the end of the syllable

As for the clusters it is assumed that the rhyme of the syllable contains

at most three skeletal p ositions at the edge of the word NowifAvowels

o ccupy already two p ositions in the rhyme for themselves there is no further

ro om for two consonants at most one consonant can o ccur after an Avowel

Bvowels on the other hand o ccupyonlyoneskeletal p osition and therefore

they leaveroomfortwo consonants in the rhyme

With regard to the o ccurrence at the end of the word it is assumed that

Dutch syllables are minimally bimoric ie they should contain at least

two p ositions in the rhyme Bip ositional Avowels satisfy this criterion

immediately monop ositional Bvowels on the other hand need another

segmentintherhyme in order to fulll this requirement

During the rst years the problem of the high vowel receives relatively

little attention This changes at the end of the s Trommelen

gives a list of prop erties of the high vowels Apart from the prop erties

already mentioned in this app endix she mentions a few prop erties which

would lead us to classify these vowels in the set of Bvowels

The consonants app ears either after short vowels or after high vow

els but not after nonhigh vowels in set A rush id rs broche

brooch brs fetisj fetish fts pistache pistachio pists douche

shower dus pluche plush plys quiche id kis

Dutch has a threesyllable window at the end of the word for main

stress Sup erheavy syllables at the end of the word can only b e

skipp ed in a few exceptional cases evenaar equator ooievaar stork

etc However the class of stressles s sup erheavy syllables including



For a description of the distribution of the diminutive which requires more elab ora

tion in this typ e of approachseeTrommelen

App endix Historical overview

a high vowel is considerably larger emir id talmoed talmud

tureluur redshank etc

Sup erheavy syllables are very rare when not at the right edge of the

word Again a large class of exceptions is formed by sup erheavies

with high vowels toendra tundra junta id bourgeois id pizza

id muesli id

Most of these words are fairly recentloansTrommelen therefore

argues that Dutchhastwotyp es of high vowels the long ones in the

native lexicon and short ones in the lexicon of loan words One of two

things will now happ en according to her prediction either the loan words

will adapt themselves to the grammar and get shortened or the grammar

will adapt itself to the loan words and start tolerating short high vowels

In our system short high vowels are actually high vowels in a closed

syllable If Trommelens observations are rightit seems to me

that they are we can say that two constraints are currently in com

p etition ConnectN lax and highlax In previous stages of Dutch

ConnectN lax was inviolable vowels in closed syllables had to b e lax

It seems that the constraint against high lax vowels is stronger in Mo dern

Dutch High vowels can o ccur in closed syllables without laxing in order to

meet the demands of highlax

One of the most imp ortant problems facing the length theory is related

to stress as discussed b efore Dutch has a quantity sensitive stress system

but CV V syllables ie syllables containing a tense vowel countaslight

i i

whereas CV V and CVC syllables count as heavy This is an extremely

i j

marked situation crosslinguistic all y

Lahiri and Koreman solved this problem by assuming that long

vowels consisted of two ro ot no des whichwere attached to only one weight

unit one mora The nonsense syllables vaa and vam got the following

representations resp ectively

vaa  vam 

  

v a a v a m

Syllable length was counted at the melo dic tier whichmakes the denition

of length somewhat awkward syllable weight at the moraic tier

The problem of syllable weightissolved by Kager in a somewhat

dierentway Like Lahiri and Koreman Kager adopts a

App endix Historical overview

moraic theory of syllable structure In his view Bvowels are monomoraic

and Avowels are bimoraic Kager suggests that Dutch syllables are

all sub ject to a bimoraicity requirement Therefore counting the number

of moras is not a p ossible way to compute the weight of a syllable in Dutch

Instead weight is computed in terms of melodic complexity ACVV

i i

syllable is light b ecause it has just one vowel at the melo dic level On the

other hand CV V and CVChavetwo dierent segments at the melo dic

i j

level dep endent on the moras Kagers prop osal therefore can b e

seen as the mirror image of Lahiri and Koremans prop osal the

former computes syllable length at the level of moras and weightatthe

melo dy while the latter computed weight at the mora tier and length at

the melo dy

An imp ortant problem for the prop osals of Lahiri and Koreman

and Kager lies exactly in this computation at dierent levels In

the rst place one of the main motivations for moraic theory is that seg

ment counting was deemed to b e an imp ossible option for the grammar

McCarthy and Prince In the second place b oth theories haveto

p ostulate a rather complicated theory of syllable length Kager has

to b e able to count to three if he wants to explain the sup erheavy status

of raam window ram Lahiri and Koreman have to p ostulate that

twovo calic ro ot no des maybeattached to one mora The same is not true

for a vo wel and a consonant at the rst mora m is heavy but it is true

for a vowel and consonant at the second mora am is p ossible

A dierent approach to solving the same problem is oered in Smith

et al and Hermans a Under each of these approaches

while there are twoskeletal p ositions involved with the Avowels the second

p osition is empty at the p oint in the derivation where stress is assigned It



therefore do es not count as making the syllable heavy

Both approaches also involve a reintro duction of the feature tense in a

certain way This is literally true for Hermans Smith et al in

stead follow the Dep endency Phonology vowel geometry of Van der Hulst

and Smith and use a dep endent element I denoting pharyngeal

expansion instead All Avowels are identical to Bvowels except that they

have an extra dep endent feature I and of course there are twoAvowels



This typ e of account can essentially b e traced backtoTrager and Smith who

prop ose that there are twotyp es of vowels in English simple vowels and complex

vowels The latter have the form of a simple vowel followed by a glide This glide could

be any element of the set fhjwgFor instance the long vowel of bought actually

consists of a sequence h

In order to see the connection b etween this prop osal and the ones discussed in the text

it is necessary to recall that h is a consonant without any supralaryngeal sp ecication

If it is a glide at all it is the glide variantofschwa Nowtheemptyskeletal p osition of

Smith et al and Hermans is also schwa This is made explicit in Hermans a

App endix Historical overview

which do not have a corresp onding Bvowel

Smith et al also recognise an underlying length distinction represented

by underlying xslots This length distinction is used for the representation

of the vowels of frele frail frql freule dame fryql zone id zqn

and also for the lengthened high vowels which o ccur in some loan words such

as surprise id syrpriqs louvredeurtje salo on do or luqvrdqrtj and

centrifuge spinner sntrifyq According to Smith et al these vowels

underlyingly havetwo xslots

Furthermore the second xslot of the nonhigh Avowels is lengthened

by general rule after stress assignment has applied The high Avowels do

not spread in this way except p erhaps b efore r b ecause otherwise no

distinction could b e made b etween underlyingly long i y and u and short

i y and u which are lengthened by rule

Hermans a has an analysis whichisvery similar in all rel

evant resp ects to the one prop osed in Smith et alAll Avowels havean

underlying feature tense This feature ensures that the vowel is sup

plemented with an emptyskeletal p osition via some technical steps which

need not concern us here This p osition is lled up after stress assignment

in the case of the nonhigh vowels In the case of the high vowels it re

mains empty and gets eventually deleted for lack of phonetic content The

solution of Hermans a for really long vowels is also very similar to

the one of Smith et al The main dierence is that for technical

reasons Hermans do es not want to assume underlying xslots He is mainly

interested in Maasbracht Dutch which has long variants of the high vowels

and of the low lax vowels just like Standard Dutch He prop oses that

these vowels underlyingly havetwo ro ots

The main problem with the SmithHermans typ e of approach apart

from the empirical problems amply discussed in the b o dy of the chapter

is that it is highly abstract It p ostulates a vo calic length that in some

cases never surfaces and that is also irrelevantformany comp onents of

the grammar that normally do consider weight Basically the length is

only seen by the comp onent that needs to see it viz the syllabication

algorithm

All these recent prop osals are based on the insight that the main argu

ment for vowel length is syllable structure They all try to establish that

this length is invisible or irrelevant for other phonological mo dules suchas

stress assignment If however syllable length is irrelevant also for syllable

structure itself no argument for vo calic length except in the truly long

vowels is left

Tilburg Dutch and Standard DutchVowel

Length

In the discussion of Avowels versus Bvowels in the previous chapter I

have put aside several vowels In particular these were the truly long

vowels and the diphthongs All of these have to b e treated as cases of real

vo calic length One of the main p oints of this chapter is that the laxbased

pro jection theory presented in the previous chapter also has to oer an

interesting p ointofview for those vowels I will showthisonthebasisof

Standard Dutch facts and also on the basis of Tilburg Dutch a dialect in

which the parameters length and tenseness arguably b oth play a role in the

underlying system Undeniably long vowels and diphthongs typ ologically

b ehavemuch more like real long vowels for instance they b ehaveasheavy

in the stress system Their head is also most often lax a fact whichI

argue to follow from Connect N lax

Another goal of this chapter is to b e somewhat more sp ecic on some

asp ects of Dutchword structure that have b een left untouched in the pre

vious chapter For instance I will treat notions suchasambisyllabicity

extrasyllabicity and catalexis and the phonetic content of the feature lax

This chapter is structured in the following way In section I discuss

the relevant asp ects of the Standard Dutch long vowel system In sec

tion I then turn to Tilburg Dutch and discuss long vowels and vowel

shortening in that dialect In section I giveanoverview of the fea

ture co o ccurrence constraints that are needed to derive the whole Dutch

segmentinventory The last section is devoted to some concluding remarks

Details in the Standard Dutchvowel system

In this section I rst discuss the Dutchvowel system as I have established

it until now and sp ecically the role of the diphthongs in that system In

the second section I briey discuss my assumptions on the ambisyllabicity

of consonants Thirdly I lo ok at the b ehaviour of tense vowels b efore tau

tomorphemic r The topic of section is the phonetic nature of the

Details in the Standard Dutchvowel system

feature I hitherto called lax In section I conclude with an overview

of dierent forms of exceptional syllable structure at the edges of words

Diphthongs The three Dutchdiphthongs i y u have

b een left out of consideration until now I will assume eachconsistsofalax

vowel followed by a high vowel and that b oth vowels have the same vo calic

place features Schematically they therefore will b e represented here as i

y and u resp ectively and they will receive the following structure

cons cons cons cons cons cons

lax high lax high lax high

coronal labialcoronal labialdorsal

The rst ro ot no de gets the sp ecications of a lax and the second

ro ot no de those of a high tense vowel

These diphthongs are problematic in one resp ect Sometimes for in

stance in the Dutchversion of Goat Latin cf section they b ehave

as unities If diphthongs are really long in the sense that they havetwo

ro ot no de p ositions this weakens the argumentinfavour of length theory

My prop osal which is in some asp ects similar to prop osals of Calabrese

for Italian dialects and to the prop osals of McCarthy a to

explain the almost identical b ehaviour of English diphthongs is to treat

diphthongs as underlyingly short lax vowels

If we lookatthevowel system wehave established until now wenda

gap There seem to b e no lax high vowels

tense lax

high i y u

e o y

low a

Some authors assume is actually a high vowel but there is some evidence

that this vowel is in opp osition to e rather than to i In morphological

tensing environments ie the plural of a certain class of nouns where

lax vowels b ecome tense the vowel shifts to e systematically lidleden

memb ers ltled smidsmeden smiths smtsmed While it is

true that in this same environment also shifts to e whichwemight

The exact organization of the lower part of the system is to b e discussed in section

Details in the Standard Dutchvowel system

attribute to a repair rule since there is no tense counterpart to crucially

never alternates with i in this environment

In the next chapter we will see that e and are the tense and lax

vowel that are most susceptibl e to reduction to while other vowels are

harder to reduce

The same is true in rcolouring to b e discussed in section where

e laxes to q not to q leer leather lqr lqr High i only lengthens

in this environment Finally the rules of shortening in Tilburg Dutchalso

treat as the short variantofe

If this is true there really are no high lax vowels in Dutch This gap

has to b e accounted for somewhere in the theory p erhaps by the lter in



high

lax

Wehave already seen this lter can get a phonetic motivation at least if

we assume that lax is dened as the retraction of the tongue ro ot since

we need dierent almost opp osite tongue movements for high vowels and

for lax vowels Archangeli and Pulleyblank

Usually it is assumed that is a restriction on underlying representa

er that this is not a p ossible option in a theory in which tions Notice howev

it is assumed that any combination of features forms a p ossible input for

the grammar I think the latter assumption makes an interesting prediction

in this particular case Also vowels with the feature combination highlax

plus any of the p ossible place feature combinations are allowed as input

The combination of high and lax is however not allowed on the surface

according to if read as a surface lter whichisvery strong in Dutch

Agiven language can use one of several strategies to avoid violation of

feature co o ccurrence constraints like One p ossibilityisleaving either

high or lax unparsed and replacing it by a nonoending feature value

high or laxtense resp ectivel y

This typ e of cleanup strategy is attested in some Italian dialects Cal

abrese and p ossibly also in some nondiphtongizing Dutch dialects

whichhave pin like for instance Aalten Dutch and Maasbracht Dutch or

pq n like for instance The Hague Dutch and Tilburg Dutch instead of

Standard Dutchpin for pijn pain Aalten and Maasbrachthavechosen

to parse high The Hague and Tilburg havechosen to parse lax in order

to satisfy cf Van Eekelen et al

Standard Dutch on the other hand seems to have a preference for pars

ing underlying features It cho oses a dierent strategy also attested in

dialects of Italian according to Calabrese ibid which is inserting a new

Details in the Standard Dutchvowel system

ro ot no de Both ro ot no des are linked to the same place features but only

one gets high and the other one lax

The lax high front labial vowel for instance b ecomes either y or y

The surface order of the twovowels is determined by ConnectN lax

Because wehavetwovowels in the rhyme this rhyme technically counts

as heavy Therefore its head must b e the lax vowel and b ecause the head

precedes the complementwe get y Similar reasoning leads us to p osit

i and u for the front unrounded diphthong and the back rounded

diphthong resp ectivel y

I will now pro ceed to formalize these observations in terms of interacting

constraints highlax seems to b e unviolated in Dutch Other constraints



we need are listed b elow

a Parsehigh Parselax high lax has to b e parsed

b NoLongVowel NLV Two adjacentvo calic ro ots maynot



be linked to the same material



high

Parsehigh Parselax NLV

lax

As can b e seen hardly any ordering b etween these constraints can or need

to b e established at this p oint It is only relevant that NoLongVowel is

rank ed b elow most of them Only the ranking of ConnectN lax with

resp ect to this constraint has b een imp ossible for me to establish

The candidate set generated over the input high lax coronal gets

the following evaluation for convenience I representvowels with matrices

rather than with feature trees in this b ox and the dierentParse and Fill



constraints for all features have b een conated



Another inviolable constraintofDutch as indeed in most if not all languages of

the world is the one against high low combinations Also the constraint Parselow

may b e surmised to have a high ranking b ecause diphthongs havea low part and a high

part as we will see in the next chapter Strictly sp eaking surface diphthongs mayalso

b e derived from underlying high low combinations The rest of the argumentwould

work in the same way



I will show b elow that this constraint probably is not needed in the analysis of Dutch

where it can b e replaced by a constraint against the insertion of the feature low plus a

pro jection constraint



In this chapter I implicitly assume that emptycons ro ots are avoided wherever

this is p ossible an assumption for which I refer to the next chapter

Details in the Standard Dutchvowel system



high

Candidates Parse NLV ConN lax

lax

 

high

lax

coronal

 

high

lax

coronal

 

high

lax

coronal

 

high lax

coronal coronal

 

p

lax high

coronal coronal

The constraint NoLongVowel gets violated in the optimal candidate

However there is no alternative which do es not violate at least one of the

higherranking constraints Splitting the oensive feature combination in



two ro ots is the only thing wecando

Furthermore the rst part of the diphthong phonetically or p ostlexi

cally gets lowsothatwehave y i and u resp ectively probably in

order to makethetwo parts of the diphthong maximally distinct in qual

ity This desire for a maximally falling diphthong apparently overrides the

constraint against insertion of low We could assume this lowering is pho

netic or p ostlexical b ecause it is not structurepreser ving and are

no part of the vowel inventory of Standard Dutch except in diphthongs

This argumentmightintheendprove not to b e to o relevant In that case

the lowering might just as well b e assumed to b e lexical Both following



In particular it is imp ortant that Parselax and Parsehigh dominate the constraint

against long vowels Beside high lax some other feature co o ccurrence constraints are

op erative in Dutch One instance is low labial since there are no low rounded vowels

in Standard Dutch Yet none of these other constraints seem to havediphtongization as

an eect Rather in these cases one of the oending features gets deleted This means

that wehave evidence for the following constraint ordering

i Parsehigh ParselaxNoLongVowel Parselow or Parselabial

Interestingly there is evidence that Parsehigh and Parselax are ranked much higher

in Dutch than the Parse constraints relating to other features This evidence comes

from reduction We will see in the next chapter that high and lax vowels can hardly

b e reduced to schwa whereas other vowels can much more easily This will b e taken as

evidence that the features lax and high are much harder to get rid of than the other

features ie that ParsehighP arselaxParselowParselabialetc

Details in the Standard Dutchvowel system

forms will still b e in the candidate set after the part of the hierarchyin

has applied



ProjectN low Heads of branching nuclei need to b e supp orted

by low



low Do not insert low



Candidates ProjectN low low

 



low

p

high

 

lax

coronal

coronal

 

lax high

coronal coronal



ProjectN lowis yet another memb er of the family of pro jection con

straints under a certain traditional interpretation of the structure of the



nucleus If we assume that all vowels are dominated by the nucleus N long

vowels and diphthongs by denition imply a branching nucleus cf chapter

for more discussion



Furthermore ProjectN low and low together have the same ef

fect as NoLongVowel in the grammar of Dutch together they also pro

hibit the formation of unnecessary long vowels Reference to NoLongVowel

is therefore not necessary in the analysis of Dutch I refer the reader to the

last chapter for fuller discussion of b oth the notion of a branching nucleus

and of the status of NoLongVowel in the theory

If we assume the condition on secret languages like Goat Latin is that all

vowel features have to b e copied the unitlikebehaviour of diphthongs in

this system follows Both lax and high have to b e copied The oending

vowel splits b oth in the original and in the copied p osition

FinallyIwanttomention that sometimes o j a j uj and iw ew are

mentioned in the literature as diphthongs I will treat these as sequences

of an indep endentvow el plus glide following Bo oij among others

That is to say the tense vowel would b e in an op en syllable and the glide

would b e in the onset of a degenerate syllable It is interesting that these

sequences seem sub ject to the OCP and that sequences like ij yj and

ej with a frontvowel followed by j or uw ow with a labial vowel



followed by w do not exist We do nd yw clusters zenuw nerve



This constraintmight b e b etter seen as a conation of structurepreserving con

straints blo cking lowlabial combinations etc



The status of aw is unclear it do es not seem to b e attested in Standard Dutch

but it is found in dialects such as Rotterdam Dutch or Tilburg Dutch

Details in the Standard Dutchvowel system

schwaduw shadow which is somewhat surprising b ecause y is round as

well I will return to these issues in chapter

Ambisyllabicity In this section I will briey discuss the syllable

structure of Dutchwords like the following

appel app eal pl

hobbel knob hbl

al lofoon lofon

kudde herd kd

rol lade rolled b eef rlad

All these words contain a V CV sequence of whichV is lax This means

i j i

that the consonant should b e syllabied into the co da of the rst syllable to

satisfy ConnectN lax Yet there is also evidence that this consonantis

syllabied into the onset of the second syllable One reason to assume this

is that the consonantcanbevoiced which is only allowed in onset p ositions

in Dutch a language with Final Devoicing Another reason is that onsetless

syllables headed byschwa do not exist as we will see in the next chapter

This means that the consonant in question is in two syllabic p ositions

In most analyses at least since Trommelen and Van der Hulst

this is taken to mean that the consonant in question is literally ambisyl

labic ie one ro ot no de linked to two syllables In principle it could b e

that phonologically these consonants are even geminates which are short

ened p ostlexically or phonetically Borowsky Ito and Mester I will

arbitrarily adopt the former assumption that these are single consonants

attached to two syllabic p ositions so that kudde gets the following repre

sentation

N N

N N

 

N N

k d

As wehave seen in section the voicing of fricatives in V FV p osition

i j

is almost predictable from the vowel qualityofVifthisvowel is tense

i

the fricativeisvoiced griezel horror xrizl and if this vowel is lax the

fricativeisvoiceless mossel mussel msl The problematic cases again

include a or Pasen Easter mazzel luck cf section b elow

Details in the Standard Dutchvowel system

This dierence might b e attributed to a dierence in syllabic aliation

of the fricatives fricatives linked to one syllable would acquire voice

whereas ambisyllabic fricatives would stayvoiceless We nd a similar

dierence in Northern Italian with resp ect to s Voicing Nesp or and Vogel

a simple intervo calic s voices in that dialect but a geminate ie

ambisyllabic s do es not see Slis and Van Heugten for a rep ort that

voiceless fricatives are generally longer than voiced ones

Van der Hulst has shown that lax vowels cannot o ccur b efore

a OL sequence O an obstruent L a liquid While there are words such

as zebra id zebra and microfoon microphone mikrofon there are no

words like zbra or mkrofon

In order to rule out these surface forms wehave to rule out two syllabi

cations One is zbbra with an ambisyllabic b and the other is zbra

with a syllable b oundary b etween the obstruent and the liquid In order

to rule out the rst syllabication we need to assume that only segments

which are under the lowest N node can be ambisyllabic In order to rule

out the second syllabication I prop ose to use the Syllable Contact Law

Vennemann Clements

Syllable Contact Law SCL In any sequence C C C must

a b a

exceed C in sonority

b

The SCL exlcudes zbra syllabications on the grounds that the word

contains a less felicitous syllable contact We will need this constraintagain

in chapter where it will b e used to rule out certain clusters b efore

rlengthening I will now pro ceed to discuss a pro cess of Dutch

phonology which is related to the issue of vowel length lengthening and

colouring of tense vowels b efore rThisr has to b e tautomorphemic to

the vowel and furthermore has to o ccur within the same fo ot Gussenhoven

These vowels get a dierent quality b efore this consonant The

following minimal pairs are copied from Trommelen and Zonneveld



The phonological representations used here have b een somewhat idealised from the

phonetic forms

Gussenhoven has p ointed out that the domain of lengtheningcolouring seems

to b e the fo ot and that there is also a pro cess of colouring without lengthening at the level

of the phonological word cf Europa Europ e ropa Oeralisch Uralic uralis cf also

Van der Hulst I only deal with the fo otlevel pro cess and have no explanation of

the wordlevel pro cess The fact that unfo oted vowels do not lengthen seems to b e due

to the strong correlation b etween vowel length and stress

Details in the Standard Dutchvowel system

giek gig ik gier vulture giqr

beet bite b et beer b ear bqr

huub name hyp huur rent hyqr

kleum shiver klm kleur colour klyqr

boet mend but boer farmer buqr

stoof stove stof stoor disturb stqr

blaat bleat blat blaar blister blaqr

kegel cone kel kerel guy kqrl

eega sp ouse ea era id qra

ethica ethics etika Erica name qrika

fusie fusion fyzi furie fury fyqri

According to the phonetic measurements of t Hart the colouring

eect in high vowels is basicaly lengthening to overlong in mid vowels it

is lengthening plus laxing plus some diphthongization towards In the



lowvowel a the eect is hardest to observe if it can b e observed at all

Iwant to claim that the eects of colouring on high mid and lowvowels

can nevertheless get a uniform explanation in terms of the theory devel

op ed in this chapter I will takethebackmidvowel o as a paradigm case

Lengthening of vowels b efore sonorants and esp ecially b efore r is a famil

iar pro cess in many languages see for instance Giegerich on Scottish

English I will attribute the pro cess to an admittedly ad hoc constraint

against CVr a sequence of a short vowel followed by r p ending further

phonological and phonetic analysis as to why short tense vowels would b e

disallowed b efore r

RColour CVr

Because of the way lexical syllabication works in Dutch the syllablenal

vowel will always b e tense Notice that this condition gives some evidence

for a degenerate syllable analysis of sup erheavy syllables b ecause suchan

analysis makes it p ossible to treat vuur re vyqr and furie fury fy qri

in the same way

As far as I can see the analysis can only work if we recognize a lexical

p ostlexical distinction It is essential that heer lord is rst syllabied as

her in the lexicon and that this is later changed into hqr Otherwise we

cannot explain whywe prefer to add an extra ro ot no de instead of simply

laxing the vowel and syllabifying r into the co da so that wewould get

hr a nonexisting wellformed word of Dutch

Some schwalike element is inserted b etween the a and the r in manyDutch

dialects so that blaar is pronounced blar See also section for some discussion

of schwaepenthesis in these cases

Details in the Standard Dutchvowel system

What we need then is a rst lexical round of syllabication in which

her is created and a constraint which I will call Faithful and which

says that previously built syllable structure maynotbechanged I do not

see any other way to derive this same eect

The eect of this constraint should b e that the candidates to b e consid

ered all have lengthened their tense vowels b efore r they have added a

ro ot no de and made the vo calic features dep end on b oth ro ots Furthermore

these vowels have received a feature lax

ro ot ro ot

vo calic

Vplace lax

O

Wehave to explain why lengthening and laxing go hand in hand in this

case

Only lax vowels can p ossibly b e long in this system if we assume

that the twohalves of a long vowel should b e tautosyllabic This is so

b ecause long vowels automatically make a syllable heavy And according

to Connect N lax only lax vowels can head a heavy syllable

This reasoning is summarized in the following tableau

or

Candidates RColour ConnectN lax Faithful

or

oor

o or

r

p

r

I already noted that I assume therefore that the analysis given here applies

at the p ostlexical stratum The Faithfulness constraint ruling out r

the result of merely laxing the formerly tense vowel not lengthening it is a

constraint to the eect that previously built syllable structure may not b e

aected In this case o and r have b een syllabied into dierent sylla

bles at the rst lexical stratum This heterosyllabici ty relation is preferably

not changed Therefore r is preferred over r

It is necessary that RColour is ranked very low in the lexical hierarchy in order for

the underlying sequence or not to surface as r already in the lexicon

Details in the Standard Dutchvowel system

As mentioned ab ove the long lax vowels b efore r also have a tendency

to diphthongize towards I will return to this in section

Wenowhave the following explanation for the mid vowels These vowels

have to lengthen Because in this way they create heavy syllables they also

have to b ecome lax Laxing alone is not sucient b ecause syllable structure

built in previous strata has to b e satised

The phonetic eect of colouring on the lowvowel a is much harder to

discern I claim that this is just a sup ercial phonetic fact The quality

dierence b etween a and seems to b e smaller in Dutch than the dif

ference b etween other tenselax pairs Furthermore long vowels get some

slight phonetic tensing automaticallyq and a therefore are phonetically

very close This is the reason why it is hard or even imp ossible to observe

a dierence b etween the two We will see b elow that this asymmetry in the

system is even clearer in Tilburg Dutch section

The b ehaviour of the high vowels can b e understo o d in terms of already

established principles These vowels have to lengthen and in order to satisfy

ConnectN lax they also have to b ecome lax If we assume the con

straint against highlax is undominated in Dutch b ecause lax high vowels



never surface in the language this laxing is not p ossible however



high

Candidates RColour ConnectN lax

lax

ir

iir

p

iir

ri

iir

The high vowels thus are predicted to only lengthen Which is conrmed



by the ndings of t Hart

The phonetic nature of the tensing feature Wehave es

tablished that the dierence b etween Avowels and Bvowels can b est b e

describ ed in terms of a feature Ab ove I informally suggested ATR as



The symbol i is used to indicate a lax version of i



Under a slight reformulation the RColour constraint against Vr could b e made

to predict that also diphthongs are dissallowed b efore r This prediction is indeed

conrmed at least as far as wordnal p ositions are concerned for u and y ir

is disallowed in tautomorphemic words altogether indeed Trommelen and Zonneveld

argue that this diphthong colours to q The facts are somewhat complicated

and it seems dicult to work out an analysis why the colouring of diphthong is so

restricted to apply only to ir not to the other twodiphthongs

Details in the Standard Dutchvowel system

an interpretation but until now I informally used the lab el lax to refer to

this feature However quite a few other lab els have b een suggested and I

have not yet provided any argumenttofavour ATR or tense over their

comp etitors

In this section I will briey discuss some of the lab els that can b e found

in the twentieth century literature I will argue that each of these lab els

has its advantages but its problems to o My conclusion will b e that while

no conclusive argumentation is p ossible at our present state of knowledge

the strongest phonological arguments may indeed b e p ointing towards a

tongue ro ot feature

tense This probably is the lab el that is most commonly

used in the literature Unfortunately it is not very welldened indeed

the lab el tense has b een used to refer to almost all relevantvo calic prop

erties mentioned b elow De Rijk assumes that we may regarda

tense vowel as a sequenceoflaxones and uses tense to describ e what

wewould have called phonological length Very often the term is also

used to describ e what Lindau and Lab ov among others call

p eripheral This use of the term has b een common in Germanic stud

ies at least from Winteler and Sievers until Giegerich

and Bo oij a The most p ersistentproponent of the hyp othesis that

tense and ATR denote the same phonological ob ject is Halle Jakobson

and Halle Halle and Stevens Halle Halle and Clemen ts

Tense thus can b e seen as a cover term and the same is true for its logical

counterpart lax The terms themselves are of course somewhat impression

istic which makes them very useful for informal discussion as long as we

have not settled the exact nature of the distinction at hand It is just one

step less abstract than the Avowel vs Bvowel distinction I used ab ove

Its very imprecision makes the term useless in a maximally explicit theory

of sound structure however In the end tenseness should b e reduced to

one of the following features or to another one yet unknown

p eripheral Peripherality within the vowel triangle is prob

ably the most common denition of tenseness in linguistic theory Lindau

Lab ov is the only scholar who uses it as an indep endentfea

ture in recentwork Lab ov uses tense as an additional purely abstract



feature to crossclassify English vowels

The Dutchvowel triangle for instance can b e roughly drawn as follows

lax vowels are underlined



The very rst to prop ose the lab el p eripheral at all is Lindau

Details in the Standard Dutchvowel system

i y u

e o

a

In a sense the lax vowels form an inner triangle and the tense vowels an

outer triangle If a vowel is back and round its tense counterpart is even

more back and more round If a vowel is low its tense counterpart is even

lower

A lot of problems have b een raised against this interpretation of the fea

ture tense It seems to b e a comp osite feature and most tenselax contrasts

can also b e expressed in terms of the indep endently needed parameters

vowel height and backness vs frontness cf Gilb ers Furthermore

it is the only relative feature in mo dern phonological theoryWe cannot

tell of a given vowel in isolation whether it is p eripheral or not This pred

icate can only b e attributed to a vowel within avowel triangle Lass

Donegan Furthermore it is not very clear how to use this lab el to

nonarbitrarily distinguish eg b etween and y while not at the same

time distinguishin g b etween for instance i and e in exactly the same way

The lab el do es have some explanatory use with resp ect to the constraints

prop osed in this chapter If the articulatory muscles havetomovetoan

extreme p osition and syllables can just use a limited amount of time the

strong syllable cut of tense vowels can b e explained It is still dicult to

see the phonetic reason why lax vowels should o ccur in closed syllables but

none of the features discussed here can provide us with a ready explanation

for that fact

More problematic is the sp ecial b eha viour of high vowels The con

straint against the combination of high and p eripheral is dicult to

understand Esp ecially i and u are real extremes in the vowel triangle

There is no apparent reason why they could not have less extreme variants

in Dutch and many other languages while this p olarityisavailable for

roundness and backness In section we will see that the a pair

is somewhat sp ecial to o In general the strong corresp ondence b etween

vowel height and tenseness cannot b e explained if weinterpret tense as



p eripheral



The reason why Lab ov prop osed p eripherality as a feature can unfortunately

not b e discussed in this dissertation He uses it to describ e phenomena

mainly in the history of English and in mo dern American and British English dialects

The vowel shift and most asp ects of unfortunately fall b eyond the

scop e of this study

Details in the Standard Dutchvowel system

op en The reasoning ab ove leads us to the p osition of Clements

i

a who argues that the distinction in Bantu languages previously de

scrib ed in terms of tense or ATR should b e describ ed byanaperture



feature op en low and high should similarily b e replaced byin

stances of op en so that we get a scale of ap erture features The Dutch

system could under Clements a prop osal b e analysed as follows

 

op en

op en i y u



op en



 

op en

y op en



op en



 

op en

e o op en



op en



 

op en

op en



op en



 

op en

op en a



op en



Ihave assumed op en low and op en high Wethenget



to the conclusion that op en corresp onds to ATR quite surprisingly



b ecause ATR seems to b ehavelike high hence likeaop en feature

in pro jection constraints not like a op en feature suchaslow Another

problem is that there is no inherent reason to numb er the dierentvowel

heights this wayWhy shouldnt wesaythateg op en op en



op en



Indep endent of the p ossible uses of the feature in the analysis of Bantu

see however Kenstowicz a for discussion of a p ossible reanalysis of

Clements a examples in terms of high and ATRthe multiply

o ccurring feature op en seems problematic in the analysis of Dutchpre

sented here

On the other hand Clements a and Odden undeniably

make a forceful argument that tense should b e attached to a separate no de

together with the height features and to the exclusion of all other vo calic

features They show that these features spread together in some Bantu

languages Because we also established a sp ecial relation b etween vowel

height and tenseness in feature co o ccurrence restrictions I conclude that



Although I do not discuss these explicitly in the main text I think similar ob jections

can b e raised against prop osals in the framework of Dep endency Phonologysuchasthe

one in Smith et al as I raise here against op en

Details in the Standard Dutchvowel system

there is sucient evidence for an ap erture no de but that the daughters of

this no de are high low and ATR

ATR RTR Several arguments have b een broughtup

against the equation of ATR and tense One is that these features seems

to have slightly dierent phonetic reexes In particular ATR do es not

usually involve the of backvowels It remains a question whether or

not languages mayvary in their phonetic realization of a given phonological

feature Another ob jection is that in real ATR languages as sp oken for

instance in WestAfrica there is no connection b etween ATR and syllable

structure I think that the fact that a feature can have an eect in certain

languages which it do es not have in others is not necessaril y an argument

for not using that feature esp ecially not in a theory likeOTinwhich

constraints which are very strong in one language may b e ranked very low



in another language

According to Clements an imp ortant problem with the as

sumption that high ATR and low are sisters is that ATR normally

is dened in terms of the activity of a sp ecic articulator the tongue ro ot

Painter Lindau As an articulatorb ound feature wewould

exp ect it to pattern with other articulatorb ound features This means

that its most natural place is under a place no de for which assumption

Clements refers to McCarthy b The features high and lo w on

the other hand are articulatorfree features they can b e brought ab out by

movements of b oth the jaw and the tongue b o dy and ro ot

Although the feature theory sketched byClements a in whicha

sharp division is made b etween articulatorb ound and articulatorfree

features each with their own no des seems very interesting it remains to

be worked out Furthermore a precise phonetic articulatorfree denition

of op en is not provided so that wehave no exact idea of the naturalness

i

of Clements alternative Finally as Clements a himself remarks the

hierarchical nature of op en is not really mirrored in other phonological

features except p erhaps tone So whereas Clements a criticism in

itself is to the p oint and should lead to a more precise idea of the phonetic



Yet another ob jection Stewart Ramers is that whereas tongue ro ot re

traction seems to b e the marked value in Europ ean languages as wehave seen African

languages often seem to treat tongue ro ot advancement as the marked feature Several

answers are p ossible to this ob jection In the rst place it do es not seem necessarily true

that African languages only refer to ATR Archangeli and Pulleyblank analyse

several languages as having the ATR value as the active value amongst them Wolof

and Yoruba In the second place we could assume either that languages can cho ose

between having the or the value of ATR as the marked value or we can assume

that there are two complementary features ATR and RTR

Details in the Standard Dutchvowel system

and phonological nature of the features involved it is not clear whether his

own theory do es not suer from very similar problems

In my view the fact that highlow on the one hand and ATR on the

other interact with each other byway of feature co o ccurrence restrictions

has to b e accounted for by reference to the fact that all these features refer

to movements of the tongue b e it that the former two refer to the b o dy of

the tongue and ATR refers to the tongue ro ot By now familiar constraints

like a therefore get a natural explanation in phonetic terms while the

constraints in b are predicted to b e imp ossible in a grounded phonology

 

high high

low ATR a

ATR low

 

high ATR

b lowhigh

ATR low

A theory of op en cannot makethistyp e of prediction at all The equiv

alents of the forms in a as well as of those in b seem to refer to

arbitrary sets of ap erture features

 

op en op en

a op en op en



op en op en

 

op en op en

b op en op en



op en op en

In the remainder of this work I will use ATR wherever phonetic explicitnes s

is required and lax everywhere else

Extrasyllabicity and catalexis Iwillnow discuss two formal

mechanisms I have used without dening in the previous chapter catalexis

and extrasyllabicity I will briey give these twomechanisms a somewhat

more precise account in this section It is very imp ortant to dene them

without referring to underlying proso dic structure My claim is that no un

derlying proso dic syllable structure is necessary to describ e Dutchvowel

length I do refer to catalexis and extrasyllabicityhowever so these notions

should get a nonproso dic interpretation

yp es of Extrasyllabicity Ihave distinguished b etween twot

wordnal extrasyllabicity the extraprosidicity of coronal consonants and

the extrasyllabici ty of other segments

At the right p eriphery of a Proso dic Word more than one coronal voice

less obstruent can b e extrasyllabic In real monomorphemic words the



The constraint low ATR will b e shown to b e op erativeinDutch in section

Details in the Standard Dutchvowel system

maximum is toorts torch torts herfst autumn hrfst and in derived

forms vreemdst strangest vremtst but it is not clear whether this up

p er limit is an accidental fact ab out the Dutch lexicon or a consequence

of some grammatical principle I will assume that these coronal segments

are extraproso dic in a broader sense They are outside the Proso dic Word

altogether where they are licensed in some sp ecial way See Paradis and

Prunet McCarthy and Taub and references cited there for

discussion of the sp ecial status of coronals in this resp ect and others I will

not further develop the coronal extraproso dicity here

My real concern is with the other class of extrasyllabic segments which

are not necessarily coronal These are the nal segments of boom tree and

ramp disaster It is absolutely crucial that these segments are extrasyl

labic b ecause this allows us to say that tense vowels o ccur in op en syllables

only It is also crucial that these noncoronal extrasyllabics cannot cluster

otherwise wewould get unattested forms like raamp I assume these seg

ments o ccupy a nal p osition within the proso dic word These I will call

extrasyllabic whereas the coronals I call extraproso dic just to havea

convenienttermathandwhich can distinguish b etween the two classes

It seems that segments are always attached at the lowest p ossible p osi

tion In kat cat kt the t lls a rhyme p osition to satisfy ConnectN

lax In president t is still within the Proso dic Word b ecause it creates

the eect of a sup erheavy nal syllable Only in herfst where it cannot

get a regular proso dic p osition it stays outside the Proso dic Word

I already discussed the two theoretical p ossibilitie s wehaveforthede

scription of extrasyllabics One was to let these segments b e unsyllabied

altogether the second was to syllabify them into a degenerate syllable I

will cho ose the second option here even though nothing in my analysis

seems to crucially hinge on this assumption My reason for cho osing it

is stress where sup erheavy syllables b ehave as disyllabic A degenerate

syllable is a syllable with just consonant and an empty head The notion

will b e further develop ed in the next chapter For now wemight assume

the following representation where C is the consonant p osition

N

N



N

C

Because degenerate syllables are marked constituentsthey cannot b e freely

distributed over the wordwe need a pro jection constraint banning them

Details in the Standard Dutchvowel system

whichwehave already seen at work in the previous chapter

N V A branching N should have a head with vo calic Project

features

In order to make sure that degenerate syllables are in most cases disallowed

wehave to stipulate that Project N V dominates Parseconsonant

It is generally b etter to leave a consonant unparsed than to put it into a

degenerate syllable

The problem that has to b e solved now is howwecanmakesurethat

extrasyllabic segments escap e Project N V in absolutely wordnal p o

sition

In the next chapters we will see that there is quite a lot of evidence

that phonological word edges in Dutch are almost inalterable Pro cesses of

vowel ep enthesis for instance never insert a schwavowel at the edge of a

word I assume that here wehave another result of an Alignment constraint

on the edges of words McCarthy and Prince b comparing the edges

of lexical sp ecication and output form for the correct formal denition of

this constraint see chapters and app endix A

Align The edges of a word may not b e altered

If Align is ranked suciently high at least ab ove the pro jection con

straint against degenerate syllables it will blo ckwordnal deletion of con

sonants but not wordinternal deletion It will similarily blo ck insertion of

ep enthetic material to add weight to the head of the degenerate syllable

To illustrate this I will give the tableaux for some simple examples such

as maan mo on man and ma mum ma

a maan

Candidates Align ProjectN V ParseC

ma n

p

man

mane

man

b ma

Candidates Align ProjectN V

mat

p ma

Details in the Standard Dutchvowel system

The t in the last tableau is an arbitrarily chosen ep enthetic consonant such

consonants do not surface in Dutch The underlined e in the rst tableau

similarily is an arbitrarily chosen ep enthetic vowel not equal to schwa such

vowels never surface either

Since Align refers exclusively to wordedge p osition it cannot savede

generate syllables wordinternally a It cannot savemultiple stacked

degenerate syllables either b I arbitrarily assume that ProjectN

V is satised by deletion of the consonant it could also b e satised byin

sertion of a schwavowel as we will see in chapter where schwaepenthesis

is discussed

a maatka

Candidates Align ProjectN V ParseC

p

ma t ka

matka

b raamp

Candidates Align ProjectN V ParseC

ramp

ra m p

p

ra m p

ram p

ramp

Candidates Align Contiguity ProjectN V Parseconsonant

p

rmp

r m p

rm p

Iintro duced one new constraint in the last tableau Contiguity

Contiguity Given a string X  Y if there is a syllable such

that   are dominated by should b e dominated by

Contiguity helpsustocho ose rmp over r m  p

Since Align crucially refers to both word edges as we will see in the

chapters that follow one could ob ject that at least we predict degenerate

syllables to o ccur also at the left edge of the word Thus for instance we

would predict nono ccurring words suchas xvar Yet in this case there

is a way to satisfy all constraints mentioned We can insert a schwa in the

There are a few words suchas psychologie or mnemoniem whichmight b e seen

Details in the Standard Dutchvowel system

nucleus wordnally this schwawould have to b e inserted at the end of the

word thereby still violating AlignYet at the b eginning of the word the

nucleus is wordinternal and Align is not violated byschwaepenthesis

as having an extrasyllbic initial consonant but these typ e of clusters are sub ject to

very sp ecic conditions the real onset has to consist of a coronal and furthermore it

may not b e complex For the latter reason I susp ect that these clusters are exceptional

bisegmental onsets not a degenerate syllable followed by a normal syllable A language

where degenerate syllables seem to b e allowed b oth wordinitally and wordnally is

p ossibly Polish Gussmann

Details in the Standard Dutchvowel system

xvar

Candidates Align Contiguity ProjectN V ParseC

xvar

xva r

x va r

x v ar

p

var x

In this case there is a way to satisfy all constraints mentioned We can insert

aschwa in the nucleus wordnally this schwawould have to b e inserted

at the end of the word thereby still violating Alignbutwordinitially the

schwa is inserted after the consonantsothatAlign is not violated

The preference for the edges of Proso dic Words to remain unaltered

has the eect in Dutch that consonants are put in a degenerate syllable

at the end of a word only This explains the distribution of sup erheavy

syllables in Dutch which o ccur only at the end of the word This topic in

connection to the b ehaviour of syllables headed byschwa will b e discussed

in the next chapter

Catalexis Inow turn to the other phenomenon that is related

to the p eriphery of words catalexisWe need this device to explain i

whyCVwords still satisfy Proso dic Minimalityandiihowwords with

exceptional wordnal stress are marked in the lexicon

In the original prop osal of Kiparsky catalexis was an underlying

mora or syllable without features attached to it CV words in this way

could b e seen as bimoraic or bisyllabic and nal syllables could b e marked

as b eing sup erheavy thereby attracting stress

Clearly the option of underlying moras or syllables is not compatible

with the assumptions whichIhave made until now Up to this p oint I

have b een able to stick to the null hyp othesis that no proso dic structure

is underlying Notice however that nothing excludes underlying bare ro ot

no des These ro ot no des have the same eect as underlying moras Supp ose

they are placeless obstruents which means they will b e represented only

as a cons sonorant ro ot I assume this will b e interpreted as one of



the empty consonants h or b A similar prop osal has b een made by

Dunlap for Spanish whichalsohasatrochaic system and wordnal

stress and for which catalectic analyses have b een prop osed as well see also

Nouveau and for Italian Vagra



On the emptiness of h and b see chapter

Details in the Standard Dutchvowel system

Because they are obstruents they will surface as degenerate syllables

after heavy syllables I give an example tableau for ma mum b elow this

tableau supplements b ab ove it do es not replace it

Details in the Standard Dutchvowel system

ma

Candidates Align ProjectN V ParseC

p

mah

ma h

Aword like chocola can also get a nal h in underlying representation

so that the nal fo ot will b e bisyllabic This h is actually phonetically

realised in the case of words with a nal stressed op en syllable cf No ote

b o om

The question remains how the nal syllable in mah is treated by the

phonetic comp onent It might b e that a syllable without any place sp eci

cation at all is simply left uninterpreted On the other hand esp ecially in

wordnal p osition after Avowels a phonetic h can b e observed

Another question that arises is whether this line of reasoning do es not

destroy our explanation of the dierence b etween tense and lax vowels al

together Whyismh not p ermitted to surface as a word The answer

is that h can never o ccur in co dap osition in Dutch not wordinternally

and not at the p eriphery It can only o ccur in onset p osition This fact has



to b e stipulated byany theory in one way or another I prop ose to do it

by the following constraint

CodahAhmay only o ccur in an onset

CodaHParselax

FinallyIwant to discuss one prediction I make If these words with nal

stress really end in a catalectic consonant wewould exp ect this consonant

to have other eects as well Indeed one such eect can b e found in an

area not directly related to stress viz inectional morphology

Adjectives take an inectional ending in some syntactic contexts for

instance in indenite DPs of common gender This inectional ending only

surfaces when the word in question ends in a consonant not when it ends



in an unstressed vowel



In a theory of phonological pro jection the constraint Codah mightbeseenasthe

result of the interaction of twoweakness constraints Weak N C the weakness con

straint requiring weak p ositions in rhymes to have at least some consonantal feature

apart from cons son would b e very strong dominating for instance ParseC

N C would b e muchweaker and b e dominated by for instance Parse whereas Weak

C Since I consider the whole matter not suciently imp ortant I will not go into it any

further



For some of these cases the lackofovert inection might also b e due to totally

dierent factors For instance it mightalsobethattheword albino is more nounlike

than real adjectives and therefore not able to carry overt inection Henk van Riemsdijk

A dialect with real length Tilburg Dutch

a een jn tijd

a happy time

b een albino man

an albino man

c een mica tafel

a micaceous table

d een sexy dame

a sexy lady

Words ending in a stressed vowel and monosyllables ending in a vowel

b ehave as if they were ending in a consonant in this resp ect as predicted

They are the only vowelnal words that takea inection

a een weej geur

a sickly smell

b een continuw stro om

a continuous ow

It is hard to see whywords ending in stressed vowels would b ehavelike

words ending in consonants if we do not adopt a theory of catalectic con

sonants

A dialect with real length Tilburg Dutch

This section deals with vowel length and vowel shortening in the Tilburg

dialect of Dutch Tilburg Dutch has a much larger set of vowels than

Standard Dutch Two prop erties of the Tilburg vowel set are of sp ecial

interest to us In the rst place it lls in a few gaps in the Dutchvowel

system since it has low rounded vowels In the second place it seems to

have a real length distinction on top of the tenseness distinction familiar

from the standard language Furthermore it has some phonological and

morphological pro cesses that seem to refer to length I will showinthis

section that even in Tilburg long vowels haveavery limited distribution

Ihave collected my data on Tilburg mainly from the dictionary of Van

Rijen and from the dialectological study of Boutkan The

pro cesses describ ed here are not unique to the Tilburg dialect however

They are quite widespread in the Brabant area which ranges approx

imately from Brussels Belgium in the south to sHertogenb osch The

pc nounlike adjectives ending in consonants suchasgenoeg sucient are not able

to carry inection either The same is not true for a form suchassexy whichisaclear

adjective The set of data mighthowever b e considered to o small to settle the p oint

A dialect with real length Tilburg Dutch

Netherlands in the north and in its broadest sense includes the provinces

NorthBrabant in the Netherlands Antwerp Flemish Brabant and the

EastFlemish Dender area Belgium See for instance Zonneveld on

Brussels Dutch Keymeulen and Taeldeman on Hofstade Dutch and

Nuyts on Antwerp en Dutch Furthermore traces of the pro cess can

also b e found in Standard Dutch and Standard German

Below I will rst describ e the Tilburg vowel set in some detail Subse

quently I will giveanoverview of the various lengthening and shortening

phenomena attested in the dialect After this I will give an analysis of

Tilburg I will show that vo calic length has a very limited distribution and

that the Tilburg system helps us dene the Standard Dutchvowel system

in a more principled way In section I briey discuss the b ehaviour of

long vowels in some other Brabant dialects In section I will discuss

the limited distribution of long vowels in b oth Standard Dutch and the

Tilburg dialect

The key hyp othesis will b e that Tilburg has the same typ e of syllable

structure as Standard Dutch in spite of its somewhat dierentvowel sys

tem Also the stress system and the relation b etween syllable structure

and stress seems to b e the same in the two dialects Ideally we therefore

wanttohave an analysis of Tilburg proso dic structure which is maximally

close to the theory wehave for Standard Dutch There is one minor su

p ercial dierence b etween the two dialects whichIwant to note at the

outset Tilburg Dutc h has a small numb er of functional items suchaswe

what w and de that d which end in the lax vowel and sometimes

or Historically these forms are derived from words ending in a t

dt wt Synchronically I have no explanation for this fact

The vowel system The Tilburg dialect of Dutch has dierent



vowels The consonant system of Tilburg Dutch is exactly the same

as in the Standard language Weijnen All vowels of Standard

Dutch are found in this dialect but in addition we nd long versions of

the lax vowels and a height distinction in the lax rounded vowels Wecan

divide the Tilburg vowel system in groups the tense vowels the short lax



vowels the long lax vowels the dorsal vowels a and schwa All

of these vowels have distinctive status According to Boutkan the

only vowels for which it is dicult to nd minimal pairs are and yq



WhileIwas completing this thesis I b ecame acquainted with the work of Boutkan

and Kossmann who give an ample overview of Tilburg phonology I refer the interested

reader to that work for a list of minimal pairs showing convincingly that eachofthese

vowels has a phonemic status in the language



The term dorsal vowel is meanttoputthesetwovowels apart from the rest of the

vowel system I will argue b elow that low is not an adequate term to set this pair apart

A dialect with real length Tilburg Dutch

He mentions rook smoke V ryqk and reuk smell rk as the only pair

he found Boutkan do es not discuss diphthongs Van Rijen

claims that older sp eakers of Tilburg do not haveanydiphthongs Younger

sp eakers havedevelop ed a j and au presumably under the inuence of

Standard Dutchi and u Yet here I will concentrate on the diphthong

less variant of the Tilburg dialect I will now briey discuss eachoftheve

subsets Where I give Tilburg words in non I follow

the conventions of Van Rijen

Nondorsal tense vowels The set of nondorsal tense vowels

of Tilburg Dutch is exactly the same as that of Standard Dutch mo dulo

some minor phonetic dierences I listed the six vowels in this set in



Belowevery vowelIgive an example word and its gloss

front back

unrounded rounded

i y u

plisi yprt sjurt

p olice man b ed he lo oks

e o

me sl lotr

with forest meadow draw lots

The set can b e further divided into front unrounded i e front rounded

y and back rounded u o or alternatively high i y u andmide

ovowels

According to Van Rijen there is also a set of overlong high

tense vowels fiq yq uqgYet these vowels seem to b e in complementary

distribution with the normal high tense vowels The long variants o ccur

b efore tautosyllabic r and the other variants in all other contexts I will

ignore this contrast in the rest of this section see section ab ovefora

discussion of a similar pro cess in Standard Dutch

Nondorsal short lax vowels The next group to consider are

the nondorsal lax vowels In Standard Dutch this subpart of the system has

two gaps as compared to the tense vowel set There are just two rounded

vowels one fronty and the other one back Standard Dutch

makes no height distinction in this part of the system These two gaps are



The fact that I call these vowels nondorsal is purely descriptive As a matter of

fact the back labial vowelsofcoursearesupposedtohave a dorsal comp onent

A dialect with real length Tilburg Dutch

lled in as it were in the Tilburg dialect which has six nondorsal short lax

vowels Compare to

front back

unrounded rounded

t dr rms

eats through Roman Catholic

d bk sxp

that I sto op sheep

If we compare to it might seem that they are exactly parallel

Because the vowels in are high and mid the vowels in could then

also b e classied as high and mid I will argue b elow however that this is

not the case y and in Tilburg Dutch as in Standard Dutch are mid

vowels and are low

Technically we can then get the relevantvowel system by the following

relative rankings in Standard Dutch and Tilburg Dutch



low

a Standard Dutch Parselow or Parselabial

labial



low

b Tilburg Dutch Parselow Parselabial

labial

In Standard Dutch the constraint against low rounded vowelsissostrong

that it can cause at least one of the two incompatible features to disapp ear

In Tilburg Dutch on the other hand it is more imp ortanttokeep b oth

low and labial in the output than it is to av oid low rounded vowels

With resp ect to language acquisition wecansay the following I assume

that in the initial stage all faithfulness constraints are ranked very low and

all other constraints very high With resp ect to the three constraints under

discussion the Standard language therefore represents the unmarked case

A Tilburg child at a certain p oint will hear low rounded vowels and from

those she can conclude the reverse ranking that is needed for her dialect

Nondorsal long lax vowels The most dramatic dierence

between Standard Dutch and Tilburg Dutch is found in the realm of long

lax vowels Standard Dutch has only three of these vowels qin serre

sqr qin freule frql and qin zone zqn These vowels

haveavery limited distribution and are standardly considered to b e loan

A dialect with real length Tilburg Dutch

phonemes of Dutch b ecause they only o ccur in wordsofFrench origin I

will return to these Dutchvowels b elow

This typeofvowel o ccurs much more abundantly in Tilburg however

In this dialect we nd long lax vowels in a lot of lexical items not only

those of French origin Apart from that wehave a height distinction for all

three dierent places of articulation so that all in all we get the following

picture

front back

unrounded rounded

q yq q

blqk knyqp sxqn

app eared button b eautiful

q q q

rqm jqn klqr

rhyme onion ready

Phonetically what distinguishes these vowels form the ones in unam

biguously is length

In Standard Dutch all frontlongvowels are low lexically wehaveq

but not q The dierence b etween Standard Dutch and Tilburg Dutch

thus might b e accounted for in the following way



a Standard Dutch ProjectN low low



b Tilburg Dutch lowProjectN low



The constraint ProjectN low requires long vowels to b e supp orted by

a feature low In Standard Dutch this constraint is undominated so that

all long vowels surface as low In Tilburg Dutch on the other hand lowering

of all long vowels is blo cked by the high ranking of the constraint against

insertion of low

Again the Standard dialect can b e assumed to b e the unmarked initial

stage of the language acquisition pro cess and a Tilburg child knows that it

has to reverse the ranking of the relativeFaithfulness constraint vis avis



ProjectN low once it hears a long nonlowvowel

Dorsal vowels It seems that for all nondorsal vowel articu

lations wehave at least three p ossibilitie s in Tilburg Dutch tense short

lax and long lax The dorsal vowels fall somewhat outside of this system

however here wehave only twovariants a and Phonetically a could

be characterized as somewhat longer than but we can also see a as

tense and as lax All in all wehavetwo dorsal vowels instead of three

A dialect with real length Tilburg Dutch

tense long lax short

a

kans s

chance if

Wehave seen that the a pair b ehaves in a similarly exceptional way

b efore tautomorphemic r in Standard Dutch Unlike other tense vowels a

do es not seem to change colour in that environment at all My explanation

there was that q and a are phonetically to o similar This seems to b e

conrmed here in the sense that also in Tilburg where length and laxness

combine as phonetic features much more freely still no distinction can b e



made b etween q and a

The analysis of the a pair is further complicated by an idiosyncratic

rule of Tilburg which tenses the dorsal b efore nt mp and k braand

re brant brnt laand land lant lnt schaand shame sxant

sxnt raamp disaster ramp rmp baank bank bak bk and

xt aacht eight axt xt naacht night naxt nxt The Standard

language has the lax varian t in all this cases The precise explanation for

these pro cesses remains to b e found

Stro op discusses similar more general pro cesses of vowel length

ening b efore homorganic nasalobstruent clusters in southern Dutchas

comp ensatory lengthening A nasal which is homorganic with an obstru

ent lo oses its indep endent place features b ecause it also slightly nasalizes

the preceding vowel it might seem to disapp ear altogether and the vowel

can take up its place We might also see a parallel esp ecially for the axt

clusters in Belfast Philadelphia and New York State tensing Fergu

son Dunlap Harris Lab ov which is triggered

by nasals fricatives and voiced stops It is still unclear however i why

in Tilburg only a are aected ii what uniquely and nontrivially



characterizes the nt mp k and xt contexts

Ileave these problems op en for further research since they seem to b e

suciently indep endent of syllable structure It will b e clear that all these

considerations taken together makea a less suitable pair for testing



In most variants of German the alternation b etween tense and lax a has dis

app eared Vater Also the transcription and the phonetic realisation of tenselax

pairs is in some languages sometimes reversed Vater uses for the tense vowel

and a for the lax vowel I follow the Dutch tradition in this thesis



Ihave argued ab ove that Standard Dutch and Tilburg Dutch twaalf twelve mayb e

the result of a similar atensing pro cess extending also to haalf in Tilburg Dutchbut

not in the Standard dialect The f in this case is the result of Final Devoicing of an

underlying v which surfaces in the plural twaalven In the next chapter wewillseethat

mayb e in Standard Dutch the context rz is also a trigger for tensing of a

A dialect with real length Tilburg Dutch

the morphologically triggered shortening and lengthening pro cesses under

discussion I will therefore put them apart until further discussion of their

status in the Tilburg vowel system p

Schwa The nal vowel of the Tilburg vowel system is schwa

It stands on its own b eing essentially featureless

schwa

rm

arm

I will not discuss the b ehaviour of Tilburg schwainthischapter I assume

it to b e basically the same as the b ehaviour of schwa in Standard Dutch



which will b e discussed in the next chapter

Why only lax vowels can b e long The rst question that

arises with resp ect to the Tilburg vowel set is why it is asymmetric in the

way it is Why is there a length contrast in the lax vowels but not in the

tense vowels

As a matter of fact this typ e of asymmetry is predicted by the constraint

ConnectN lax as wehave seen in the discussion of rcolouring Supp ose

that wehave a long vowel ie a vo calic no de linked to tworootnodes

o o

vocalic

Assuming that the two parts of the long vowel can only b e syllabied tauto

sylabically they cannot each head their own syllable the resulting struc

ture will always include a branching Nnode

N

o o

vo calic



However the Tilburg schwashows up one somewhat unexp ectedcharacteristic We

nd at least one word eweg away wx as a counterexample to the claim made for

Standard Dutch that no lexical word starts wit schwa

A dialect with real length Tilburg Dutch

Nbranches and therefore ConnectN lax demands that the head should

dominate a feature lax Therefore if there is any asymmetry at all we

predict that this head is lax

There is however one p ossible exception In some dialects also the high

tense vowels can have long variants This may b e true for Tilburg Dutch

and Standard Dutchwhichhavesomeloanwords suchascentrifuge id

sntrifyqz and team id tiqm It certainly is true for Limburg dialects

where there is a relatively large numb er of long high vowels Weijnen

Hermans a The interesting thing ab out this is that exactly

the high vowels do not have lax counterparts If we assume that in these

dialects the constraint against the combination of high of lax dominates

ConnectN lax wegettheright result cf the table in Wewill

see indep endent evidence for this constraint ranking b elow

Vowel shortening In the immediately preceding section I have

shown that Tilburg Dutchhasvowel length In this section I will showhow

long vowels b ehave in morphological contexts I will show that quite a few

morphological pro cesses involve shortening Indeed this seems to b e true

for all pro ductive inectional suxation pro cesses After suxation long

lax vowels shorten and tense vowels turn lax I will rst discuss some of

the relevant data An analysis will b e attempted in section

Diminutives The diminutive form of nouns is derived byadd

ingasuxktjsk to the stem the feature content of the con

sonant dep ends on the stemnal segment Long vowels shorten in the

pro cess

a baosboske b oss bqsbsk

b straotstrotje street strqtstrtj

c kn eenknentj rabbit knqnknntj

d beenbintje leg bqnbntj

e bookbokske b elly byqkbyksk

f steelstiltje shaft stelstltj stltj

g haandhendje hand hanthntj

As can b e seen in g Tilburg Dutch also shows some traces of an umlaut

pro cess in diminutives The pro cess nowadays seems to b e highly morphol

ogized in this dialect and applied to a restricted set of stems only I will

not discuss the prop erties of the umlaut pro cess here cf Nijen Twilhaar

Wetzels and Bennink in prep and section of this

A dialect with real length Tilburg Dutch

dissertation for discussion of Dutch umlaut in dialects where it is more

pro ductive and also of the ktjks alternations

If we concentrate on the nonumlauting forms we see that long lax

vowels shorten to their short lax variants ae More interesting is

the b ehaviour of the tense vowel e in f The fact that this vowel

changes to in the diminutive probably has to b e seen as an instance of

the shortening pro cess that applies to the lax vowels Because tense vowels

in closed syllables do not exist the vowel changes to a lax version at the

same Two p ossibilitie s are logically p ossible and

Wewould exp ect the to o ccur nowifwe assumed b oth e and

are mid vowels whereas is high Yet the ischosen here not the

This once again shows that is the mid lax vowel and islow

This raises the question what happ ens to the other tense vowels if they

o ccur in a shortening environment Consider the following examples

a liesliesje oatgrass lislisj

b tuuttuutje tire tyttytj

c boekboekske b o ok bukbuksk

d deukdokske dent dkdksk

e sloopslopke pillowcase slopslpk

The mid vowels all change to a lax variant changes to y and o to

which gives us an argument to consider also these vowels as mid and

and as low

The high tense vowels do not shorten at all It may seem therefore that

these vowels count as short in the system

Comparatives and sup erlatives Wenow turn our attention

to the comparative forms We do not nd shortening uniformly in the

comparative paradigm As a matter of fact we only nd shortening in

those cases where an ep enthetic d is inserted b etween the stem and the

comparativesuxer r This happ ens when the stem ends in l r or

n The same applies to Standard Dutch but only when the stem ends in

r

a schoonschonder b eautiful sxqnsxndr

b feenfender ne fqnfndr

c zwaorzworder heavy zwqrzwrdr

d geelgilder yellow elldr

e grootgrotter big rqtrtr

A dialect with real length Tilburg Dutch

f hooghogger high hqhr

The forms e and f are exceptional They are the only forms with

shortening but without d ep enthesis It is very dicult to nd adjectives

ending in tense vowel fl r ng but from the few forms wehave wecan

conclude that the comparatives b ehavelike the diminutives in this resp ect

Shortening o ccurs also in sup erlatives The sup erlative sux is st and we

nd o ccasional forms like schonst and fenst The pro cess do es not seem to

b e pro ductive The o ccurrence of shortening dep ends on the frequency of

the adjective The more frequent the adjective the more likely shortening

b ecomes

Ordinal numbers The next forms to consider are the ordinal

numb ers Only the rst ten numb ers need to b e considered b ecause all

other numb ers either end in tiendewhich is already in the list or end in

a sux that is irrelevant b ecause its nal vowel is already short or schwa

tig ty honderd hundred duzend thousand etc

a eenirste one qn rste

b tweetwidde two twetwd

c driedriede three dridrid

d viervierde four viqrviqrd

e veefvefde ve vqfvfd

f zeszesde six zszsd

g zevezevende seven zevzevnd

h aachtaachtste eight axtaxtst

i negenegende nine nenend

j tientiende ten tintind

Only the rst ordinal is formed in a somewhat irregular way in Tilburg but

all other forms are derived byaddingde to the cardinal numb er Although

eavery limited set of examples here vowel shortening of course wehav

b ehaves exactly as wehave come to exp ect the long lax vowel b ecomes

short the tense e b ecomes in twidde the high and low tense vowels

are not aected by shortening and neither is e if it is protected by a nal

schwa

A dialect with real length Tilburg Dutch

Verba prsentia sg Apart from the diminutives verbal

inection oers the most pro ductive paradigm of vowel shortening Below

Igive the innitive and the p erson singular form of a selected number of

verbs and the complete paradigm of the present tense of eten eat

a ooteot whistle yqtt

b keekekekt lo ok kqkkkt

c slaopeslopt sleep slqpslpt

d koopekopt buy kqpkpt

e eteit eat ett

f koomekomt come komkmt

g speulespult play splspylt

h begienebegient b egin binbint

i zuukezuukt lo ok for zykzykt

j roetseroetst run rutsrutst

k verbraandeverbraant burn vrbrandvrbrant

singular plural

k et wet

t g t

h t zet

According to Boutkan wealways nd shortening b efore fk p t l m

n g the Tilburg dialect has Final Devoicing We nd it incidentally b efore

the fricatives ffgrgr is called a fricativeby Boutkan b ecause of its

uvular pronunciation in the Tilburg dialect I have no real explanation for

this dierence b etween fricatives and other segments It might b e that again

wehave a reex of tensing b efore fricatives also observed in Philadelphia

English and for axt clusters in Tilburg ab ove This tensing tendency would

sometimes b e stronger than shortening Below I will ignore the cases b efore

the fricatives and r and concentrate on the cases where shortening do es

o ccur

Other categories In the previous subsections wehave seen

that long lax vowels and mid tense vowels shorten in inected forms if we

take inection to b e a rather broadly dened category including diminu

tives if the inectional sux starts with a consonant There are a few

residual categories to consider where we also nd shortening pro cesses

First of all there is a class of lexicalised comp ounds as I will provi

sionally call them These are comp ounds whichhave acquired a sp ecialize d

A dialect with real length Tilburg Dutch

meaning and therefore might b e analysed as constituting a single entry in

the lexicon

a hooshosbaos houselandlord hyqshysbqs

b gereetgeritschap readyinstruments rqtrtsxp

c spaoresporspot savemoney b ox spqrsprspt

d toontonman gardengardener tyqntynmn

With newlyformed comp ounds we do not normally nd shortening Com

pare for instance d to the relatively new form toonsentrum garden cen

tre which is probably not lexicalized and which is pronounced as tyqnsntrum

and not tynsntrum On the other hand it is not crystal clear what ex

actly counts as a lexicalise d comp ound For instance while we do nd

shortening in erbezie strawb erry rb ezi lit earth b erry wedonot

nd it in eerpel p otato qrpl litt earth apple It is unclear what

constitutes the essential dierences b etween these two examples

Another category where we nd incidental shortening are prep ositions

and prexes like the following



a neevenee next to nevnf

b aonon on qnn

c ootot out yqtyt

It is however very unclear what the p ossible conditions could b e on the

shortening pro ces applying to these forms b oth in derived words and in

prep ositional phrases It is p ossible that there is a relation to sentence

stress Furthermore shortening seems to b e optional in most cases For

this reason I assume these prexesprep ositions to havetwo lexical forms

one with a long vowel and one with a short one

FinallyIwanttomention the form keend kender child children

ndr The er plural sux is the only sux that do es not start kqntk

with a consonant but nevertheless triggers shortening in kender On the

other hand er is not the pro ductive morpheme for expressing the plural As

a matter of fact it only o ccurs on one or two forms aajer eggs Again I

assume that in this case the plural form is lexicalise d There is no shortening

b ecause the short lax vowel is underlying



The vf alternation in this form probably is connected to a lter excluding V CV



morphemes where V is a lax vowel and C v or z see p

A dialect with real length Tilburg Dutch

Analysis In this section I will showhow Tilburg ts into the

theory established earlier in this chapter Four problems will havetobe

dealt with In the rst place given the p ossibilityofoverlong syllables why

do es shortening o ccur at all In the second place whydoshorttensevowels

shorten along with the long lax ones Thirdlyhow should we classify the

dierentvowels in a suciently constrained feature theory And nally

do wehave a case of real underlying length here These questions will b e

dealt with one by one b elow

Shortening in derived contexts Wehave seen that long lax

and tense vowels shorten if the syllable in which they o ccur gets overlong

b ecause of some morphological pro cess For most suxes this is not prob

lematic b ecause they consitute syllables If we assume a sux syllable

gets incorp orated in the proso dic word of the stem the stemnal degener

ate syllable loses its p eripheral p osition after suxation As wehave seen



ab ove degenerate syllables can only o ccur at the end of a word

The verbal sux t causes more problems Take for instance the verb

slaope sleep slqp The stem of this form slqp consists of a sylla

ble slq plus a degenerate syllable p If we attach a t this cannot b e

incorp orated into the proso dic structure of the base I leaveouttheless

imp ortan t details in the following tree

N N



N N

s l p t

For reasons discussed ab ove there can only b e one degenerate syllable p er

word the t cannot even get its own degenerate syllable

If we shorten the vowel the p can b e incorp orated into the rhyme

and the t can get its own degenerate syllable

N N



N N

s l p t



The problem therefore is rather with Standard Dutch This reasoning seems to

indicate that inectional suxes seem to have some cliticlikestatusinthislanguage

from a phonological p oint of view

A dialect with real length Tilburg Dutch

This notion of an overlong syllable turns out to b e not very easy to for

malise however First of all we nd shortening also in forms like etenit

to eat It is not very clear why et would b e to o long Even if e do es not

allow consonants in the rhyme b ecause of ConnectN lax we could still

make t extrasyllabic or extraproso dic It is dicult to see what would b e

wrong with this structure

Given that we nd lexical words in Tilburg like kaort map kqrt

with an extrasyllabic r and extraproso dic t as discussed for Dutchin

the previous section section it is even questionable that shortening

should have a p ositive eect in slopt you sleep slpt from slaopen

slqp

It seems as if the nal t of a stem can b e extraproso dic while this



option is not op en if the t formsasuxofitsown in Tilburg We

will also havetotakeinto account that Standard Dutchdoesnotmakea

dierence b etween derived and underived forms in this resp ect the second

p erson singular form of to sleep is slaapt pronounced as slapt with

tense long a

The standard pro cedure of accounting for this typ e of variation in

constraintbased phonology is to assume there is a constraintwhichhas

sucient strength in one dialect but is less strong in the other I prop ose

the relevant constraint in this case is one whichsays that every morpheme

should b e parsed into proso dic structure at least partially

MorPaAt least one element the most sonorous element of a

morpheme is incorp orated into a proso dic word

We will return to this constraint several times in the chapters that follow

I prop ose MorPa is ranked rather high in the Braban t dialects while

it has sunk to irrelevance in Standard Dutch In particular in Brabant

the constraintisranked higher than the constraintonvowelparsing In

the following brackets indicate Proso dic Word b oundaries since Standard

Dutchdoesnothavevowel length I assumed the relevantinteracting con

straint here is the one against insertion of lax in Tilburg Dutch it is the

constraint ParseV requiring cons ro ots to surface

Tilburg Dutch MorPa ParseV lax

a slaopt slqpt



The pro cess is reminiscentofthevarious vowel shortening pro cesses in English which

also apply in derived environments onlyMyers has argued that all of these

shortening rules are instances of one pro cess repairing overlong syllables See Prince

Borowsky Yip for other fairly recent treatments of the English

phenomena

A dialect with real length Tilburg Dutch

Candidates MorPa ParseV

p

sl q pt

slqpt

b kaort kqrt

Candidates MorPa ParseV

k q rt

p

kqrt

c kneupt knpt

Candidates MorPa lax

knpt

knpt

d werkt

Candidates MorPa ParseV

p

wrkt

Standard Dutch laxMorPa

a slaapt

Candidates MorPa lax

slpt

p

slapt

b kaart

Candidates MorPa lax

krt

p

kart

c werkt

Candidates MorPa ParseVowel

p

wrkt

In this case wemight assume that the Tilburg Dutch ranking is unmarked

reecting the initial state of Universal Grammar The Standard Dutch

child learns to rerank the constraint lax once it observes that the vowel

in a morphologically complex form like slaapt is not laxed contrary to exp ectation

A dialect with real length Tilburg Dutch

The question still remains what is the explanation for the dierence

between the rst p erson singular stqt and the third p erson singular stt

of the verb stoote to push stqt Why is the t allowed in one form to

remain outside of the proso dic word and why should it incorp orate in the

other form

Zonneveld has argued that Dutch has an empty theme vowel in

the case of the rst p erson singular This vowel could in Tilburg Dutchallow

the t to incorp orate into an onset in this form The same is not p ossible in

the third p erson singular b ecause that form do es not have a theme vowel

This analysis cannot b e adopted in the approach suggested here b ecause it

is unclear what the nature of the empty theme vowel would b e As wewill

see in the next chapter an emptyvowel ro ot no de is interpreted as schwa

and it is imp ossible to b e more empty than an emptyroot

Fortunately it is not necessary to assume this typ e of emptynodeeither

since the facts already follow from the assumptions made plus the fact that

in the case of the third and second p erson singular a sux t is added

a stoot st p erson

Candidates MorPa ParseVowel

st q t

p

stqt

b stott nddperson

Candidates Geminate MorPa ParseVowel

st q tt

p

st q  t t

st q t t 

st q t t 

stqtt

I added a constraint against geminate consonants in b Tilburg Dutch

do es not have geminate consonants under any circumstances and certainly

not at the end of the word so wemay safely assume this constraint has a

very high ranking

The b ehaviour of tense vowels Iwillnow discuss the b e

haviour of tense vowels in shortening pro cesses Given the nature of these

vowel the term shortening is of course rather infelicitous but I will nev

ertheless stick to it as a descriptive term Iwillnow rst explain the

b ehaviour of the mid vowels and then I will discuss the high vowels

A dialect with real length Tilburg Dutch

As wehave seen the mid vowels e and o change to y and

resp ectivel y The reason why they b ecome lax should bynowbeobvious

In the form neemt takes the t is forced into the proso dic word and

therefore the m is forced into the co da of the ne syllable This syllable

b ecomes heavy and b ecause of ConnectN lax its head should b e lax

Boutkan oers a diachronic explanation for some of the facts

discussed here The alternation b etween e which is the most common

alternation b etween mid tense and high lax vowels according to Boutkan is

explained by reference to According to Van Helten the

Wachtendonck Psalms a translation of the psalms which at presentisour

main source for Old named after a th century owner of

the only copywehave had the following verbal paradigm for the present

tense

Verbal paradigm in the Wachtendonck Psalms

singular plural

on un

is itet

it untontint

Furthermore the ProtoGermanic rule whichchanged an e to i b efore

a syllable containing i or j was still op erative in Old Dutch Wecan

therefore construct the following paradigm for gevan give in the language

of the Wachtendonck Psalms

e i ij

Paradigm of gevan in the Wachtendonck Psalms

singular plural

gevon gevun

givis givit

givit gevunt

Although the i of the sux disapp eared in later stages of Dutch some



dialects like Tilburg retained the i in the stem The alternation has

later b een generalized to a rule all tense vowels in the dierent

p ersons

Regardless of the value of this as a diachronic explanation we cannot

explain the synchronic facts of Tilburg DutchthiswayHowdoesthechild

which presumably do es not knowtheWachtendo ck Psalms acquire the pro

ductive pro cess of e alternation in the dierent inectional paradigms



We nd the same alternation in some Standard German verbs as well gebengibt

give nehmennimmt take etc

A dialect with real length Tilburg Dutch

Furthermore the alternation in presentday Tilburg can also b e found in

lexical comp ounds even if no i is present in the second stem at all Also

synchronically the pro cess is no longer restricte d to the e pair which

is another unexplained fact under the diachronic analysis Finallyitisnot



clear why the raising pro cess should b e restricted to tense vowels

It seems more reasonable to assume that simply is the lax variantof

e yofand of o The upshot of this is that and are low

vowels Before I will delveinto the consequences of this nding I prop ose

to rst take a closer lo ok at the high vowels which nishes our study of the

eects of shortening on all classes of vowels

As wehave seen i y u do not alternate in shortening environments

a hebegint he starts

b he styrt he steers

c he vlukt he curses

The reason for this is that the constraint against lax high is inviolable in

Tilburg as it is in Standard Dutch On the other hand MorPa still forces

the stem nal segmentinto the co da The constraint that gets violated is

Connect N lax

b egient



lax

Candidates MorPa ConnectN lax

high

bint

bnt

p

bint

The system should of course work in the same way for words with high

vowels other than y and u

The Tilburg vowel system Wehavenow established the fol

lowing vowel system for Tilburg Dutch disregarding length for a moment

ATR ATR

high i y u

mid e o y

low



Ihave to admit that there is one verb mentioned by Boutkan freete to eat abun

dantly frqt of which the second and third p erson forms is frt although the form

frt is also found This seems to b e an isolated case however which therefore in my

view should b e treated as an exception mayb e on a par with umlaut cases suchas

stadsteden cities

A dialect with real length Tilburg Dutch

I assume features can combine freely but with resp ect to ap erture features

the following two output conditions apply



lax

a

high

b low lax

Wehave seen that b oth constraints are wellmotivated phonetically A

problem is that wenowhavetota in the system These vowels

are standardly assumed to b e low Phonetically they are certainly lower

than If weintro duce a fourth vowel height distinction we get the

strange situation that b holds for midlowvowels but not for really low

ones This would weaken the phonetic motivation for such a constraintto

a large extent

I therefore prop ose to distinguish the a pair from the other vowels

not by an ap erture feature but by a place feature These are the only

vowels that are back but not rounded Adding place features to we

get the following picture

ATR

 

cor dor s

cor dors

lab lab

high i y u

mid e o a

low a

ATR

 

cor dor s

cor dors

lab lab

high

mid y

low

The fact that there are no high unrounded dorsal vowels p ossibly has the

phonetic motivation that dorsal vowels can only b e pro duced by op ening

the mouth to a considerable degree or that raising the back of the tongue

involves tongue ro ot retraction The question whether is mid or lowis

more dicult to answer Mayb e it is most reasonable to saythatitislow

b ecause we do not want to p ostulate a constraint against the dorsal low

combination b ecause that combination seems phonetically so desirable and

therefore a constraint banning it is unlikely to haveuniversal status If is

lowwe only have to p osit which is not an unnatural constraint Stan

dard Dutch has basically the same system b e it that it has an additional

A dialect with real length Tilburg Dutch

highranking constraint against labial lowonlyoverridden in the case

of diphthongs

dorsal low

If this constraintisranked b elow the constraint banning labial low com

binations we get the right result

Inowturntothevowel length parameter First I have already ex

plained whyonlylaxvowels can b e long I assume that the parts of a

long vowel can only b e tautosyllabic If this is true long vowels always

are in heavy syllables therefore by ConnectN lax they should b e lax

The only tense vowels that could p ossibly b e lengthened without laxing

dep ending a little bit on the ranking of constraints are the high vowels

b ecause they have an indep endent constraintpreventing them from laxing

Indeed it is the case that a few English and French loan words can b e

found whichhave these long high vowels Smith et al team id

tiqm centrifuge dryer sntrifyzj etc There are absolutely no such



words with nonhigh tense vowels

In Standard Dutch three vowels are lax and long These are the vowels

in the th column of the table in on page whichIhave neglected

until now I prop ose to analyse these as q qandq resp ectivel yThe

latter ones are normally analysed as yqandq b ecause it is assumed that

long vowels havetobevariants of existing short vowels In a phonologi

cal theory of interacting and comp eting constraints this assumption is no

longer necessary The representations q and q seem closer to the pho

netic reality I assume there is a constraintwhichsays that doubly linked

vo calic no des have to b e supp orted bylow long vowels are lowered We

have already seen this constraintabove namely in the discussion of diph

thongs see page This constraint outranks the constraint against low

labial vowels which as I claimed ab ove is activeontheDutch short vowels

Standard Dutch therefore has three truly long vowels All three of these

are lax b ecause they are in a heavy syllable All three of them are low



b ecause of ProjectN low

Long vowels in other Brabant dialects Although most Bra

bant dialects have a morphologicallyconditioned pro cess of vowel short



Typ ologically I make the prediction that length either is found in b oth tense and

lax vowels when Connect N lax is ranked very low or it only o ccurs in the lax vowels

but not in the tense vowels with the p ossible exception of the high vowels the Brabant

situation It is predicted that no language can o ccur where only tense vowels havea

length distinction but no lax vowels Although this seems to b e in disagreementwith

the implicit assumptions of most phonologists I have not b een able to nd data that

shed light on the issue

A dialect with real length Tilburg Dutch

ening and also a much larger set of long vowels than Standard Dutch the

actual structure and content of the vowel set varies considerablyInthis

section I will briey discuss two other Brabant systems Hofstade Dutch

Keymeulen and Taeldeman sp oken in the extreme south of the Bra

bant area and Antwerp Dutch Nuyts sp oken in central Brabant

In the latter dialect all long vowels are lax and all tense vowels are

short This is the exact opp osite situation of what is normally assumed to

b e the case in Standard Dutch Wethus nd words like the following all

examples have b een copied from Nuyts

i stipt prompt q qr scream

y dyrf dare q vqr re

u buk book Uq vUqt fo ot

q bqk bro ok e sp el toy

a kat cat q mq sleeve

The right generalisation can b e obtained if we assume that i Connect N

lax do es not visibly play a role in Antwerp Dutch and ii Antwerp Dutch



hasavariantof ConnectN lax with lax in stead of low



ConnectN lax Antwerp Dutch Vo calic features are at

tached to more than one ro ot they are supp orted by the feature

lax

In Hofstade Dutch the parameters tenseness and length are indep endent

In this dialect we nd a complete vowel system All tense and lax vowels

have b oth long and short variants In all there are therefore dierent

vowels in Hofstade some of which are listed b elow

i wit white iq wiql wheel

yq sxyqr barn y mys mouse

e b elt image eq veql much

m skirt q wqt wide

a kat cat q mq sleeve

As in Antwerp Dutch the constraint ConnectN lax do es not playa



visible role in Hofstade The same is true for the constraint ProjectN

under any of its guises in this dialect however All these constraints are

ranked b elowFaithfulness Every combination of length and tenseness may

surface as they are sp ecied regardless of syllabication with one exception

long vowels cannot o ccur in syllables that are also closed by a consonant

This however presumably follows from the general structure of rhymes in

whichatmosttwo p ositions are available not from any violable constraint

A dialect with real length Tilburg Dutch

The limited distribution of long vowels Trommelen and

Zonneveld haveshown that the loan phonemes q qandq

in Dutchhavea very limited distribution They only o ccur in either the

absolutely wordnal sup erheavy syllable or in the p enultimate when

the nal syllable is headed byschwaqisvery rare

gene embarassment zqn creme cream krqm scene scene

sqn

freule noble lady frql

zone id zqn record id rkqr controle control kntrql

Trommelen and Zonneveld note that when a full vowelsuxis

added to these forms the vowel shifts in qualitytowards the nearestby

vowel tense in an op en syllable lax in a closed syllable

a gene embarassment zqn genant embarassing zennt

or with reduction znnt

b expert id kspqr expertise id ksprtse

c controle control kntrql controleer to check kntrolqr

d zone id zqn zonaal zonal zonal

In Trommelen and Zonnevelds analysis this distribution should b e ac



counted for by the following rule

foreignforeign

The loan phonemes of Dutchthus have a feature foreign under Trom

melen and Zonnevelds analysis this feature can incidentally b e replaced

bylow under the analysis presented here The nonlowversion of qwill

have to shorten to e b ecause of the constraintlowlax in combination

N lax The latter constraint forces the short vowel in an with Connect

op en syllable to b ecome tense and the former requires that then it also has

to lo ose its feature low The result is a frontvowel without lax or low

ie the tense front mid vowel

The counterpart of in a constraintbased theory would b e a con

straint against the feature low on a vowel when followed by another full

vowel

Another way to lo ok at this fact however is the following In all the

examples given here the long vowels have primary word stress in the un

derived form After derivation stress shifts to the sux Therefore if we



Trommelen and Zonneveld adopt an analysis under whichschwadoesnot

head its own syllable I refer to the next chapter for details on this typ e of analysis

A dialect with real length Tilburg Dutch

assume that long vowels in Dutchmay only o ccur under primary stress the



alternations in also follow

Wenow exp ect long vowels also to show up b efore full vowels Indeed

a few cases are known from the literature although their number is admit



tedly small and limited to instances of long q

prairie id prqri aerodynamica id qro beta id bqta

In Tilburg Dutch the number of suchwords is somewhat larger as is ex

p ected b ecause long vowels in general are more frequent in this dialect In

all cases the long vowel gets primary stress

euriejaant b ossy p erson qrijant

rinnewaosie damage rnwqsi

mooniekaa concertina mqnika

peeresol parasol pqrs l

sjaokies quiet skis

Conclusion In this section I haveshown that the vowel system

of the Brabant dialect Tilburg Dutch can b e explained under the assump

tions made for Standard Dutch in earlier sections The assumptions re

garding syllable structure and extraproso dicity together with a stipulation

that all morphemes should at least partly b e incorp orated into the proso dic

word have b een used to explain the distribution of vowel length and mor

phological shortening pro cesses Even though the Tilburg system contains

six nondorsal lax and six nondorsal tense vowels I have argued these

two systems are not symmetric Tense vowels are high or mid lax vowels

are mid or low The dorsal vowels are set apart altogether They do not

show a distinction b etween long lax on the one hand and short on the other

and they similarly do not showanyheight distinction The argumenthas

b een carried over to Standard Dutch which is presumed to havetwoad

ditional comp eting constraints one which prohibits low labial lax vowels

and one which requires that all long vowels are low The fact that there



A problematic case for this analysis is the pair hygiene id hiijqn hy

gienisch id hiije nis b ecause primary stress in this case is still on the former

long vowel Yet the sux isch shows a somewhat exceptional b ehaviour in the stress

system normally wewould exp ect it to b e stressed I have no sp ecic analysis for this

form but the exceptional stress b ehaviour might help to explain it Another explanation

Harry van der Hulst pc mightbethatisch more generally seems to require that

vowels immediately preceding it are tense witness pairs like alcohol lkohlalcoholish

alcoholic lkoholis



According to Trommelen and Zonneveld the long q b efore r is the result

of rColouring applied to i and words like beta form a sp ecial stratum in the lexicon

Derivation of the Dutchvowel system

is an asymmetry b etween lax and high vowels on the one hand and non

high tense vowels on the other in that the rst class can o ccur in closed

syllables whereas the second cannot has b een argued to follow from the

general mechanisms of the system in particular of ConnectN lax

Derivation of the Dutchvowel system

In this thesis I assume the theory of sp ecication and undersp ecic ation

of segments in which in principle anycombination of features or feature

values can b e underlying Illicit feature combinations will b e ruled out by

grounded constraints like the ones summarized in the previous subsec

tion and repaired by deletion rules Features not present in underlying

representation will similarly b e lled in automatically This means no con

straints on underlying representations are needed as in other theories of

undersp ecicati on

For Dutch I assumed the following vo calic features coronal labial

dorsal low high and lax For the sake of simplicity I assume all these

features are monovalent but the following discussion would not haveto

change dramatically if they were not It would just have to b e complicated



Free combination of these features gives us dierent p ossible

inputs represented in the following table Hhigh Llow RRTRlax

ccoronal llabial vdorsal a plus indicates that a feature is present a

blank that it is absent

H

L

R

c

l

v

H

L

R

c

l

v

H

L

R

c

l

v

H

L

R

c

l

v

The vowel in row has no features at all I will argue in the next

Derivation of the Dutchvowel system

chapter this is the schwa The vowels in the rows through only have

ap erture features no place features These would b e high lowandRTR

variants of schwa None of these o ccur in Dutch We need therefore to have

constraints to the eect that ap erture features can only surface if they are

supp orted by place features The following is the structure of the vo calic

no de

vo calic

Vplace Ap erture

coronal labial dorsal high low lax

Aschwa do es not havea vo calic no de at all Some other vowels the tense

mid vowels only have place features but there are no vowels with only

ap erture features and no place features in the output ie high or lowmid

vowels these would b e the surface corresp ondents of row in the table

I assume this is the result of several constraints which require ap erture

features to b e accompanied by sp ecic place features for instance

a laxdorsal lowlax



b highlabial

c etc

These constraints should b e ranked higher than the constraints against

the insertion of dorsal coronal and labial In that case every ap erture

feature will always b e accompanied by a place feature and schwa will b e

the only p ossible vowel without a Vplace no de

Rows through represent the coronal vowels is e i and

will b e lled out with lax b ecause of the following constraint

ordering

a lowlax

b lax

Rowis Rows and have illegal feature combinations high

el y They will b oth surface as the diphthong lax and high low resp ectiv

i



Constraints like this express the idea that all vowels need a place feature when they

have an ap erture feature I will not go into the question to what extent such constraints are grounded

Derivation of the Dutchvowel system

Rows through represent labial vowels without dorsal sp ecication

However all of these vowels except the ones that have a coronal sp eci

cation will acquire a sp ecication dorsal I will assume this is the result

of the following two constraints



dorsal

a

coronal

b labialdorsal

The constraint against a combination of dorsal and coronal seems as

universally highranked as the constraint against low high

These feature combinations surface then as the back rounded or front

rounded vowels Conicting ap erture features cause diphthongization

The rows in through all represent dorsal vowels Combinations

through and through do not surface b ecause they contain

conicting place sp ecications Furthermore the dorsal vowels that have

a high marking will get labial sp ecication and end up as u by the

interaction of the following constraints



a highlabial



dorsal

b highcoronal

coronal

The rows through also representback labial vowels Finally the

forms b etween and that do not have conicting ap erture features are

p ossible inputs yielding a and

The main dierence b etween Standard Dutch and Tilburg Dutchisthat

in the former dialect a constraint against the combination of labial and

low is active which has no eect in the latter Another dierence is that

doubly linked vowels in Dutch always get laxed b ecause of ConnectN



lax and lowered b ecause of ProjectN low whereas in Tilburg

Dutch they only get laxed

In all we can derive the Dutchvowel system by the following four blo cks

of constraints within each blo ck it is not p ossible to establish anyinternal



ranking

  

dorsal high low

a lowlax

coronal low labial



This constraint has to b e ranked under labial coronal to make sure that underlying

is do not surface as y



Because of their low ranking most constraints in the last blo ck are actually irrelevant

for the description of Dutch I added them b ecause their eect can sometimes b e seen in other languages

Conclusion

b highlabial labialdorsal laxdorsal highcoronal

Parsedorsal Parsecoronal Parselow Parselabial

Parsehigh Parselax

c lax coronal low dorsal labial

d laxlow dorsallabial dorsallax

coronalhigh labialhigh etc

I refer to the top ology of the Dutch lexical phonology in app endix C

Conclusion

In this chapter I have discussed real vowel length as opp osed to the

phonetic length which is a result of tense vowels ending up in op en syllables

thatwe nd in Dutch and in BrabantDutch dialects It is quite inter

esting that long vowels and diphthongs always have a lax head regardless

whether they are underlying or the result of rcolouring or diphthongisation

This fact is unexp ected from the p oint of view of a lengthbased theorybut

it is exactly what a laxbased pro jection theory predicts Because in ad

dition the head of long vowels and diphthongs in Standar d Dutchislow I

prop osed to follow the tradition of letting the twohalves of a long vowel b e

dominated by the nucleus allowing me to formulate a pro jection constraint

on branching nuclei ProjectN low A similar constraint could also b e

used for Antwerp Dutch where all long vowels are lax and all short vowels

tense

Wehave also reached some results that are not directly related to the

theory of phonological pro jection For instance I haveshown howambi

syllabicity extrasyllabicity and catalexis can b e expressed in the theory of

proso dic structure used here I have also argued that lax is b est seen as

a tongue ro ot feature to b e group ed together with the ap erture features

high and low Finally the analysis of Tilburg vowel shortening gave evi

dence for a constraintMorPa to the eect that all morphemes should at

least partly b e incorp orated into the proso dic structure All of these results

will turn out to b e quite useful for us in the chapters that follow

Derived schwainDutch

Intro duction

Schwa probably is the most wellstudied vowel of Mo dern Dutch Its ex

ceptional b ehaviour in syllable structure its stressles sne ss and its use as

a reduced and ep enthetic vowel have inspired phonologists to a varietyof

theories ab out this mysterious segment

In my view schwaprovides us with convincing evidence for the hyp oth

esis that vo calic feature comp osition and syllable structure are strongly

related Schwa has only one feature cons it therefore is a very defective

typ e of vowel and it heads a very defectivetyp e of syllable In terms of

the pro jection constraints there is no feature that allows schwa to o ccur in

the head p osition of anytyp e of proso dic constituent except for the ma jor

class features cons and p ossibly son itself I will show these two

facts are connected and that we can derive the latter from the former

In the next twochapters I develop a theory of Dutchschwathatiscom

patible with the general framework of this thesis At the outset it is useful

to make clear that I distinguish b etween three typ es of schwainDutch

This distinction is pretheoretical in the sense that we can empirically dis

tinguish b etween these typ es without prejudice as to whether one or more

of these typ es should have some sp ecial theoretical status indep endentfrom

the others

Rschwa Stressless vowels alternate with schwainDutch as they do in

English However this alternation is much more restricted in the

former language than in the latter The restrictions are manifold

Some vowels never alternate at all some p ositions in the word do

not allow for reduction etc Also even if a vowel can reduce this

reduction is never obligatory in Dutch The more formal the style of

sp eech the less we nd I will therefore takestyle

dep endent alternation with a full vowel as a litmus test for rschwa

r for reduction

Intro duction

Eschwa Words ending in a consonant cluster ie a closed syllable fol

lowed by a degenerate syllable can get an optional schwaintervening

between the two consonants Here I take the existence of a variant

without schwa as the deciding test for eschwa e for ep enthetic



Rschwas are never eschwas and vice versa

Ihave to note however that the variants without schwaofwords like

melk milk Marc name hulp help etc sound overly formal to

manynativespeakers of Dutch including the author of this thesis

Uschwa This is empirically a rest category If a schwa is neither an

u for underlying rschwa nor an eschwa I call it an uschwa



Uschwa do es not alternate

The reason why I coined the somewhat abstract terms eschwa rschwa

and uschwa is that not every author agrees on the analysis that is im

plicit in terms like epenthetic schwa and underlying schwaFor instance

some authors have argued that also uschwas are the result of ep enthesis

andor reduction Even those authors have to explain however why in some

contexts reduction and ep enthesis are obligatory while in others they are

optional I will discuss some previous theories on Dutchschwa in section

in the next chapter However I am not aware of any study trying to

approach all instances of schwa at the same time

Interestingly the three dierent instances of schwahave some phono

logical prop erties in common which set them apart from the rest of the

vowel system Most prominent among these prop erties no doubt is the im

p ossibility for either of these vowels to b ear primary stress but I will show

that there are other prop erties that put the schwas apart as well

On the other hand the three schwas also dier from one another in

signicantwa ys For instance underlying schwa and ep enthetic schwa seem

to b e in complementary distribution

The fact that all schwas are clearly dierent from other vowels and the

fact that they slightly dier among themselves are the main issues of the

next twochapters This chapter will deal with rschwaandeschwa the

next chapter will deal exclusively with uschwa

I will try to make the rst fact follow from the observation that schwais

a featureless vowel It is almost absolutely central to the vowel triangle

This is not true if wetakeinto consideration some very informal styles of sp eech

where vowels which can b e reduced in somewhat less informal styles are deleted For

instance the rst a in banaan banana can b e reduced to in some styles bnan

but in very informal styles of sp eech it can b e deleted altogether bnan From this

point of view the schwa is b oth an rschwa b ecause it alternates with a full vowel and

an eschwa b ecause it alternates with null



See however in the next chapter for a weakening of this rm statement

Intro duction

so it do es not have its own place or ap erture sp ecications I will assume

the following prop ertyofschwa to b e the most central one from which

most of the others can b e derived

Prop erty Schwa is exceptional in the vocalic system because it bears no

phonetic features

The informal observation of prop erty gets its empirical underpinning in

Ko opmansvan Beinum Her phonetic research has shown that

schwa sounds in natural continuous speech areconsiderably shorter than

the other short vowels no strong consonantal inuence exists on schwa du

ration schwa sounds display a spectral spread larger than any other vowel

and surrounding consonants seem to play a role with respect to the mid

point distribution of the schwa within the whole vowel system

Ko opmansvan Beinum cf also Browman and Goldstein

on targetless schwa in English Esp ecially the latter two criteria seem to

me to b e esp ecially relevant indications that schwa do es not haveitsown



place sp ecications

It is attractivetohave this prop erty as the most basic prop ertyofschw a

from the p oint of view of language acquisition Prop erty is a phonetic fact

that is op en to direct observation Most of the prop erties to b e discussed

b elow are more abstract It seems therefore more plausible to assume the

child observes the featureless nature of schwa and derives its other prop er

ties from this than to takeany of the other prop erties which are much more

dep endent on other more abstract prop erties of the Dutch phonology as

we will see as basic

I will therefore make this prop erty the starting p oint of the analysis for

all three o ccurrences of schwa From this single basic prop erty I will derive

other prop erties whichIhave found to hold for Dutchschwa The reader

can nd a complete list of these prop erties at the end of the next chapter

on page

Schwa do es not b ear any of the vo calic features high low lax

coronal labial or dorsal Jakobson Anderson I as

V V V

sume all these vo calic features are monovalent or otherwise only one value is

relevant for the constraints stated b elow In the feature geometry adopted

here prop ertywould b e stated as follows

Schwa is a ro ot no de with no feature material dep endentonit



In many dialects schwa is p erceptually quite close to the mid

y In other dialects it comes closer to eg in Rotterdam Dutch or eg in The

Hague Dutch I assume that these are matters of phonetic implementation

Intro duction

Following McCarthy I assume that the ma jor class features are re

alized on the ro ot no de directly An empty ro ot no de still has the ma jor

class features on it

This means we can still sp ecify the schwaasanelement b earing ma jor

class features Given these assumptions Dutchschwa gets the following

representation



cons



son

This is a maximally undersp ecie d segment if we assume ma jor class fea

tures are never undersp ecie d Clements If consonantal turns out

to b e nonexsistent as has b een argued by Hume and Odden or de

p endent on the ro ot no de as defended byPadgett the ro ot would

consist of only son The interesting prediction wewould make then is

that there are exactly two totally empty segments son and son



b Ihave nevertheless decided not to work out this line of thought To

the contrary I will often use only cons as a representation of schwa

Now the schwabehaves in some ways as a normal vowel b ecause it

has exactly the same sp ecication of ma jor class no des as other vowels

However in one very imp ortantway it is a defectivevowel it has no vo calic

class no de We will see that manyoftheschwa prop erties listed b elowcan

b e reduced to this defective nature Schwa often cannot o ccur where other

vowels can

Following this line of reasoning we should b e able to draw the same

conclusions ab out schwa in other languages that havethevowel in their

inventory I will therefore discuss the b ehaviour of schwaintwo languages

other than Dutch viz French and Norwegian in chapter

I try to make the dierences b etween eschwa rschwa and uschwa

follow from the dierent phonological levels in which they arise and from the

fact that dierent factors interact with the featurelessnes s of schwaineach

of these cases Eschwa and rschwa are derived at later p ostcyclic levels

Uschwa is underlying hence already present in the Lexical Phonology



This representation is very similar to the one prop osed bySzpyra and Bethin

for the Polish yer Yet Szpyra assumes that the ro ot no de is completely

empty ie also the ma jor class features are not sp ecied In other words Polish yer for

Szpyra is Since I assume class no des do not exist without features it is imp ossible

to interpret such a structure under the assumptions defended here



This assumes that b is the consonantal counterpart of schwa In what follows it

will b e shown that this consonant as well as h indeed stands in a sp ecial relation

to the emptyvowel h could b e seen either as a continuantversion or as a sonorant

version of b ie mayb e even the glide of See Keating McCarthy

Pulleyblank Stemb erger Steriade for discussion of the glottal stop

and h as empty consonants

Prop erties of rschwa

The reason why it is imp ortant to distinguish b etween a Lexical and a

Postlexical phonology is not that derived rschwaandeschwa sometimes

cannot o ccur where uschwa can This can b e seen as a particular instance of

the emergence of the unmarked McCarthy and Prince we cannot

derive marked congurations but we can allow them to surface when they

are underlying b ecause deleting the vowel would b e even more costlyThe

real reason for distinguishing b etween Lexical and Postlexical phonology is

that in some cases uschwa cannot o ccur in environments where rschwa

and eschwa can One schwa is deleted in exactly the same conguration

where another schwaisderived Here I can only see one solution viz to

let uschwa b e present on a phonological level the lexical level where e

schwa and rschwadonotyet o ccur Atthislevel uschwa can then b e

deleted whereas at a later level the oended constraints are no longer as

strong as they used to b e Rschwa and eschwamay therefore arise at

these particular levels

In this chapter I concentrate on the two p ostlexical o ccurrences of

Dutchschwa rschwaandeschwa I discuss these b elow one byoneEach

time I rst formulate the prop erties of schwa in a more or less theory

indep endentway b efore I givemyown analysis

Prop erties of rschwa

It seems most convenient to start the discussion with rschwa the schwa

that alternates with a full vowel in unstressed p osition and therefore is

commonly analysed as a reduced variant of the full vowel I will supp ort

this analysis As I stated ab ove reduction of schwa is far more restricted in



Dutch than it is in English This is what makes Dutchrschwa particularily

interesting

Restrictions on rschwahave b een discussed in Martin No o

teb o om Bo oij Van Zonneveld

Ko opmansVan Beinum Neyt and Zonneveld Slo otweg

and Wester Kager Visch and Zonneveld Kager and

Trommelen and Zonneveld among others Most of the generative

phonologists in this list have concentrated on the relation b etween stress

p osition and reduction Other factors are sometimes mentioned but not

worked out

One of the reasons for this state of aairs p ossibly is that phonological

theory at present cannot incorp orate all relevant factors in a natural way



On the other hand from Giegerich Harris and Burzio wecan

infer that all conditions on reduction in Dutchmentioned here are also at least partly

active in some English dialects

Prop erties of rschwa

In the rst place reduction is heavily inuenced by extralinguistic factors

suchasword frequency and so ciolinguisti c parameters such as so cial class

Secondlysomeoftheinternal factors are not very well understo o d For

instance reduction seems to b e easier b efore r than b efore other seg

ments I am not aware of any phonological theory that can explain this

centering eect of r in a satisfactory wayInthischapter I will ignore

all extralinguistic factors except style register Of the linguistic factors

I only discuss those that are reasonably well understo o d and that can get

an explanation in my theory or anyb o dy elses I refer to Martin for

the completest description of facts and factors

Why do es an unstressed vowel reduce to a schwa I assume that ideally

vo calic features have to b e supp orted by stress and vice versa So if a vowel

dominates lax or labial or some other vo calic feature this vowel in a

certain way ideally heads a syllable whichinturnheadsafootInverselyif

a syllable is stressed it tends to have a full sp ecication for vo calic features

def

ConnectFt V ProjectV Ft ProjectFt V N

  



dominates a vo calic no de i N heads a branching fo ot

I argue b elow that in a few cases Dutchdoeshave unary feet b ehaving in

a slightly dierentway

These two pro jection constraints are clearly related to Connect N lax

which I used and defended in the previous chapters Where ConnectN

lax states that the feature lax preferably is dominated by the head of a

branching rhyme and that the head of a branching rhyme inversely domi

nates the feature lax these constraint state that i a vo calic no de prefer

ably is in the head of a fo ot and ii the head of a fo ot dominates a vo calic

no de see chapter for extensive discussion of this p oint

The question arises whether wehave to distinguish b etween dierent

vo calic features or to distinguish b etween dierentlevels of stress Wewill

see b elow that we do need to distinguish b etween twotyp es of metrical

p osition one which is in a fo ot but not its head and another whichis

outside all feet This distinction is to b e very imp ortant also for uschwa

Of course other constraints are interacting with ConnectFt V In



some style registers hardly anyvowel is reduced at all This shows that

full vowels can freely o ccur in unstressed p osition in that register I assume

astyle register is a separate grammar Generally it seems to b e the case

across languages that the more formal the style of sp eech the higherranked



Cf Mo ortgat and Van der Hulst Charette Van der Hulst b

Kenstowicz b among others for other formalisations of similar intuitions



On rhythmic p ostlexical stress in forms like helderdere clearerINFL see section

Prop erties of rschwa

the faithfulness conditions Parse and Fillare I will not pursue this

conjecture here except that I will takeitasmy guideline for the study

of Dutch see section for some discussion In more formal styles of

sp eech the requirement to parse vo calic features gets more weight than

ConnectFt V If for instance an o ccurs in unstressed p osition



it is deemed more imp ortant to parse the feature bundle dorsal low lax

than to satisfy ConnectFt V



Because schwa has no vo calic features at all it is the most ideal vowel in

unstressed p osition according to the pro jection constraint The rst prop

erty of rschwa apart from its lack of features is now trivially explained



Prop erty Rschwa is the reduction vowel

Vowel reduction is a consequence of ConnectFt V Since schwa has the



representation in it is the only vowel that do es not violate ConnectFt



V when it o ccurs in unstressed p osition From whichproperty follows

directly

Inowhave set up the basic mechanism for vowel reduction Iwill

pro ceed to discuss some restricti ons on it My main sources for the data

are Martin Bo oij and Kager

The rst two of these restrictions have to do with p eripheral p ositions

in the word Rschwa is disallowed b oth absolutely wordinitially and ab

solutely wordnallyHowever for wordinitial p ositions this can b e shown

to b e related to some principle not directly referring to the p eriphery of

words

Wordinitial p osition In wordinitial onsetless syllables vow

els can never reduce not in anystyle of sp eech

elite elite elt lt egaal plain eal al

ovaal oval oval val agent p oliceman ant nt

It can b e shown that the prohibition against schwa in onsetless syllables

is much more general not restricted to the rst syllable of the word It

seems that schwa cannot o ccur in onsetless syllables In addition wewill

see b elo w that rschwaavoids all wordp eripheral p ositions This implies

For instance in French the more formal the style of sp eech the more ie

parsing of consonants Encreve In Turkish the more formal the style of sp eech

the less ep enthesis ie insertion of morphologically unsp onsored material

A pretonic tense vowel can sometimex reduce to a lax vowel esp ecially if this

vowel is a eg banaan banana banan can b e pronouncedasbnan Although this

pro cess is very interestingfor instance b ecause it cannot b e straightforwardly describ ed

as reduction in my theory since the change from tense to lax involves addition of a feature

rather than subtraction I cannot discuss the phenomenon here

Prop erties of rschwa

there are two indep endent factors prohibiting rschwawordinitiallysince

in this p osition schwa o ccurs b oth in an onsetless syllable and in the word

p eriphery I will rst discuss the former factor

Reduction in onsetless syllables is also prohibited inside the word This

is demonstrated in the words in Only words with an aV sequence

where V is the p otential reduction vowel are relevant b ecause tense vowels

other than a all can develop a glide which makes the following syllable

not onsetless

Maoist id mabost maost mabst maestoso id mabstoso

mastoso mabstoso

It is not clear whether a glottal stop is inserted b etween the twovowels it

seems at least to b e p erceptually much less salient than it is in German Yet

schwa reduction will b e blo cked regardless of whether we insert the glottal

stop or not A related restricti on seems to b e the one against reduction

after h

helaas alas helas hlas heraut herald herut hrut

hotel id hotl htl habijt monks fro ck habit hbit

As we will see h and b pattern together in many other contexts It seems

not unreasonable to analyse them as empty consonants Neither of them

seems to have its own supralaryngeal place of articulation

At rst sight this might b e seen as a condition on syllable structure

It is sometimes supp osed that vowels immediately before h do not reduce



either Facts like the following are given in supp ort of this assumption

mahonie mahony mahoni mhoni alcohol id lkohl lkhl

Yet the unacceptability of these facts is not as strong by far as that of the

examples in or As a matter of fact these examples sound quite

grammatical b e it very informal to me and I will treat them in this way

The same seems to b e true for reduction of the rst vowel in hiatus

Also this reduction do es not seem to b e as strongly blo cked as in the case

of the second vowel An additional problem in this case is that the only

sp ot where we could p ossibly exp ect any eect at all is the place where the

stressless vowel is a cf If the vowel is any of the other tense vowels

e i y u o a glide can b e inserted lexically to resolve hiatus and the

vowel can freely reduce in the p ostlexicon cf

According to Kager the constraints against reduction b efore h or bhold

for wordinitial pretonic syllables only reduction in medial prevo calic vowels is only

slightly inhibited If this is true I have no explanation for the fact My own observations

do not really conrm Kagers p oint For me all the examples in and are

on the same level

Prop erties of rschwa

naef naive naf nf cocan id kokankokn

theater theatre tejatr tjatr oceaan o cean osejan osjan

The reduced forms do not seem to b e very bad I will therefore concentrate

on the cases of tautosyllabic b h and

Cohen et al observed that we can generalize over the hiatus

context and the p ositions adjacent to h Because in hiatus a glottal stop

is automatically inserted wemay assume that a syllable may not consist

of only empty unarticulated segments

a A ro ot no de is empty i it do es not dominate a place no de

b Contour If the head of a syllable is empty its onset maynot



be empty

In the chapters that follow in particular chapters and we will see

that the contour constraint is more general its actual formulation should

b e something like if the head of a syl lable is x its onset may not bex

This will also rule out illegitimate syllables suchas ji and wy The

constraint Contour is very closely related to the OCP with the syllable

as its domain but it diers from that principle in twoways In the rst

place the OCP always refers to two adjacent features while Contour in

this case is violated b ecause of a lack of features In the second place a

syllable such as ji could also consist of one segment i linked at the same

time to b oth the onset and the nucleus cf also chapter This is ruled

out by Contour but not by the OCP

We can generalize this observation furthermore over all instances of

schwa Eschwa and uschwa are never found word initially as the second

vowel in hiatus p osition or after h either We therefore have the following

prop erty of Dutchschwa

Prop erty Schwa cannot be tautosyl labic to h or b

Because h bor are never found in Dutch we

may assume the constraint Contour in b is ranked in a fairly high

p osition In particular it dominates the pro jection constraint demanding

reduction ConnectFt V As we will see b elow it will also dominate



the constraints on the o ccurrence of eschwa and uschwa

Wordnal p osition Like absolutely wordinitial vowels abso



lutely wordnal vowels cannot reduce to schwa



This is an adaptation of Noskes OnsetNucleus Contour Constraint



On the somewhat exceptional status of wordnal schwa in English historical phonol

ogy see Harris

Prop erties of rschwa

toee id tfe tf cola id kola kol

herrie noise hri hr foto picture foto fot

In this case the eect cannot b e attributed to the syllable or any other level

lower than the word As we will see b elow op en syllables generally favour

reduction and also the weak p osition of a tro chee is very favourable for this

pro cess We will also havetotakeinto account that the constraint against

wordnal p osition is not shared byuschwa which o ccurs in wordnal

p osition fairly often though it do es apply to eschwaaswell

It can also b e demonstrated that it is not nal syl lables that blo ck

reduction since wordnal closed syllables do reduce whenever comparable

wordinternal syllables do cf eg radar id radr radr The eect

is therefore to b e attributed to the segmental level

Prop erty A wordnal segment cannot be rschwa or eschwa

What could b e the reason that p eripheral segments are so reluctanttobe

come schwa In the previous chapter wehave already seen another instance

of the particular b ehaviour of p eripheral segments wordnal consonants

in degenerate syllables are not deleted This was then attributed to the

theory of Generalized Alignment The denition of the generalized Align

mentschema of McCarthy and Prince b ex is copied in

b elowFor some more discussion of this constraint family see app endix A

def

AlignCat Edge Cat Edge Cat Cat suchthat

     

Edge of Cat and Edge of Cat coincide

   

where Cat Cat PCat GCat Proso dic and Grammatical

 

categories

Edge Edge fRight Leftg

 

What do es it mean to say that the edges of two categories coincideIn

all work done on Alignment Theory the relation seems to imply that the



categories involved share a segment Let us adopt this p osition The

segment I will take to b e a ro ot no de This seems reasonable b ecause ro ot

no des are the lowest phonological ob jects with proso dic aliation at least

in the approach defended here The edge lab el Right of Cat now refers to

x

the rightmost ro ot no de R of Cat ie the ro ot no de R Cat suchthat

x x

there is no ro ot no de SCat S following R A similar denition can b e

x

provided for the lab el Left

Wecannow formulate the following denition of the coincidence rela

tion



On the p ossible role of nul l strings cf McCarthy and Prince b and Ito and Mester

Prop erties of rschwa

An edge E of a category C coincides with edge E of category

  

C i E E and feature F dominated byE a feature F

     

dominated byE suchthatF F and F dominated byE

    

F dominated byE suchthatF F

   

In this denition the relation is used as a primitive Two class no des

and features havetohave the same lab el feature value and index for the

relation to hold So what says informallyisthattwo edges coincide

if they are exactly the same The crucial part of this denition is the

requirement that all dominated features of the edge segments havetobe

the same This is a stipulation but in my opinion not a very unreasonable

one

We assume the underlying phonological representation of any morpheme

is a set of strings of segmental material What I assume then is that a

grammatical category immediately dominates a string of ro ots McCarthy

The morphology of toee id therefore lo oks as follows feature trees

have b een conated to IPA sym b ols

Stem

t f e

Crucially one of the morphological edge no des is e The Principle of

Consistency of Exp onence says that no changes in the exponenceofa

phonological lyspecied morpheme arepermitted McCarthy and Prince

This means that the Stem no de will always have an e edge no matter

what we do

Reduction means that the vowel features of e are are no longer visible

for the phonology This implies that a Proso dic Word built on tfe will

not see coronal In other words the right edge of the Proso dic Word will

be By our denition of coincide the edges of Proso dic Word and Stem

will therefore not b e aligned prop erly

If uschwa is really underlying schwa Alignment will not cause a prob

lem for that vowel b ecause in that case the edge of the morphological word

as well as the edge of the proso dic word will b e schwa Also wordinternal

reduction can of course not b e prohibited by Alignment whichonlylooks

at edges

Because wordnal vowels are never reduced wemay assume that Align



dominates ProjectFt Noweven if wewould want to distinguish b e

tween tw oversions of Align corresp onding to the two dierent edges like



Align itself might of course b e ranked b elow other constraints in particular the

constraint enforcing nal devoicing which also applies in wordnal p osition in Dutch

Prop erties of rschwa

AlignLeft and AlignRight all evidence in Dutchwould seem to p oint



in the direction of AlignLeft dominating AlignRight This has as

a consequence that wehave an extra reason for banning reduction in word

initial p osition even if we do not insert a glottal stop at the b eginning of

the word

Vowel quality An interesting factor in reduction is the quality

of the vowel that has to b e reduced Some vowels reduce very easily other

vowels resist reduction under almost all circumstances In particular e is

virtually always reduced when it is in unstressed p osition with the excep

tions discussed ab ove while y hardly reduces at all If reduction means

getting rid of features which seems the most reasonable assumption to

make this leads us to the conclusion that some features seem to b e harder

to get rid of than others

The most prominent constrast can b e observed b etween Avowels and B

vowels The latter category reduces under very sp ecic circumstances only

However since the relevant feature lax interacts with syllable structure

which itself interacts with stress and since stress is an imp ortant indep en

dent factor in reduction the direct inuence of exactly this feature lax is

particularily unclear I will therefore put o its discussion until the next

few sections Here I will concentrate on the other features

Kager establishes the following reduction hierarchyforvowels

I havechanged his feature lab els in order for them to b e compatible with

the vowel theory develop ed in the previous chapter

a e high labial dorsal

b a high labial dorsal

c o high labial dorsal

d i high labial dorsal

e u y high labial dorsal

Ihave to add that some parts of the hierarchy are not really wellestablished

For instance Kager remarks that hardly occurs in stressless

positions such as initial pretonic position and that its grouping with o

is therefore by necessity more based on theoretical extrap olation than on

empirical observation Also there is a lot of variation b etween individual

but not for instance in Yiddish cf Katz Lombardi and section in

the next chapter



For instance the prexsux asymmetry in syllabication in German is taken to b e

an indication for this ranking in McCarthy and Prince In Dutchwe nd exactly

the same typ e of asymmetries as will b e briey discussed in the next chapter

Prop erties of rschwa

sp eakers and within one sp eaker b etween dierentstyles of sp eechandeven

between individual words Sometimes reduction do es not really lead to a

pure schwa but to a schwalikevowel containing some phonetic traces of

the original colour I havechosen to nevertheless put the gradient facts into

a discrete scheme A discussion of style registers will b e taken up b elow

Two generalisations are particularly strong I will list them as sep erate

prop erties here

Prop erty Rschwa alternates with e and less often with aand

o

Prop erty Rschwa almost never alternates with a high vowel

By denition these prop erties are prop erties of rschwaonly However

we will see in chapter that in other languages underlying schwas also

alternate with frontmidvowels

We see a clear correlation b etween markedness and reducibilityeis

probably the most unmarked full vowel If all vo calic features are mono

valent this vowel only b ears the sp ecication coronal the activevalue for

ap erture features high and low is F in the system prop osed here y

on the other hand is a very marked v owel with features coronal labial

and high

The feature high seems particularly activeinblocking reduction High

vowel reduction seems only p ossible in some very frequentwords like minuut

ziek music myzik mzik but normally it minute minyt mnyt and mu

is imp ossible It seems as if this feature has a particularly strong attachment

to the feature tree In a constraintbased theory we can formalize this by

assuming the constraint forcing an underlying feature high to b e parsed

in the output is ranked very high higher than ProjectFt in anystyle

register of Dutch

I will followDeSchutter and Kager in assuming that

round or labial as I call it is another feature blo cking reduction Re

duction of the labial mid vowels o and sounds quite informal Finally

a similar consideration leads us to p osit a constraint on parsing the feature

dorsal ranked very low but ab ove Parsecoronal in order to capture

the dierence b etween e and a on the one hand and the dierence

between a and o on the other

Kagers hierarchy in can now b e reected in the constraint

hierarchyin

Parsehigh Parselabial Parsedorsal Parsecoronal

These constraints are never in direct conict with one another so that

the ranking given here can only b e established on the basis of interaction

Prop erties of rschwa

with other constraints In chapter wehave seen some evidence for a high

ranking of Parsehigh as well as of Parselax and Parselow on the

basis of diphthongisation Here we see additional evidence for this ranking

coming from the unrelated area of vowel reduction

It is very well p ossible that parts of this ordering have a universal char

acter For instance the low priority assigned to parsing coronal might

reect the univeral unmarkedness of this feature It is wellknown that

in English the only nonreducible vowels are high cf Fidelholtz

Giegerich for recent discussion The reduction hierarchy of Breton

Dressler also seems very similar to the one prop osed here for

Dutch high vowels do not reduce at all and the b est candidates for reduc

tion are e and a

I will suggest b elow that the relative ordering of ProjectFt with re

sp ect to this hierarchy dep ends on style register In very informal styles

only Parsehigh dominates ProjectFtsothatonlyhighvowels are

excluded from reduction In very formal styles al l Parse constraints dom

inate ProjectFt except mayb e Parsecoronal so that all vowel seg

ments other than e are exempt from reduction

Notice that it is imp ossible to distinguish b etween i on the one hand

and uy on the other in the approach presented here All three vowels

resist reduction b ecause this would violate Parsehigh If Kager is

right reduction of the latter twovowelsisworse b ecause they are round

in addition to b eing high Such a generalisation cannot b e captured in



the formalism of OT We cannot add the violations of several constraints

to weigh them against one other constraint If yu are ruled out in a

certain grammar they are ruled out b ecause they violate Parsehigh b e

cause this constraint dominates Parselabial Then i is out as well

We cannot rearrange the order of Parsehigh and Parselabial b ecause

then wewould incorrectly predict that o are very marked reducers to o

I am not sure whether Kagers distinction b etween yu and i

is so strong that it p oses a serious problem His main motivation seems to

b e that it is easier to nd words where i reduces fi guur gure bikini

vier river than to nd words where the other two reduce Yet id ri

also for i it is p ossible to nd numerous cases where this vowel seems to

never reduce at all and for y and u there are some cases of optional

reduction eg muziek music myzik mzik as Kager indicates

It mightwell b e that there simply are more frequentwords with stressles s

i than with stressles s y or u Given this lack of empirical clarityI

do not consider my nding that all high vowels should b ehavealikeasa



Cf Mohanan for a theory where violations against constraints can be

accumulated

Prop erties of rschwa

problem

Stress The next factor in reduction I want to study is the only

really strong condition on reduction in English only stressles s vowels are

reduced Stressless ness is one of the most central prop erties of schwa It

holds without exception for all instances of Dutchschwa and it is wide

spread in other languages as well

Prop erty Rschwa eschwa and uschwa cannot occur in stressedpo

sition

Interestingly Dutchmakes a distinction b etween twotyp es of unstressed

p osition in vowel reduction This has b een the main topic of most of the

studies on reduction mentioned ab ove The facts can b e demonstrated on

the word fonologie phonology fonolo In very formal sp eech this word

is pronounced as just indicated Two alternative less formal pronuncia

tions are p ossible fonlo and fonl The latter one is even more

informal than the former What is crucially imp ossible is the pronunciation

fonol If the second vowel reduces the rst one should reduce as well

Bo oij

Various prop osals have b een made to deal with this p osition eect Most

of these prop osals refer to suprasyllabic metrical structure Wehavetorst

establish what the metrical structure of a form suchasfonologe is b efore

we can account for the reduction facts Since this level of representation is

not the topic of this thesis I will not review the literature here

In a pro jection theory the fact that b oth no and lo are candidates for

reduction is an indication that they b oth are in a dep endent p osition at

some level of pro jection The fact that lo is more resistant to reduction is

an indication that it is still a head at some other level of pro jection where

no is still a dep endent For this reason I havechosen to use the following

representation I assume that the rst two syllables are joined together in a

fo otlike constituent and that the third syllable is joined to this constituent

into a larger constituent say a sup erfo ot

Prop erties of rschwa

Word

Ft Ft Ft

   

fo no lo i

I assume that something like the Strict Layer Hyp othesis holds Selkirk

all syllables are organized into feet all feet into sup erfeet all su

p erfeet into words The heads of sup erfeet are the b earers of primary and

secondary accent In the normal case a sup erfo ot consists of one binary

branching fo ot but in exceptional cases when there is a ternary pattern a

unary fo ot may o ccur in the dep endent p osition of a sup erfo ot This analy

sis mayormay not b e the most optimal one but at least it provides us with

the necessary terminology to distinguish b etween twotyp es of unstressed

p osition

The rst unstressed syllable is in the weak p osition of a fo ot and I will

call it a weak p osition The second unstressed syllable I will call semi

weak

Kager prop oses that there is a hierarchy based on metrical p osition

Kager his ex like there is a hierarchy based on vowel



quality In the formalism of pro jection constraints it is however imp os

sible to refer to the dierence b etween a weak and a stray p osition in the

appropriate way In order to account for the fact that no reduces more

easily than lo wewould need to saythat if a vowel lacks vocalic features

it occurs in the dependent position of a foot Yet this is neither a p ossible

pro jection constraint nor a p ossible weakness constraint

Prop erty Reduction to schwa is general ly easier in weak positions than

in a semiweak positions



Kager uses an analysis in which the second syllable of fonologie is in the weak

p osition of a fo ot and the third syllable is stray

i

fo no lo i

Prop erties of rschwa

Dep ending on ones metrical theory one can rephrase the statementof

this prop ertyinvarious ways One of the few frameworks in whichit

is dicult to make this typ e of distinction is Burzio who would

presumably assign a ternary fo ot without anyinternal structure to the rst



three syllables of fonologie Yet even in this case we could for instance

refer to a dierence b etween fo otinternal and fo otnal p ositions

The idea is that if we reduce a vowel V we also obligatorily reduce all

vowels which are easier to reduce than V

First let us see what the reason is for reducing as manyvowels as p ossible

in the most formal register The imp ortant pro jection constraint here is of

course ProjectV Ft



ProjectV Ft N dominates a vo calic no de N heads a



branching fo ot

The b est output of fonoloi for ProjectV Ft would b e fonli



b ecause all full vowels o ccur only in the heads of branching fo ot A candi

date like fonloi violates ProjectV Ft b ecause it has a full vowel



the o of lo which is not the head of a branching fo ot For a similar rea

son also the candidate fonoli gives one violation of ProjectV Ft



and the candidate fonoloi gives even two violations

The dierence in reducabilitybetween the twotyp es of syllable has to b e

attributed to their dierence in p osition The syllable lo in is still the

head of a fo ot even though this fo ot do es not branch I prop ose to explicitly

distinguish pro jection constraintatfootlevel which refers to branching feet

and one which refers to feet in general The sp ecic constraintwillbeof

the form in but I will add to this a slightly more general constraint

ProjectV FtN dominates a vo calic no de Nheadsa Foot

These two constraints ProjectV Ft and ProjectV Ft are in a so





called Paninian relation If ProjectV Ft is violated a syllable is

outside the head of a fo ot but not reduced Because it is outside the head

of a fo ot it is also outside the head of a branching fo ot Therefore there

will also b e a violation of ProjectV Ft The reverse is not true a non



reduced syllable in the head of a nonbranching fo ot will give a violation

Burzio himself notes a similar kind of distinction for English in the fo otnote

on page in English the rst unstressed syllable of ttamagouchi or panama

reduces much more easily than the second one Burzio also notes that similar

facts hold for syncop e cf memorzation memrization Heeven refers to the historical

phonology of French Jacobs where Latin similitudine simlitudne sembletude



Two constraints A and B are in a Paninian relation if A applies to a prop er subset

of the candidates to which B applies Prince and Smolensky

Prop erties of rschwa

of ProjectV Ft but not of ProjectV Ft Concretely fonloi



violates ProjectV Ft but not ProjectV Ft and fonoli violates



b oth constraints

In Dutch we can make an empirical dierence b etween weak syllables

and semiweak syllables Therefore wemay assume the ordering of these



constraints is

ProjectV FtProjectV Ft



Again this ranking can b e established only indirectlyby studying the in

teraction with other constraints I assume this ordering is stable in Dutch

The only p ossible dierence b etween styles of sp eech is the ordering of

faithfulness constraints In the case of fonologie the relevant faithfulness

constraintisParselabial b ecause we are dealing with rounded mid vow

els There are three p ossible rankings of this constraintvisa vis the two

instances of ProjectV They corresp ond to three styles of sp eech

a formal Parselabial ProjectV FtProjectV Ft



b semiformal ProjectV Ft Parselabial ProjectV

Ft



c informal ProjectV Ft ProjectV Ft Parselabial



Wenow get the following tableaux for each of the relevantstyles

a formal

Candidates Parselabial ProjectFt ProjectFt



p

fonoloi

fonloi

fonoli

fonli

b semiformal

Candidates ProjectFt Parselabial ProjectFt



fonoloi

p

fonloi

fonoli

fonli



Given the Paninian relation just established if ProjectV Ft would b e ranked



higher than ProjectV Ft the eect of the latter wouldbeinvisible What wewould

get is a language where unstressed syllables are reduced regardless of their precise met rical p osition

Prop erties of rschwa

c informal

Candidates ProjectFt ProjectFt Parselabial



fonoloi

fonloi

fonoli

p

fonli

The form fonoli could never b e generated byany ordering of these

constraints The reason is that this form violates all constraints while

there are always alternative candidates which fare considerably b etter I

will provide some more discussion on style registers b elow section

Closed syllables Reduction is much easier in op en syllables

than in closed syllables It is not altogether excluded in the latter category

esp ecially not if the closing segment is a sonorant or s Furthermore we

nd reexes of the same kinds of constraints that count for tense vowels

For instance the frontmidvowel reduces most easilyAlsowordinternal

reduction is much easier than p eripheral reduction and reduction adjacent

to h or a glottal stop is blo cked Nevertheless reduction of vowels in closed



syllables is more marked than reduction of vowels in op en syllables Given



the theory outlined ab ovewehaveseveral options to analyse this eect

Prop erty Rschwa prefers open syl lables

As we will see b elow the same prop erty holds for uschwa but not for

eschwa

In the rst place we could assume this is a vowel quality eect lax

resists reduction just like high lo w or labial Wehave already

seen in chapter that diphthongization provided us with an argumentthat

Parselax should b e ranked high

Alternativelywe could analyse this eect as the result of stress Closed

syllables are heavy and we could assume for instance that even though they



See also Burzio on English reduction and Hawkins Kenstowicz and

Kisseb erth on reduction in Carib Jacobs on reduction and vowel deletion

in the history of French all of which seem also blo cked by the closed nature of the

syllable



There is some variation b etween sp eakers with resp ect to the strength of prop erty

Some sp eakers such as Bo oij nd reduction of the underlined vowel in words

suchasbe nzine id portier p orter only p ossible if the following consonant is deleted

OK OK

bnzn bzne prtr ptr Others such as Kager do not

necessarily have to delete the consonant For them the forms bnzn and prtr are

acceptable pronunciations

Prop erties of rschwa

are not stressed they still form their own abstract fo ot of some sort Under

suchahyp othetical approach they would always end up in a metrical

p osition that disfavours reduction as wehave just seen

Finally the constraint ConnectN lax could b e broughttobearupon

this issue It could b e that this constraint is higher ranked than Project

Ft Therefore lax cannot get lost in a closed syllable even if this syllable

is unstressed

Given the very intimate relationship in Dutchbetween vowel quality

syllable structure and stress it seems imp ossible to nd empirical evidence

that would shed light on this issue There is a conceptual reason whyI

opt for the feature analysis lax is a feature that wants to b e parsed

like other features This reason is that the reduction or nonreduction of

closed syllables is very muchstyledep endent andIwant to argue that style

dep endency is always a matter of reranking of faithfulness constraints It

is dicult to establish a precise p osition for Parselax in the hierarchyof



Parse constraints I will establish in the next section

Style registers Summarizing what wehave found so far wemay

say that some constraints on reduction are absolute in Dutch viz the con

straints on p eripherali ty and against adjacency to the placeless consonants

h and b Other constraints are relative viz the constraints related to

vowel quality and those referring to the precise typ e of unstressed p osition

wehave

As wehave already seen ab ove this variation is normally attributed

een three to style registers I followed Bo oij in distinguishing b etw

such registers Style I informal Style I I semi formal and Style I I I



formal

Normally it is assumed that eachsuchstyle register is a separate gram

mar This concept had to face some problems in earlier approaches based

on extrinsically ordered rules In the rst place style registers could dier

from one another in arbitrary ways in a more informal register rules could

b e i ranked dierently than in a formal register ii b e present while they

were absent in a formal register or iii b e absent while they were present

in a formal register Furthermore the discrete division of style dierences



Yet myintuition is that lax vowels vowels in closed syllables are at least easier to

reduce than high vowels If this is true at least the ranking Parsehigh Parselax

holds



Bo oij used a somewhat dierent terminology

Tomy knowledge the rst to distinguish b etween style registers within generative

phonology was Harris Of course there has b een a strong tradition of studying

registers in pregenerativework and for instance Selkirk refers to Fouche

rather than to Harris when she intro duces the concept in her study of French liaison

See Dressler for discussion of the notion style register in generative grammar

Prop erties of rschwa

in for instance three registers is somewhat arbitrary It is not easy to see

whywe should not make a division into for instance ve or ten registers

Observationally an imp ortant dierence b etween more formal and less

formal styles of sp eech seems to b e that the former are closer to the under

lying forms while the latter allow for more reduction ep enthesis deletion

and other op erations In Optimality Theory this can b e easily expressed

The more formal the style of sp eech the higherranked the faithful

ness constraints

Mayb e is a principle of universal style register comp etence I already



expressed the conjecture that it might b e the only principle This would

solve some of the problems with earlier approaches In the rst place the

relation b etween style registers would no longer b e arbitrary but regulated

by This would solve also an imp ortant learnability problem The

child only has to learn one phonological grammar the dierences in style

would b e regulated by the universal principle Furthermore weno

longer have to p osit an arbitrary number of style registers Wecanhavea

register where the faithfulness constraints are lowest in the hierarchyone

in which they are highest in the hierarchyandeverything in b etween

For Dutchwe established the following partial constrainthierarchies

a Align Contour ProjectFt ProjectFt



b Parsehigh Parselow Parselax Parselabial

Parsedorsal Parsecoronal

c Parselax

Icontend that the rst two hierarchies can b e combined in many dierent

ways I decided to disregard lax vowels hence c here Every hierarchy

is p ossible as long as the partial orders of a and b are preserved

Kagers style registers are characterized as follows

Reduces in Weak p ositions Semiweak p ositions

e formal formal

a semiformal semiformal

o i semiformal informal

y y informal excluded



Laver identies two other factors that might crosslinguistically indicate in

creasing informality of sp eech rising pitch level and range and dropping loudness level

and range Sp eech rate on the other hand do es not seem to b e a contributing factor at all

Prop erties of eschwa

As discussed ab ove in our system i will always pattern with the other

high vowels rather than with o With this exception it is p ossible to give

the rankings that will generate these three registers

a Style I I I formal Align Contour Parsehigh Parse

labial Parsedorsal ProjectFt ProjectFt Parse



coronal

b Style I I semiformal Align Contour Parsehigh

ProjectFt Parselabial

ProjectFt Parsedorsal Parsecoronal



c Style I informal Align Contour Parsehigh Project

Ft

ProjectFt Parselabial Parsedorsal Parsecoronal



More subtle dierences are p ossible in this system It mighteven b e that

vergenerate style registers For instance according to wenow somewhat o

Kager e always reduces and the high vowels in stray p osition

never do This means that Parsecoronal has a xed highest p osition

it is always b elow ProjectFt andParsehigh a xed lowest p osition



itisalways ab ove ProjectFtIamnotvery sure this observation is

strong and if it is whether this could not simply b e a so ciolinguistic

p erformance eect Mayb e we could say some styles are so overly formal

or so overly informal that they are never used Yet it seems more plausible

to say that languages x an upp er and a lower limit for their faithfulness

constraints

Prop erties of eschwa

Of the three dierentschwas we distinguish here eschwa the real svarabakhti

vowel is the least wellstudied The reason probably is that on the one

hand its distribution is relatively narrow and on the other hand this o ccur

rence is somewhat problematic A typical instance of eschwa o ccurs in the

consonant cluster at the end of the word help id hlp A pronunciation

a sounds somewhat overarticulated in the ears of of this word without schw

some native sp eakers but it is not very clear howepenthesis improves the

word Of course on the one hand wedoawaywithamarked sup erheavy

syllable On the other hand however the result is a closed syllable with

schwa as its head which is also marked in Dutch Furthermore the sono

rant l has to b e resyllabied into the onset of the schwasyllable All

in all the slight improvement of syllable structure seems to demand a high price

Prop erties of eschwa

I will assume here that eschwa really is the result of ep enthesis an

uncontroversial p oint of view as far as I can see cf Berendsen and Zonn

eveld and De Haas for the most extensive discussion of eschwa

to date Below I will discuss the prop erties of this schwa one byone

The ep enthetic vowel is schwa The rst prop ertytobedis

cussed is that it is precisely schwa that is the ep enthetic vowel and not

some other vowel In other languages other vowels may indeed act as

ep enthetic vowels For instance the Icelandic ep enthetic vowel seems to

b e u the ep enthetic vowel in Spanish is e and in Yokuts it seems to b e

i see Archangeli for an overview As far as I know however all

languages that allowvowel ep enthesis and that haveaschwa on the sur

face the twovowels are the same So the fact that the ep enthetic vowel in

Yokuts is i is an indication that this language do es not haveanyschwaat



all

The reason whyschwaisafavourite default vowel should b e attributed

to its b eing essentially featureless For one reason or another we sometimes

need to insert a vowel which is not underlyingly present but wealways do

this in the most economic way This means in the rst place that wedonot

insert unnecessary features such as coronal or high If schwa is allowed

as a vowel in the language at all schwa therefore is the preferred ep enthetic



vowel

Prop erty Eschwa is the epenthetic vowel

In Dutch the interaction of several constraints forces us to insert a vowel

between the l and the p of hlp In principle we can insert anyvowel

also an a or an orany Yet each of these vowels will b ear a set

of vo calic features viz dorsal labial lax and labial coronal lax re

sp ectivelyEvery feature constitutes a violation of a constraint against the

insertion of exactly that feature low labial lax etc Schwagives

a minimal violation of these constraints This I contend is the reason why

crosslinguisti call y schwa is the preferred ep enthetic vowel just likeitisthe



preferred reduction vowel



It is however p ossible that dierentvowels play the role of default vowel at dierent

lexical levels In that case schwamight b e p ermitted only after the lexical level where

some other vowel has played the role of default vowel This would cause some complicated

pattern of surface violations of the generalisation made in the main text I am not aware

of any system that has b een convincingly shown to p ossess these prop erties



The same line of reasoning would lead us to p osit basthefavourite ep enthetic

consonantinany language that allows the consonant at all I will not go into this matter

any further The glottal stop certainly is the ep enthetic consonant in Dutch



The ep enthetic vowel is not normally transcrib ed in Dutch writing If one wants to

Prop erties of eschwa

In languages whichdonothaveschwa a constraint should b e active

that bans this vowel The most straightfoward waytodothiswould b e the

following

EmptyRoot No empty ro ots are allowed

This constraint states that every vo calic ro ot no de needs to dominate a

vo calic no de which in turn needs to dominate a place no de andor an

ap erture no de whichhave to b e lled These constraints therefore demand

that even the default vowel in the language gets some minimal sp ecication

On the other hand we could also use a pro jection constraint to do the same

work

ProjectN V N is the head of a branching N N dominates

avo calic no de

The advantage of this constraint clearly is that wehave used it already

Since vo calic ro ots usually o ccur in the head p osition of a syllable b oth

constraints have the same empirical eect I cho ose to use ProjectN V

b ecause it is formally related to the other pro jection constraints and it ts



well within their family

The precise nature of the required feature sp ecication under the vo calic

no de to satisfy ProjectN V dep ends on the weight the language assigns

to the dierent features In one language it might b e that insertion of a

place feature is sucient and that coronal is the most unmarked place

feature In such a language the default vowel will surface as e as in

Spanish except when the feature co o ccurrence restricti on coronalhigh

is ranked suciently high in whichcasewe will get an i likeinYokuts

In other languages insertion of labial will lead to the smallest number

of violations and we will get an u like in Icelandic I predict that y can

never b e an ep enthetic vowel since it contains two place features coronal

and labial and there seems to b e no feature co o ccurrence restriction in

UG forcing the two features to o ccur together

mimic informal sp eech in writing usually one of two transcriptions is chosen viz either

hel le p oru hel lu p The rst sp elling is probably chosen b ecause e is the normal e

transcription also for uschwamode mo d and the second sp elling u b ecause

the vowel normally transcrib ed by this letter is phonetically closest to schwa in most

Dutch dialects



Indep endent evidence for either of these constraints may come from the fact that

in languages likeTurkish or Yoruba in whichvowel harmony seems blo cked internal to

a morphemeschwalikevowels are often an exception to this blo ckage they can only

o ccur in harmonic contexts Clements and Sezer Pulleyblank Bennink in

N V or EmptyRoot preparation This might b e seen then as a result of Project

dominating the constraint that blo cks morphemeinternal spreading For more discussion

on the constraint EmptyRoot see chapter and app endix A

Prop erties of eschwa

Thus for Spanish Yokuts Icelandic and Dutchwe get the following

rankings

a Spanish

N coronal labialProject

highcoronalhigh

b Yokuts

labialProject N coronal

coronalhigh high



c Icelandic

N labial coronalProject

labialhigh high

d Dutch

coronal labialProject N

coronalhigh highlabialhigh ordering irrelevant

BelowIgive the relevant rankings for an ep enthetic vowel in each of the

four languages just mentioned

a Spanish

Candidates ProjectN V labial coronal

u

o

p

e

p

i

high Candidates coronalhigh

p

e

i

b Yokuts



The ordering coronal labial seems very marked given the fact that coronal

place is in most languages the most unmarked place for b oth consonants and vowels It

seems indeed true that ep enthetic vowels in most languages are chosen from the set fi

e g Steriade ie either a frontvowel or a placeless vowel

Prop erties of eschwa

labial coronal Candidates ProjectN V

u

o

p

e

p

i

Candidates coronalhigh high

e

p

i

c Icelandic

Candidates ProjectN V coronal labial

p

u

p

o

e

i

high Candidates labialhigh

o

p

u

d Dutch

Candidates coronal labial ProjectN V

p

u

o

e

i

high Candidates labialhigh coronalhigh

p

This theory predicts that at least the vowels a e i o u can function

as an ep enthetic vowel in some language On the other hand it predicts

that a universally marked vowel like y can never b e an ep enthetic vowel

since it contains two place features coronal and labial and there seems

to b e no feature co o ccurrence restiction in UG forcing the two features to

o ccur together In other words there probably is no grounded constraint

Prop erties of eschwa

coronallabial parallel to labialhigh To the b est of myknowledge

this prediction is b orne out

ProjectN V thus constrains the ep enthesis of schwa as an empty

vowel It cannot in and of itself b e the reason for schwaepenthesis b ecause

a syllable with schwa violates Project N V justaswell as a syllable

without it I prop ose therefore that the real reason for schwaepenthesis

is another pro jection constraint viz ProjectN cons a constraint

requiring syllables to have at least a vo calic ro ot in their head

N cons N heads a branching N N dominates a Project

cons ro ot

Insertion of schwa is the most economical way of complying with Project N

cons for syllables which start out without anyvowel since schwa is ex

actly a cons ro ot

N Wethus could assume that there are twoslightly dierent Project

constraints at work in Dutch A dierent solution is also feasible how

ever This would b e to include cons in the set of vo calic features V

In that case and if we assume that the constraints against insertion of

place features are suciently high we could replace Project N cons

N V ie the following constraint ranking has the same eect by Project



N cons as Project

coronal labial dorsal ProjectN V

Wehave just seen that this ranking holds for Dutch For the sakeofsim

plicity and ease of reference I will keep however to the constraint name

Project N cons

Eschwa do es not o ccur at the end of the word One of

the biggest mysteries of Dutcheschwa is the following whyishlp in

informal styles of sp eech improved to hlp and not to hlp The

latter certainly seems b etter from the p oint of view of metrical structure

it has less violations of ConnectN lax It seems that eschwaisbanned



from absolutely wordnal p osition



Wewould also have to assume that a vowel with a vo calic no de in addition to a

N V b etter than a vowel with only a vo calic ro ot in vo calic ro ot satises Project

other words ProjectN V should b e a gradual constraint



As we will see in the next chapter there is crosslinguistic variation with rep ect to

the allowance of ep enthetic schwainwordnal p osition Many languages would prefer

wordnal ep enthesis in this case Yet Moro ccan and Berb er are instances of

languages where ep enthetic schwa never o ccurs at the end of the word either

Prop erties of eschwa

Wehave already seen that rschwa has the same prop ertyofavoiding

the word edge prop erty page The explanation I havegiven for

that schwa can b e used for eschwaaswell I assumed the constraintthat

the edges of the morpheme and the proso dic word should coincide Align

ment is very strong in Dutch barring all kinds of changes of these edges

except nal devoicing The morpheme help ends in a p Therefore

the corresp onding proso dic word should end in a p as well It preferably

should not end in an ep enthetic schwa

Wehave already seen an additional blo ckade against ep enthesis of schwa

at the end of the word One reason for putting the nal p of help in a

degenerate syllable rather than leaving it unparsed is the constraint Align

as I argued in the previous chapter hlp is b etter than hlp b ecause

its morphological and phonological edges are b etter aligned At least one

of these two constraints outweighs the requirements on syllable structure

Eschwa only o ccurs in the last syllable of the word This

constraintcontrasts with the previous one Together they have asaneect

that eschwa can only o ccur in closed syllables This makes it dierentfrom

the other schwas which preferably rschwa or even exclusively uschwa

o ccur in op en syllables

This generalisation has also b een contested byseveral authors such

as Kager and Zonneveld De Haas and Kager Ac

cording to these scholars there is one other environment for ep enthesis

VCCC ie b efore a wordnal syllable headed by uschwa

They use this observation as an argument that uschwabehaves likeaword

edge They cite paradigms like the following to demonstrate the eect I

underline eschwatocontrast it with uschwa in this example

m helmet b hl mrname c hl ma name a hl

wrm warm mrmr marble mr mtid

In my view the ep enthetic schwainhl m has a dierent status from

the one in hl mr As I already stated ab oveepenthesis is for many

sp eakers almost obligatory in the former case In the latter case however

pronunciations without schwa are p erfectly acceptable In a normal mo de

of sp eech mostofmy informants would sayhlm but hlmr wrm



but marmr

So while technically b oth schwas should countaseschwa and while

we should also explain whyeschwa is almost absolutely forbidden b efore



This observation is conrmed by Kager

Prop erties of eschwa



a full vowel as in mrmt I will put apart the marmer cases Having

done this the following prop ertyofeschwa emerges

Prop erty Eschwa only occurs in the last syl lable of the word

The reason for this seems to b e that the app earance of eschwa is closely

linked to the degenerate syllable Eschwa is found only b etween a closed

syllable and a degenerate syllable

This result conforms the facts A degenerate syllable is very marked in

manyways For instance it lacks a head which is a violation of one of the

central principles of syllable constituent theory the pro jection constraint

ProjectN V

Degenerate syllables are formed at the lexical level Schwaepenthesis

seems in all resp ects to b e a p ostlexical or wordlevel cf Trommelen

N cons gets quite strong in pro cess Assume therefore that Project

this level of phonology whilst it do es not visibly play a role in the lexical

phonology This will ban all degenerate syllables whichnow should get

vo calic heads An empty head is clearly not vo calic

The ideal syllable head is a vowel The most economic waytoget



a syllable head is byepenthesis of a vo calic ro ot For reasons discussed

ab ove this syllable head cannot follow the original content of the degenerate

syllable ie the p of help it therefore has to precede it Furthermore the

l is resyllabied into the onset of the newly created syllable I assume it

stays within the co da of the rst syllable and therefore gets ambisyllabic

N N

N

N

N

N

h l p

The p ostlexically created syllable violates some constraints that are oth

erwise unviolable in Dutch Most imp ortantly this is the only o ccurrence



These cases remain to b e a problem under any analysis I am not awareofany

prop osal that can describ e them in a satisfying way Itseemstobetruehowever that

eschwa can only o ccur in a cluster which is p otentially a wordnal cluster It can not

m since there is no draxm and bytlr since there is no btl o ccur in dr



I assume sonority restrictions are still in force excluding the p ossibility of consonan tal heads

Prop erties of eschwa

of a closed syllable without a lax feature on the head If we assume p ost

lexically the prohibition against insertion of lexical features is very strong

much stronger than ConnectN lax this apparent anomaly can b e ex

plained away More in general at several places b elow we will see that

pro jection constraints have lost their force at the p ostlexical level It seems

that p ostlexically the relation b etween feature content and proso dic struc

ture is lost More in general it is sometimes observed that for instance p ost

lexical rhythmic stress is less sensitive to subtleties of syllable structure

than lexical phonology it also do es not seem sensitive to the heavylight

distinction in syllables

Wordinternal contexts in whicheschwa do es not o ccur

There are three contexts in whichwe donotndeschwa contrary to our

exp ectations in the discussion so far This is the case if a degenerate syllable

is preceded by i an op en syllable ii a syllable that ends in a consonant

which is homorganic with the consonant in the degenerate syllable or iii

if the nal segment is a coronal obstruent These contexts are exemplied

in a b and c resp ectively

a slaap sleep slap slabp groot big rot rot

b bank id bk bk ramp disaster rmp r mp

c hart heart hrt hrt

herfst autumn hrfst hrfst hrfst

The third case is the simplest The fact that a coronal obstruentisinvolved

directly leads to the conclusion that there is no degenerate syllable in these

cases at all The coronal obstruent is simply outside the proso dic word al

together Sp ecically it is not in a syllable therefore it do es not constitute

N cons Ep enthesis is therefore not necessary a violation of Project

The relevant economy principles for instance the constraint against in

serting a ro ot no de will therefore blo ck it as they blo ck the o ccurrence of

anyschwa adjacenttoavowel

I prop ose that in the rst two cases ep enthesis is blo cked by indep endent

factors viz the Onset constraint and a constraint against intro ducing

consonantal place features We will see b elow that also uschwa disapp ears

after and p ossibly b efore a full vowel and wehaveseenabovethatrschwa

did not arise immediately adjacent to a full vowel either

The explanation in the case of rschwawas the constraint Contour

If we assume this constraint outranks ProjectN cons we get the

following result Tomake things more explicit I draw the following tableau

for the word slaap sleep slap

Prop erties of eschwa

N cons ContourProject

Candidates Contour ProjectN cons

p

slap

slap

In English a schwa is inserted b etween a dipthongized long vowel and

l or r example McCarthy Halle and one might argue

that a similar case can b e made for long vowels b efore r in some variants

of Dutchasisshown in see section in the previous chapter for

more discussion of the b ehaviour of tense vowels b efore r

feel jlfeeling jl foal fowlgoalie gowlij sure suw

assuring sur re fa j reit fa jrt

zwaar heavy zwar duur exp ensive dyr boer farmer bur

The reason might b e for instance that the high sonorancy of the liquids

and esp ecially of r makes them b etter co das than other segments I will

not however try to work this out

We are now left with the cases of homorganic clusters These include

at least the words ending in nasalobstruent clusters like bank id bk

bk and ramp disaster rmp rmp Wemay also wish to include

cases suchaskans chance kns kns and hand id hnt hnt

and even held hero hlt hlt All these latter cases however can also

b e analysed as including extraproso dic coronals A condition against schwa

ep enthesis in those clusters was indep endently needed to account for cases

suchasherfst autumn hrf st hrfs t

In previous analyses the resistance of these clusters against ep enthesis

y eect See Selkirk for was usually ascrib ed to a geminate integrit

an overview The nasal and the obstruent shared a place feature

cons cons

nasal voice

Place of Articulation

No segment could intervene b etween the two halfs of this semigeminate

Yet under the assumptions made here there are two problems with this

approach

Prop erties of eschwa

In the rst place we could think of the No Crossing Constraint of Gold

smith But if schwaisanempty ro ot no de insertion of schwabe

tween the two ro ots in should not cause any line to b e crossed at

all

cons cons cons

nasal voice

Place of Articulation

An alternative might b e to argue that the relevant constraintisNo Skip



ping Van der Hulst and Smith Archangeli and Pulleyblank

No Skipping a feature that is asso ciated to  and  in the con

guration  should also b e asso ciated to 

No Skipping Project N cons

The result of schwaepenthesis in is a violation of No Skipping Yet

there is at least one way to repair this violation and wemightwonder why



this reparation is not made We could degeminate the cluster so that

weget

cons cons cons

nasal PofA PofA voice

If weepenthesize a schwainto this cluster this will cause a violation of nei

ther the No Crossing Constraint nor the No Skipping Constraint However

this form is worse than in at least one other asp ect It contains more

instances of the Place of Articulation no de and of course also of the place

features dep endent on that no de In other words it has more violations of

the constraints against coronal dorsal or labial As wehave already

seen the reason why schwa is ep enthesize d rather than some other vowel

was to avoid violations of exactly these constraints Here we see that they

are so strong that they can even block ep enthesis



This constraint is clearly related to the Contiguity constraint at the level of syllable

structure as discussed in the previous chapter



Some other crazy reparation p ossibilities not discussed in the text are linking the

consonantal place features to the schwa deleting the consonantal place features moving

the features from the nasal to the schwa etc Although all these steps would avoid the

violation of No Skipping they would obviously result in quite absurd constructions to b e

ruled out by indep endently needed and very strong feature co o ccurrence constraints

Summary and conclusion

A third logical p ossibility is also excluded by the constraints we already

intro duced ab ove This is to disasso ciate the Place features from one of the

two segments the nasal or the obstruent altogether In that case ep enthesis

will also not result in a violation of either No Skipping or No Crossing

However if we delink the place features from the obstruent the result will

be a b or hlike element and wehave already seen that schwanever

surfaces next to such a consonant If we delink the place features from the

nasal it is not very clear what the result will b e Wemight either supp ose

that placeless nasals are blo cked altogether there is no nasal variantof

the glottal stop or the h or we might extend the interpretation of the

Contour to exclude schwa adjacenttoany placeless segment

Style registers Even though ep enthesis in words like help and

kerk church is almost obligatory in a formal style of sp eechpronunciations

without schwa are still p ossible

Very formal styles of sp eecharecharacterized by a high ranking of

faithfulness constraints as I have argues at various places ab ove In this

case the relevant faithfulness constraint is the condition against insertion

of nonunderlying ro ot no des Normally this constraintisranked under

N cons the constraint forcing vowel ep enthesis In a very Project

formal style of sp eech the constraint raises to a very high level where

ep enthesis b ecomes imp ossible in any morphological or phonological con

text

Summary and conclusion

In this chapter I have discussed the two o ccurrences of p ostlexically derived

schwawe nd in Dutch a schwa that is derived by reduction from full

vowels in unstressed p osition and a schwa that is derived byepenthesis in

consonant clusters

In b oth cases I have argued that the relevant constraint is a pro jection

constraint Reduction follows from the fact that full vowels prefer to o ccur

in the head of a fo ot when they o ccur outside of that p osition in particular

the weak p osition of a fo ot every vo calic feature F means a violation of

a constraint whichsays if a segment dominates F the segment should b e

the head of a fo ot Since schwa is the minimal vowel it gives the smallest

p ossible numb er of violations of this constraint Ep enthesis on the other

hand is an eect of a pro jection constraintwhichsays that every syllable

should haveavo calic head A degenerate syllable consisting of only a

consonant violates this constrain t We therefore prefer to insert a vowel

and if we do this in the most economic waywe again cho ose the minimal

Summary and conclusion

vowel ie the schwa

In b oth cases schwa app ears to b e a compromise b etween on the one

hand a requirementofhaving a vowel in a certain p osition and on the other

hand of havingafewvo calic features in that p osition as p ossible Making

this compromise has some cost in this case that wehavetoallow an empty

ro ot This empty ro ot cannot o ccur in all environments for instance not

after a hiatus or a h and this causes reduction and ep enthesis to b e

blo cked in those environments

It is quite striking that the blo cking environments for reduction and

ep enthesis are almost exactly the same In a rulebased theory of phonol

ogy this similarity is dicult to capture since probably reduction and

ep enthesis should b oth b e the results of dierent sets of rules in such

a theory In the constraintbased theory prop osed here there are simply

some constraints blo cking schwa to arise in some environments There are

other pro jection constraints which cause full vowels to reduce or schwas

to b e ep enthesized and these can b oth b e sub ordinate to the same surface

constraints

Similarly the constraintbased analysis allowed us to express the eects

of style of sp eech on b oth phenomena in a uniform way In b oth cases we

could say that the more formal the style of sp eech the closer the similarity

between input and output This means that in more formal styles of sp eech

w e exp ect less reduction and less ep enthesis Again this correlation is less

easy to capture in a rulebased analysis

As for the representational side of the phenomena discussed here I

think these phenomena provide fairly strong evidence that schwas are re

ally emptyvowels Phenomena of reduction of schwa and ep enthesis of

schwa instead of some other vowel are b est to b e understo o d in terms of

feature loss and representational economy resp ectivel y The pro jection the

ory predicts that emptyvowels haveavery defective pro jection b ehaviour

In the next chapter I will show that this b ehaviour is evidentinDutch

underlying schwas as well

Dutch Uschwa

Intro duction

Uschwa is probably the most hotly debated of all o ccurrences of schwain

Dutch Its place in the vowel system has b een sub ject to quite some contro

versy Some authors haveeven denied a separate status in the Dutchvowel

triangle for this vowel arguing instead that it is a sp ecial case of either

eschwa or rschwa Although I will endorse an ep enthesis hyp othesis for

some of the facts b elow I will also argue that some other uschwas necessar

ily are underlying Indeed in a theory without constraints on underlying

representation it is dicult to ban underlying schwas esp ecially if there

are schwas on the surface If everything can b e input to the grammar also

emptyvo calic ro ots can

My idea is that underlying schwainmany cases is blo cked from surfac

N V the constraint against syllable ing for instance b ecause of Project

heads without a vo calic no de Only when there are other constraints that

force the schwa to surface an exception can b e made to the general rule

One instance of such a constraint is the constraint Projectcons N

again a pro jection constraint this time forcing underlying cons ro ots to

o ccur in the heads of a syllable

Projectcons N A segmentShasacons ro ot S is the

N head of a branching

This constraint can also b e read as ParseV underlying vo calic ro ots have

to b e parsed It is hard to distinguish b etween ParseV and Projectcons

N in Dutch b ecause empty vo calic ro ots when parsed always get parsed

into a syllable

Another p ossibility sometimes suggested eg in Van den Berg is that schwa

is a variantofy or to which it p erceptually is very similar The suggestion is not

worked out seriously byany recent author however and given the resistance of other

front rounded vowels to reduction it seems dicult to reconcile with any of the other

facts ab out eschwa

Prop erties of uschwa

The underlying nature of uschwa explains some of the dierences b e

tween this vowel and the two other schwas In the rst place uschwais

already presentatthelowest level of the phonologywhichaccounts for the

fact that in some ways it is more restricted than the other schwas But on

the other hand uschwa is underlyingly present which means that it can

sometimes surface in contexts where the derivation of eschwa and rschwa

was blo cked

Prop erties of uschwa

Because of the extensive literature on this sub ject and b ecause there is

a rather large range of known facts I have decided to devote this whole

chapter to my discussion of this vowel In this section I will study the

prop erties of Dutch that have b een discussed in most recent literature and

that every theory of this vowel has to takeinto account I will defend the

hyp othesis that schwas defective nature has as an eect that it can only

head a minimal syllable viz a CV syllable without a co da or a complex

onset In the next section section I will contrast this theory to some

of its comp etitors

Syllable weigh t The rst prop erty of uschwa is already known

to us b ecause it is true also for rschwa but not for eschwa It is prop erty

rep eated here for convenience

Prop erty Uschwa can occur in an open syl lable

The standard example for this prop erty is that whereas mica id mika

and Mieke girls name mikarevalid Dutchwords the same is not

true for mik

Prop erty is esp ecially surprising under length theory if only long

vowels can o ccur in op en syllables schwa phonetically the shortest vowel



of all is not exp ected to o ccur in the same environment Yet tenseness

theory also do es not have a ready explanation for these facts b ecause schwa

is not exactly tense either

In the theory defended in the previous chapter however this problem

disapp ears The relation b etween vowel quality and syllable structure was

expressed by Connect N lax as follows



N lax N dominates lax i N branches Connect



See Trommelen and Bo oij a for the prop osal that schwa phonologically

is long

Prop erties of uschwa

If schwa is featureless it also do es not b ear the feature lax ConnectN

lax therefore makes an even stronger prediction than what is listed here

as prop erty It predicts that uschwacanonly o ccur in an op en syllable

On rst sight this prediction may seem to o strong I will nevertheless

argue b elow that it is correct For now it should b e sucient to note that

prop erty is a corrollary of ConnectN lax and the representation of

schwaasanempty ro ot

Wehave already seen that also rschwa had a preference for op en sylla

bles while the fact that eschwa o ccurred in closed syllables was the result

of indep endent and stronger factors An imp ortant dierence b e

tween uschwa and the two others is that the former can also o ccur in op en

syllables at the end of the word Wehave seen that rschwa and eschwa

never o ccur in that environment

mod fashion tant aunt lind linden tree oranj or

ange

If uschwa is really an underlying schwa this dierence is what we exp ect

The reason why eschwa and rschwawere banned from the p eriphery was

Alignment theory Metaphorically sp eaking the edges of morphemes have

to b e absolutely clean We are not allowed to reduce anything and we are

not allowed to insert anything if these op erations would change the edges

Aschwa which is underlying of course cannot b e banned from a p e

ripheral p osition in the same way As a matter of fact we predict that

p eripheral underlying schwas should always surface as such This gives us

the desired result for wordnal schwa

Uschwa does not occur wordinitially At this p oint the

question might arise whether the discussion in the previous subsection also

gives us a satisfactory explanation for wordinitial uschwa b ecause of the

following prop ertyofuschwa

Prop erty Uschwa does not occuratthebeginning of the word

There are absolutely no Dutchwords like dm This prop erty is attested



in other languages likePolish French and German as well



See Hall for German and Wetzels for French RowickaandVan de

Weijer note that the observation can b e made for the Polish yer and Hermans

pc observes that it is true for Serb oCroatian yer as well Some discussion of non

Dutchschwas will b e given in chapter

In Dutch the prop erty is restricted to lexical forms As a matter of fact most clitics

do start with schwa er r there em m him t t it es s once and een

n a Even p ostlexical schwa observes most of the other prop erties stresslessness

supp ort of sonorants coronals or k only no complex onsets See section for discussion

Prop erties of uschwa

In the case of rschwa and eschwa whichwere also banned from word

initial p osition I claimed there were twocontributing factors The rst one

was Align and the second was a prohibition against syllables of which

neither the head nor the onset haveany features

As explained ab ove Align cannot b e the explanation for a ban against

underlying schwainwordnal p osition b ecause uschwa does o ccur in such

a p osition It seems therefore reasonable to assume it also cannot ban schwa

wordinitiallyIhave already p ointed out that even if wewould assume

an asymmetry b etween Alignleft and Alignright all evidence seems

to p oint in a direction where Alignleft is stronger than Alignright

for instance the b oundary b etween prexes and stems cannot b e crossed

by syllabication whereas the b oundary b etween stems and suxes can If

Align do es not work we are left with the second explanation The word

initial syllable seeks an onset This onset can only b e lled by a glottal

stop which results in an illicit completely empty syllable Such an illicit

syllable will also surface when we do not insert an onset segmentatall

Wehave already seen that the constraint against totally empty sylla

bles called Contourisinviolable in Dutch grammar Since it do es not

directly conict with Onset its actual p osition in the hierarchyishowever

irrelevant for the present analysis as long as its dominates Align

ContourAlign

dm

Candidates Contour Align

dm

p

dm

bdm

No matter where werank Contour with resp ect to Onset the form dm

will always come out as the optimal form as long as ContourAligna

ranking for whichwe will see some indep endent evidence b elow

As was the case with eschwa and rschwa we exp ect schwa to b e blo cked

also after h This exp ectation is b orne out indeed

a hV b h c h

abrahm abrahm abrh

There are no forms where we nd a lexical schwa after h

Some other segmental eects Other prop erties of schwathat

seem of relevance for any theory of Dutchschwa are the following

Prop erties of uschwa

Prop erty Consonant clusters before schwa undergo as if

they were syl lablenal

m helmet b hl mr name c hlma name a hl

wr mwarm mr mr marble mrmt id

Prop erty In some Dutch dialects sp is metathesized at syl lable end

and before schwa but not before ful l vowels Stroop

a vsp vps wasp

b mspl mpsl medlar

c spirin psirin Aspirin

Both of these prop erties seem to show that the consonant cluster b efore a

schwabehaves as a syllablenal consonant cluster as has rst b een noted

by Kager and Zonneveld I will call this typ e of theory which claims

that schwa do es not license a full syllable the NoSyl lable Theory

Unfortunately the scop e of these two prop erties is rather limited In

particular the variant of Dutch with b efore a schwasyllable is

anything but widespread Yet many dialects have metathesis at the end of

the word This seems to b e an argument against rather than in favour of

an analysis in whichwe generalize over schwaandword end

The same is true for the ep enthesis facts as wehave seen Ep enthesis

b efore schwasyllables seems to b e of a dierent nature than ep enthesis at

wordend Another thing is that the NoSyllable Theory has some problems

in explaining metathesis itself If schwa do es not form a syllable there is

no obvious reason whyitwould b e inserted at all

The next generalisation sometimes listed as a prop ertyofschwa Kager

and Zonneveld Kager is even more questionable

Prop erty x and diphthong plus r are excludedbefore schwa as

wel l as syl lablenal ly whereas they may occur before ful l vowels

The eects of these constraints are illustrated in whichlikemany

other examples in this section are copied from Kager

a V V rV b V V r c V V r

i j i j i j

urora ur ur

a xV b x c x

xora xl x

It is questionable whether these facts have to b e attributed to the presence

or schwa Indeed Gussenhoven has shown that these facts might

Prop erties of uschwa

b etter b e seen as fo otlevel wellformedness constraints The wellformed

diphthongr and x sequences in and are separated bya

fo ot b oundary The stress patterns of aurora and angora are urora

and xora resp ectivel y

Now the fact that these sequences cannot o ccur to the left of a schwa

can b e seen as a result of the fact that schwa cannot o ccur at the leftedge of

a fo otin other w ords since Dutch feet are leftheaded that schwa cannot

o ccur in the stressed p osition of a fo ot

The onset of schwasyllables Inow turn to a prop ertyof

Dutchschwa which is quite sp ectacular

Prop erty Schwa cannot have a complex onset

This prop erty is demonstrated in the following example

a katrl pulley b katrl c katr

dyplo duplicate dypl dypl

Also this prop erty seems go o d evidence for the NoSyllable Theorysince

p ossible complex onsets also are not allowed wordnallyasissho wn in

c Clusters b efore schwabehave as co das not as complex onsets b e

cause they are co das if schwa do es not pro ject a syllable and if we further

more assume that all consonants preferably incorp orate into a syllable

In its original form Kager and Zonneveld prop ertywas some

what stronger stating that consonant clusters b efore schwa cannot havea

rising sonority

More recent literature has brought up a signicantnumber of counterexamples

words with rising sonority sequences b efore schwawhich are disallowed at

the end of the wordto this version of prop ertyhowever some of which

are quite systematic the following examples are from Kager and Zon

neveld

drachme drachma drxm Ariadne name aritn

butler id bytlr ritme rhythm rtm

Dafne name dfn franje fringe frnj

kubisme cubism kybsm partner id prtnr

tarwe wheat trw orgasme orgasm rxsm

ordner le rtnr rapalje rabble raplj

drxm bytl dfn etc

All of the words in have a rising sonority sequence b efore schwa In

terestingly these clusters not only cannot o ccur wordnally but they also

are not p ossible onsets For instance the tl sequence of butler wealso

Prop erties of uschwa

nd in atlas id which is syllabied as atlas tlis not a p ossible onset

in Dutch

Admittedly a signicantnumb er of these words are fairly recentloans

Yet isme is a pro ductive stressattracting sux words like franje part

ner tarwe and rapalje seem to b e very much adapted to the Dutch sound



system and wealsohavetoexplainwhyaword like ritme is b orrowed in

exactly this form unlike for instance its English counterpart

The fact that words like are p ossible is a severe problem for the

NoSyllable TheoryIfthel of butler is not licensed in the co da of the rst

syllable and schwa do es not pro ject its own syllable this segment should

b e licensed in some other way In that case it is dicult to imagine why

for instance the r of the imaginary but imp ossible katrl could not also

b e licensed in this other way

I b elieve that in contrast prop erty is fairly wellestablis hed for word

nal schwa Counterexamples are a few learned words like oeuvre and

timbre which at least in the ears of the present author sound very foreign

very Frenchlike the full vowel of the rst word still has its French quality

the full vowel of the second is even nasalized while nasal vowels do not exist

in Dutch phonology They seem to b e real loan words

Interestinglyschwa in other p ositions in the word more in particular

wordinitially do es not seem to ob ey the same restrictions Here we nd

words like the following

plezier joy plzir brevet certicate brvt bretels braces

brtls

It seems to me that the schwainvolved here really is rschwa All of these

forms havesomewhat markedpronunciations with e instead of In

terestingly for some sp eakers the surface schwaofprcies seems to b e

reinterpreted as uschwa These sp eakers pronounce the wordasprcies

so with a simple onset and a sonorant segment in the co da

I p ostp one the question whyuschwa do es not license a complex onset

to section b elow Here I want to note that wealsohave to exclude the

logically p ossible syllabication ktrl but that this can b e accomplished

by indep endent means since lax vowels can never b e followed by p otential

complex onset clusters as wehave seen in section in chapter

The co da of schwaheaded syllables The facts just mentioned

are very interesting but b efore we can start to analyse them we rst haveto

consider one other prop ertywhich is probably somewhat more controversial

than the previous ones



See chapter for discussion of words ending in Cj with C a coronal consonant

Prop erties of uschwa

Prop erty Schwa can license at most one sonorant in the coda

Prop erty a somewhat weaker version of prop erty implies that while

bekr b eaker and vogl bird are Dutchwords the same cannot b e said

for kagk or vogp This observation needs some motivation b ecause

it is usually ignored or even denied in the literature Cohen et al

Kager and Zonneveld

First of all observe that there are no words ending in C C ifC is

i j j

not a coronal obstruent

a vadrk

wndlp

b lieverd darling livrt

anders dierent ndrs

vervelend b oring vrvelnt

Since coronal obstruents at word edge are arguably outside the Proso dic

Word the Proso dic Words in b end in schwa plus one consonant a

shows that after a schwaheaded syllable a degenerate syllable is not al

lowed

Secondly the consonantinarhyme b efore a coronal can only b e a

sonorant Compare the words in b to the ones in a with schwa

plus a noncoronal obstruent b elow and the ones with normal tense and

lax vowels in b

a adpt

ob elks

ktarkt

OK

b adpt adept

OK

nks nothing

OK

ktarkt cataract

OK

haks squared

OK

feks shrew

If we assume prop erty is true this gap can b e explained

Inow turn to the words where schwa is followed by one consonant and

no coronal Prop erty claims that this consonant can only b e a sonorant

or a coronal obstruent which is not incorp orated into the proso dic word

but Kager and Zonneveld claim that virtually anything can follow



schwa They give the following list



Native words ending in t are hard to nd if any exist at all The ones listed are

the only ones I could nd and they almost all seem English loans I have no explanation for this gap

Prop erties of uschwa

n m s

wapen weap on bezem bro om gratis free

baken b eacon bodem b ottom vonnis judgement

zegen blessing bliksem lightning kermis fun fair

oefen exercise goochem clever dreumes to ddler

adem breath stennis trouble

k uw

havik hawk schwaduw shadow wereld world

monnik monk zwaluw swallow arend eagle

perzik p each zenuw nerve mosterd mustard

hinnik neigh peluw pillow mieters sup er

t fp l

lemmet blade tinnef garbage dubbel double

pocket pap erback sherri id koppel couple

racket id hennep hemp wikkel ller

ticket id tokkel pluck

There are on rst sight three classes of problems in this table for our pro



p osal Those are the words ending in k in p and in f resp ectivel y

Words ending in f and p are very rare The words ending in f apart

from the two mentioned by Kager and Zonneveld I have only found

gannef swindler are loan words from Yiddish or English Words ending

in p are p erhaps even more marked The only examples other than the

one Kager and Zonneveld mention are Wezep and Gennep b oth

names of towns I will put them apart for this moment only to briey

return to them b elo w p

So the real problem is the relatively large class of wordsendingin



k and a class of words not mentioned by Kager and Zonneveld

probably b ecause it is hard to nd relevantwords that are uncontroversially

monomorphemicviz the class of words ending in x

x

gezel lig cosy zlx



I will not discuss the uw cluster here b ecause the problems related to this cluster are

not central to my present account It is included for the sake of completeness Trommelen

has argued that unstressed uw should b e derived from underlying w This

theory has some attractive prop erties which will b e briey discussed in chapter



Apart from the words listed in this table there also is a large group of adjectives

ending in the denominal derivational sux lijk lk eerlijk erlk honest litt honour

ly heerlijk herlk delicious litt lordlike gevaarlijk varlk dangerouslitt dangerly

Prop erties of uschwa

keurig neat kqrx

heftig heavy hftx

overhandig hand over ovrhndx

korzelig cantankerous krzlx

Most of these words ending in ig are adjectives although there are some

exceptional verbal forms suchasoverhandig probably historically derived

with the adjectivizing sux x which is still pro ductiveinmod

ern Dutch This etymology is now opaque b ecause the stems of the forms

in are no longer available as indep endent elements

I prop ose that in all these cases the schwa is not underlying but is

an instance of rschwa the result of a reduction pro cess applying to an

underlying b efore a dorsal obstruent in an unstressed syllable This

assumption is supp orted by the following facts of Dutch phonology

There are no Dutchwords ending with unstressed x or k on the

surface This can b e explained if there is a reduction pro cess applying

obligatorily Otherwise it is an unexplained gap in the system

In Dutch there are a few allomorphs of the agentive sux most im

p ortantly r and aar The morphological rule governing the choice

between the allomorphs considers the preceding syllable if this con

tains schwa the choice will b e aar otherwise it will b e r Compare

for instance wandlaar wandlr wanderer luistraar

luistrr listener beoefnaar beoefnr practitioner

OK OK

with sprekaar sprekr sp eaker and beztaar beztr

o ccupant etc

r pre The words in k and g all select r instead of aar predik

dikaar preacher punnikr punnikaar someb o dy who knits with



There is one recalcitrantword havik hawk which seems to b e pronounced by some

sp eakers with in spite of stress on the rst syllable Zonneveld suggests that

the actual reduction rule should refer to the quality or length of the preceding vowel

reduction of p osttonic short frontvowels would only apply when the immediately

preceding syllable is short

Yet the few Dutchverbs ending in m are problematic for this prop erty b ecause

they select r instead of aar admr someone who breaths bezmr someone

who uses a bro om sweep er

There also are some forms in nr etc but they have to b e analysed as lexical

exceptions For instance the word opener do es not just mean someone who op ens

but like its English alternative has the instrumental reading the instrumentby which

one can op en something This instrumental reading is not available for wandelaar

luisteraar beoefenaar etc

One nal remark is that agentive nouns formed on the basis of some stems ending

r schildraar schildrr painter in r are somewhat irregular schild

timmrman timmraar timmrr carp enter

Prop erties of uschwa

a sp o olknitter zaniker zanikaar someb o dy who nags frunnikr

frunnikaar ddler

If an adjective ends in a derivational sux r or n there are no

suxes m or l it cannot get overt inection cf

het mo oimo oi huis

the b eautiful agr house

the b eautiful house

het houtnhoutn huis

the woodaff agr house

the wo o den house

de AmsterdamrAmsterdamr meiden

the Amsterdamaff agr girls

the girls from Amsterdam

The adjectivizing sux ig x do es not pattern with the schw asuf

xes nor do es lijk lk

a het baasx baasx meisje

the bossaff agr girl



the b ossy girl

b het eerlk eerlk meisje

the honourly agr girl

the honest girl

Again this b ehaviour can b e explained if one assumes that ig has

the underlying structure not and similarily that lijk

lk not lk

The Dutch rst p erson singular pronoun ik uncontroversially has an

underlying if it is stressed In clitic p osition however this

can b e reduced to schwa

In very formal styles of sp eech the vowel in words as aardig nice

and monnik monk surfaces as I have to admit that the relevant

style of sp eechisvery formal and therefore I am not sure that this

pronunciation is not to b e attributed to inuence of sp elling

In some pragmatic contexts one can nd het bazig meisje and het eerlijk meisje

indeed Inectionless adjectives give some sp ecialized meaning to the DP Crucial is that

one can never say de opene deur

Prop erties of uschwa

Morphological comp ounding is a pro ductive pro cess in Dutch Nor

mally one of the segments s or is inserted b etween the two

memb ers of the comp ound Historicallythismight b e related to the

fact that b oth segments also served as plural inection and genitive

case but this seems no longer relevant in the contemp orary morpho

logical system Trommelen and Zonneveld

The choice b etween s and null is very complicated One of the

most imp ortant factors undoubtably is the phonological makeup of

the rst element If this wordendsinavowelconsonant group the

normal choice seems to b e a Dutch sp elling conventions re

quire that the schwa b e sometimes written as en If the rst element

of the comp ound ends in schwaconsonantwe never nd as the

intermediary segment Most of the time they shownointermediary

segment at all b The only exception is the agentivesuxr

which combines with s c

Nound ending in k there are no nouns in x like monnik demand

a as the intermediary segment the signs denote morpheme

b oundaries just like most other p erson nouns ending in a syllable

headed by a full vowel

a mannenwerk mnwrk mens work

zonneschijn znsxin sunshine

schapevlees sxapvles litt sheeps meat

mutton

b kleuterwerk kltrwrk to ddlers work

heuvelrug hvlryx chain of hills

bezemsteel b ezmstel bro omstick

havenloods havnlots harb ourpilot

c schilderswerk sxldrswrk painters work

d monnikenwerk mnkwrk litt monks work

tedious drudgery

This again shows that words ending in k pattern with other words

ending in full vowel plus consonant not with words ending in schwa

plus consonant

I conclude that there is evidence for a reduction from toinDutch

It probably is not a coincidence that the underlying lax vowel that reduces

so easily is In the previous chapter I have already argued that is

the lax counterpart of e and wehave seen in this chapter that e is the

vowel that is most susceptible to reduction

One could observe that most of these forms have a simple onset just like forms with

a real schwa This is unexplained by the reduction analysis since Dutchvowel reduction

Prop erties of uschwa

Ihave no explanation why exactly the dorsal obstruents trigger this

pro cess in so many cases Mayb e this is only a historical coincidence We

can analyse the words hennep gannef and even tennis and others in the

same way The tests listed in the main texts seem to p oint in this direction

wherever they applyFor instance wend tennisr tennis player in stead

of tennisaar Finallypronunciations like tenn s sherr f and henn p

seem to b e marked but not imp ossible in contrast to forms suchas adm

breath adr vein and opn id According to this criterion all



schwas b efore an obstruent are therefore rschwas

Degenerate and schwaheaded syllables Inowhave explained

why all the cases of surface schwaobstruent sequences have to b e treated

in a way dierent from schwasonorant sequences The former are cases of

reduction the latter can b e real cases of uschwa

Only the former are to b e seen as really unproblematically o ccurring at

the surface From this it follows that every theory of Dutchschwa should

trytogive an explanation for prop erty

The rst solution that comes to mind is that these wordnal schwas

may b e followed by a degenerate syllable But then new problems arise

First why should this degenerate syllable only contain sonorants The

same is certainly not true for degenerate syllables following other vowels

Second why is the same prop erty also attested in nonnal syllables cf

vrlies lose vkbies Third why are there no words like the following

vadrk voglp konk

These facts seem to indicate that the following prop erty holds for uschwa

Prop erty Uschwa cannot b e fol lowedbya degenerate syl lable

This prop erty has to b e explained rst Why do syllables headed byu

schwa and degenerate syllables exclude each other I want to relate this

to the fact that in monomorphemic forms we do not nd sequences of

seems to b e hardly blo cked by complex onsets at all On the other hand since almost

all these forms are historically derivations with x I b elieve that the complex onset

test cannot b e used as a go o d test



Another p ossibility is that the schwainvolved actually is eschwa A relatively large

numb er of these words have a nasal b efore the schwawhich is not homorganic with the

obstruentfollowing it It can b e that in one way or another these words have underlying

nasalobstruent clusters which are marked for not b eing homorganic and that ep enthesis

applies almost obligatorily in these cases It remains a puzzle under that analysis why

hn p sounds more acceptable than rm I will not explore this p ossibility further here

Prop erties of uschwa

schwaheaded syllables Forms suchas kabml or kantrl are



absent from the Dutch lexicon

Ihave already argued that the favoured metrical p osition of schwais

in the weak p osition of a fo ot Although this has b een left implicit in the

discussion of the degenerate syllable in chapter the same should b e said

for this constituentaswell Word nal sup erheavy syllables almost always

attract stress as in speculaas biscuit emigrant id just like syllables

followed byschwa Stress also falls normally on the syllable just b efore

schwa as in oranje orange

In the analysis presented here sup erheavy syllables as such do not ex

ist Sequences like laas in speculaas or grant in emigrant are analysed as

sequence bisyllabic units sp ekylas emirnt parallel to the ranje

of oranje ornj

Since the Dutch fo ot is leftheaded and since schwaheaded syllables and

degenerate syllables prefer to b e in the weak p osition of a fo ot rather than

b eing unfo oted this probably means that a fo ot is built on the last full



syllable and the degenerate syllable in the case of socalled sup erheavies

I assume therefore that b oth schwasyllables and degenerate syllables

have to b e in the weak p osition of a fo ot as a result of the following

instance of ProjectFt





ProjectFt V N is the head of a branching fo ot N domi



nates a vo calic ro ot

By logical transp osition ProjectFt V is equivalent to the statement





N does not dominate vocalic features Nisintheweak position of a

branching foot In other words schwa has to o ccur in the weak p osition of

a fo ot

This constraint is the reverse of ProjectV Ft that I have discussed



in the previous chapter I assume it is inviolable at least in the lexical

phonology of Dutch Therefore every syllable that do es not have a normal

head ie either a schwasyllable a or a degenerate syllable b can

only o ccur in the weak p osition of a fo ot



Sequences of degenerate syllables are also absentbutwehavegiven an indep endent

explanation for this fact in chapter Furthermore descriptively sequences of a degen

erate syllable followed byaschwasyllable if the consonants in this schwasyllable are

coronal are also found eg aarde earth ard I will return to these latter cases

b elow



Apparent exceptions to this generalization forms in which degenerate syllables o ccur

wordinternally or in which they are p eripheral but do not receive primary stress very

often include a high vowel which means they may b e analysed in a dierentway namely

as one exceptionally closed syllable not followed by a degenerate syllable as wehave seen

in chapter

Prop erties of uschwa

N b N a

 

N N

k k

This combined with the assumption that Dutch feet are tro chaic and max

imally binary Kager gives us the desired result Two incomplete

syllables cannot o ccur next to each other b ecause in that case they would

b oth have to app ear in the weak p osition of a binary fo ot Which is imp os

sible by denition

If sequences of a schwasyllable plus a degenerate syllable are not al

lowed we still have to nd the reason why sonorants can follow uschwa

My hyp othesis is that in these cases the schwa is not the actual head of

the syllable It mayeven not b e present in the relevant part of phonology

at all Iwanttocontend that in the lexicon the level relevant for the

present discussion the nal syllable of theater id tejatr is tr with

r the head

N



N

t r

The schwa is later ep enthesized in a way to b e discussed b elow An em

pirical argument for this analysis may come from level I nonGermanic



suxes such as adjectivizing aal when combined with theatr this sux

gives tejatral not tejatral Bo oij De Haas and Trommelen

Similarily we nd pairs suchaslter id N fltr ltreer

to lter fltrqr and monster id mnstrmonstrueus monstrous

mnstrys On the other hand this criterion is not watertight since

there are many other pairs in whichtheschwa either is not deleted at

all or otherwise changes into a full vowel b efore exactly the same suf

xes puber adolescent pybrpuberaal like an adolescent pubral

lapostolaat ap ostolate apstolat ether pubral apostel id apst

id etretherisch ethereal eteris etc The morphologyphonology

interface cannot b e sub ject of the present study

The reason why it is exactly the class of sonorants that is allowed to

followschwacannow b e understo o d Many languages that allow syllabic



All vowelinitial derivational suxes are level I except for adjectivizing achtig

which is the only bisyllabic sux without schwa

Prop erties of uschwa

consonants restrict this set to the class of sonorant consonants The



same assumption could b e made for the lexical phonology of Dutch

Obligatory versus optional ep enthesis This leaves us with

the following two questions

i why is there obligatory ep enthesis at all

ii why are the sonorants after ep enthesis lo cated in the rhyme

br rather than in the onset br We will see in chapter

that in French the latter option is chosen rather than the rst one

The rst question has already b een answered in the discussion of eschwa

There we assumed that at the p ostlexical level a constraint holds which

states that syllables havevo calic heads cf example on page

Clearlyvo calic heads are missing from syllables with consonantal heads

as well as from degenerate syllables without anyheadatall Both are

therefore predicted to undergo ep enthesis in the p ostlexical phonology

Apotential problem is that there are very formal styles of sp eechwhere

no ep enthesis applies in degenerate syllables like help Even in those styles

of sp eech theater is still pronounced tejatr never as tejatr There is

a and ep enthetic uschwa a dierence b etween eschw

Wethus havetodraw a distinction b etween syllables without a head

which are allowed in some styles of sp eech on the one hand and sylla

bles with a consonantal head whicharenever allowed to surface from the

p ostlexical phonology

This is the p ointtointro duce the rst weakness constraintofthis

thesis The reader might recall that weakness constraints are formally the

opp osite of pro jection constraints The latter connect certain features to

head p ositions whereas the former connect certain features to dependent



p ositions

In chapter I will discuss a weakness constraint related to the feature

high In this case I prop ose that we can make the relevant distinction

between degenerate syllables and syllables headed by a sonorant consonant

with a weakness constraint which refers to the feature consonantal

Weakcons N A segmentSbearscons S app ears in the

dep endent p osition of N



Some Eastern Dutch dialects allow nasal consonants to stay syllabic also in the p ost

lexical phonology Most German dialects also allow syllabic liquids in p ositions which

corresp ond to r sequences in Standard Dutch



A similar distinction is drawn b etween Peak constraints and Margin constraints in

Prince and Smolensky Yet the theory presented there crucially hinges on the

notion of an intrinsinc whereas the one here do es not

Prop erties of uschwa

A syllable with a consonantal head violates but a syllable without a

head do es not If wenow assume the following ranking

N cons formal style ProjectN cons cons Weak cons

informal style

The weakness constraint dominates the constraint against insertion of schwa

ie a bare vo calic ro ot no de cons in all styles of sp eech It is therefore

always b etter to insert a schwa in syllables with consonantal heads For

degenerate syllables only the constraint Project N cons is relevant

and the p osition of this constraint with resp ect to the faithfulness constraint

cons is variable across style registers as wehave seen ab ove

Wethus have to conclude that the p ositioning of faithfulness constraints

in the hierarchy is not totally free Apparentlywedohave to p osit some

b oundary on their p osition in this particular case at least an upp er b ound

ary for cons b elow Weak cons N

Again on complex onsets The answer of the second question

in lies in the interaction of several constraints we discussed b efore

Let us lo ok at the two p ossible outcomes of schwaep enthesis into a head

p osition of tr

a N b N

N N

 

N N

t r t r

Given general conditions on syllable structure eg sonority restrictions

in the co da these are the only p ossible ways of providing a vo calic head

The reason why a should b e chosen at least at the end of the word

rather than b is clear bynow It is the same reason whyanytyp e of

nonunderlying schwa is forbidden to arise at absolute wordedge Wehave



The need for another theoretical subtlety arises in the analysis of Dutch dialects

sp oken in northern and eastern parts of the Netherlands hinted in the previous subsec

tion In these dialects schwaepenthesis is obligatory in syllables headed by a liquid ie

r or l but it is blo cked if the head is a nasal ie m n or Nijen Twilhaar

These dialects thus have syllabic nasals on the surface but no syllabic liquids

The reason why nasals in some grammars are more acceptable than liquids despite there

lower level of sonority in other asp ects of syllable structure is an interesting issue not to b e explored here

Prop erties of uschwa

already seen that this is true for rschwa and eschwa It should b e true for

the particular ep enthetic schwa in trtyp e syllables as well The reason

for this was the constraint Align

This constraint do es not apply to nonnal trtyp e syllables so that

in these cases other constraints b ecome relevantinthechoice b etween a

and b One imp ortant condition is of course ConnectN lax Since

schwa do es not b ear any features it also do es not b ear the feature lax

Therefore its o ccurrence in a closed syllable is disallowed in the lexical

phonology and the representation in b should b e preferred

Yet the scarce data that wehave seem to p oint in a dierent direction

The reader might recall that some sp eakers of Dutchsayprsis for precies

precise normally pronounced as prsis I already argued ab ovethatit

might b e that in this case an original rschwa alternating with presis

is reanalysed as uschwa This would corresp ond to a change in syllable

p osition All schwas preceded by a complex onset seem to b e rschwa

The only other piece of evidence I have b een able to lo cate is that there

is a prex ver of the relevant structure There are no prexes starting

with a complex onset and schwa The string vr furthermore o ccurs in

anumber of words in which synchronically it no longer functions as a

prex verstand exp ertise verlies lose verdriet grief

In so far as we are allowed to make generalisations over this very small

set of evidence we seem to get a rather than b There should

therefore b e another factor favouring the r in the co da over the r in the

onset Recently some researc hers have suggested that complex onsets

p erhaps have to b e licensed by a fo ot in a certain wayDavis Van der

Hulst a Takahashi If this is true and if furthermore it is true

that schwa cannot pro ject its own fo ot this could explain whywordnal

and wordmedialwordinitial schwasyllables b ehave the same with resp ect

to syllable structure However the data remains scarce and therefore it

might b e to o early to draw denite conclusions

Another question that now arises is whywe never nd syllables with

both a complex onset and a sonorantcoda In other words wehaveto

exclude the syllable typ e in b elow b oth wordnally katrl and in

other p ositions trlzir

Prop erties of uschwa

N

N



N

t r l

As far as I know there is not one single exception to this generalisation

neither wordinternally nor wordnally Since degenerate syllables and

syllables headed by a real underlying schwaalsodonothave complex

onsets wemayformulate the generalisation that syllables without vo calic

features do not allow more than one branching N

Let us lo ok more closely at the syllable structure used in this dissertation

o designates the terminal no de of syllable structure the ro ot no de of

feature trees

N

N

N



N

o o o o

Nisdepen Wehave seen that the branchingness or nonbranchingness of

dent on a quality of the head viz the presence or absence of lax We

have also seen that vo calic features play a role in proso dic structure higher

than the syllable where total absence of vo calic place and height features

implies a weak p osition in a fo ot and vice versa Wehaveseenthateven



the internal structure of the N might b e dep endentonvo calic features It

seems only natural then to assume that vo calic features also playarole

in the branchingness of the highest level of syllabic Xbar structure N

Because all full vowels allow complex onsets wemay assume the require

ment on the branching of Nisvery similar to the one on the branching of

higherorder structure anyvo calic feature of the set fhigh low lax

coronal labial dorsalg is sucient but at least one of these features

Prop erties of uschwa

N We can therefore use the constraint is necessary to license a branching

Project N V encountered at various places ab ove

This is one of the twoversions of ProjectN that we encounter The

other one was ProjectN cons the constraint forcing at least some

vowel to b e the head of a syllable even if this vowel is schwa

To complete the set of p ossible Project N constraints I can mention

ProjectN F This constraintwould require syllables to have at least

some feature material in their head Since VFand Vcons ab

stracting away from secondary articulation on consonants for a moment

on which see chapter these three constraintsareinaPaninian relation

If a syllable lacks features from F initsheaditalsolacks the feature

cons or any feature from V Similarily if a syllable has a head of which

therootisnotcons this head also has no vo calic features from V

The three constraints thus corresp ond to the following markedness hi

erarchy

Candidates ProjectF Projectcons ProjectV

no segmental head

p

consonantal head

p p

schwahead

p p p

full vo calic head

Ihave found no strong evidence for p ositing ProjectF when wehave

already p osited the other two constraints Wehaveseenabove that the real

distinction b etween degenerate syllables and syllables with a consonantal

N More head has to b e made byaweakness constraint Weak cons

discussion on this problem will b e provided in chapter

There are two N no des in We exp ect there to b e no dierence

between the higher and the lower syllable no de in If the lower

instance of Nisnotallowed to branch the higher instance similarily should

not b e allowed to do so Strictly sp eaking this means that schwa syllables

should not b e allowed to have an onset at all

This is not in accordance with the facts wehave found Schwasyllables

allow for onsets but not for complex onsets A lot of the argumentation

ab ove is based on this assumption

There is one constraint whichisrelevant for our present concerns

This is the constraint Onset rememb er that this constraint can also b e

The theory presented here is crucially based on the absence of an onset constituent

Onsetbased theories that do refer to notions similar to the ones here head branching

etc are presented byVan Lit and Charette

Prop erties of uschwa

formulated as a pro jection constraint as shown in chapter A syllable

wants to have a sp ecier an onset segment regardless of the feature

structure of its head Wehave already seen at various o ccasions ab ove

that this wish for an onset is very strong in Dutch In chapter we

will see that Onset can force a syllable to cross morpheme b oundaries in

many languages Supp ose wehave an underlying p otential syllable tr

Supp ose furthermore that Onset dominates ProjectN V inDutch

Four parses are relevant

Candidates Onset ProjectN V

tr

p

t r

p

t r

tr

If we leave b oth consonants unparsed we create an onsetless syllable Al

though this is exactly the structure ProjectN V favours it is ruled out

b ecause it do es not meet Onset On the other hand if we parse b oth

consonants this will certainly satisfy OnsetButwe violate Project N

V twice unnecessarily so b ecause derivations where one consonantisin

the Onset and one is unparsed hence with one branching N only do also

exist Which of the latter two torrwilleventually b e chosen has

to b e decided by indep endent constraints for instance constraints having

to do with sonority slop es or Contiguity of parsing McCarthy and Prince

b Wordinitially Align will probably cho ose the rst consonant ie



in this case the t

N Taken together the three pro jection constraints Onset Connect

lax and Project N V have as an eect that syllables headed byuschwa

or a sonorant consonant are core syllables ie syllables consisting of a head

preceded by one single consonant This typ e of syllable seems universally

unmarked It is interesting to see that in Dutch the segments with sub



minimal vo calic quality can only head this minimal typ e of syllable



Given the curious fact that no words exists starting with a sonorant schwa

m r etc wemay also b e led to conclude that in the cases at hand

t will b e chosen as the prop er output On the other hand as I have p ointed out

ab ove it is dicult to draw conclusions from the absence or presence of schwa syllables

with a certain onset given the overall scarcityofsuch syllables



In the previous chapter wehave seen that reduction in Dutch is not blo cked by

complex onsets b ecause reduction is p ostlexical There is at least one language how

ever where reduction schwa seems to b e sub ject to the same conditioning as Dutch

uschwa This language is Macushi Carib Hawkins Kenstowicz and Kisseb erth

Prop erties of uschwa

Schwa surrounded by identical consonants Schwacanthus

b e followed by sonorant consonants in Dutch It also can b e preceded by

every consonant except for h and the glottal stop Yet the following

sequences seem never to o ccur monomorphemically

mm rr ll

It is not true for instance that schwa cannot b e surrounded by consonants

b ecause other combinations do o ccur

rl rn rm rr

kerel guy toren tower harem id

korrel p ellet koren corn

ml mn mm mr

stamel stammer samen together marmer marble

rammel rattle amen id emmer bucket

nl nn nm nr

ponem face toner id

panel id binnen inside

ll ln lm lr

molen mill golem id trailer id

propel ler id

Only words ending in C m are hard to nd but this maysimply

son

reect the fact that m sequences in general are rare Furthermore for

rm and lm sequences after lax vowels this probably has to b e related

to the complementary distribution of uschwa and eschwa section

More in general we can thus observe the following prop ertyofschwa

Prop erty C C theconsonants preceding and fol lowing schwa may

i i

not be the same exceptwhenCn

i

Both in derived and underived environments wedondnn sequences

n serves among other things as the most common pluralitymarker on

nouns binnen inside boonen b eans baanen jobs etc On the

other hand rr clusters even seem imp ossible to derive in those con

e morpheme which normally shows up texts For instance the comparativ

as rkwaader angrier takes a der allomorph when combining with

Normally words in Macushi show an alternating pattern of schwas and full vowels

starting with schwawnamri mirror wanmarr mu mirror but this pattern

can b e disturb ed by closed syllables which do not reduce as well as syllables with com

plex onsets which also do not reduce Thus wendsibmrikpe little now instead of

sbmirkip b ecause the rst syllable is closed and kratpe alligator now in stead of

krtup b ecause the rst syllable has a complex onset

Prop erties of uschwa

a stem ending in r zwaarder zwaarer heavier There are no suf



xes l m so that we cannot test the validity of prop ertyfor

p olymorphemic domains with other consonants

In chapter we will see that prop erty is also attested for Norwegian

p olymorphemic words Now the explanation for prop ertyisactually

quite straightforward under the theory defended here Since schwaisas

sumed to b e inserted only p ostlexicall y in these cases all these sequences

would lexically have the form C C with the two consonants in the onset

i i

and the nucleus of the syllable resp ectivelyWehave seen go o d evidence

in chapter and also earlier in this chapter that there has to b e a contour

between onset and nucleus This explains whywe do not nd ji or h

sequences and now it can als help us explain the blo cking of ll rr mm

etc

As for the derived forms it is actually unclear to me whynn se

quences are freely allowed whereas rr are blo cked Wehave seen that

also in underived words we do nd nn sequences What should make the

coronal nasal so sp ecial is at presentamystery to me

Schwa after ng The next prop ertymay at rst sightseemto

b e a problem for the p oint of view just outlined

Prop erty mayoccur before schwa as wel l as syl lablenal ly whe

reas it is excludedbefore ful l vowels



This prop erty is illustrated in the following example

a V b c

ora l

bo

As is shown by the example bo this pattern cannot b e reduced to a

constraintonwellformed feet This prop erty seems a rather strong argu

ment for the NoSyllable Theory The distribution of is probably to b e

explained by a lter banning dorsal nasals from the onset p ositon This

prop ertysho ws that the schwainengel is the only vowel that do es not

attract this consonantinto its onset All other vowels force this consonant

in a p osition where it is banned by indep endent constraints



I assume that the sux ing in for instance verdringing suppresion vrdr

litt surpressing is underlyingly where the is optionally reduced to schwa as

it is b efore other dorsal consonants



Notice that the theory presented here is not compatible with an analysis of as

the placeless nasal as defended in Trigo If where a placeless ro ot wewould

exp ect it to avoidbeingintheonsetofschwa just likehandb

Prop erties of uschwa

This would explain the nonexistence of aora the cannot b e in

the co da probably b ecause of the Syllable Contact Law but it also cannot

app ear in the onset If in al the schwa has no syllable it do es not force

into its onset

Any theory contesting the NoSyllable Theory will havetoprovide for

a explanation of this fact A rst observation we might makeisthatis

only found if the head of the syllable is a sonorant consonant more precisely

a liquid or n I have found no instances of plus real uschwa while

the numb er of examples with consonantal heads esp ecially l is suciently



large

angel sting engel angel zwanger pregnant

honger hunger wingerd vineyard Engels English

tenger slender klungel bungler pingel higgle

The also has to b e preceded byalaxvowel aangel eengel etc

which probably is to b e explained by the fact that it can only o ccur in

co das I supp ose that the pattern therefore is the result of some constraint

blo cking to o ccur to the left of vowels Lacking any evidence on what

the form and nature of this constraint could b e however I will leavethis

matter op en for future research

The reasons whyeschwa and rschwadonothaveproperty on the

other hand is quite clear Ep enthesis cannot apply after b ecause

can b e followed only by coronal obstruents or by consonants that are

homorganic to ie dorsals But wehaveseenthatepenthesis never

applies directly b efore coronal obstruents ie at the end of the proso dic

word or in homorganic clusters Reduction on the other hand cannot

happ en to full vowels in V clusters b ecause as wehave just seen

full vowels simply are never allowed to o ccur in these p ositions lexically

Postlexical reduction to schwaafter is thus bled by the lexical constraint

p osited here

Uschwa and stress The next two prop erties of schwa are

related to the stress system

Prop erty Schwa is stressless



There are a few names ending in like Inge Slinge etc There are not

manywords ending in an n or ns jongen b oy tangens tangent and a few other

Ihave found no words in m or Many inectional suxes start with schwa

and combine freely with words ending in showing that the lters do not prohibit sequences crossing morpheme b oundaries

Prop erties of uschwa

Prop erty When schwa is the nal vowel of the word

a If schwa is preceded by one or moreconsonants stress is on the penul

timate syl lable

b If schwa is not precededbyconsonants stress is either on the penultimate

or on the antepenultimate syl lable

As we will see in the next chapter the stressles sne ss of schwa prop erty

is a recurring feature of languages that include schwa in their vowel system

Wehave also seen that it is a prop ertyofDutchrschwa and eschwa as

well

Prop erty is less widespread crosslinguistic all yTogether with prop

ertyitsays that schwaisinvisible for stress In a way a syllable headed

byschwa b ehaves as an extrametrical syllable Normally stress in Dutch

is on the nal or prenal syllable If the nal vowel of the word is schwa

however stress falls on the prenal or the antep enult It is sometimes said

that schwa is stressattracting

The rst clause of this prop erty has already b een explained ProjectFt



V required stressless schwatobeintheweak p osition of a binary fo ot This

implies the syllable immediately preceding schwa should b e in the strong

p osition of the fo ot It gets stress and since primary stress in Dutch is gen

erally on the nal fo ot this means it gets primary stress A schwaheaded

syllable b ehaves like a degenerate syllable in this resp ect

The second clause is somewhat more problematic It describ es the

stress b ehaviour of words like Belgie Belgium blij Italie Italy

italij Sicilie Kirgizie Alexandrie alksndrj Veluwe velyw

Betuwe betyw the last two are names of regions in the Netherlands

er patrician patrsijr agrarier farmer ararijr patrici

We do not exp ect there to b e a dierence b etween schwa preceded bya

closed or by an op en syllable In b oth cases the schwa should b e forced into

the weak p osition of a fo ot ie we exp ect all the words just mentioned to

b ehaveasAleksandrie But in fact the latter word b ehaves as exceptional

and is itself sub ject to regularization towards Aleksandrie in the sp eechof

manyspeakers Something seems problematic with these examples

A few things are o dd ab out the examples mentioned All relevant cases

haveahighvowel The reason for this is not very clear It cannot b e

b ecause these vowels are the source of glide insertion b ecause in p olymor

phemic forms glides are also pro duced after mid vowels zeeen seapl

zejn reuen male dogpl rjn Furthermore all these examples

are either top onymsviz those ending in or names of p ersonsviz those



ending in r The adjectival variant of these top onyms do es not include a

sc hwa Belgisch blis Kirgizisch krizis Veluws velyws It might



Iamaware of one nontop onymic example ending in weduwe female widow

Prop erties of uschwa

therefore b e that these nouns are morphologically complex formed bya

ro ot ending in i and a stressneutral sux consisting of schwaoreven

j The corresp onding adjectives then consist of the same ro ot plus s

We nd a few forms not ending in ijthatmight give some evidence for

this SpanjeSpaans SpainSpanish spansspnj As in many languages

the top onymic stratum is however full of exceptions for instance twoad

jectives corresp ond to Italie Italisch Italic and Italiaans Italian which

makes it dicult to test this hyp othesis

Adjacency b etween schwa and full vowels Prop erty

again is found in Dutchaswell as in other languages that haveschwa

Prop erty Uschwa cannot occur next to a vowel

In my discussion I want to distinguish b etween two cases viz prevocalic

and p ostvo calic schwa I will discuss these cases in this order

In monomorphemic forms we cannot see what happ ens if V

sequences are input to the grammar Such a sequence do es not surface

but there is no way to observe whether this is the result of schwa deletion

consonantepenthesis or some other means

We can directly see what happ ens if we add a sux starting with a full



vowel to a stem ending in uschwa One example stem is elite id and an

example sux is adjectivizing air qr The resulting adjectiveiselitair

elitqr not elitqr The schwa gets deleted or remains unparsed

I assume that after suxation wehavetwo relevant candidates elitqr

where the ax forms an onsetless syllable and elitqr where schwais



deleted and the t is parsed into the onset of the sux syllable The

weduw In the rst place it is not clear whether this word is not morphologically

complex cf weduwnaar widower weduwnar In the second place it mightbethat

the second syllable of this word contains a schwa itself at some level of abstraction

Trommelen See Zonneveld for a line of reasoning similar to the one

presented here

Ihave also found a form ending in ir which do es not denote a p erson viz terrier

id



Dutch shows an asymmetry b etween prexes and suxes in that the former but

generally not the latter form a syllabication domain on its own syllable b oundaries

do not generally cross the b oundary b etween prex and stem whereas it do es cross the

b oundary b etween stem and sux although there is a class of suxes which also form

their own domain One eect of this is that across the prexstem b oundary wedond

V sequences eg geeerd honoured qrt I refer to Bo oij

Langeweg and Van Oostendorp for discussion



Of course there are innitely many other p ossibilities like leaving the full vowel of

the sux unparsed or inserting a glottal stop I assume these p ossibilities are ruled

out by constraints on the insertion and deletion of vo calic and consonantal material

resp ectively

Prop erties of uschwa

rst form violates Onset the second violates the requirement to parse

schwa ie the feature cons

Wehave already seen that Onset is virtually inviolable in Dutch We



may therefore assume the following hierarchy

N A vo calic cons ro ot should b e the head Projectcons

of a syllable

OnsetProjectcons N ParseV

This gives us the following tableau for eliteair

Candidates Onset Projectcons N

elitqr

p

elit qr

It is b etter to ignore the emptyrootoftheschwa than to create an onsetless

syllable or a sequence of empty ro ots Since stemsux combinations seem

to b ehaveinidentical ways with resp ect to syllabication as monomor

phemic forms I assume that the b ehaviour of these two domains with re

sp ect to schwa deletion is also similar Also underlying V sequences

in monomorphemic forms will b e resolved by deletion of the schwa

I already discussed the most imp ortant reason for the ban on V

sequences in the chapter on eschwa and rschwa An empty syllable would

result in these cases ie a syllable with empty ro ots in b oth nucleus and

onset and insertion of a glottal stop do es not help b ecause the glottal stop

itself is an empty consonant Again it is dicult to test what happ ens to

underlying schwas that are underlyingly adjacenttovowels in monomor

phemic forms so that again wehave to take recourse to sequences arising

from suxation

The schwa seems to never surface in these cases but it is dicult to

nd really hard evidence for deletion of schwa There are three schwainitial

axes an adjectival agreement marker r forming the comparative

on adjectives and n forming the plural of nouns and verbs As far as

bines freely with stems ending I can tell neither of these suxes ever com

in nonstressed vowelsthey com bine with stems ending in stressed vowels

but these have b een analysed in chapter as ending in an empty consonant



underlyingly On the other hand there is no real evidence for schwa

deletion in these cases Often simply a dierent allomorph is chosen For

instance the plural sux after an unstressed vowel is neither n nor n



Ihave already argued that Projectcons N has a similar eect as ParseV



This consonantwould probably diasapp ear after suxation

Prop erties of uschwa

but s The plural of familie family famli is famlies not famlieen

or famlien cf genie genius zn genieen genies see Lieb er and

Bo oij for more discussion Similar patterns are to b e observed for

the other schwainitial suxes

Thus while the suxed forms do provide us with some evidence that

schwa cannot followfullvowels they do not inform us as to what exactly

happ ens phonologically when a V sequences arisethey are a voided



by morphological means already

Complementary distribution of uschwa and eschwa The

next prop erty of uschwaIwant to discuss is prop erty

Prop erty Uschwa and eschwa areincomplementary distribution

In all phonological contexts where wehaveuschwa we do not haveeschwa



and vice versa As wehave seen ab ove ep enthesis applies b etween two

consonants if they are together at the end of a syllable and they do not

form a homorganic nasalobstruent sequence We can say that uschwa

o ccurs everywehere but there The question is whyuschwaisblocked in

the environments just mentioned

This prop erty is demonstrated in the following table

eschwa arm id rm rm

help id hlp hlp

kurk cork kyrk kyrk

uschwa al les all ls ls

ritme rhythm rtm rtm

ticket id tkt tkt

The correct generalisation seems to b e that we cannot haveuschwa ie

nonalternating schwa b etween two consonants if these consonants could

b e the co da of a syllable and a following degenerate syllable

There are no words like arrem rm with a stable uschwa meaning

they could not have an alternative realization rm This fact may seem



There are a few monomorphemic forms of which the stem ends in a vowel which

use the form stemn to denote the plural and the innitive gaan go doen do

zien see staan stand The problem is however that these forms are monosyllabic

so that we do not exp ect these to need schwa deletion and furthermore that these are

strong verbs with a very exceptional paradigm also for other forms so that they cannot

b e used as evidence



Two names are problematic in this resp ect Wil lem William whichdoesnothave

apronunciation without a schwa wlm wlm and contrasts with lm whichhas

an optional eschwa flmflm and Col lem which similarily contrasts with olm elm

Prop erties of uschwa

quite curious but I b elieve the explanation b ehind this distribution is quite

straightforward Supp ose wehave an input rm

The rst two segments are parsed into one syllable What can wedo

with the other two They cannot form one closed syllable b ecause uschwa

is not allowed to o ccur in a closed syllable in the lexical phonology as

wehave seen in the previous section They also cannot each form their

own separate syllable b ecause this would involveaschwasyllable and a

degenerate syllable which are not allowed adjacent to one another b ecause

they b oth need to nd a place in a weak p osition of a fo ot

This means it is imp ossible to parse b oth segments One of the two

has to go There are two reasons why this should b e the schwa In the

rst place schwa is only a ro ot no de with one feature cons whichis

very easy to get rid of as wehave seen ab ove In the second place the

m is at the edge of the word whichwewanttokeep clear as muchas

p ossible b ecause of AlignTheschwa do es not get parsed and m forms

a degenerate syllable with ambisyllabic r in its onset This means the

output of lexical phonology for rm will b e rm just as for rm

An underlying schwa has no eect in these circumstances

Let us now turn to another pair in for a moment al les versus

als I assume the rst word has the underlying structure ls and the

second form has the underlying structure ls In b oth cases the s can

b e extraproso dic b eing a coronal obstruent This means that in the second

form there is no need for ep enthesis as wehave seen in the section on e

schwa In the rst form on the other hand the schwa stands at the end of

the proso dic word It can surface in a normal op en syllable with the again

ambisyllabic l in its onset There is no need to leave the underlying ro ot

no de unparsed Therefore the rst form will leave the lexical phonology as

ls and the second as ls The two forms are correctly predicted to b e

dierent

Schwadeletion To conclude this discussion of uschwa I have

to saya word ab out schwa deletion In some informal mo des of sp eech

schwa is deleted in some phonological environments This pro cess seems to



aect all instances of schwa uschwa and rschwaalike if it applies at all



I assume eschwa is excluded from deletion b ecause it makes no sense to assume

aschwa is rst ep enthesized just to get deleted afterwards Since I assume that b oth

ep enthesis and deletion are p ostlexical phenomena and since the theory of derivations

used here is Optimality Theory in which they b oth are the result of output lters it is

not even p ossible to assume some vowel is rst ep enthesized and then deleted

I also have to remark that in the history of Dutch a rule of ap o cop e has applied

deleting some wordnal schwas As a result of this we nd a few isolated cases of

alternation suchasHereheer lord That this is not related to the optional pro cess

discussed here is easily observable the rst form can only refer to the Lord in a religious

Prop erties of uschwa

In my description of the facts I partly follow Bo oij a but my

judgements of some of the facts are partly dierent Bo oij found the fol

lowing generalisation

Prop erty When a Dutch word has two consecutive syl lables with schwa

as their vowel the rst of these schwas may be deleted

As wehave seen sequences of schwasyllables can only b e derived The

second schwa can b e in an ax or in an enclitic It do es not really seem

to matter however whether the rst or second schwa are uschwa eschwa

or rschwa The two considerations together lead us to the conclusion that

this pro cess is most probably p ostlexical

The context for the deleted schwamust b e obstruent liquid accord

ing to Bo oij a The second schwa can never b e deleted the nal n

in some of these forms is pronounced in some dialects but not in others

a tokkelen to pluck tkln tkln

b dobberen to oat dbrn dbrn

c verbeterem correct him vrb etrm vrb etrm

d knuelen cuddle knfln knyn

e kietelen to tickle kitln kitln

Ihave to note here that there app ears to b e some disagreementonthese

data among nativespeakers of Dutch Although I have no problem in

accepting Bo oijs judgementsmostofmy informants do not seem to agree

Henk van Riemsdijk pc notes that to his ear this typ e of sounds

more typical of p o etic language than of informal sp eech and Harry van der

Hulst pc feels that the liquid gets a syllabic head rather than part of a

complex onset I will nevertheless assume that the facts in are correct

Ben Hermans pc observes that in his dialect the elision is not p ossible

at all

In e we see that at this p ostlexical level of the derivation wecan

derive onsets which are not allowed at earlier stages There are no words

starting with tl

Bo oij observes that universally obstruentliquid clusters are the most

favoured typ e of onset In myspeech deletion is actually allowed b etween

anytwo consonants as long as the second is more sonorous than the rst

So I can delete a schwabet ween kn in tekenen draw teknn

teknn or b etween ml in rammelen rattle rmln rmln

Tomy ear kietlen is in no way b etter than teeknen This probably means

sense whereas the second can also refer to a wordly lord or gentleman

Prop erties of uschwa

that in my grammar the notion p ossible onset is dened even more lo osely

at the p ostlexical level than in Bo oijs grammar For me anything with a

rising sonority slop e is acceptable at this level

Bo oij a p oints out that it is not really necessary that the vowel

immediately following the deleted schwaisaschwa itself Full vowels can

also o ccur A more relevant generalisation seems to b e that schwadoes

not delete if the next vowel b ears stress geraamte skeleton ramt

beloven promise bloven beraad delib eration brat Some lexicalize d

cases are exceptions to this suchasgeloven b elieve lovn lovn

gereformeerd Calvinist refrmert refrmert terecht rightly trxt

trxt Wemay assume that these words have acquired a lexical entry in

which the rst schwa is no longer present cf also Standard German glauben

b elieve

Some sp eakers can also delete rschwas in syllables that are followed by

syllables with primary or apparaat apparatus parat

prat prat kapelaan chaplain kpelan kplan kplan koppeling

clutch kplkpl Again high frequency of words is a furthering

factor here and it might b e that these sp eakers have a separate entry for

these words

The generalisation I want to explain is thus the following

Prop erty Schwa can be deletediitapp ears before a nonstressed

vowel and between two consonants C C whereC has a lower sonority

i j i

than C

j

In the next chapter we will see that in Frenchvowel deletion is much more

common and much less restricted to certain styles of sp eech There I will

analyse the deletion in terms of a constraint Charette has termed

the Avoid Vowel Principle In the terminology used here the Avoid Vowel

Principle is a constraint against cons ro ots This constraintwillonly

aect schwas if the Parse constraints on the vo calic features high low

lax coronal labial and dorsal are ranked ab oveit

I adopt Bo oijs a suggestion that the second restriction means

that schwa can only b e deleted when the resulting cluster is an onset to

the remaining vowel I cannot answer the question why the restrictions on

p ossible onsets are less strict at the p ostlexical level It seems generally to

b e the case that this typ e of restriction is lifted at later levels Borowsky

Mohanan

Consider the nal part of tokkelen The following two partial syllabi cations are p ossible

Prop erties of uschwa

a N N b N

N N

N N N

N

k l n k l n

In very formal styles of sp eech the rst form will b e chosen b ecause it con

tains more underlying material In less formal styles of sp eech faithfulness

b ecomes less relevant In particular the constraint Projectcons N

forcing the surfacing of the feature cons is relatively lowranked as we

haveseeninvarious sections ab ove Other constraints b ecome more rele

vant such as the constraints against vo calic ro ots b is in this resp ect

more optimal than a b ecause it contains less vo calic ro ots ie less

schwas Wehave also already seen that the ranking of cons can vary

in the Dutch p ostlexical phonology corresp onding to style register

The reason whyschwa cannot delete b efore a stressed vowel is probably

to b e attributed to an integrity eect Since Dutch has a tro chaic stress

system a pretonic schwa is separated from the following syllable by a left

fo ot b oundary It may b e that syllables cannot b e built over formerly

established fo ot b oundaries cf Rice

Conclusion Ihave tried to give a full accountofthebehaviour

of those instances of schwa that alternate neither with full vowels nor with

zero Although I found supp ort for the ep enthetic nature of uschwain

some cases namely when it is followed by a sonorant this is not neces

sarily true for all instances of uschwa Both sonorants and uschwacan

pro ject only a very limited typ e of syllable Full vowels can pro ject more

complex syllables obstruents cannot pro ject any syllable at all Neither

supp orts co das b ecause neither b ears a feature lax Neither supp orts

complex onsets b ecause Nnodeshave to b e supp orted byvo calic features

so any extra N no des apart from the one needed to satisfy Onsetis

banned When ep enthetic schwa is inserted in a syllable headed by a sono

N is resp ected even if this means disresp ect for rant consonant this ban on

ConnectN lax Thus p ostlexicallywehave the ordering ProjectN

V ConnectN lax

Like all other schwas uschwa can never o ccur in stressed p osition and

it even has a very strong preference for the weak p osition of a fo ot It

Prop erties of uschwa

seems to b e banned from other p ositions b oth the strong p osition of a fo ot

and the unfo oted p osition like a degenerate syllable I consider it one

of the more imp ortantinsights of recent theories of Dutchschwalike the

No Syllable Theory that they have drawn a parallel b etween degenerate

syllables and syllables headed byschwa This parallel is retained in the

prop osal made here

All these prop erties follow from the pro jection theory plus the assump

tion that schwa is totally empty As a consequence schwawillalways count

as a more marked syllable head than a full vowel b ecause it lacks a vo calic

no de but it will also count as less marked than a consonantal head or a

completely empty head

Since in the previous chapter I noted that it is attractive to take the

phonetic emptiness of the schwavowel as our starting p oint I will add a

note on the learnability of the system presented here We could assume

that initially all pro jection constraints are ranked very high Simplifying a

bit this means that the child exp ects to hear no schwa this b eing sucha

bad syllable head At a certain p oint she hears the word mode From this

word she can drawseveral conclusions i some faithfulness constraints the

constraints against insertion of features whichwould makethevowel a full

vowel and parsing constraints havetoberanked higher than ProjectN

V ii OnsetProject N V She still do es not exp ect schwas with

complex onsets with co das or in stressed p osition Since she never encoun

ters suchwords and furthermore will never nd other evidence for a low

ranking of the relevant pro jection constraints she will continue to correctly

assume such forms do not exist

Assume furthermore a child encounters the word theater She then

knows that schwa can o ccur in closed syllables at least at some phonological

level If she has already encountered instances of eschwaar m she

knows that it is most economic to assume that this level is the p ostlexical

N lax at that level anyway level b ecause she needs to rerank Connect

This leaves her with teatr for the lexical level Since complex onsets cannot

o ccur with schwa at that level they certainly also do not o ccur with empty

heads since b oth typ es of syllable violate ProjectN V in the same way

The child therefore adopts an analysis in which r is the nucleus of the

syllable The minimal change to get this would b e to assume that sonorants

can b e syllable heads Since the child never encounters any evidence that

also obstruents can b e such heads this will b e her nal conclusion

Previous analyses of uschwa

Previous analyses of uschwa

As I havementioned in the intro duction to this chapter Dutch uschwa

has b een the topic of several studies In this section I will discuss some of

these previous studies and compare them with my ndings I distinguish

roughly b etween three typ es of analysis Ep enthesis Theory according to

which uschwaisalways the result of ep enthesis Reduction Theory accord

ing to which uschwaisalways the result of reduction and the NoSyllable

Theory according to which uschwa is underlying but for one reason or



another do es not pro ject a syllable

The Reduction Theory and the Ep enthesis Theory have the advantage

of global economy we indep endently need reduction and ep enthesis rules

to account for rschwa and eschwa resp ectively The problem is that we

nd uschwa in lo cations where neither rschwa or eschwa o ccur and vice

versa The NoSyllable Theory has the advantage of making a lot of inter

esting and correct predictions But some of its predictons are wrong and

furthermore it has little or nothing to say ab out the similarities b etween

uschwaontheonehandandeschwa and rschwa on the other

Some or mayb e most authors adopt mixed versions of these theories

For instance Zonneveld assumes that schwaisalways ep enthetic

except in absolute wordnal p osition where it is the result of reduction

The p osition I defended in the previous sections can b e seen as a combi

nation of the insights of Ep enthesis Theory and the NoSyllable Theory

Nevertheless it is useful to study the extreme p ositions to see their relative

merits and problems I will therefore discuss these three theories and some

of their representatives b elow each in their own subsection

Reduction Theory I will discuss two instances of a reduction

approachtouschwa Brink and Zonneveld

Brink Reduction Theory has probably b een intro duced

at least in generative grammar by Brink This scholar claimed that

e and inDutch are in complementary distribution e is found exclu

sively in stressed p osition while schwa is found exclusively in unstressed

p osition For reasons of economy of underlying representations then Brink

prop osed to take e as the underlying vowel in b oth cases and havearule

changing this vowel into schwa in stress environments



In earlier literature eg Cohen et al it is sometimes assumed that schwa

is not dierent from other vowels at all I hop e to have made clear that at least this

p osition is untenable

Previous analyses of uschwa

Wehave already seen that rschwa most often alternates with em uch

more so than with any other vowel It has sometimes b een observed for in

stance by Kager that the reduction rule that allegedly applies in this

case has some prop erties that makes it dierent from the optional reduc

tion rule which results in rschwa For instance the latter do es not apply

wordnallyaswehave seen Yet a substantivenumb er of examples exist

whichshowwordnal schwa like mode elite etc Another dierence is

that in the words justmentioned the nal vowel must b e pronounced as

schwa Even in the most formal styles of sp eech the nal vowel cannot b e

pronounced as mo de elite

In some Dutch dialects the schwahasaslightly fronted and lowered

pronunciation approximately as or I consider this to b e a matter of

phonetic implementation b ecause it applies to all schwas also for instance

to eschwa which nob o dy supp oses to b e derived from underlying e

Although these dierrences b etween the two reduction rules make Brinks

analysis somewhat less attractive they are not necessarily crucial

ob jections against his theory which is stated in the SPE framework It

simply is not excluded that there are two dierent rules

Amuch stronger ob jection is that a lot of the o dd prop erties of schwa

remain unexplained under this account like the restriction to simple onsets

the prohibition of following degenerate syllables etc Of course eachof

these restrictions could in principle b e incorp orated into the reduction rule

but only at the cost of complicating the rule to an extent that has proven

to b e unacceptable to most authors

Zonneveld Interestingly Brinks theory has



b een partly revived in Zonneveld Zonneveld prop oses a threefold

analysis for uschwa

Uschwa b efore sonorants is the result of ep enthesis just as in my

prop osal laid out ab ovemy prop osal is inspired by Zonneveld

in this resp ect

Uschwa in axes is underlying schwa The distribution of this vowel

is restricted to these axesit do es not o ccur in stems

Stemnal Uschwa as well as rschwa is the result of reduction of

e

The dierences b etween rschwa and stemnal uschwa for instance the

absolute imp ossibilit y of pronouncing mo de is still unaccounted for in



It is also defended in Trommelen and Zonneveld

Previous analyses of uschwa

this framework Furthermore most of the sp ecial prop erties of uschwa are

still unexplained Zonneveld mentions the prop erties broughtup

in Kager and Zonneveld and as wehaveseenatvarious o ccasions

ab ove he explains away most of these This is not true however for most

of the new observations made here For instance while Zonneveld

seems right in disproving the claim that schwa syllables can have no onsets

at all he do es not account for the fact that the schwasyllable can havea



simple onset only

Also the admittedly small group of words ending in unstressed ee is

at rst sight a problem These are words like toee mentioned earlier

Zonneveld observes however that these words are always bisyllabic

Polysyllabic forms like dominee vicar domine do not count b ecause they

havestressontheantep enult For technical reasons in Zonnevelds

ideas ab out stress this means the nal e forms a fo ot of its own Poly

syllabic forms with p enultimate stress and nal stressles s e do not o ccur

However in my account it is also true that leftadjacency to a stressed

syllable and b eing a coronal mid vowel form very favourable conditions

for reduction This do es not mean we cannot make a distinction b etween

rschwa and uschwa

The strongest argument against the reduction analysis of wordnal

schwa in my view is that wehave seen that the grammar in all other cases

avoids deriving a schwa in the absolute wordnal p osition The schwa

derived from e would b e an exception to this The only way to understand

this is that in a way stressles s e would b e much more oensivethanany

other vowel Yet e is apart from schwa itself the most undersp ecied

vowel It is unclear to me whyavowel with only a coronal sp ecication

could b e so much more unnatural then an y or u that it can override

the otherwise inviolable prohibition against wordnal derivation of schwa

One other somewhat surprising feature of Zonnevelds theory is

that real schwaisonlyallowed in axes I am not aware of any other

convincing analysis in which the set of segments allowed in axes is bigger

than the set allowed in stems Furthermore this prop osal fails to capture

the similarities b etween axal schwa and the derived schwa in stems for in

stance its stressless ness the fact that it can only b e followed by a sonorant

etc

On the other hand the analysis of the Dutch stress system and the

parallelism b etween sup erheavies and schwa syllables seems very convincing

to me I have tried to incorp orate those features of Zonnevelds theory into

the one presented in this thesis



He mentions a few examples but these are either the clearly foreign forms uvre

and timbre with a foreign and a nasalized vowel resp ectively or names of French p eople

or lo cations Sartre Montmartre

Previous analyses of uschwa

Ep enthesis Theory Schwa can b e derived in a way other than

reduction Wehave seen that there is indep endent evidence that schwacan

also b e the result of ep enthesis Another p ossible way to go is therefore to

assume that all u schwas are the result of ep enthesis

Wehaveseenabove that this hyp othesis is succesful for the uschwas

b efore wordnal sonorants like theater and vogel bird It has b een pro

p osed for this context byDeSchutter Ho eksema Ter Mors

Van der Hulst and Van Lit Zonneveld and Hermans

a

In other contexts for instance word nally mo dulo coronal obstruents

the p osition is much harder to maintain The problem is that there are quite

a lot of near minimal pairs of which one form has a schwa while the other

one do es not

a sonate sonata sonat sonat

stramien canvas stramin stramin

als if ls ls

trein train trin trin

b paraat prepared parat parat

lawine avalanche lawin lawin

al les everything ls ls

terrein terrain trin trin

Wehave already seen that eschwa and uschwa are in complementary dis

tribution It is unclear how to distinguish b etween als and al les if all schwas

are the result of ep enthesis

For this reason most of the authors pleading for an Ep enthesis Theory

of uschwa either ignore the facts in or otherwise supplement their

theory with some additional theory on the schwas in For instance

Zonneveld assumes these schwas are the result of reduction and

Hermans assumes these schwas are extrametrical vo wels to which

some version of the NoSyllable Theory applies

NoSyllable Theory An ingenious and inuential theory ab out

the idiosyncratic b ehaviour of uschwa has b een prop osed by Kager and

Zonneveld According to this theoryschwa is not able to head a

complete syllable If the bimoraicity requirement or ConnectN lax is a



requirement on syllables it do es not aect schwa This approachhasbeen



However the problem is that as we will see undeniably at a certain level of repre

sentation schwa has to form a syllable If we do not assume this syllable is bimoraic

Trommelen the problem is that even at that level short syllablenal vowels should b e excluded

Previous analyses of uschwa

further formally worked out in eg Kager and Hermans I

will henceforth refer to this family of approaches as the NoSyllable Theory

The NoSyllable Theory dep ends on level ordering It has to b e assumed

that the bimoraicity requirement applies only at an early level of lexical

derivation It can b e demonstrated that at a later level the consonants in

frontofschwa actually have to b e resyllabied into the onset in other

words that schwa has to form a syllable

Like many other languages Dutch has a Final Devoicing pro cess which

can b e provisionally formulated as the following rule see section for

a fuller discussion

Final Devoicing

son voiced

This rule explains whyaword like moed mud courage is pronounced

as mut Bo oij and Rubach argue that the Final Devoicing rule has

to b e assigned to the last stratum of Lexical Phonology cf Trommelen

The precise statement of Final Devoicing and its interaction with

other principles will b e discussed in section For now the formulation

given here is sucient

This implies that the Bimoraic Constraint has to b e checked at an earlier

r level Final Devoicing do es not apply to a word like moeder mud

mother This shows that at the level of Final Devoicing the d no longer

is uniquely in the co da of the rst syllable

I will now turn to several variants of the NoSyllable Theory that have

b een prop osed These are Kager and Zonneveld Kager Her

mans and Nijen Twilhaar

Kager and Zonneveld According to the original pro

p osal of Kager and Zonneveld wordnal schwa can form a sp ecial

typ e of app endix the terminology is from Halle and Vergnaud to



phonological words called a schwappendix This schwapp endix consists

roughly of schwa followed by an arbitrary numb er of consonants The

schwapp endix could b e preceded by a regular app endix consisting of either

one noncoronal consonantoroftwo coronal consonants Both app endices

are optional C stands for a coronal obstruent

c

 

C C

o

word

C C C

c c c

It is true that there is one prop ertyofschwasyllables whichIdidnot

takeinto account in the previous sections For instance words like klooster



See Visser for an application of this theory to Frisian

Previous analyses of uschwa



monastery klostr are not uncommon in Dutch However the struc

ture of these words is not very clear under the accountgiven ab ove The

syllabication should b e something like klostr The problem is the s

There is no ro om for this segment in either the rst syllable whichendsin

a tense vowel or in the second one which cannot have a complex onset

It can also not form a degenerate syllable b ecause degenerate syllables and

syllables headed byschwa cannot b e adjacentaswehave seen I will return

to this problem in section b elow

Kager and Zonnevelds theory makes a lot of interesting predic

tions For instance it predicts the lack of complex onsets b efore schwabe

it by merely stipulating that a wordnal schwa can only b e preceded by

an app endix consisting of one noncoronal while the schwapp endix itself

can not start with a consonant the stressneutral and stressattracting

b ehaviour the nono ccurrence of some clusters b efore schwa Wehave seen

however that some of these alleged prop erties were later argued to b e less

essential to schwathanwas thoughtby Kager and Zonneveld On

the other hand some of the new prop erties listed ab ove cannot b e cap

tured in their framework like the obligatoriness of an onset

One imp ortantdrawback of the schwapp endix analysis is that it predicts

that uschwaisalways rightp eripheral in the word Wehave seen that this

is simply not true and uschwa can o ccur virtually anywhere in the word

provided sucient other material is present to build a legitimate syllable

Furthermore the theory seems to b e dicult to incorp orate into a some

what more restrictive theory of extrametricality Usually it is assumed that

either a segment or some phonological constituentsuch as the syllable can

be marked extrametrical None of the two app endices conforms to this

criterion

More imp ortantly the organisation of the two app endices and esp ecially

of the schwapp endix in is largely arbitrary There is no sp ecial reason

whythevowel in this app endix can only b e schwa or why it cannot b e

preceded bytwo noncoronal consonants instead of only one This means

that many of the most crucial prop erties are simply stipulated and some

imp ortant generalisations are missed

I think Kager and Zonnevelds pap er is empirically the most

imp ortantcontribution to the discussion ab out schwa that has b een made

hitherto It intro duced two ideas into the discussion In the rst place this

was the No Syllable hyp othesis the idea that schwas do not license syllables

or complete syllables In the second place this was the idea that schwas are

extraproso dic and fall outside of the proso dic word I think however that

Zonneveld has shown quite convincingly that the latter part of the



The Dutchword is not morphologically complex

Previous analyses of uschwa

theory is not tenablethe most imp ortan t problem is that it cannot explain

why stress falls invariably on the antep enultimate syllable in words ending

in schwa Final stress in the proso dic word is otherwise not the regular

pattern in Dutch at all

Kager Most of the problems just mentioned were also

noted by Kager Instead of the schwapp endix he develop ed an

even stronger version of the NoSyllable Theory According to Kager

the schwa is the only vowel that has no underlying mora All other vowels

have one or two moras dep ending on whether they are long or short Kager

assumes that at level I schwa do es not build a syllable at all b ecause only at

that level those rules apply that pro ject syllables from moras Furthermore

Kager assumes that the segments preceding the schwa get incorp orated into

the preceding syllable which thereby b ecomes sup erheavy The structure

of bende band thus b ecomes at level I

b n d

At later levels the d has to b e resyllabied into the onset of a newly

pro jected syllable headed byschwa in order to escap e nal devoicing

Because in Kagers theory wordnal sup erheavy syllables attract stress

and Dutch feet are built on top of syllables the stress facts are readily

explained schwanever is stressed b ecause it is not in a syllable at the

relevant level of the derivation and the syllable preceding schwa is stressed

b ecause it is sup erheavy The fact that schwa do es not allow for a complex

onset is explained by the following lter

Complex Onset Condition Level

C C

 

cons

son

son

unless the two consonants o ccur in the same syllable

Kagers Complex Onset Condition is stated just in terms of obstruents

and liquids b ecause they form the only p ossible complex onset clusters

in Dutch At least one cluster tl is incorrectly included into this lter

the word butler bytlr butler is incorrectly predicted to b e illformed in

Dutch

Previous analyses of uschwa

The reason for this exceptionalityofbytlr seems to b e that tl is the

only stopliquid sequence that is not a p ossible onset cluster for indep endent

reasons p erhaps having to do with the OCP

In order to account for this fact Kager would have to revise to

Complex Onset Condition Level

C C

i j

unless C and C app ear in the same syllable or C C is not a

i j i j

potentially wellformed onset cluster

This version of the Complex Onset Condition says if some consonantclus

ter can b e an onset it must b e an onset Yet this formulation is not without

problems for Kager b ecause it crucially refers to the notion of onset

and there is no such thing as an onset in moraic phonology Kager

claims there is one piece of indep endent evidence for this constraint There

are no words of the form zbra ie a lax vowel followed by a complex

onset followed by another vowel If ambisyllabicity is not a p ossibilityat

level I while the b has to b e in the co da of the rst syllableb ecause of

Connect N lax or b ecause every syllable has to b e bimoraic dep ending

on ones theory ab out the dierence b etween Avowels and Bvowels

explains why this structure is ruled out The constraint therefore parallels

what wehave termed the Syllable Contact Law the unless clause would

probably now b e captured in terms of constraintinteraction

As wehave seen ab ove Kager noted as a problem for Kager and Zonn

evelds theory that it predicts schwasyllables can only b e p eripheral

which is not true Yet Kager himself has to assume that at level I

remains unsyllabied in the middle of a word like Hengelo which actually

is a place name

Level I

h x l o

Schwa apparently is not incorp orated into the Proso dic Structure at this

level of representation yet This means that the Principle of Proso dic Li

censing in anyofitsformulations is violated as far as this structure is

concerned The fact that preschwa consonants are incorp orated into the

previous syllable shows us that at least there is a tendency for all struc

ture to b e incorp orated in the existing material This in turn shows that

Previous analyses of uschwa

Proso dic Licensing at least should hold in some weaker form and that the

schwa is an exception to it

Kager and Zonneveld analyse this exceptional b ehaviour as a

consequence of a certain version of extraproso dicity but Kager do es

neither give an account for schwas exceptional b ehaviour in this resp ect

nor of the segments that can followschwa In kater tomcat katr the

r is not syllabied at level I

k a t r

Later on it gets syllabied together with the schwa One of the curious

facts now is that these unsyllabied segments are sonorants Under a moraic

view of syllable structure wewould probably wanttosay that these are in

away the moraic consonants But it is crucially imp ossible to saysucha

thing in Kagers framework If these consonants were to b ear moras

they would also come to b ear syllables b ecause that is what happ ens at level

I But in that case the whole argumentaboutschwa b eing amoraic would

break down I do not see a way to solve this problem

Another problem for Kagers approach is that it is still stipulative

as far as the nature of schwa is concerned Kager stipulates that schwa

do es not b ear a mora There are three typ es of vowels in this approach

monomoraic vowels like bimoraic vowels like o and amoraic vowels

like the O stands for the features of and o

o

O O

There is no formal relation b etween the phonetic and phonological empti

ness of schwa and its b eing amoraic One could just as well imagine a

language which has three schwas one amoraic one monomoraic and one

bimoraic or a language which has just a bimoraic schwa and amoraic a

i and u in its vowel inventory In other words there is no theory of the

relation b etween segmental content and number of moras

Furthermore as wehave seen eschwa and rschwa share some of the

prop erties of uschwa Kager do es not provide us with a theory of

eschwawhich indeed seems hard to incorp orate into his analysisbut he

Previous analyses of uschwa

do es give a partial analysis for rschwa from which it b ecomes clear that

for him this vowel is p ostlexical He do es not drawany relation b etween

the dierent o ccurrences of schwa Uschwa is sp ecial b ecause it is amoraic

rschwa is sp ecial b ecause it is derived p ostlexically

Hermans a The relation b etween the lack of features

of schwa and its defective metrical b ehaviour seems to b e b etter estab

lished in an unpublished manuscript by Hermans a who furthermore

defends a less radical version of the NoSyllable Theory

Hermans assumes that all pro jection no des of proso dic structure haveto

see certain nonphonetic features on another plane Since schwa is empty

it also lacks these features Therefore proso dic no des on top of such empty

ro ots are illegitimate Hermans assumes this holds for all no des but for

technical reasons there is an exception for the lowest one the nucleus

Schwa in Dutch can therefore only license a nucleus Hermans a

assumes the Dutchnucleus can only contain sonorant segments This ex

plains why only sonorants are allowed after a schwa It also derives Kager

and Zonnevelds claim that schwasyllables have no onset In or

der for an onset to exist a syllable no de is needed which cannot b e there

b ecause it is not licensed b yany feature Unfortunatelyaswehave seen

Kager and Zonnevelds generalisation is not totally adequate and

should b e replaced by schwasyllables have exactly one segment in their

onset Exactly this generalisation can not b e captured under Hermans

account

Also the nono ccurrence of schwainwordinitial p osition is dicult to

explain under this approach Hermans a assumption is that at a

later lexical level all schwas have to pro ject to a syllable whichinturn

needs an onset This onset needs to b e lled by a glottal stop but sequences

of unarticulated segments are assumed to b e blo cked Thus far this is more

or less the traditional approach Cohen et al which is also defended

here But it is unclear how the sequence of glottal stop plus schwacan

b e excluded under Hermans a analysis Because p ostlexically schwa

apparently builds a syllable it should have acquired the feature licensing

it But then it presumably is dierent from the glottal stop whichisnota

fully sp ecied syllable head but a consonant

In general it seems that Hermans a theory can account for the

claims ab out schwa made in Kager and Zonneveld rather neatlyUn

fortunatelyhowever most of these claims have b een shown to b e untenable or sub ject to revision

Previous analyses of uschwa

Nijen Twilhaar An interesting interpretation of the

analysis of schwa along the lines of Kager and Zonneveld has b een

provided by Nijen Twilhaar Nijen Twilhaars pap er esp e

cially concentrates on the assumption that schwas are extraproso dic

Nijen Twilhaar p oints out that seemingly underived nouns ending in

schwa all select the common gender determiner de In dialects whichdis

tinguish b etween three genders like Hellendo orn Dutch they even are all

feminine He p oints out that nonfeminine nouns ending in schwa often

changed their grammatical gender while inversely feminine nouns some

times develop ed a wordnal schwa

On the basis of this Nijen Twilhaar argues that these forms have

the following morphological structure

root

N

The schwa in this analysis is a ro otselecting lowlevel sux Ro ots cannot

o ccur with this sux Nijen Twilhaar assumes Williams Right

Hand Head Rule holds for Dutch A consequence of this is that the sux

determines the gender of the noun If we determine that the ro ot initially

is the domain of syllabication wederive a lot of Kager and Zonnevelds

schwa prop erties

Interestingly it app ears that the nouns which are exceptions to Kager

and Zonnevelds generalisation like ritme and uvre select the de



terminer het in Standard Dutch and are neuter in Hellendo orn Nijen

Twilhaar assumes that these forms are monomorphemic so that schwabe

haves like a normal vowel and that there is a lexical redundancy rule like

If then neuter

W

Even though there are a few counterexamples and problematic examples

as noted by Nijen Twilhaar himself it seems that these observations

are basically correct They require further investigation b ecause they are

problematic for the approach defended here

On the other hand an approach along these lines has to face many

problems that are not addressed in Nijen Twilhaar In the rst

place this theory makes exactly the same predictions for nouns as Kager

and Zonneveld and therefore he has the same problems including the

prediction that wordinternal schwa should b ehaveentirely dierentlyIn

the second place other than Kager and Zonneveld it also seems to predict

that wordnal schwa followed by a sonorant consonantbehaves entirely



On the other hand there are exceptions to this generalisation viz words like kas

tanje chestnut and tarwe wheat

Remaining problems

dierentlyunless wewould b e willing to supp ort sux analyses for r

m n etc as well There seems to b e no evidence for this p osition The

criterion of determiner selection Nijen Twilhaar prop oses for do es not

seem to work

a de adder id het anker the anchor

b de deken the blanket het teken the sign

c de asem the breath het desem the leaven

d de stoppel the stubble het koppel the b elt

Yet phonologically these forms b ehave exactly like the forms ending in

In the third place it remains unclear whyschwasyllables in adjectives

and verbs b ehave similar to those in nouns except again if there would

b e evidence that in those cases the schwaapp endices are suxes

Remaining problems

In the previous sections wehave put aside some technical problems either

b ecause their treatmentwould unnecessarily slowdown the discussion or

b ecause they needed some extra information we could not yet provide at

er useful the time In order to makemy prop osals more explicit it is howev

to also solve these problems I will briey discuss eachoftheminthis

section

Final Devoicing The rst problem I want to discuss is Final

Devoicing The problems are mainly technical and involve the following

p oints In the rst place wehave to make sure that Final Devoicing applies

at the end of a word notwithstanding the fact that most other pro cesses

are prohibited to apply at word edge Secondly I cannot say that Final

Devoicing applies to obstruents in the co da b ecause under my assumptions

the nal underlying d of brood bread is in the onset of a degenerate

syllable and still Final Devoicing applies to this segmentaswell

The rst p oint is not really much of a problem in a theory where con

straints can b e highly ranked Wehave seen that Align the constraintthat

is resp onsible for the relatively static b ehaviour of word edges has a high

ranking in Dutch This do es not mean however that Align is inviolable

under all circumstances In the preceding sections wehave already seen

evidence that it is dominated by for instance the constraint Contour

causing dm to surface dm

We can assume that at least the constraint resp onsible for Final Devoic

ing call it FinalDevoice is ranked higher than Align

Remaining problems

FinalDevoice Align

The inverse ordering of these constraints is also attested viz in Yiddish

This language shows devoicing of syllablenal obstruents just likeDutch

However wordnal obstruents exceptionally do not devoice

a red I sp eak red ret

b retst you sp eak retst redst

c shrayb I write sra jb sra jp

d shraypst you write sra jpst sra jbst

e ayz ice a jz

f ayskastn ice b ox a jskastn

g briev letter briv

h brieftreger p ostman briftregr

The dierence b etween Yiddish and Dutch is describ ed by Lombardi

by stipulating a nal exceptionality parameter which has the plus value



in the former language and the minus value in the latter We can now

understand somewhat b etter the nature of this parameter In Dutch the

necessity to devoice nal obstruents is absolute In Yiddish it is overridden

b y the desire to keep the edges of words clean

This brings us to the next p oint How can weformalisenaldevoicing in

a theory of degenerate syllables It lo oks as if wehavetomake a disjunctive

statement for Dutch obstruents devoice in a co da of a regular syllable or

in the onset of a degenerate syllable Iwillhowever here again follow

Lombardi who argues that the relevant constraint has the following



form



FinalDevoice voice except in the environment

sonor sonor

Laryngeal



Other languages with the same parameter setting are Romanian and Serb oCroatian

according to Lombardi



Lombardi also claims that voiced is a privative feature a claim which I will follow

here



The except in the formulation makes us susp ect FinalDevoicing is actually the

conation of several constraints at least one of which prohibits the feature voiced on

obstruents in anyenvironment and another which enforces an obstruenttobevoiced

when followed by a sonorant I will not work this out

Remaining problems

It is as yet formally unclear whyvoiced obstruents are restricted to this

environment preceding a sonorant but neither a segment in a co da nor a

segment in the onset of a degenerate syllable satises this condition Both

are therefore correctly predicted to b e sub ject to Final Devoicing

Sup erheavy syllables b efore schwa The next problem con

cerns the class of words exemplied by klooster monastery klostr The

problem with these words is that they seem to have a sup erheavy syllable

preceding a schwa syllable while I have argued that degenerate syllables and

schwa syllables can never b e adjacent to one another On closer scrutiny



the problematic forms app ear to fall into three classes

a klooster klostr monastery meester master mestr

heester shrub hestr langoeste langouste lust buste

bust byst hamster id hmstr etc

b aarde earth waarde worth aarzel hesitate daalder dollar

gaarne readily vaandel banner

c pienter clever pintr boerde fabliau burd ottielje otil

la tilj

The segment s is the only segmentwhich can freely stay in extraproso dic

p ositions throughout the word b oth at the leftp eriphery as in s troop

treacle and at the rightp eriphery as in melaats leprous It can also

stay in an extrasyllabic p osition b etween two syllables b oth headed bya

full vowel ekstra In the latter case the syllable following s starts with a

t Exactly the same conguration can b e found in the forms in a so we

may assyme the s in this case is extraproso dic not the onset of a degenerate



syllable

The second class consists of words with an a It is interesting that

these forms contain an a followed by a sonorant and a voiced obstruent

or in one case a nasal In chapter wehave seen that in Tilburg Dutch

tenses in similar circumstances It maywell b e that here wehavea

reex of a similar historical tensing pro cess whichwould just apply in

front of rz in Standard Dutch

The third class nally consists of words with high vowels Wehave seen

in chapter that these vowels do not havealaxcounterpart and therefore

can exceptionally o ccur in a closed syllable If this is true the examples in



I assume the t in pantser armour pntsr and similar forms is the result of a

phonetic rule of homorganic stop insertion The only counterexamples of whichIam

aware and which do not fall into either of these classes are deerne lass taveerne tavern

and heinde en verre far and near



English has the same typ e of exceptional words cf Harris

Remaining problems

c are not counterexamples to our claims at all since they consist of a

normal closed syllable followed bya schwaheaded syllable

Postlexical uschwa Wehave seen that uschwa can app ear

in a rightp eripheral p osition while this p osition is prohibited for rschwa

and eschwa Lexicallyuschwa cannot o ccur wordinitiallyhowever Even

this changes at the p ostlexical level In Standard Dutch a lot of clitics

have the form C where C is either a sonorant or a coronal consonantas

is shown in

m him n a r herthere s once t itthe

While these clitics b ehave as enclitic s if they can showing Onset is still

op erative if it can these clitics can phonologically still app ear in a phrase



initial p osition

a Een n man huilt niet

a man cries not

Real men do not cry

b Er r k omt iemand

There comes someb o dy

Someb o dy arrives

c Het t regent

it rains

d m m vertrouwen kan ik niet

him trust can I not

I cannot trust him

Some dialects also have an inectional prex usually denoting the p er

fect Examples are Katwijk Dutch next to the coast Overdiep

and Aalten Dutch next to the German b order Bennink pc The

following is an example sentence from Katwijk Dutch

Ik heb Krijn n zien

I have K case epenthetic n seen

I have seen Krijn



For one reason or another examples with phraseinitial er like b are not ac

ceptable for some nativespeakers

Remaining problems

According to Overdiep p ersonal names and some other categories

in Katwijk Dutchhave a case inectional element in the accusative In

this case the name Krijn b ears this inection The immediately following

participle starts with a schwa In this case an n is ep enthesized according

to Overdiep As far as I can tell an alternativewould b e to assume that

the inection is n and the n gets deleted in all contexts except b efore a

vowel In any case it can b e seen that Onset is still active although it no

longer prohibits wordinitial schwa

The prex in these dialects corresp onds to a prex in most other

dialects and the standard language Interestingly nonprexal wordinitial

is also found for instance in the word geluk luck lyk and gera

nium id ranijym In cases such as these Katwijk Dutch and Aalten

Dutch also have and never

Van Haeringen observes that in Dedemsvaart Dutch another

eastern Dutch dialect even which are used as attributivead

jectives again showthevariant Bennink pc notes that the same

alternation holds in Aalten Dutch

a Wij hebt melk eko okt

we have milk co oked

Wehavecooked milk

b Wij hebt eerepp els eko okt

we have p otato es co oked

Wehavecooked p otato es

c Mij lust liever gebakken eerepp els as geko okten

me likes better baked p otato es than co oked

I like fried p otato es b etter than co oked ones

It seems then that in these cases the adjectival forms are formed already in

the lexicon where schwainitial words are not allowed whereas formation

of the real participles can b e p ostp oned until the p ostlexicon

Notice however that in the previous chapter wehave seen that the

derivation of schwainitial words is still blo cked p ostlexically The only

initial schwas that can surface are the underlying schwas that arise only

p ostlexically ie initial schwas in clitics and suxes In the p ostlexical

phonology it thus seems that Contour gets ranked b elow the constraint

Projectcons N andor Align

Finally I have to note that p ostlexical derivation also allows schwato

b ear some weak form of secondary stresssee for instance the stress pat

terns in words like gemakkelijkere easyerAGRmklkr

Remaining problems

Also the constraint ProjectFt V seems to get a lower ranking b e



lowarhythmic constraint requiring every second syllable to have stress in

the p ostlexicon

Umlaut Finally I want to briey discuss umlaut Many eastern

dialects of Dutchaswell as most German dialects have an active umlaut

ing pro cess which is triggered by one or more schwacontaining suxes It

seems to b e a distinctivepropertyofschwa that only this vowel can trigger

umlaut in most of these dialects

Prop erty Schwa in suxes can trigger umlaut

Wehave seen that some remnants of the pro cess can b e found in Tilburg

Dutch but it seems to b e really pro ductive in for instance Schinnen Dutch

Province of Limburg Wetzels Hellendo orn Dutch Province of Over

ijssel Nijen Twilhaar and Aalten Dutch Province of Gelderland

Bennink in preparation All of these dialects have their own p eculiariti es

Ihavechosen to only discuss Aalten Dutch for practical reasons a native

sp eaker of this dialect was close at hand

Umlauting involves fronting of a vowel usually the nal vowel of the

stem Some examples are provided b elow all copied from Bennink in prepa

ration with some adaptations to t the phonological transcription system

used here

underived form derived form gloss

mus myz mouse pl

vust vyst st pl

bs byskn bundle dim

bk bkskn traydim

Historically the umlaut triggering schwas are derived from frontvowels

mostly i Synchronically there is no sp ecial reason to see them as r

schwas in presentday Aalten Dutch since they never alternate with any

full vowel

The authors just mentioned all p oint out that this causes at rst sight

a somewhat curious situation b ecause schwa as a noncoronal vowel trig

gers fronting ie coronalization of a preceding vowel I will basically

adopt the solution put forward by Nijen Twilhaar and supp ose the

representation of umlauttriggering schwaisasfollows

cons coronal

Remaining problems

The representation consists of two indep endent elements an emptyroota

schwa and a feature coronal The diachronic development can b e seen

as the loss of all connections underlying asso ciation lines and class no des

between these two elements

The derived form muzwillnowlookschematically as in

m u z

coronal

The rst constraint that is relevantisParsecoronalthevo calic place

feature coronal has to b e linkedtoavo calic place no de Logically sp eak

ing there are two optionscoronal can b e link ed to the u yielding myz

or it can b e linked to the schwa creating a lot of intermediary no des C



place vo calic Vplace and presumably yielding muze The former is

clearly the option that is selected

There are at least two p otential reasons why this is the case In the

rst place Aalten Dutchshows the eect of a curious constraint against

nal coronal vowels in a word All words which in Standard Dutch end

inafrontvowel either have defronted this vowel or end in a schwain



Aalten The feature coronal can therefore not do ck on the nal vowel of

the phonological word but it has to go to the stemnal full vowel This

is approximately the line of reasoning defended by Bennink in preparation

Yet this reasoning cannot fully explain all facts of umlaut In the rst

place many other Dutch and German dialects that have umlauting do not

prohibit coronal vowels in wordnal p osition So at least in those dialects

some other factor should b e at work In the second place umlauting in

words like vaderken father dimvdrkn aects the full vowel not

the stemnal schwa If the reasoning just laid out would b e correct we

would exp ect the pronunciation vadrkn Yet this is not the case

It seems therefore more correct to supp ose that it is a characteristic

prop erty of the schwavowel as opp osed to other vowels that it can not

serve as an anchor p oint for the place feature regardless of whether schwa

is in a sux or a stem

I already indicated that in one sense it is more costly to attach a place

feature to schwathantoany other vowel The reason for this wehave



I assume schwa coronal e in op en syllables or in closed syllables in Dutch



Bennink in preparation rep orts that Aalten sp eakers refer to outsiders unsucces

fully trying to imitate the lo cal dialect as sp eakers with pien in de buuk pain in their

stomach pin nd byk To outsiders this seems an adequate Aalten phrase since it

contains nondiphthongized versions of words which in Standard Dutch are pronounced

as pin and byk resp ectively Lackofdiphthongisation is a prominentcharacteristic

of eastern Dutch dialects Yet the real Aalten pronunciation of this phrase would b e pine

in de boek pinnd buk

Remaining problems

already seen It is the constraint ProjectV Ft whichsays that un



stressed syllables should not dominate a vo calic no de Since the vowel in

the sux as well as that in a nal schwaheaded syllable is unstressed

attachment of the feature coronal to that vowel will cause unnecessary vi

olation of that constraint Attaching of the feature to a full vowel can never

cause such a violation b ecause that vowel already has a vo calic no de hence

already violates the constraint against such no des in unstressed p osition

Also the umlauting b ehaviour of schwathus follows from this vowels

minimal nature It has very little structure and therefore it is not able to

serve as a linking p oint for many features

FinallyIhave to discuss one additional case namely one in whichwe

add an umlauting schwa to a stem containing at least two backvowels An

example is foto photograph Given the preceding discussion it is not clear

whether the output should b e fotkn or ftokn Both rank eqally well

with the constraints mentioned b efore Howdowe explain that it is the

former output that is selected More in general why is the last full vowel

always selected rather than some other vowel

I prop ose the answer lies in the fact that the coronal feature is part of

a sux What this means is that it should b e aligned with the right edge of

the proso dic word on top of the stem as much as p ossible McCarthy and

Prince Akinlabi Wethus have a constraint whichsays that

the elements of the diminutive sux should o ccur as much to the rightas

p ossible Therefore the form foteuken is preferred over feutoken b ecause

in the latter case the coronal feature has shifted unnecessaril y far to the



left

For convencience I will call this constraint Suffixalthoughitcanbe

formulatedasamemb er of the AlignmentfamilyAligndim X PrWd

R

The ideal output for Suffix has coronal at the rightmost syllable of

the word Yet this constraint is outranked by the pro jection constraint

ProjectV Ft Wethus get the following tableaux assuming only can



didates which satisfy Parsecoronal



N do es not head a branching fo ot N do es ProjectV Ft



not dominate a vo calic no de

Suffix The elements of a sux should o ccur maximally close to

the right edge of a proso dic word



Logically we should also exclude a realisation feuteuken in whichbothvowels are

umlauted I assume some kind of economyprevents this double linking Once Parse

coronal is satised multiple linking of this feature gives only unnecessary asso ciation lines

Conclusion

muz

Candidates ProjectV Ft Suffix



p

myz

muze

vaderkn

Candidates ProjectV Ft Suffix



p

vdrkn

vadrkn

vadrkn

fotokn

Candidates ProjectV Ft Suffix



p

fotkn

ftokn

fotokn

This concludes my discussion of umlaut Uschwa can cause umlaut even

though it is not itself front b ecause it can b e accompanied by a feature

coronal

Conclusion

In this chapter I have discussed many prop erties of Dutch uschwa the

schwa that never alternates with either zero or full vowels It is argued

that only the schwa that o ccurs in op en syllables is really underlying The

other uschwa is the result of an obligatory rule of ep enthesis in syllables

which lexically havealevel in which a sonorant consonant is the head of

the syllable

In the lexical phonologyschwa only licenses CV syllables I have argued

that the reason for this is that schwa lacking a vo calic no de cannot b e the

head of anybranching constituent at allit do es not license a co da or a

dep endent syllable at the fo ot level It also do es not really license onset

segments but since at least one such segment is forced to b e presentby

forces from outside ie the constraint Onset the result is a core syllable

Exactly the same reasoning causes also sonorant consonants to only o ccur

in CV syllables with V in this case the p osition lled by the sonorant

Pro jection constraints served to formally express all of these generalisations

Another consequence of the emptiness of schwaisthatitavoids the

neighb ourho o d of other emptysegments In particular it cannot o ccur in a

Table of Prop erties

syllable of which the onset is empty a glottal stop or h Again this was

expressed in terms of a pro jection constraint

The pro jection theory thus plays a crucial role in my analysis of schwa

Schwa is a defectivevowel this is the reason why it pro jects a very defective

typ e of syllable

In this resp ect there is a dierence b etween the lexical and the p ostlex

ical level Postlexical schwa can o ccur in manymoreenvironments than

lexical schwa as we can see in the comparison b etween eschwa and rschwa

on the one hand and uschwa on the other Postlexical schwa can o ccur in

a closed syllable it can have a complex onset etc All of these things are

not p ossible for lexical schwa

It seems then that certain topdown pro jection constraints requiring

the heads of pro jection levels to b e supp orted byfullvowels havelostmuch

of their force in the p ostlexical phonology Other pro jection constraints

b oth topdown the constraint against degenerate syllables requiring in

sertion of cons ro ots in ar m and b ottomup the constraint forcing

unstressed full vowels to reduce to schwa b ecause full vowels wanttooccur

in the head of a fo ot are very forceful in the p ostlexicon whereas they do

not playsuch a strong role in the lexical phonology I am not sure whether

anything more general can b e said ab out the dierence b etween the two

dierentlevels of analysis

Table of Prop erties

Below I give a table of all prop erties of DutchschwawhichIhaveidentied

in this chapter and the previous one For every prop ertyIhave indicated

p

whether the prop erty holds for each of the three typ es of schwa A

mark indicates that the schwa has that prop erty an asterisk that it do es

not have it a dash indicates that the prop erty is not relevant and a P of

preference indicates that the prop ertymay b e violated although schwas

which satisfy the prop erty surface in more style registers than schwas which do not

Table of Prop erties

Prop erty Rschwa Eschwa Uschwa

p p p

Phonetic emptiness

p

Reduction vowel

p p p

Not tautosyllabic with h or b

p p

Not wordnal

p

Alternates with e a o

p

No alternation with i

p p p

Unstressed

p

Not outside fo ot P

p

Op en syllables only P

p

Ep enthetic vowel

p

Only in wordnal syllable

p

Not in wordinitial p osition

p

Allows ep enthesis b efore itself

p

Allows sp metath b efore itself

p

x V V r

i i

p

No complex onset

p

Only sonorantincoda P P

p p

degen syll

p

OK

p

C C

i i

p p

Stress on preceding syllable P

p p p

V V

p p

Compl distr uschwa and eschwa

p p

Schwadeletion

p p

Deletion only in p ossible onset sites

p

Umlaut

SchwainFrench and Norwegian

Intro duction

In the previous twochapters I havederived some prop erties of Dutchschwa

from the phonetic and phonological emptiness of this vowel From this it

was made to follow that schwa only pro jected to a core syllable in the

weak p osition of a fo ot Since this emptiness by denition is the same for

schwalikevowels in languages other than Dutch we exp ect the phonological

b ehaviour of these vowels to b e similar also they should pro ject only a

minimal structure In this chapter I study two languages other than Dutch

with a schwa in their surface vowel inventory to nd out whether or not

this exp ectation is b orne out These languages are French and Norwegian

The reason whyI havechosen these languages is that their schwabehaves

dierently than Dutchschwaininteresting and signicantways while still

the phonologies of these languages are not to o dierent from that of Dutch

so as to obscure the comparison

Some prop erties of schwa seem universal This is esp ecially true for its

stressless nes s Schw aavoids stressed p ositions not only in the languages

discussed here but also for instance in KP Mohanan T

Mohanan German Hall Wiese Noske

Indonesian Cohn and many other languages It also seems true that

if a language has schwa on the surface and either vowel ep enthesis or vowel

reduction schwa tends to b e the ep enthetic or reduced vowel Finallyword

p eripheral p ositions are disallowed for schwalikevowels in many languages

All of these facts ab out schwa can b e made to follow from pro jection theory

On the other hand some prop erties I mentioned in the preceding chapter

seem less universal and more typical of Dutch One of the problems studied

in this chapter is that schwa can have complex onsets in b oth French and

Norwegian Both languages havea word ordre pronounced more or less as

rdr I will try to explain this as an eect of a minor reranking of some

constraints Essential is that neither language likeDutch allows a word

like katrl where a schwaheaded syllable has b oth a complex onset and

acoda

French

In Pro jection theorywe exp ect schwa to license only a very limited typ e

of syllable structure in every language This is a prediction which seems

to b e b orne out at least for the languages under discussion Yet it will b e

shown that languages may slightly dier as to what they consider to b e a

minimal onset

French

There is an overwhelming amount of literature on Frenchschwa I refer to

Anderson Basbll Charette Dell

Durand Morin Noske

Schane Selkirk b Tranel

among others for discussion of the manyinteracting factors that

inuence the b ehaviour of this segment Interestingly the range of theories

for Frenchschwa put forward by these authors is approximately the same

as the one discussed in the previous chapter for Dutchschwa For instance

some authors eg Tranel have prop osed that Frenchschwa

is a oating melo dic element which is not attached to a skeletal p oint and

others have assumed it is an emptyskeletal p oint without melo dic content

eg Charette

It is imp ossible to discuss all known facts ab out Frenchschwainthis

chapter As compared to the fairly comprehensive discussion of Dutch

hwa in the previous twochapters the discussion presented here inevitably sc

is somewhat sup ercial I only discuss the observations that seem most rel

evant with resp ect to the discussion of Dutchschwa in the previous chapter

Within the terminology of the preceding chapter wecansaythatFrench

has instances of eschwa and uschwa but not of rschwa The language

do es not allowvowel reduction under any circumstance I will takethis

as an indication that French has a suciently low ranking of the relevant

pro jection constraint relating vowel quality to heads of feet reduction in

Dutch takes place b ecause full vowels preferred to o ccur in heads of feet

and apparently this constraint is not visibly activeinFrench

Still this relation b etween feet and features is not totally irrelevant As

we will see there are a few contexts in whichschwa alternates with full

vowels esp ecially with and although it is usually asssumed that this is

not reduction to schwa but rather enhancement from schwa to a full vowel

the notion of a Fo ot can b e argued to b e relevant so that some version of

Pr ojectFt still has a role to playinFrench phonology

Underlying and ep enthetic schwa are dicult to distinguish in French

b ecause this language allows muchmoreschwa deletion than Dutch A

consequence of this is that almost all schwas alternate with zero Never

French

theless some convincing tests have b een develop ed to distinguish b etween

underlying and ep enthetic schwainFrenchaswe will see in section

and therefore I will pro ceed to assume this distinction

My discussion of French is organized in approximately the same wayas

my discussion of Dutch The most imp ortant prop erties of Dutchschwa

are listed and each time it is discussed in what resp ects the Frenchvowel

is dierent or similar I skip the prop erties of Dutchschwathatseemtoo

much connected to the rest of the Dutch phonological system For instance

the b ehaviour of diphthongs or b efore schwa cannot b e used as a test in

French b ecause this language do es not havediphthongs and uses only

marginally I do not discuss the prop erties of rschwa either since reduction

do es not o ccur in French On the other hand I will mention some prop erties

that are more sp ecic to Frenchschwa A complete graph of the constraint

interactions needed in the analysis of Frenchisprovided in app endix C

Of course schwainFrench like in all other languages by denition has

Prop erty

Prop erty Schwa is exceptional in the vocalic system because it bears no

vocalic features

The variation in the phonetic realisation of schwaisvery large in French

In some dialects the realisation of this vowel is very close to the realisation

of the lax rounded coronal mid vowel For this reason some authors

have prop osed to give the twovowels the same representation eg Morin

Although it is in itself curious that the emptyvowel phonetically

is so close to one of the most sp ecied vowels coronal labial lax I

will follow the authors mentioned ab ove and assume and havean

essentially dierent representation and that Frenchschwa has Prop erty

in most cases

Yet it seems that wehavetomake one exception It will app ear b elowthatschwa

deletion o ccurs muchmoreofteninFrenchthanitdoesinDutch Yet a few words havea

socalled stable schwaa schwa that cannot b e deleted although schwas in comparable

nu id femel le female debout environments can instances of stable schwa o ccur in me

stand up etc Morin Durand Slater and Wise and Charette have

suggested that in these cases wedohave a full vowel probably rather than schwa

in the underlying representationIfwewould want to implement this in the theory

presented here we could follow this suggestion stable schwa is not a schwa at all but

a full vowel Real schwas in French are empty ro ots In the remainder of this chapter

I will concentrate only on those instances of schwa that do alternate these are either

underlying schwas which can b e deleted or ep enthetic schwas which can b e inserted

The only workedout alternative prop osal byTranel and Noske holds

that normal schwa is a oating vowel not linked to a skeletal slot underlyingly while

stable schwaislinked to such a slot Such a prop osal cannot b e adopted in the framework

prop osed here For a general criticism of the idea that schwa is a oating vowel cf the

discussion of Kagers prop osal for Dutchschwa in the previous chapter

French

Again we exp ect all other prop erties of Frenchschwa to b e related to

this central prop erty This is trivially true for the rst prop ertyofEschwa

which is discussed in the next section

Eschwa is the ep entheticvowel Prop ert y As noted

it is dicult to nd go o d evidence for schwas that are unambiguously

ep enthetic in French Because underlying schwas so often alternate with

zero it is hard to nd cases in which a surfacing schwaisunambiguously the

result of ep enthesis not an underlying schwa that happ ens to have escap ed

deletion

Still there are a few go o d reasons to distinguish b etween underlying

schwa and ep enthetic schwa In the rst place there are a few French

dialects eg Midi French sp oken in southern France in whichschwa

deletion is much less common than it is in Standard French Still in these

dialects some words showaschwa zero alternation which is not attested in

other words

For instance Durand gives fo c fk jib versus phoque fk

seal as a minimal pair In Standard Paris French b oth words are pro



nounced as fk It is imp ortanttokeep this distinction in mind since the

distinction b etween real consonantnalwords and words ending in schwa

in French is sometimes denied for instance in the Government Phonology

analysis defended by Charette

A similar test can b e designed for dialects that are closer to Standard

French It is usually assumed that underlying schwa in these dialects sur

faces obligatorily when adjacent to a socalled h aspireFor instance Du

rand notes that phoque havane tobacco brown seal is pronounced

fkavan while fochavane tobacco brown jib is pronounced as fkavan

It is therefore reasonable to assume that fk has a schwathatislacking in

fk Still there are contexts in whichwords like foc surface with a schwa

aisepenthetic I assume at least this schw

In my view the most plausible case for schwaepenthesis is the context

between twowords the rst of which ends in a consonant cluster and the

second of which starts with an onset I have copied the following examples

from Noske and underlined the ep enthetic vowel

a CC C

penible b un contact

painful a contact

a painful contact



Because Paris French is a so ciologically very prestigious dialect the twowords tend

to b ecome homophonous in Midi Frenchnowadays as well

French

c un index formidable

an index terric

a terric index

I consider the schwa in these examples ep enthetic b ecause there is no reason

to assume it is underlying in either the preceding noun contact or index

or the following adjective penible of formidable the insertion of schwais

optional even in the context of an haspire word

It seems that coronal segments do not have the same exceptional sta

tusinwordnal p osition in Frenchwhich they have in Dutch The most

imp ortantargument for this is that there are no Frenchwords ending in

three obstruents The nouns in exhibit the maximal numb er of obstru

ents allowed in wordnal p osition words like contacts kts or indext

kst are not p ossible in French Coronals are not allowed after clusters

of noncoronals either For instance there are no words like armt In

general words allowatmosttwo consonants in nal p osition regardless of



the place features on those consonants It therefore do es not seem unrea

sonable to assume Frenchwords allo w at most closed syllables followed by



a degenerate syllable at their end but no extraproso dic segments

It is nevertheless true that the nal segmentinthewords in is a

coronal Yet this seems to b e due to another eect namely that in the case

of sequences of equalsonority consonants within one word at least one of

the two segments most often the second one is coronal For instance we



nd acteur actor aktr but not akpr or atkr I therefore assume

here that the coronals in are in the proso dic word unlike coronals in

similar p ositions in Dutch This assumption makes it easier to understand

why these segments trigger ep enthesis The Dutchword contact do es not

trigger ep enthesis in any phonological environment at all I assumed the

reason for this is that the coronal can b e outside the proso dic word where

it do es not need to b e in a syllable Apparently the t in French contact

prefers to b e in a syllable t I take this as an additional argumentthat

it is in the proso dic word

In environments like these the ep enthetic vowel is always schwainFrench

like it is in Dutch The reason for this is the same in the two languages

For one reason or anotherprobably related to syllable structure given the

conditioning environmentwe need to insert a vowel Since Frenchallows



Apparent counterexamples like encre ink can also b e dealt with in the theory to

be worked out b elow b ecause they consist of a closed syllable followed by an onset



For the sake of completeness I mention that wordinitially we do nd extraproso dic

ss however station id stasj strict id strikt etc



This observation has b een made and given a formal account in K Rice who

prop oses a notion of binding for heterosyllabic consonant clusters where binds  i

has a less marked place feature than  and precedes  cf also Harris

French

schwas on the surface it is most economical to insert a schwa All other

vowels would intro duce additional features that are not strictly necessary

and therefore forbidden for reasons of economy

Since the alternation here is b etween a degenerate syllable and a schwa

headed syllable wemay assume the relevant constraintisProjectN

cons the constraintwhich forces syllables to havevo calic segments as

their heads just as in Dutch p ostlexical phonology

Eschwa do es not o ccur at the end of the word Prop erty

This prop erty is dicult to test to the letter as is Prop erty E

schwa only o ccurs in the last syllable of the word The ep enthetic schwas

in o ccur typically b etween twowords so it is imp ossible to tell whether

phonologically they b elong to one word or to the other

Prop erty therefore do es not seem to b elong to the Frenchvowel gram

mar It is simply contradicted by the facts in In Dutchthereasonfor

not ep enthesizi ng in the wordnal p osition was Alignment morphologi

cal and phonological edges had to b e aligned with one another as muchas

p ossible The second reason p ossibly was the constraint FinalCforone

reason or another proso dic words prefer to end in a consonant

There is an indep endent reason to assume that Frenchhasalower rank

ing for the Align constraint than Dutch This constraint should b e ranked

lower in Frenchbecauseofliaison and enchanement These are two pro

cesses of resyllabication of the nal consonant of one word into the onset



of the rst syllable of another word The fact that resyllabication is al

lowed over a word b oundary implies that Align should b e ranked relatively

low For a general discussion of the dierences b etween DutchEnglish typ e

languages on the one hand and Frenchtyp e languages on the other see Pul

gram I will return to this b elow

Dutch for instance do es not haveany phonological lexical resyllabi

cation crossing a morphological word b oundary The reason for this presum

ably is that the constraint Align dominates Onset Consider for instance



the NP roodoog red eye

ro dox



In the terminologyintro duced by Encreve liaison is the pro cess that allows

wordnal consonants to surface b efore vowelinitial words while enchanementisthe

resyllabication itself



This statement needs to b e qualied b ecause syllabicationover the b oundary b e

tween a stem and one of a numb er of suxes is actually p ossible In McCarthy and

Princes terminologywe are referring to AlignLeft in this particular case The

same is true for the French example in

French

Candidates Align Onset

p

ro tox

rodox

Final devoicing has b een used here as a test for resyllabicati on The rst

syllabication is preferred even if it includes a violation of the Onset con

straint b ecause repairing this restriction would make the proso dic structure

less wellaligned with the morphological b oundaries

This apparently is not the case in French It seems wehavetoreverse

the ranking of Onset and Align in this language as can b e seen in forms

like petit ami small friend

Candidates Onset Align

p

ptitami

pti t ami

This gives a piece of evidence that the Alignment constraintislower in

French than it is in Dutch It therefore is not very surprising that also

the ordering of the constraints Align and Connect N laxis reversed In



Dutch the ordering is AlignConnectN lax

arm Dutch

Candidates Align ConnectN lax

p

rm

rm

In French the ordering is reversed again Align is ranked lower than

ConnectN lax



It seems unnecessary to distinguish b etween lexical versus p ostlexical phonology in

most asp ects of this particular analysis of French as opp osed to the case of Dutch This

however might b e simply a result of the less detailed nature of my discussion of French

as compared to the discussion of Dutch Also exactly with resp ect to the lo cation of

N lax in the hierarchyFrenchdoesseemtoshow some limited evidence for Connect

a distinction b etween lexical and p ostlexical phonology Lexically schwa can never o ccur

in an closed syllable but p ostlexicallywe will see that the second schwainaword like

devenait b ecame dvn can get deleted leaving the rst schwa in a closed syllable

dvn

Ihavechosen the example arm b ecause it happ ens to b e the same in French and

Dutch except for the p osition of schwa It is however p ossible that the Frenchword ends

in an underlying schwa rather than an ep enthetic schwa I b elieve this do es not aect

the argumentation I also abstract away from the prop er way of treating the a pair

in French Dell and transcrib e all lowback unrounded vowels as a

French

arme French

Candidates ConnectN lax Align

arm

p

arm

The family of Alignment constraints is not totally irrelevantinFrench Li

aison do es not apply b etween anytwowords but basically only b etween

words that are in some higher order constituent for instance the phonolog

ical phrase Nesp or and Vogel Selkirk Kaisse It seems

therefore that instead of aligning morphological to proso dic wordsFrench

rather aligns syntactic to proso dic phrases Below I will argue that French

often seems to have a parameter setting phrase in most highranking con

straints where Dutch has word

Aschwa can therefore only arise inside a phrase not at the p eriphery

of one The pronunciation ktkt or for that matter p enibl in

isolation as an indep endent phrase is therefore excluded since it has a

nonunderlying schwa causing misalignment of morphological and syntactic

b oundaries and proso dic b oundaries but the same is not true for the phrase

kntktp enibl in which the schwa only causes misalignment at the word

level which as I argued ab ove is irrelevantinFrench

This concludes my discussion of Frenchepenthetic sc hwa Let us now

turn to underlying schwa

Uschwamust o ccur in an op en syllable Prop ert y

While this prop erty required some argumentation in the case of Dutchbe

cause it is not directly visible in that language where schwa can b e followed

by a sonorant on the surface in Frenchwe can immediately observe its

truth b ecause there are no forms with schwatautosyllabic consonantin

that language So while there are words like those in a there are no

words like those in b

a tabl id mod fashion quatr four

b tab l mod m quat r

I argued that the reason why Dutch only allowed schwas or sonorant conso

nants in open syllables was the constraint ConnectN lax Since schwas

and consonants havenovo calic features they do not have a feature lax

either and since a syllable can only b e closed if its head is lax the fact

that schwa and sonorants can only o ccur in op en syllabls is explained

The role which Connect N lax plays in the vowel grammar of French

is somewhat complicated It is stronger with resp ect to the constraint

French

Align than it is in Dutch as wehave just seen In chapter I p ointed out

that some southern French dialects like Languedo c French show a clear

eect of this constraint all mid vowels e and o are laxed to

and in these dialects Yet the syllable structure constraints on schwa

are not limited to these dialects As far as I know schwa cannot o ccur in

closed syllables anywhere in the area

Traditionally two pro cesses are used in the analysis of Standard French

phonology which seem to b e related to the constraint ConnectN lax

Both pro cesses are known as Closed Syllable Adjustment CSA the

rst pro cess is called eADJ byTranel and is resp onsible for the

alternations in the second pro cess is called ADJ by the same author

and resp onsible for the alternations in I have added the forms in to

show that there are also instances of underlying segments that do not

alternate with e or atall

e

esperer hop e inf sp ere espere I hop e spr

ceder cede inf sede cede I cede sd

lever stand up inf lve leve I stand up lv

mener lead inf mne mene I lead mn

rever dream inf rve rev I dream rv

feter celebrate inf ft fete I celebrate ft

Traditionally it has b een assumed that there are three underlying vowels

e and The vowels in and are the result of laxing

The trigger for laxing in these cases is the fact that the underlying schwa

or e o ccurs in a closed syllable

This analysis ts with the theory prop osed in the previous chapter The

alternation b etween tense e in op en syllables and lax in closed syllables

is exactly what wehave come to exp ect In Dutchthelaxvowel alternating

with e was rather than but given the fact that Standard French

do es not haveany height contrast in the lax vowels at all it is not altogether

unexp ected that in this language should surface as the lax counterpart

to e Igive the representations of e and b elow

Queb ec French seems to have Y U but these really b ehaveashighlaxvowels

phonologically They o ccur only in closed syllables and whenever they alternate they

do so with high vowels See Dechaine for more discussion of the very interesting

Queb ec alternations

French

e

cons cons

Vplace

Ap erture Vplace

coronal

lax coronal

The only real problem is that laxing aects only the unrouded mid vowel

even though French has a lax back rounded vowel and a lax front

rounded vowel on the surface Why do not o and show the

eect of laxing The examples in furthermore showthatConnectN

lax only works in one direction in French although e cannot o ccur in

closed syllables can o ccur in op en syllables

There are ways of technically solving the rst problem The second

problem will b e addressed b elow For instance we could assume there is a

feature co o ccurrence constraint against the combination of lax and labial

Such a constraint seems to b e suciently grounded in the phonetics given

the close connection b etween laxness and lowness and the more general ban

on low labial vowels It could b e that this feature co o ccurrence constraint

is ranked higher than Connect N lax so that laxing will not takean

eect in closed syllables that are headed by rounded vowels On the other

hand the feature co o ccurrence restriction should b e ranked lower than the

relevant ParseFeature constraints so that underlying andcan

surface



labial

Parselabial Parselax ConnectN lax

lax

Inow turn to schwa alternations The fact that the emptyvowel is

also sub ject to Closed Syllable Adjustment is predicted by ConnectN

lax As wehave seen this constraint has the eect that schwa can only

o ccur in op en syllables This restriction to op en syllables can havevarious

eects Supp ose that in French the constraint ConnectN lax is ranked

suciently high It should at least b e ranked higher than the constraint

against insertion of lax as wehave just seen

ConnectN lax then forces insertion of a feature lax under a schwa

just as it did under an e in a closed syllable Mere insertion of lax

in the representation of would mean however that we get the following structure

French

cons

vo calic

Ap erture

lax

As wehave also seen for Dutch Frenchdoesnotallowvowels with only

a sp ecication for Ap erture features In b oth languages ap erture features

need to b e accompanied by a place feature Universally coronal seems

to b e the most unmarked place feature For these reasons the following

representation is preferred over

cons

vocalic

Ap erture Vplace

lax coronal

This is the representation of Wemight therefore say that Pro jection

theory predicts Closed Syllable Adjustmentinexactlytheway it happ ens

In Dutch the coronal mid vowels are the ones that are most likely to al

ternate with schwa eg e and can b e most easily reduced Under

dierent conditions the same typ e of alternation is found in French and

in Norwegian as we will see shortly b elow It also seems true that in

languages whichlack surface schwa frontmidvowels play the role of that

vowel The theory presented here claims that schwa is a truly undersp eci

ed vowel and that coronal mid vowels are the least marked vowels This

seems to yield the correct result in all cases at hand

A dierence b etween French and Dutch is that underlying can sur

face in op en syllables in the former language but not in the latter This

could b e attributed to a dierence in ranking of Parselax with resp ect

N lax in Dutch ConnectN laxParselax so that to Connect

lax vowels that end up in an op en syllable lose their underlying feature

In French on the other hand Parselax ConnectN lax so that un

derlyingly lax vowels always surface as lax even if they end up in op en

syllables

Laxing in the head of a fo ot It is relatively easy to show

however that Closed Syllable Adjustment alone is not enough to explain

the alternation b etween and e on the one hand and on the other

French

Since the work of Selkirk it is known that metrical factors might

also b e involved Schwa do es not only surface as in a closed syllable but

also when followed by another syllable headed byschwa

In Southern French dialects for instance mene is still pronounced as

mn Similarilyaword like sevre I wean sqvr cf sevrer wean

inf svre in which the lengthening of the rst vowel seems to indicate

that the syllable is op en still has the variantof e also laxes in

this environment and so do the other mid vowels in Midi French It would

be interesting to see whether a No Syllable Theory as discussed for Dutch

in the previous chapter could make the required predictions Given the

ob jections raised against such a theory I will not try to work this out

In other Frenchdialectsvowels in op en syllables are lengthened so that

wehave a contrast b etween serve id srv on the one hand and Sevres

id sqvr on the other The fact that the latter is lengthened

shows that it is in an op en syllable This syllablenal lengthening is even

more apparentinQueb ec French where the long vowels get diphthongized

Charette

Queb ec French

i u

a rever dream ra ve  rqve palir to b ecome pale pa lir

i

 paqlir pretrise priestho o d pra triz  prqtriz

u

encadrer to frame aka dre  akaqdre

u

b partir to leave partir pa rtir merci thank you mrsi

i

ma rsi

Still there are no words suchas sqvr or seqvr This shows

in my view that neither schwa nor e can o ccur in a stressed syllable even

when this syllable is op en

This argumentthus shows that it is not p ossible to reduce laxing in

theheadofafoottoCSAWecouldnowwonder whether it is p ossible

to reduce the CSA to laxing in the head of a fo ot The Queb ec data of

Charette seem to p oint in that direction

i u u

a bete stupid ba t  bqt rage madness ra z  ra z

i i

reve dream ra v  rqv pretre priest pra tr  prtr

u u

b palpe feel palp pa lp p arc park park pa rk

i

verte green fem vrt va rt

Vowels followed by one consonantorby a consonant group which can b e an

onset lengthen as if they are in an op en syllable whereas vowels followed

See Charette for a more fully develop ed theory along the lines of Selkirk and

Basbll Tranel and Verluyten for criticism

French

by a consonant cluster of decreasing sonority do not lengthen at all I think

this is convincing evidence that word nal consonants can form degenerate

syllables as Charette also argues But in that case wemightbeled

to think that indep endent reference to the closed syl lable is not necessary

We do not nd b et or bt b ecause in these cases e or would o ccur

in a branching fo ot not b ecause they are in a closed syllable

The reason why I think that the notion closed syl lable is still needed is

that wendFrenchwords suchasprdri but not prdri or p erdri

There is no obvious wayinwhich the rst syllable in these forms constitutes

a footofitsown

Selkirk argues that the French fo ot is usually monosyllabic In

addition she claims that thereare instances however where the French

foot is morecomplex I submit that yet another principle is at work

in the denition of the foot in French a principle according to which a

syl lable containing may join together with a preceding syl lable and with it

constitute a foot Notice that this is a prop osal of conicting constraints

since a fo ot built according to the last principle will by necessityviolate

the monosyllabicity requirement on feet This prop osal has several eects

some of them having to do with schwa deletion which I will discuss b elow

section One other eect is that twoschwas form a fo ot next to one

another at the end of a word

F

s w

 

s vr

Selkirk prop oses that in the strong p osition of a fo ot is sub ject to

arulechanging it to Although this analysis is very interesting and I will

follow it in spirit I think it fails to capture one imp ortant generalisation

In this theory the fact that it is exactly schwa that is adjoined to other

syllables to form a bisyllabic fo ot and the fact that schwachanges to

when it is in the strong p osition of a fo ot are formally unrelated Intuitively

b oth pro cesses seem to b e triggered by the fact that schwa cannot b e in

a stressed p osition In a constraintbased analysis this typ e of statement

is easier to express we simply say that an unstressed p osition in a fo ot

implies schwa and vice versa than in the rulebased approach of Selkirk

where the generalisation has to b e made bywayoftwo formally

indep endent rules

In Optimality Theoretic terms we could say that there is a constraint

French

forcing stress to b e as much to the right as p ossible I will use the shorthand



AlignFoot for the constraintwhich has the following formal sp ecication

h

AlignFoot AlignF R PrWd R

h

I used the sign F to indicate the head fo ot of the word I assume

every Proso dic Word has a designated fo ot which functions as its head

says that the head fo ot of the word in French should b e at the rightedge

of that word Since primary stress in Frenchisnever further awayfrom

the word edge than one schwaheaded syllable I assume this constraint

is undominated

Because French do es not have reduction I have assumed that the pro

jection constraint ProjectV Ft whichsays that vowels in unstressed



p osition should not contain vo calic features hence b e schwa is ranked rela

tively lowinFrench To b e more precise all constraints on feature parsing

are ranked higher than this constraint Underlying vo calic features have

to b e parsed in French even in unstressed p osition We therefore have the

following partial ordering

ParseFeature ProjectV Ft



In this example ParseFeature stands for the set of all parsing constraints

on vo calic place features fParsecoronal Parsedorsal Parselabialg

and mayb e also the ap erture features Parselax Parsehigh Parse

low This set of constraints seem to cluster together in French it is not

p ossible to establish an internal ranking within this set

Yet I will show that one version of ProjectFt the version which



forces the strong p osition of bisyllabic feet to contain vo calic features ProjectFt



V is still visibly op erativeinFrench

def

ConnectFt V ProjectFt V ProjectV Ft Nis

  

the head of a branching fo ot i N dominates lax

I also assume French feet are leftheaded The following constraint is there

fore undominated



I will assume that French has only one fo ot p er word since there is no evidence

for secondary stress and furthermore sequences of schwa syllables do o ccur in nonnal

p osition eg genevois genevian znvwa This can only b e explained under my

assumptions if neither schwa forms the head of a fo ot which in turn implies under the

assumption that French feet are binary and leftheaded that b oth are unfo oted

If this assumption is right the Alignment constraint in can b e replaced bya

constraint on plain feet without reference to the head of the proso dic word If AlignF

R PrWd R is undominated it can only b e satised by candidate outputs whichhave

one fo ot at the outer right edge

This is a technical p oint of little relevance to the argumentation in the text

French

TrocheeFeet are leftheaded

Trochee and the ConnectFt V constraints together haveasaneect



that the ideal binary fo ot contains a full vowel and a schwa in that order

also in French

We can observe that the nal syllable of a word is always stressed unless



this syllable contains schwa in which case the stress is on the p enult

Wecannow extend the hierarchy just established with two additional

constraints FootBin requiring feet to b e binary and lax Both con

straints should b e ranked relatively low FootBin b ecause stress is so often

on the nal syllable in this supp osedly tro chaic system which means the

nal fo ot contains only one syllable and lax b ecause the feature lax is

inserted in leve mene appel le etc

ParseFeature AlignFoot Trochee



V FootBin lax ConnectFt



This constraint ranking gives us the right results for forms like paru ap

p eared containing only full vowels ordre order with one full vowel and

schwa and appel le call with one full vowel and twoschwas In the fol

lowing tableaux I have put the undominated constraints together in one

column for convenience In this column I have indicated the actually vio

lated constraintforevery candidate



pary

lax Candidates Undominated ConnectFt V FootBin



pary

p

pary

par ParseFeature

pary AlignFoot

rdr

Candidates Undominated ConnectFt V FootBin lax



p

rdr

rdr

rdr



I will turn to the sp ecial case of enclitics b elow



The ranking b etween FootBin and lax is established in the tableau in b elow

Compare the optimal candidate with pl



It is p ossible that fo ot binarity is not a violable constraint see app endix A If that

is true and if furthermore French feet are tro chaic words ending in a stressed syllable like

pary should b e assumed to have an extra empty syllable and FootBin in these tableaux

should b e replaced by another constraint probably one against empty syllables

French

apl

Candidates Undominated ConnectFt V FootBin lax



apl AlignFoot

p

apl

apl

apl

apl

apl

The question nowiswhywewould turn schwainto the lax vowel inthe

head p osition of a fo ot and not for instance into the tense vowel e even

though the latter vowel has fewer features hence is less marked It seems

that it is not sucienttohave the constraint ConnectFt V ask for any



vo calic feature to supp ort the head of a fo ot ConnectFt V inFrench



demands a supp orting lax feature just like Connect N lax

To conclude this section consider what happ ens with a form ending in

e suchasappelez call aple

aple

lax Candidates Undominated ConnectFt V FootBin



aple AlignFoot

aple

aple

p

aple

apl

aple

One minor problem arises in the analysis of this form In the rst place

the reader mightwonder why the actual output aple do es not vio

late the constraint ConnectFt lax For this it is imp ortanttosee



that ConnectFt lax like all pro jection constraints refers to heads of



branching constituents Since the e in this case is not in a branching

constituent it can by denition never violate this constraint

Although French cannot apply reduction to create an optimal fo ot of

a full vowel and schwa it can in some cases ll an underlying schwa

with vo calic material to comply with this requirement The constraint

ConnectFt lax can therefore b e seen to play a role even in a language



without reduction

Uschwa do es not o ccur at the b eginning of the word

Prop erty As I noted in the discussion of Dutch this prop erty seems to

French

b e close to universal LikeDutch Frenchdoesnothaveanyword of the

form I assume the same solution for French as I did for Dutch It

is b etter to leave the schwa unparsed than to satisfy the Onset constraint

by inserting a glottal stop In other words I assume the ranking for b oth

languages

N ContourParseVowel Projectcons

It is b etter to leaveavowel unparsed than to create an empty syllable

This reasoning app eared to extend to h as well in Dutch schwawas also

absent from p ositions adjacent to this segment It is not p ossible to test this

hyp othesis in French b ecause this language do es not have h consonants

on the surface An exception has to b e made for some Northern French

dialects and for Wallo on in which this h b ehaves like Standard French h

aspire

The phenomenon of h aspire aspirated h in general seems to b e of

particular interest As wehaveseenabove haspire words blo ck deletion

of underlying schwa while at the same time they do not trigger schwa

ep enthesis See Tranel for a p ossible solution of this problem

Consonant clusters b efore schwa cannot b e p ossible com

plex onsets Prop ert y This seems to provide us with the clear

est dierence b etween DutchschwaandFrenchschwa French has lots of

words with complex p ossible onsets b efore schw aasisshown in example

When these words are b orrowed and adapted to the Dutch system

the liquid shifts its p osition

French papavre p oppy papavr cadre id kadr cable id

kabl debacle id debakl etc

Dutch papavr kadr kabl debakl etc

In Dutch the constraint against empty heads in closed syllables Connect N

lax is ranked lower than the constraint against empty heads in syllables

with an onset ProjectN cons ApparentlyinFrench the constraint

ranking is reversed

Input papavr

a Dutch p ostlexical

Candidates ProjectN V ConnectN lax

p

papavr

papavr

French

b French

Candidates ConnectN lax ProjectN V

papavr

p

papavr

Neither French nor Dutch allows for a sequence trl In Dutch this se

quence is disallowed b ecause of Project NVschwaheaded syllables do

not have complex onsets In French it is disallowed b ecause of ConnectN

lax schwaheaded syllables do not havecodas

Schwa is stressless Prop erty Other than the previous prop

erty this generalisation holds for FrenchasmuchasitdidforDutch I have

already explained why and howschwa is stressles s in French since part of my

explanation for the schwa alternation dep ended on this prop erty Here

as in Dutch the relevant constraintwas ConnectFt lax connecting



feet to vo calic feature material

As in Dutch clitics form an exceptional domain for some schwa prop

erties in French In the latter language enclitics can get word stress

donnele

dnl

givehim

When wehave a cluster of schwasyllable clitics stress is on the last one

donnemele

dnml

givemeit

giveittome

These forms are exceptional in twoways Assuming for a moment that the

stem clitic combination is the prop er domain for word stress wehaveto

explain why the stress falls on the nal rather than the prenal schwagiven

the ranking laid out ab ove wewould exp ect donml or dnml

instead of and in the second place given the fact that the last clitic

is stressed why do esnt the vowel surface as

The null hyp othesis in these cases would b e that French stemclitic

combinations do indeed form the domain for the normal stress rule I think

the most imp ortant observation to make is that in all these cases is that all

French enclitics are monosyllabic Nowwemight assume that Frenchfeet

are sub ject to some requirementofIntegrity Kager b

French

IntegrityAFoot may not cross the b oundary b etween stem and

clitic

Supp osing the Integrity requirement is undominated we get the following

candidate evaluations for donnele and donnemele

dnl

Candidates Undominated ConnectFt V FootBin lax



dnl AlignFoot

dnl Integrity

dnl Integrity

p

dnl

dnl

dnl Integrity

dnml

Candidates Undominated ConnectFt V FootBin lax



dnml AlignFoot

dnml Integrity

dnml Integrity

p

dnml

dnml

dnml Integrity

The only serious candidates are those in which the fo ot is built on top of

the schwasyllable only These candidates by necessityhave a monosyllabic

fo ot Since ConnectFt V is a constraint on branching feet it is not



applicable to these forms Insertion of a feature lax therefore do es not

help in anyway but gives a violation constraint against the insertion of

this feature

Schwa cannot o ccur next to a vowel Prop erty This

is again a prop ertywhichFrench shares with Dutch As a matter of fact

the prop erty shows up even more strongly in the former language than in

the latter In Dutch schwa and full vowel can b e adjacent to one another

if they are separated by a morpheme b oundary to b e more precise the

b oundary b etween a stem and a prex

b eargumenteer

bbrymnter

give arguments for

French

In Frenchschwas do not surface in environments comparable to those in

either

a refaire

rfr

do again

b reamener

rmne rmne

to bring back

c p etite

ptit

small fem

d jolie

zli zlij

pretty

The facts are somewhat more complicated than this example might suggest

One imp ortant problem is that nal schwa do es not surface at all in many



dialects of French

Before I discuss these problems however I rst have to make more

explicit my analysis of monomorphemic forms In these forms Frenchdoes

not showany V or V sequences

I assume French and Dutch are exactly the same in this resp ect For

Dutch I argued that the main reason for the ban on vowelschwa sequences

was the Onset constraint combined with a constraint against sequences of

unarticulated segments like glottal stops and schwas Wehaveseenabove

that the same typ e of argumentation should hold for Frenchaswell given

that French do es not havewords starting with a schwa

Wecannow turn to monomorphemic forms I have already p ointed

out that French and Dutch dier with resp ect to the force of Alignment

constraints In Dutch morphological word b oundaries havetobewell

aligned with their phonological counterparts This caused ep enthetic schwa

to b e inserted inside the word rather than at the edge In French on the

other hand morphological Alignment constraints are not that strong This

has as one eect that ep enthetic schwa can o ccur at the edge of the word

in the latter language

Wehave already seen that there is additional evidence for this relative

strength in Frenchversus Dutch Frenchallows for resyllabication over



A further problem is that re is the only schwanal prex in French and that the

analysis of this prex is not unproblematic De Jong and Hietbrink Yet pro clitics

suchasne not je I le him etc also b ehaveintheway indicated here

French

the word edge liaison whereas Dutch do es not I prop ose that here we

have a similar eect b oundaries b etween stem and ax are resp ected in

Dutchasmuch as p ossible

What I want to argue then is that in languages likeFrench or Italian

Onset is ranked ab ove Align I also assume that Onset is ranked ab ove

ParseVowel the constraint forcing vo calic ro ots to b e parsed In a form

like na pas do es not have we then get the following tableau I used square

brackets to indicate the b oundaries of the lexical words

Candidates Onset Align ParseVowel

napa

p

n apa

Dutch shows an asymmetry b etween prexes and suxes with resp ect to

schwa deletion as discussed in the previous chapter French do es not have

this asymmetry at all For suxes we nd exactly the same pattern

eliteair

Candidates Onset Align ParseVowel

elitr

p

elit  r

An interesting contrast b etween DutchandFrench can b e observed if we

consider the b ehaviour of the inectional sux it happ ens that this suf

x marks adjectival agreement in b oth languages when it follows a stem

ending in a vowel As wehave seen in the previous chapter Dutch zeeen

seas surfaces as zej The schwa do es surface in the case of tran

sjunctural vowelschwa sequences I argued that the reason for this is a

constraint called MorPa which forces every morpheme to get some reali

sation in proso dic structure at least partlyIf is the only phonological

element in a sux it is this sux that should b e realized

Yet in French jolie pretty surfaces as zli not as zlij What

this should mean then is that the MorPa constraint is not as strong in

French as it is in Dutch

There is indep endent evidence for this dierence b etween the twolan

guages In Dutch all suxes always surface even if they consist of just a

coronal consonantliket the third p erson singular marker on v erbs or s

a plural marker on nouns slaapt sleeps slapt adders id drs

Wehave seen that the Tilburg dialect of Dutcheven forces these consonants

to b e inside the proso dic word if p ossible so that we get slpt instead of

slqpt

French

In French on the other hand many inectional axes do not surface ex

cept under very sp ecial circumstances One instance is the plural marker s

It is imp ossible to nd a phonological reex of this grammatical marker on

the form bons good b if pronounced in isolation even though phono

tactically there is no reason why this form should not o ccur in Mo dern

French The s only surfaces in liaison contexts bons amis go o d friends

bzami There is therefore indep endent evidence to assume that French

do es not attachmuch imp ortance to the phonological parsing of all inec

tional axes Unlike Dutchitdoesnothave a reason to treat the nal

schwainjolie dierent from any other schwa

Schwadeletion As I have indicated several times ab ove schwa

deletion in Frenchismuch more common than it is in Dutch Noske

distinguishes b etween six typ es of schwazero alternation One of these

is ep enthesis discussed in sections and ab ove Two others are

prevo calic and p ostvo calic sch wadeletion discussed in section The

remaining three contexts are

Schwa deletion in a two sided op en syllable This alternation

is perhaps the most general one according to Noske A schwain

atwo sided op en syllable is a schwa in the context VCCV or

VCOLV Oobstruent Lliquid In other words schwahasto

b e in an op en syllable has to have a simple onset and b e preceded by



an op en syllable Ihave copied the following examples from Noske

a tu devenais you b ecame

i tydvn

ii tydvn

iii tydvn

iv tydvn

b Henri devrait partir H would havetoleave

c ridvrpartir

i ari dvrpartir

ii aridvrpartir

d Jacques devrait partir Jacques would havetoleave



On variations of this p ointbetween Standard FrenchandQueb ec French see Charette

French

i zakdvrpartir

ii zakdvrpartir

Schwa deletion in phrasenal syllables Apart from this most

general typ e of alternation Noskeshows that wehave to distinguish

twocontexts in which underlying schwas do not surface both at

the p eriphery of the phrase Phrasenal schwa is deleted without

exception in Standard French

a la terre est plate the earth is at latrplat

b la route est longue the road is long larutlg

Schwa deletion in phraseinitial syllables Although somewhat

more marked and under somewhat more restricte d conditions phrase

initial schwas can also b e deleted

a Revenez demain Come back tomorrow rvnedm

padbil b Tefaispas de bil Dont worry tf

I will discuss Noskes rst two cases b elowbutIhavevery little to say

ab out his last instance of schwadeletion Noske remarks that this is the

most marked form of deletion I basically accept Noskes analysis that more

complex onsets are allowed at the b eginning of the phrase than in other

p ositions just like more complex onsets are allowed wordinitially than in

other p ositions in other languages for instance in Polish cf Gussmann

Below I will argue that the phrase in Frenchplays the role of the

word in Dutch or English in many other resp ects

According to Charette the reason whyschwas in Frenchhave

a tendency to not surface at all is the Avoid Vowel Principle

Avoid Vowel Principle

An emptynucleus remains uninterpreted whenever p ossible

The dierence b etween an interpreted and an uninterpreted nucleus in

Charettes terminology for all practical purp oses is the same as that b etween

aschwaheaded and a degenerate syllable in our terms Within the formal

ism develop ed here this constraint can therefore b e stated as cons the

ban on vo calic ro ots The whenever p ossible clause in Charettes formu

lation is an indication that there are constraints which are in conict with

the AVP

In the chapter on DutchIhave already discussed the most imp ortantof

these constraints For instance we need to make sure that only schwacan

French

b e deleted The reason for this is that all other vowels havevo calic features

like coronal labial high etc which need to b e parsed Apparently the

parsing of these features is more imp ortantthantheAvoid Vowel Principle

but the parsing of ro otsie cons featuresis not otherwise schwa

would never b e deleted either

ParseFeature cons ParseVowel Projectcons N

I use ParseFeature as a shorthand for the set of related constraints

Parsecoronal Parselabial Parselow etc

Another constraint that is of relevance is the constraint against degener

ate syllables Project N cons Degenerate syllables are disfavoured in

French except in phrasep eripheral p ositions Vowels can therefore only

b e deleted if their surrounding consonants can b e parsed in a dierentway

N cons cons Project

I assume that schwa deletion is a p ostlexical pro cess in French Given the

fact that two sided op en syllable deletion is p ossible only when the schwa

syllable is preceded by an op en syllable I think there is no ob jection against

accepting Noskes analysis that the consonant preceding underlying schwa

gets syllabied into the co da of the preceding syllable It is signicantthat

the denition of this two sided syllable seems to b e exactly cast in such

awaysoastomake this resyllabicati on p ossible we can haveridvr

but not zakdvr b ecause the latter form necessarily contains a non

p eripheral degenerate syllable

A problem might seem to arise in the case of devenait when pronounced

as dvn b ecause now the rst syllable is closed while its head still sur

facesasschwa not as Yet wehave already seen in chapter that

in Dutch p ostlexical phonology where we insert schwaincontexts like

theat r the constraint Connect N lax can get lower ranked so that it

is no longer op erative in cases like this For French we could for instance

assume that p ostlexically dorsallaxdorsalConnectN lax We

will see b elow that Norwegian actually shows dialectal dierence with re

specttothispoint some dialects are exactly likeFrench and Dutchin

reranking the order b etween the constraints but in other dialects the rank

ing holds at every level of phonological analysis disallowing schwatooccur

in any closed syllable on the surface

We can now turn to wordnal schwa deletion The interesting thing

ab out this deletion site is that it allows schwa not only to b e deleted in a

twosided op en syllable but also when preceded by a closed syllable or

when the schwaheaded syllable has a complex onset Deletion is even p os

sible when schwa has a complex onset and is preceded by a closed syllable

French

as in Chartres name of a town sartr Since we are dealing with word

nal syllables the schwasyllable is actually alternating with a degenerate

syllable The nal syllable of quatre in Jen ai quatre I have four of them

therefore is either a or b

N a

N

N

t r

b N

N

N

t r

The form in a is chosen inside a phonological phrase and b efore a con

sonant in other words in exactly the same environmentasschwaepenthe

sis as in quatrcopains four friends whereas the form in b is chosen

phrasenally

Wehave already seen that the Alignment constraints do not playarole

at the word level in French b ecause liaison causes syllabication over the

word b oundary and ep enthesis applies at the edge of words but that an

Alignment constraint is necessary at the level of the syntactic and phonolog

ical phrase again in order to restrict application of liaison whichdoesnot

apply to segments in anytwo adjacentwords Yet even phrasal alignment

cannot explain the deletion of schwaifwe assume that at least in some cases

the underlying schwa is nal For instance the phonological representation

latrplat and the morphological and syntactic representation la terreest

plate the earth is at do not seem particularily wellaligned

We therefore have to nd a highranking constraint dominating at least

Align the phrasal alignment constraint I use  as a shorthand for the

phonological phrase McCarthy has prop osed a constraintwhich

ant to us in his Optimality Theoretic analysis of Eastern might b e relev

Massachussetts rinsertion This constraintisFinalC

FinalC A Proso dic Word has to end in a consonantAlignPrWd

R C x

French

McCarthy argues basically that in the Eastern Massachussetts dialect of

English an r is inserted in vowelnal words in order to satisfy this con

straintspar idear etc

It seems that a similar constraint could b e at work here although French

would have a dierent parameter setting for FinalC the phonological

phrase instead of the proso dic word and use a dierent strategy to reach

its aim and goal Instead of inserting a default consonant Frenchwould

prefer to delete vowels The following constraint ranking could achieve the

appropriate result

ParseFeature FinalCAlignParseV

The ranking FinalCAlignParseV has as a result that it is b etter

to delete phrasenal underlying schwa in order for a word to end in a conso

nant than to keep syntactic and phonological representations wellaligned

The constraint ranking ParseFeature FinalC on the other hand

ensures that only schwa can b e deleted not some full vowel which has more

phonological features

A parameter The phonological phrase in French seems to as

sume a lot of the tasks that are played by the phonological word in lan

guages like Dutch or English Pulgram This is true at least for

syllabication primary stress and ep enthesis The Alignment constraints

that are parametrized for the word level in Dutch seem parametrized for

the phrase lev el in French

Dutch English French

ep enthesis AlignPrWd X Y X Align X YP X

syllabication

h h

stress AlignF RWord R AlignF RR

FinalC FinalC Word FinalC 

I assume now that like the two alignment constraints also FinalC is

parametrized In Eastern Massachussetts English it is a constrainton

the level of the proso dic word Yet in French also this task is taken over

by the phrase it is phonological phrasesnotwords that have to end in a

consonant

In this particular place it seems preferable to depart slightly from the

ortho dox optimalitytheoretic p oint of view If the traditional way of lo ok

ing at things of Pulgram is right and there are languages in which

several pro cesses play a role at the phrase level which all play a role at

the word level in other languages there is reason to intro duce a limited

principle of parameter setting into the theory

French

Such a revision of the theory would also b e more attractive from the

p oint of view of language learnability UG would have some subset of

constraints all of which could b e sp ecied for either the phrase or the

word These constraints would refer to a parameter ranging over eg fword

phraseg and all these constraints would b e set in the same setting by the

language learner The child would observe for instance that Dutchdoesnot

haveany plausible case of phonological resyllabication and therefore she

would set the parameter in every constraint in the appropriate way

On the other hand if it would turn out that there are mixed systems

sometimes referring to words and at other times to phrases wemight still

have reason to b elieve that all four constraints in are presentinUni

versal Grammar and that it is just an accidentthatFrenchcho oses the

phrasal variants of the alignment constraints and Dutchcho oses the word

variants Extensivetyp ological study would b e needed to settle this p oint

Assuming FinalC takes precedence over Align hence by transitiv

ity also over ProjectN cons in French as it did in Dutch and assuming

that la terre est plate forms one phonological phrase we get the tableau in

ParseFeatures

FinalC 

Align

N cons Project

cons

Parsevowel Projectcons N

la terre est plate the earth is at

Candidates FinalC Align ProjectN cons

p

latrplat

latrplat

The question now is what happ ens to forms in which the nal schwaheaded

syllable contains a complex onset as in feminine autre other Given

the fact that the pronunciation ot with deletion of the r as well as

of the schwa satises all constraints just mentioned just as well as the

pronunciation otr it ends in a consonant it leaves one schwa unparsed it

has a degenerate syllable etc and since also the constraint Project N

cons banning unnecessary onset segments from emptyheaded syllables

is active at some level why do es not this rst form surface

For some dialects the answer to this question is armative For instance

Breton French Lo dge seems to allow exactly two pronunciations of

quatre four

French

kat

katr

Phraseinternally schwa can surface and r can surface with it so that we

get the form in At the end of the phrase the schwa do es not surface

p erhaps b ecause of FinalC But then there is no reason in Breton French

for the r to surface assuming the constraint Contiguity blo cks the

parse ka t r

In Paris Frenchwe can distinguish b etween several style registers In

informal sp eechwe nd kat at the end of the phrase but in more for

mal registers wendkatr The dierences b etween style registers I have

argued in the previous chapter can b e describ ed as a reranking of Faith

fulness conditions the more formal the style of sp eech the higher ranked

the Faithfulness constraints In the case at hand it could b e that in Paris

French there is a very formal style of sp eech in which for instance Parse

consonant is ranked higher than Project N cons In this style of

sp eech underlying consonants would always surface even if that means

that they surface in the complex onset of a degenerate syllable whichis

otherwise a very marked conguration

Candidates FinalC ParseC Project cons

kat

p

katr

katr

Dell has p ointed out that only an analysis which assumes that nal

obstruentliquid sequences are onsets of degenerate syllables can explain

why the liquid is deleted in exactly these words and not in others For

instance an analysis assuming an optional rule deleting wordnal sono

rants like the following can not explain why the sonorantisnever deleted

in word like parle sp eak or rhythme rhythm

C

In the theory presented here parle and rhythme are syllabied as pa rl

and ritm resp ectivel y there is indep endent reason to assume rl and tm

are not p ossible onset clusters so that the same problem do es not arise

There is no need to simplify the onsets of these degenerate syllables b ecause



they are already suciently simple



For the sake of completeness I should mention that Dell lists a few clusters

which can o ccur b efore full vowels but not b efore schwaorwordnally These are tn

French

Conclusion plus a note on learnability Despite the sup er

cial dierences b etween French and Dutch I haveshown that the grammars

of the two languages are only minimally dierent The basic dierence is

one in parameter setting What the word b oundary is for Dutch is the

phrase b oundary for French This accounts among other things for the fact

that Dutch has ep enthesis only within a word and French only within a

phrase

As for the ranking of constraints only a few rerankings were needed

In the rst place in French ConnectN laxdo es not aect the rounded

vowels whereas in Dutchitdoes



labial

a Dutch ConnectN lax

lax



labial

b French Connect N lax

lax

In the second place since in French lax vowels can o ccur in op en syllables

wehave the following reranking

a Dutch Connect N laxParselax

b French Parselax ConnectN lax

Because French do es not have reduction al l Parsefeature constraints

are inviolable This also means that there is no way to establish an internal

ranking b etween dierentversions of ConnectFt lax



Finally the stress system of French is somewhat dierent from that of

Dutch In Dutch the feet are always binary b e it that sometimes they

include a catalectic syllable In other words Fo ot Binarity is undominated

in that language In French this constraint is dominated at least by In

tegrityby ConnectFt lax and by AlignFoot



In other words b oth ConnectN lax and Fo ot Binarityhavesunkto

a somewhat lower place in the hierarchyinFrench But their eects are

still visible so that we cannot say that these constraints are parameters

switched o in French Wethus have found evidence in favour of parameters

ranging over a set of values suchasfword phraseg while at the same

time wehave evidence against straightforward binary parameters of the

ono typ e In comp ensation the Parsefeature constraints havegot

ranked higher in French than was the case in Dutch

ethnologue ethnologist tl athlete id kn acne id bz objet ob ject dm

admirer admire and gz exil id All of these clusters are wordinternally analysed

as co daonset sequences It is not clear to me what would disallow them to o ccur b efore

aschwaorwordnally

Norwegian

It seems to me that a child should b e able to acquire these rankings

fairly easily As already discussed at various places ab ove I prop ose that

at an initial stage the child assumes that all constraints are inviolable except

for the faithfulness constraints which are ranked very low cf section

in the rst chapter A Dutchsp eaking child can learn that Project

Ft constraints are ranked fairly low when confronted with dierentstyles



of sp eech in which full vowels do not alternate with schwas Given the

unpredictable nature of the full vowel in these contexts it is easy to see that

these alternations are due to reduction But in reduction Parsefeature

constraints are always violated and Pro jection constraints are probably the

only p ossible causes for reduction On the other hand a Frenchchild can

learn that ConnectN lax is ranked relatively low when she observes that

there are tense rounded vowels in closed syllables and lax vowelsinopen

syllables eg at the end of the word A Frenchsp eaking child can acquire

the low ranking of Fo ot Binarityby observing that there are twotyp es of

feet sw and s and that the latter pattern o ccurs regularly ie cannot

b e treated as an exception as is the case in Dutch It is interesting that

the Frenchchild do es not have to learn anything particular ab out schwa

while the Dutchchild only has to see that it alternates with other vowels

Everything else follows from the empty nature of this vowel

Norwegian

Inow turn to Norwegian This language also has a schwa with some in

teresting prop erties but unlikeforFrench and Dutch there is no tradition

of studying these prop erties For the data in this section I have mainly

relied on the pioneering study on Norwegian syllable structure by Kristof

fersen and myown research on a Norwegian dictionary Haugen

Norwegian is interesting for several reasons Wehave seen that French

typ e schwas can have a complex onset but no co da table and Dutch

t yp e schwas can have a co da but no complex onset tafel Norwegian

has b oth typ es of schwa tig r ordr It do es not however haveschwa

headed syllables with b oth a complex onset and a co da katrl Norwegian

dialects furthermore dier from each other in interesting ways in closed

syllables some dialects have instead of schwa and in wordnal op en

syllables some dialects haveanemptynucleus rather than schwa

I will concentrate here on Norwegian ep enthetic and underlying schwa

Ihave not b een able to nd sucient evidence for vowel reduction in any

Norwegian dialect It is true that unstressed syllables in the Germanic

stratum of the lexicon are almost always headed byschwainmanydi

Norwegian

alects of Norwegian while they are headed by e in others but as far as I

know there is no evidence that this schwa in the former group of dialects

has to b e derived from some underlying full vowel As far as ep enthetic

schwa is concerned I will concentrate only on those cases whichinDutch

I called sub cases of uschwa the schwawhichoccurswordnally b etween

an obstruent and a sonorant consonantasin tiger Neither ep enthesis nor

reduction therefore seems at issue in Norwegian

Again the starting p ointformy analysis of Norwegian is schwas Prop

erty

Prop erty Schwa is exceptional in the vocalic system because it bears no

vocalic features

Kristoersen notes that in some dialects of Norwegian schwais

somewhat more fronted and sounds almost like a coronal mid vowel e

or Yet in most dialects the distinction b etween those vowels and schwa

seems to b e suciently clear

Schwa is the ep entheticvowel Prop ert y Although

Norwegian do es not haveschwaepenthesis of the Dutch hel p typ e some

dialects show evidence for ep enthesis in words which underlyingly end in an

obstruent followed by a sonorant consonant as in Dutch theat r Kristof

fersen p oints out that Oslo Norwegian has a lot of words ending in

syllabic sonorants the sup erscript ed numb ers indicate the stressed syllable

of the word the typ e of tonal accent asso ciated to the word in question

known in the Scandinavian literature as accent and accent

 

nkklkey doqvn lazy kl simple bkn cymbals

laqkn sheet vimpl streamer mnstr pattern

In standard Norwegian orthography the words in question are transcrib ed

with an e between the obstruent and the liquid This schwa is also pro

nounced in many southern which therefore have the

following forms

 

kn laqkn vimpl mnstr nkql doqvn kl b

Kristoersen argues that these schwas should b e analysed as ep enthetic

for the following reasons In the rst place if the schwas were underlying

we could not explain why they are always followed by a liquid and never

by an obstruent given that wehaveaword like hasel hazel hasl why

are there no words like hast for Oslo hast As wehave seen in

Norwegian

the discussion of Dutch ep enthesis theory can explain this fact given the

minimal assumption that only sonorants can b e nuclear heads

A further observation supp orting ep enthesis is that there are no monomor

phemic words with identical sonorant consonants preceding and following



the vowel Thus while there are derived forms like murer mason mTrer

in Oslo there are no phonologically similar monomorphemic forms This

can b e understo o d in a theory with ep enthesis in terms of the Contour

constraint dominating ParseC as wehave seen for a similar observation

on Dutchinchapter

The fact that the ep enthetic vowel is schwa in these cases should not

come as a surprise As in Dutch and French there are reasons for inserting

avowel slot Since schwa is the most minimal vowel intro ducing the

smallest numb er of features this is the vowel that is chosen

Ep enthetic schwa do es not o ccur at the end of the word

Prop ert y Norwegian is likeDutch and dierent from French in its

selection of ep enthesis site Apparently in wordnal CC clusters Norwegian

c ho oses the p osition between the two consonants rather than the p osition

following them Consider for instance the resp ectivewords for tigre in each

of the three languages

a French ti gr

b Dutch tij r

c Norwegian ti gr

In section I connected the dierence b etween French and Dutchto

other dierences b etween these two languages French has pro ductive ex

ternal while Dutch do es not French seems to select the phrase

rather than the word as a domain for stress etc I suggested that there is

a parameterlike mechanism at play where various constraints which are

formulated at the word level in Dutchhave b een formulated at the phrase

level in French

Given the Norwegian ep enthesis site wenow predict that this language

should b e likeDutch with resp ect to sandhi as well The wordinternal

ordThe schwaisbrought ab out by a relatively high ranking for Alignw

parameter setting of this language therefore is word

The fact that there even are no t clusters means that the status of wordnal

coronals in Norwegian is dierent from their status in Dutch Wehyp othesizedthatin

Dutchschwa is allowed to o ccur b efore coronals b ecause these were extrasyllabic and

therefore did necessarily not close the syllable In Norwegian apparently these coronal

consonants are parsed into the proso dic word whenever they can thereby closing the

syllable and forcing its vowel to o ccur as wherever this is p ossible

Norwegian

In this light it b ecomes relevant that Norwegian is like the Romance

language Italian a language which arguably has the parameter setting

phrase just likeFrench in that on the surface it has b oth long vowels

and long consonants and that it has a requirement that stressed syllables

should b e bimoraic this is due to a constraint I will call Prokosch Law

in chapter Furthermore vo calic length and consonantal length are in

complementary distribution in b oth languages in the sense that we can de

rive the one if we p osit the other in the underlying representations In the

literature on b oth languages there is a debate whether we should p osit

vowel length or consonantal length as underlying

Connected to this is Italian Raddoppiamento sintatticoifaword ends

in a stressed vowel the consonant of the following word gets lengthened

if the twowords are in the same phonological phrase Italian thus is

likeFrench in that it has external sandhi The Alignword constraintis

ranked accordingly low

Interestingly Norwegian has a pro cess that is phonologically very similar

to Raddoppiamento Johnsen KayeandVergnaud Yet this typ e of



Raddoppiamento never crosses the b oundaries of the syntactic X It

therefore seems reasonable to exp ect Alignword is ranked suciently

high in Norwegian so that we predict wordinternal rather than wordnal

ep enthesis indeed

Schwamust o ccur in an op en syllable Prop ert yNor

wegian b ehaves exactly the same as Dutch with resp ect to this prop erty

ep enthetic schwas are forced inside a closed syllable for reasons of Align

ment Real underlying schwa on the other hand always o ccurs in an op en

syllable otherwise wewould still also exp ect hast

There are however some dialects of Norwegian which disallowschwain

closed syllables even in vowel ep enthesis One instance of such a dialect is

Bo d Norwegian sp oken in a town in Northern Norway In this dialect the

Oslo forms in are pronounced as follows

 

mnstr nkl doqvn kl bkn laqkn vimpl

In light of the discussion of French closed syllable adjustmentaboveitisof

course quite interesting that the vowel in the closed syllable surfaces as



Raddoppiamento Sintattico Norwegian Style as Johnsen KayeandVergnaud

call it applies b etween the two parts of a comp ound The initial consonant of the second

comp onent is lengthened if the rst comp onent ends in a stressed vowel This raises of

course the question what exactly denes the domain of this pro cess since it is usually

assumed the two parts of a comp ound are X category elements Apparently the two

parts of a comp ound do not countaswords in the sense of Alignword The most

imp ortant observationhowever is that there is no external sandhi

Norwegian

For Standard Dutchwe assumed that the reason whyepenthetic schwas

other than underlying schwas can surface in closed syllables is that at the

p ostlexical level where these schwas surface the ConnectN lax con

straint gets ranked very low The same should b e said ab out the southern

Norwegian dialects whichhaveepenthetic schwa

Bo d Norwegian is dierent from these languages in that ConnectN

lax still plays a role in this dialect In particular ConnectN lax dom

inates the constraint lax On the other hand Bo d Norwegian is similar

to Dutch and southern Norwegian in that it do es need to haveavowel

in every syllable unlike Oslo Norwegian whichallows syllables headed by



sonorants at every level

Bo d Norwegian

a ProjectN cons ConnectN lax lax

b laqkn

lax Candidates ProjectN cons ConnectN lax

laqkn

laqkn

p

laqkn

Southern Norwegian

a Project N V lax ConnectN lax

b laqkn

Candidates ProjectN cons ConnectN lax lax

p

laqkn

laqkn

laqkn

Oslo Norwegian

a cons ProjectN cons

b laqkn

Candidates ProjectN cons cons

laqkn

p

laqkn

laqkn



I do not transcrib e phonetic dierences other than the ones that concern us here In

particular there are variations across dialects in the pronunciations of aq and k which I do not note

Norwegian

The Oslo dialect is dierent from the others in that it has the Avoid

Vowel Principle cons ranked very high also at the p ostlexical level

Ep enthetic vowels are therefore disallowed regardless of their quality

Schwa do es not o ccur at the b eginningoftheword

Prop erty It seems that this prop erty is among the most universal

prop erties of schwa Norwegian do es not allowwordinitial schwas either

Apart from the Contour constraint against syllables with unarticulated

segments in b oth onset and nucleus one other factor seems at work in

Norwegian to prohibit wordinitial schwas

The fact is that schwa in Norwegian is found almost exclusivel y in the

Germanic stratum of the lexicon and that stress is on the rst syllable of the

word in this stratum Since schwa is unstressed in Norwegianjustasmuch

as in DutchandFrench this has an eect that we hardly nd anyschwain



wordinitial syllables at all even if these syllables do have a normal onset

As a consequence of the fact that most words in the Germanic stratum are

bisyllabic and are almost in complementary distribution

This solv es the rst problem just mentioned The second problem is

why the vowel in VCCV sequences like eple is always short I prop ose that

N V isstill the reason for this is that also the strong version of Project

activeinNorwegian Complex onsets b efore a schwa syllable are avoided

wherever this is p ossible and syllables are maximally bimoraic

According to nativespeakers intuitions eple is syllabied as pl not

as epl Wemight therefore assume that although the strong version of

Project N V isranked fairly lowinNorwegian it is still ranked higher



than the Syllable Contact Law

N V SCL Project

epl

Candidates ProjectN V SCL

eqpl

p

pl

If p ossible Norwegian still prefers schwa syllables to have simple onsets

even if complex onsets are not completely disallowed as we will discuss

now



Norwegian has the Germanic prex be which contains a schwa Yet prexes are not

necessarliy stressed as we will see in the next chapter



N V should also outrank the relevant faith If vowel length is underlying Project

fulness constraints

Norwegian

Consonant clusters b efore schwa cannot b e p ossible com

plex onsets Prop ert y As far as this prop erty is concerned Nor

wegian seems to b e for one part likeDutch and for another likeFrench

While ep enthetic schwas do not allow for complex onsets tiger tigler

underlying schwas in op en syllables do monstre pattern venstre left

etc Still there are no schwasyllables with b oth a complex onset and a



co da katr l Furthermore in words ending in V CC like eple

i

the vowel seems to b e always short These are the facts that I wantto

explain in this section

First let us turn to the dierence b etween schwaheaded syllables and

syllables headed by a sonorant which get an ep enthetic schwa p ostlexi

cally What I just observed is that the latter cannot have a complex onset

whereas the former can In other words a and b are allowed but

c is not

a N b N c N

N N N

N

N

p l p r p

l r

In chapter I argued that we should distinguish b etween two pro jection

constraints one stating that syllables should have heads with vo calic fea

N V and another stating that syllables should haveat tures Project

N cons least a cons ro ot as their head Project

a Project N V N branches N should dominate a vo calic

feature

b ProjectN cons Nbranches N should dominate a vo

calic ro ot cons

The two onset segments in a givetwo violations of ProjectN V

b ecause they are not supp orted byavo calic head but they do not cause any

violations of ProjectN cons b ecause at least their head is a vo calic

ro ot On the other hand the two onset segments in c violate b oth

versions of ProjectN twice b ecause the head do es not haveanyvo calic



Ionceagainwanttopoint out that I am discussing underived forms onlyPolymor

phemic forms like epler apples plural do o ccur

Norwegian

N feature and it do es not haveavo calic ro ot The twoversions of Project

are in a Paninian relation b ecause a violation of ProjectN V will also

give a violation of Project N cons but not vice versa

We can establish the following constraint ranking

OnsetProjectN consParseConsonant ProjectN

V

N V has suchalow ranking in Norwegian that it Actually Project

seems to me that there is no reason to assume it is there unless one wishes

to stick to the idea that all constraints are present in the grammars of all

languages

Ihave noted earlier on in this chapter however that it mightbede

sirable to intro duce some idea of a parameter into the theory In the case

of the pro jection and weakness constraints one could imagine that the

feature parameter could b e set for every individual pro jection parameter

Thus French has the parameter setting lax for Project N and Project

NNorwegian has the Ft but the parameter setting cons for Project



same parameter settings for ProjectN and ProjectN but presumably



a dierent setting for ProjectFt I think at least in the data presented



in this thesis there is no real counterevidence to this claim although we

have to assume that Dutch lexically has the setting V for ProjectN and

p ostlexically the setting cons Project N V will b e attributed a mi

nor role in the analysis of p ostlexical Rotterdam Dutch presented in the

next chapter but it is not clear whether this has rep ercussions for the anal

ysis of the Standard Dutch p ostlexicon cf the arguments for constraint

ranking in app endix B

Onset has to b e undominated b ecause all schwaheaded syllables in

Norwegian have onsets Wenow get the following tableaux for ac

resp ectivel y

a pl

Candidates Onset Projectcons ParseC ProjectV

p

pl

p l 

p l

p  l  b pr

Norwegian

Candidates Onset Projectcons ParseC ProjectV

p

pr

p r

c plr

Candidates Onset Projectcons ParseC ProjectV

plr

p

p l r

p

p lr

p  l r

In the case of underlying plr either the p or the l will survive The

exact decision will b e made by constraints on Contiguity of parsing Align

ment and on the sonority slop e The actual decision is not very interesting

I think and therefore I will leave the topic here

Alternation with degenerate syllables According to Kristof

fersen most masculine and feminine words whichareschwanal in

most Norwegian dialects are pronounced without schwa in northern East

Norwegian In the terminology used in this dissertation these forms end

in a degenerate syllable rather than a schwasyllable

Two questions now arise In the rst place why do es this dialect prefer

degenerates over schwasyllables And in the second place whyisthis

preference restricted to the masculine and feminine gender

The answer to the rst question is quite obviouslytheAvoid Vowel

Principle cons Apparently this principle is ranked much higher in

northern East Norwegian than it is in any other dialect In other dialects

the Avoid Vowel principle is ranked b elow the parsing constraint Parse

N cons In the northeast the ranking is exactly Vowel and Project

the opp osite

a moqn NENorwegian

Candidates ParseVowel cons

moqn

p

moqn 

b moqn other dialects

cons Candidates ParseVowel

p

moqn

moqn 

Norwegian

To see why neuter nouns are dierent it is imp ortant to note that Norwe

gian seems to b e likeDutch in one imp ortant resp ect when a neuter words

ends in a schwasyllable the latter almost always has a complex onset

Wehave seen in section that Nijen Twilhaar observed this

for loan words in Dutch In Norwegian the correlation seems to hold also

for the Germanic stratum

The question therefore reduces to the question whywords with a com

plex onset resist deletion of the schwa Here again the distinction b etween a

cons and a V parameter setting of ProjectN b ecomes relevant Schwa

headed syllables are the only weak syllables with a complex onset in Nor

wegian Degenerate syllables are not allowed to have complex onsets as we

have seen and neither are syllables headed by sonorant consonants

Conclusion and another note on learnability The dier

ences b etween Norwegian and DutchorFrench do not seem to b e very

great Norwegian like Dutch seems to treat the word edge as the sp ecial

edge as opp osed to the phrase The only real dierence b etween Dutch and

Norwegian seems to b e that in the former language sonorantandschwa

heads of syllables b ehave in exactly the same way Most imp ortantly they

b oth disallow complex onsets In Norwegian on the other hand evidence

emerges that we should draw a dierence b etween schwaheaded syllables

and syllables headed by sonorants the former do allow for complex onsets

whereas the latter do not

Again this distinction is fairly easy to learn Hyp othesizing that in the

initial state neither schwas nor sonorants license an onset the Dutchchild



do es not have to learn anything in particular The Norwegian child will

however one day hit up on saytheword ordr e At that p oint she will know

that she has to relax the condition on schwaheaded syllables rank it b elow

Faithfulness say I assume in cases like this the child reranks only the

N V The most sp ecic version of a given familyinthiscase Project

weaker version stays high up in the hierarchy and will not shift from there

b ecause there is no evidence to that eect

Notice that there is no way to get to a system in which sonorants have

a complex onsets whereas schwadoesnotAssoonasachild would hit

up on a form kabrl she would have evidence to rerank ProjectN V to

alower p osition But this would also make p ossible schwaheaded syllable

with complex onsets The reader can check that there is no way to rank the

constraints given here in suchaway that we get a system in whichkatrl



The hyp othesis here is that children rst get acquainted with somewhat more formal

style registers There seems to b e some evidence for this hyp othesis indeed Paula

Fikkert pc

Conclusion

is allowed but katrisnot

Also the distinctions b etween the dierent dialects of Norwegian seem

to b e learnable As in the discussion of the distinctions b etween French

and Dutchabove I assume that all constraints are ranked very high in

the initial stage of the acquisition pro cess except for faithfulness In this

case it means that the Bo d dialect represents the initial stage of the

acquisition pro cess The child from Oslo observes that in lakn there is

a syllable without a vowel thus hyp othesizing that a constraint against

inserting vowels should b e ranked higher than the constraint ProjectN

cons Achild from Southern Norway observes that lakn contains a

violation of ConnectN lax She therefore assumes that a constraint

against the intro duction of lax is ranked higher than this constraint Since

there is no reason for her to assume that Project N cons also has

to leave its undominated p osition this automatically implies the ranking

N cons laxConnectN lax Project

Conclusion

Kager and Zonneveld remarked that it would probably b e dicult to

apply the No Syllable Theory they prop osed for Dutch to other Germanic

languages as well It is in myeyes a curious thing that although default

vowels are a wellstudied topic in many languages comparative studies are

rare one exception is Noske In this chapter I haveshown that de

spite the surface dierences the grammars needed for Dutch French and

Norwegian schwa are very similar given an idea of reranking in addition to

some minimal idea of a parameter setting In all three languages this vowel

only pro jects a very simple typ e of syllable in accordance with the predic

tions of the Pro jection Theory The exact nature of the syllable dep ends

on other parts of the grammar but these dierences are indep endently

observable

A VowelGlide Alternation in Rotterdam

Dutch

Intro duction

In the previous chapters I have discussed the relation b etween typical vo

calic features such as lax and low and proso dic structure In this chapter

I will examine under which conditions vowels can o ccur in an extracephalic

p osition in the syllable In particular I will review a vowelglide alternation

in the Rotterdam dialect of Dutch In this dialect i alternates with j

under certain conditions which will turn out to b e interesting also from

the p oint of view of my theory of schwa I will show that all alternations

are the result of an underlying i sequence in whichtwovowels are in

comp etition

The diminutive sux in the Rotterdam dialect of Dutchhasinteresting

distributional prop erties some of which it shares with its counterparts in

other Dutch dialects For instance Standard Dutch has the alternating

forms pje kje tje and etje

raampje koningkje latje dingetje

windowdim kingdim drawerdim thing dim

little window little king little drawer little thing

This alternation is sub ject to phonological conditions in Standard Dutch

the form of the sux is dep endent on the phonological shap e of the stem

With a few dierences to b e discussed b elow the same paradigm is found

in the Rotterdam dialect In particular the consonantal alternations in this

dialect are almost the same as in Standard Dutch

I will not go into the details of the alternation b etween etje tjas

attested in dingetje and the other forms since it already received a consid

erable amount of attention in the literature Kruisinga Cohen

Ewen Gussenhoven Trommelen Van Zonneveld Van

der Hulst The form etje is found after stressed syllables ending

Intro duction

in a lax vowel and a sonorant consonant It is often assumed this is a

morphologically conditioned instance of schwa insertion

It is also commonly assumed that tje is the underlying representation of

the diminutive and that the other allomorphs are derived from it by phono

logical rule in particular by place of the initial consonantto

the previous consonant I will argue b elow that the underlying representa

tion of the rst segment of the diminutiveisslightly dierent from that of a

coronal stop but for the moment let us assume that the sux underlyingly

is tje indeed

Of central interest for this chapter is that the forms raampje and kon

ingkje are normally realized as rampi and konki in the Rotterdam

dialect so with a high frontvowel instead of j Rogier De Wilde

van Buul Oudenaarden whereas voetje and dingetje are always

realized as vutjanddtj In other words the form jshows up

after a coronal stop and the form i shows up in other contexts

In some cases the t is optionally deleted In tdeletion environments

the word has two p ossible phonetic forms one with a t and the diminutive

realized as j and one without t and the diminutive realized as i the

former pronunciation sounds more aected

kasttje kstj ksi

The same alternation can b e found in other contexts We nd it for instance

at least diachronically in monomorphemic words like garage xarazi Fr

garaz horloge hrlozi Fr rloz

A note of warning is in order here For the sake of simplicity I abstract

away from many details of the so ciolinguistic reality In particular I allow

myself the abstraction of assuming the existence of an indep endent Rotter

dam grammar In the real world probably all sp eakers of the Rotterdam

dialect know b oth variants konkj and konki and they know the

former is more in accordance with the standard variantofDutch

In formal sp eech of Rotterdam sp eakers the variantwithj is found

and even preferred I consider this to b e a matter of dierentstyle regis

ters and I will supp ose a dierentstyle register means a partially dierent

grammar as I have also argued in c hapter The more formal style register

will b e discussed in section

Furthermore the ij alternation although it is seen by many native

sp eakers as the most prominentcharacteristic of their dialect Oudenaar

I do not discuss the deletion pro cess in this chapter See De Vries et al

Trommelen and Van Hout for grammatical and so ciolinguistic discussion

of this pro cess which is quite widespread across Dutch dialects



The representations as given here do not corresp ond to the actual phonetic reality

They will b e improved b elow

Intro duction

den is certainly not unique to Rotterdam Dutch For instance I

have found some reexes of it in Tilburg Dutch monomorphemic words

as well menezie manege mnezi Standard Dutch Frenchmnez

lozzie watch lzi Standard Dutch French hrlz medol lie medal

j

mdli Standard DutchFrenchmedl e etc Historical alternations

similar to these actually seem to b e quite widespread over the Dutchdi

alect area moreover there are instances of jchanging to i as well as i

changing to jWeijnen The alternation is furthermore quite wide

spread in the diminutive forms in the Dutch dialects sp oken in Holland

It is also found in some contexts in Frisian as has b een noted by Cohen

et al my translation

One can also nd the i of middei afterno on moandei mon

day as j so mdj mndj one could consider this

j to b e a form of refraction of i We nd other examples

of less accented is that surface as j in kofje coee plysje

p olice man naesje nation Because wehave made to o lit

tle investigations into this sub ject matter we cannot saymore

ab out it

According to Cohen et al in Frisian the alternation works in the opp osite

direction I just suggested for Rotterdam Dutch i changes to j in some

contexts The conditioning environment at rst sight is also dierentfor

Frisian and Rotterdam Dutch b ecause stress do es not seem relevantinthe

grammar of the latter See Bo oij for a discussion of Frisian breaking

within the framework of NonLinear Phonology Nevertheless wehaveto

takeinto account that underlying schwa is inherently unstressed so jis

always unstressed also in Rotterdam Dutch

Yet in one context the alternation seems to b e really typical of Rotter

dam sp eech The second p erson singular clitic je is normally realized as

i when it cliticiz es to another word except when this other word ends in

a coronal stop or a vowel

a hbj h bi haveyou

b mj mi mayyou

c hat j hatj hate you

d lst j lstj lsi likeyou



With resp ect to the clitics one nds remnants of the pro cess in other Holland Dutch

dialects Amsterdam Leiden The Hague among others but often only nonpro ductively

in frequent combinations suchashebbie haveyou assie if you etc



I will argue b elow that the stemnal obstruenteventually surfaces as not as t

but I abstract away from that here for simplicity

Intro duction

e zij zij zii ziji said you

f j at jat iat you go

It do es not matter whether je is used as a p ossessive pronoun a reexive

pronoun or any other of its pronominal roles De WildeVan Buul

The syntactic conditions under which cliticizati on can take place are quite

intricate but they are not at issue here It seems that the preceding word

has to ccommand the clitic in order for the pro cess to take place Henk

van Riemsdijk pc For example the form ie can b e used in phrases like

those in a and b but not the one in c I underline the relevant

clitic

a Do et een sjaal ommie nek

do a scarf aroundyour neck

put a scarf around your neck

b Ommie oma komp mo ete we stil zijn

b ecauseyour grandmother comes must we silent be

Wemust b e silent b ecause your grandmother will come

c Azzie alles wat je heb je ie oma mo e

if you everything that you have grandmother must

your

geve be je snel arm

give are you so on poor

If you havetogiveeverything you havetoyour grandmother

you will so on b e p o or

In a the clitic is either in the sp ecier of the DPNP complement je nek

of the prep osition om or it is the D dep ending on the syntactic analysis of

this construction which need not concern us here In either case the clitic

is separated from the prep osition by at least one syntactic categoryThe

syntactic b oundary is even stronger in b In this case om probably is

in C and ie is emb edded in the complex sp ecier of the IP complement

of this C In b oth a and b the preceding word is a head and je

is in the sp ecier of the sp ecier of a complement of that head In c

the word preceding je is emb edded in the direct ob ject of a verb whereas



The term ccommand is used here as a descriptive term It remains to b e worked out

whether this alternation has to b e describ ed in terms of a socalled direct syntax approach

Kaisse or rather in terms of a separate level of phonological representation Selkirk

Nesp or and Vogel Inkelas and Zec



Some of my informants rep orted a dierence in judgementbetween a and b

the latter is considered to b e somewhat worse than the former In general the eect

seems however to b e so exceptionally light that I do not knowhow it can b e taken into

account

Intro duction

je is the sp ecier of the indirect ob ject of the same verb No phonological

cliticiz ation is p ossible

In any case I assume that the fact that the phonological alternation

involved o ccurs across syntactic b oundaries is an indication for its b eing

p ostlexical

The conditioning then is mainly phonological In e it is shown that

the clitic also do es not showupinitsivariantafteravowel In this

environmentitisalways realized as j The same is true for phraseinitial

p osition as is demonstrated in f

Wecannow ask the following questions with regard to the diminutive

sux

Why do es i alternate with j and vice versa Given the theories of

Avowels versus Bvowels and of schwa presented earlier we should b e

able to describ e the b ehaviour of these segments in a satisfying way

It will also b e shown that alternation b etween i and j is a problem

if we assume that i is long

Why do es the rst segment of the diminutive sux assimilate to the

last segment of the stem That this typ e of assimilation is typical for

the diminutive sux only can b e seen if welookatthebehaviour of

asuxwithavery similar underlying form te t or the circumx

gete t The t in these axes do es not assimilate at all

geb o omte lengte zwakte geb o efte

getreete longte weakte gescoundrelte

timber length weakness rabble

Why do the coronal stops b ehave dierently with resp ect to the ji

alternation from all other segments b oth in the diminutive and in the

second p erson singular clitic

The study of the ij alternation brings together many of the topics

addressed in the previous chapters Furthermore it also intro duces a few

new topics some of which are directly relevant to the central concerns of

this dissertation The most imp ortantoftheseis glidingWhathappens

when vowels with all their vo calic features which make them so suitable

for syllable headedness app ear in a dep endent p osition such as the onset

or the co da

Finally the interesting interaction b etween consonantal and vo calic fea

tures that surface in this dialect will also b e the ob ject of inquiry

The second p erson clitic

In this chapter I argue that the answer to the last two questions in the

case of the diminutive is basically the same and is related to the under

sp ecication of the rst segment of the diminutive sux This segment

obligatorily assimilates in its place features either to a segment on its left

or to a segment on its right

FinallyIalsowant to discuss the dierence b etween Standard Dutch

and Rotterdam Dutch From my analysis it will follow that b oth variants

have the same underlying forms for the second p erson singular clitic and

the diminutive sux Yet the two dialects b ehave dierently on the surface

so that wehave to account for this dierence in the phonological grammar

In section I rst lo ok at the facts of the second p erson clitic b efore

I analyze the diminutive in section The rst will b e analysed as i

and the second as bi underlyinglywithtwovowels comp eting for a

place in the nucleus and the coronal vowel i interacting with coronal

consonants In section I briey compare the Rotterdam facts to a

dierent glidevowel alternation Sieverss Law and I will show that this

comparison provides us with an extra argument against underlying vowel

length in Dutch Some more detailed problems will b e discussed in section

which concludes this chapter

The second p erson clitic

As far as I know the ij alternation in the Rotterdam Dutch second

p erson clitic henceforth S has previously b een analysed in a generative

frame only byVan den Berg and by Berendsen Both authors

takej to b e the underlying form of this clitic and derive i from it

by rule I will return to these prop osals b elow In this chapter I follow

these scholars in their basic prop osal and assume that b oth forms have the

underlying form i They dier only in their resp ective syllabication

a j N b i N

N N

i i

In a the glide j is represented as the high vowel i o ccurring in a

consonantal p osition This do es not seem a very controversial assumption

I will turn to the exact feature geometry of i b elow The representation

of Dutchschwaasanemptyvo calic ro ot given here has b een argued for in

chapters and The question is whether this schwa has to b e identied

as an underlying schwa in whichcaseitwould b e unparsed in the form i

The second p erson clitic

or as an ep enthetic schwa In the latter case b would consist of a high

tense vowel only I will argue b elow section that the former option

should b e chosen b is a high vowel followed by an empty ro ot Until

that p oint I will simply assume this representation to b e correct

I claim that i actually is the underlying representation of S in the

Rotterdam dialect Its precise phonetic representation is dep endent on its

syllable structure

Wherever this is p ossible the rst segmentofi is analysed as a nu

cleus i is a full vowel and all full vowels are preferably syllabied into

anucleus Wethus get the following structure for hoef je must you and

waar je where you

huvi N N wari N N

   

N N N N

h u v i w a r i

Schwa is also a vowel of course and therefore also wants to b e in a nu

cleus for instance b ecause of Projectcons N On the other hand in

chapters through I have argued extensively that schwa is not as unprob

lematically a syllable nucleus as full vowels In particular do es not have

avo calic no de Therefore it do es not seem improbable that when i and

comp ete for a p osition in the nucleus the former wins in the unmarked

case This will b e technically worked out b elow

Whenever an onsetless syllable would o ccur by the realisation as i

howeverat the b eginning of a phrase or after a vowelthe form jis

chosen instead Compare the forms in to the ones in b elow

huvi N N wari N N

   

N N N N

h u v i w a r i

The grammar cho oses whatever syllabication is optimal in a certain con

text Normally this optimal segment is i b ecause this syllabication gives

us a syllable with a maximally sonorous nucleus we will see b elowthat

this can b e formalized in terms of Project N V but after a vowel or

wordinitially it is j in order to avoid onsetless syllables that would yield

violations of the Onset constraintIhave used already at several p oints in

this thesis

We seem to have a fairly straightforward case of constraintinteracti on at

hand First and foremost Onset decides but if wehave several candidates

The second p erson clitic

which tie on Onsetwecho ose the candidate with the optimal nucleus the

one with a full vowel rather than schwa In the remainder of this section I

will discuss the dierent phonological contexts one by one and explain the

b ehaviour of i in those contexts

Hiatus We exp ect underlying i to surface as j whenever it

o ccurs phrase or wordinitially or after a vowel I will refer to these three

contexts as a hiatus context Let us rst examine lexical forms to see

whether this prediction is b orne out in that stratum

The wordinitial p osition is usually considered to b e an exception to

the Onset constraint Languages whichdonotallow for wordinternal

hiatus still allow for onsetless syllables in wordinitial p osition If Dutch

were to allow the same exception wewould actually predict i rather than

j Onset would b e irrelevant and therefore the constraint preferring full

vowels would decide

As wehave seen in the discussion of schwainchapter the Onset

requirement plays an imp ortant role in lexical words also in the absolutely

initial p osition I argued in chapter that words like d m are excluded

from the Dutch lexicon b ecause the rst syllable needs an onset this onset

can only b e ep enthetic b and b sequences are not allowed The work

ing of Onset in wordinitial p osition plays a crucial role in this line of

reasoning

But even if it were p ossible to formalize an exception mechanism to

Onset in the appropriate way the problem remains that wendboth

words starting with j and words starting with i The former class is

much smaller but it contains at least one everyday Dutchword p erfectly

acceptable to all nativespeakers Apart from some forms morphologically

related to and probably derived from the second p erson singular clitic

jezelf yourself jewelste intensier litt yourb est we only nd the list

in a as opp osed to the much longer incomplete list of words starting

in i in b



I only considered words without stress on the rst syllable Otherwise the list in

b could havebeenmademuch longer but stressed schwasyllables do not exist for

indep endent reasons amply discussed in chapters through ab ove Furthermore for

the name Jeruzalem an alternative pronunciation jeryzalm is also p ossible for some

sp eakers so that we might conclude that in this case wehave rschwa rather than uschwa

The second p erson clitic

a jenever gin b idee idea

Jeroen b oys name Irene girls name

Jeruzalem id Iran id

icoon icon

idyl le idyl

il lusie illusion

ivoor ivory

etc

We do not nd any kind of alternation among these forms Neither the

pronunciation inever of jenever nor the pronunciation jde of idee are

ever found in Rotterdam Dutchorany other dialect that I am aware of

This is an imp ortant argument for an underlying distinction b etween i

on the one hand and i on the other I assume the forms in the second

column in start simply with i without a following schwa The forms

in the rst column start in contrast with i

Wehave already seen in chapter that the following two constraints

hold for Dutch

a Onset ProjectF N A syllable should have an onset if

the head of an N no de dominates any feature that no de should

branch

b ProjectN V if N branches N dominates a vo calic no de

Onset wants the sequence to b e parsed as j b ecause that sequence has

an onset while the output i do es not On the other hand the parse

j violates the constraint Project N V b ecause it contains a branching

N syllable no de with a schwa head The parse i do es not violate Project

V b ecause it has no branching syllabic no de at all and furthermore its head

dominates a vo calic no de with features high and coronal

The only case I know which could p otentially b e attributed to this phenomenon

is the variation b etween Izabel Old Testament and Jezabel New Testament in the

socalled Statenvertaling the th century State translation of the Bible whichforms

the foundation for Mo dern Standard Dutch Yet the latter word is normally pronounced

as jezabl or jezabl so with stress on the rst syllable and the vowel realized as e

rather than Yet this may b e a reex of the fact that Tib erian Hebrew had more or

less the same alternation as wehaveinDutch BatEl

The reason why the latter forms are so uncommon probably has to do with an

observation made in chapter fo otnote p if schwa is in the rst syllable it

is always preceded by one of the consonants in f t b v d g These segments do not

form a natural class under any feature analysis of Dutch The reason for their patterning

together here seems to b e a historic one b ecause there are or have b een prexes of the

form t vr and There has never b een a Dutchprexj

The second p erson clitic

Wehave also seen in section p that the Onset constraint

N V This assumption explained whyschwa licensed outranks Project

onsets but not complex onsets Here we can use it to derive the dierence

between i and j

Candidates Onset ProjectN V

p

j

i

This constraint ranking causes the j parse to b e chosen b oth phrase

initially and in intervo calic context After a consonant Onset b ecomes

irrelevant b ecause the consonant can ll the required onset p osition of i

I conclude that the sequence iwillwordinitially surface as j Log

ically sp eaking this do es not tell us anything ab out S at all It is crucial for

the analysis that clitics qua phrasal axes do not turn up in the phonology

until the p ostlexical level This explains for instance why a lot of clitics can

start with schwa even if they are phraseinitial The moment they enter

the grammar the Onset and Contour constraints have lost much of their

force The following constraint ranking holds true

ParseVOnset Contour

But if this is the case it mightwell b e that p ostlexicall y the ordering of

the two constraints just mentioned Onset and Project N V is re

versed Aquick lo ok at the data reveals however that this is not the

case Whenever S o ccurs phraseinitially or after a vowel it surfaces

as j This means that Onset in Dutch p ostlexical phonology still has

to dominate ProjectN V even if the former constraint is no longer in

violable as is the case in the lexicon I assume therefore that the ranking

OnsetProjectN V holds b oth lexically and p ostlexically in Rotter

dam Dutch

Hiatus after high vowels One typ e of hiatus p osition is of

sp ecial interest This is the case where i follows a high vowel in the

stem In that case the question is whether it is the stemnal high vowel



Tobevery precise the exact lo cation I mean is the rst syllable slot in an Intonation

Phrase or Utterance in the sense of Nesp or and Vogel and Selkirk in all

other p ositions S b ehaves as a phonological enclitic

Wehave seen indep endent evidence that this ranking holds in chapter where it

help ed us explain why slapslap with an underlying form slap it is b etter to

have an empty headed syllable than to have a syllable without an onset

The second p erson clitic

or the suxal i that undergo es gliding Wehave to distinguish b etween

three typ es of conguration a ij b Vj where V is y or

u and c V V j so after a diphthong of which the second half

i j

is always high in Dutch All these environments are of interest b ecause

high vowels b efore a hiatus normally develop a glide to satisfy the Onset

requirement in all of these cases

The facts of ii are simplest In these cases none of my informants

OK

could pro duce the i alternantofSzie je seeS zii ziji zij

The reason for this probably is a lter excluding ji sequences combined

with the relative high ranking of Onset Indep endent evidence for such

a lter comes from the fact that there are also no Dutchwords with ji

sequences derived from underlying ji and that glide insertion is also

j OK j

blo cked b etween two underlying is Shiiet Shiite s ijit s iit

In fact wehave to rule out two logically p ossible congurations to ban

ji the i abbreviates a feature tree in the following representations

a N b N

N N

i i i

These are the two p ossible structures for ji sequences There can either

be two indep endent segments or there can b e one and the same segment

linked b oth to onset and nucleus Since ji sequences do not o ccur on the

surface in Dutch at all wehave to rule out b oth structures

I assume the relevant constraintisContour

Contour If the head of a syl lable is its onset may not be

We used this constraintinchapter to rule out C C and hsequences

i i

and onsetless schwaheaded syllables Here wetakeaninterpretation where

linked to i if the head of a syllable is linked to i its onset might

not b e linkedtoi This rules out b ot a and b as required In

order to get the result that zijizijwehave to assume the following

ranking

ContourProject N V



Although there are words with j as wehave seen in chapter



Iwant to thank MiekeTrommelen for this observation

The second p erson clitic

We could sp eculate on whether should not b e formulatedsoastoalso

exclude some other nono ccurring sequences such as syllableiniti al dl

tl bw or pw

ant manner klant client tlant a tr

b praat talk kwaad angry pwaat

In these cases as well mere reference to the OCP is not sucient In

addition we need a constraint ruling out doubly linked features in the onset

What these examples show is that something like the constraint Contour

is also crucially needed to rule out sequences of homorganic onsets The

prop er formulation could b e something like if an onset segment is the

fol lowing segment may not be and that at this level the following ranking

holds

ContourParseConsonant

It is not clear why onset segments have this prop erty that they cannot b e

followed by homorganic segments but as a working hyp othesis Contour

will turn out to do its work reasonably well

Inow turn to hiatus after the other high vowels y and u The facts

are not very clear and judgments seem to dier among sp eakers Most of my

informants allow insertion of a glide w Wim Zonneveld pc has drawn

myattention to the fact however that other sp eakers prefer the jform

in these cases Ihave not b een able to nd evidence that this dierence

corresp onds to any geographic or so ciological b order line b etween the two

classes of sp eakers



I crucially assume here that the value coronal is present on coronal consonants and

that it is not a default value undersp ecied at any relevantlevel of representationI

also assume r is not sp ecied as coronal although I will not take up the issue of the

exact representation of this segment



There is one technical problem with this sp ecic formulation however I represented

catalexis in chapter with an empty consonanthorb in a degenerate syllable If

this is correct we can have degenerate syllables without a head bur not schwaheaded

syllables without an onset This would argue in favour of a constraint if the nucleus of

a syl lable is the onset may not be The facts in would havetobeaccounted

for in a dierentway



Wehave already seen indep endent evidence for this ranking b e it indirectly

N V in example ab ove For wehave seen that Contour dominates Project

N V dominates ParseConsonant in the lexicon b e and weknowthatProject

cause this help ed us explain why sup erheavy syllables are not allowed wordinternally

matkama t  ka underlying representation matka a form with a word

internal sup erheavy syllable is worse than a candidate in which the consonant is left

unparsed



My written sources do not discuss this context They do not give relevant examples

of high vowel i sequences either

The second p erson clitic

doeje doS nu je nowS

dialect duj nyj

dialect duwi nywi

The facts of dialect are what wehave come to exp ect wi is a reasonably

normal syllable in Dutchwie who wiel wheel etc and there is no

reason why it should not o ccur The facts of Zonnevelds dialect are

therefore somewhat problematic for myaccount It mightbethatinthis

dialect homorganic glide insertion is blo cked until a later phonetic level

so that we only havea choice b etween nyi and nyj in whichcasewe

pick of course the latter A somewhat dierent suggestion will however b e

made b elow section

A similar distinction arises in the case of diphthongs After i we

always nd j After u the judgments are variable According to

De WildeVan Buul the choicebetween je and ie hereseems to

bedependent on the level of civilisation of the speakerLower so cial class

p eople say houwie p eople with a higher class background say houje De

WildeVan Buul suggests that the crucial factor is that in lower class sp eech

the u is pronounced more like a consonant

This suggestion might indeed lead us to the right solution In section

I will show that there may b e evidence for some dierences b etween

the feature structure of the glide w and of the high vowels u and y

The relation b etween w and these vowelsisdierent from the relation

between j and i which seem to b e exactly the same in all resp ects except

for syllable p osition It might therefore b e that glide formation of w is

in some way more costly than the glide formation of j I will makethis

idea of costliness more precise in section b elow This costliness would

blo ck glide formation in the higher so cial class variant of Rotterdam Dutch

For now I will assume that there is a constraint forbidding glide formation

of w in the relevantcontexts which is ranked higher than the constraint

demanding an optimal syllable nucleus since huwi is less wellformed

than huj

In the lower so cial class the representation of diphthongs may b e dif

ferent from that of Standard Dutch altogether ei and au sound more like

It is very dicult to construct reasonably natural contexts where i follows y

b ecause the latter diphthong is not very frequent I found only twoverbal stems ending in

it krui wheel and verbrui incur someb o dys displeasure These forms are disregarded

in my written sources My informants assert in these cases they would prefer j even

if they dier on the realisation of S after au I will ignore the dipthong y in the

following discussion b ecause it is hard to nd go o d testing evidence

Furthermore there seems to b e a dierence b etween lexical items and function words

or otherwise a frequency eect zouwie should you nouwie nowyou or houwie hold

you seem much more acceptable than brouwie brewyou Again I have nothing to

say ab out these eects

The second p erson clitic

a j and aw resp ectivel y Therefore the nal segmentofau already has

to be a glide and there is no extra cost to putting it into an onset In other

words the representation of hou in this dialect is as follows

N N

N N

h a w

The formulation of the constraint against glide formation in section

will b e such that it do es not aect this representation

After coronal stops The choice for j in hiatus is intuitively

quite plausible The quest for onsets has many diverse eects acrosss lan

guages as wehave seen This variation might b e one of them

More problematic is the question whywe do not nd hdi for had je

hadS or dti for dat je thatS In lexical words the syllables di

and ti have no exceptional status at all

dynamisch dynamic dinamis direct id dirkt

divers various divrs

tiran tyrant tirn titaan Titan titan tyfoon typho on tifon

Even if wewould p osit a separate stratum of clitics di and ti cannot b e

disallowed b ecause they are the regular shap es of the third p erson singular

masculine clitic cf section

I ignore Dutch y for reasons mentioned in fo otnote



I do not fully discuss the facts of coronal nonstops in this chapter but the data

seem to b e very interesting The fricatives allowtwo forms kastje little case can b e

pronounced kse with a palatalized sound or ksi For l and n the facts are even

more complex We nd a wide range of idiolectal and lexical variation According to

my informants the lexical verb kennie can S sounds b etter than the auxiliary bennie

are you for instance but b oth forms have a preferred form without the nasal cosonant

with a nasalized vowel and j for the clitic kj bj The same is true for l

verveelie vrveli sounds somewhat b etter than wil lie wli but the informants would

prefer to sayvrvej and wj

De WildeVan Buul notes there also is some dialectal variation with resp ect

to these facts For instance in Vo orschoten a town approximately kilometers to the

north of Rotterdam of which the dialect has b een studied byOverdiep wedond

j after nwhile the preceding v owel still nasalizesand frequently also after the

other coronal consonants and after r There is no reason to assume r is coronal in

Rotterdam Its pronunciation is uvular

I cannot oer an explanation for the facts in these other dialects if only b ecause more

precise details of their system are unfortunatelylacking

The second p erson clitic

On the other hand the sequence tj is not found in any lexical form

outside the class of diminutives As a matter of fact the sequence Cj

is nearly nonexistent with some regulated classes of exceptions to b e dis

cussed in section

I will rule out these congurations by the following ad hoc constraintat

this p oint but I will give a more principled explanation based on ProjectN

V below p

C j

N N

Given all these considerations it seems hard to explain then why tjis

preferred over ti in the case of clitics

Now as a matter of fact the representation tj in the case of stemS

or diminutive forms do es not provide an adequate reection of the phonetic

facts It is more correct to saythatthetwo coronal consonants merge

j

into one palatalized consonantt Van den Berg Tobeeven more

precise my impression is that with manyspeakers the primary articulation

j

sounds almost like a palatal This segment has to b e banned from

lexical words in which it do es not o ccur

A similar eect has b een addressed in Hermans b discussing

palatalization in Limburg Dutch which is triggered by a totally dier

entenvironment but this need not concern us here Hermans adopts the

mo del of Clements in whichvo calic and consonantal place features

app ear on dierent tiers Hermans assumes the representation of a palatal

ized consonantisschematically as follows

cons

Cplace

coronal Vplace

i

coronal

i

The two place features are crucially coindexed Hermans b observes

that the place of constriction for coronal vowels is ususally more back than

that of coronal alveolar consonants If two features are coindexed they

have to get the same interpretation phonetically Therefore the coronal

feature under Cplace is interpreted as palatal rather than alveolar in this

case

Later work Hume Clements and Hume has worked out a

slightly dierent view on the interaction b etween consonantal and vo calic

features These authors argue that consonantal and vo calic place features

The second p erson clitic

app ear on the same tier Such a theory would give us the representation of

palatalized coronals in b In my opinion this view allows us to express

Hermans observation even more elegantly without the use of coindexa

tion on features In this case it is literally the same feature that acts as

a consonantal and a vo calic place feature It can only get one phonetic

interpretation Since alveolar vowels do not exist or at least since Dutch

do es not allow them this interpretation can only b e a palatal one

Van den Berg working in an SPElike framework claimed that

j written by him as ju b ecause he assumed schwawas the unstressed

variantofu was the underlying form and i was the result of

a reduction rule conating the feature matrices of two segments into one

new matrix

This reduction was blo cked after a coronal stop b ecause there was a

palatalisation rule extrinsically ordered b efore jreduction whichcon

j

ated the t and j into a palatalised t Because j did not survive

the palatalisation pro cess as an indep endent element palatalisation bled

jreduction Berendsen oers basically the same analysis but

stated in somewhat more mo dern terms I prop ose to also adopt this anal

ysis once again mo dernizing the technical apparatus and using insights of

mo dern syllable structure and autosegmental theory

I therefore assume that the two segments t and i are merged In the

following representations I mark features that are directly dep endenton

the Cplace no de with a subscript C and those that are directly dep endent

on the Vplace no de with a subscript V

a Input

 

cons son cons

Cplace Cplace

coronal vo calic

C

Vplace

coronal

V



This is necessary b ecause the coronal feature in the output has to countina

sense as a valid parse of b oth the consonantalandthevo calic coronal feature Wecan

always reconstruct the indices from the representation however a place feature that is

directly dominated by a Cplace no de has an index C and a place feature that is directly

dominated by a Vplace no de an index V

Presumablywealsohavetomention in the sp ecication of the input the fact that the

coronal vowel should b e high b ecause similar palatalisation is not found with e

this condition is discussed in section On the conditions on the consonantsee

the discussion on p b elow

The second p erson clitic

b Output

 

cons son cons

Cplace

vocalic

Vplace

C

coronal

V

A few prop erties of this pro cess have to b e discussed here In the rst place

why do es it only aect coronal stops and not for instance p and k The

most straightforward thing to do seems to b e to relate this to the fact that

the j is also coronal We therefore havetwo coronal segments adjacent

to one another This is an OCP violation whichmight b e seen to trigger

the pro cess at hand In other words wehave evidence for the following

ranking of constraints

OCP EmptyrootParseVocalicroot

In the case of p and k there is no external reason for creating the

complex segment in b In the framework adopted here if there is no

reason for doing something we are not allowed to do it b ecause doing

something will always cause violation of the constraints that demand input

and output to b e maximally similar

In the second place in what waycanwe analyse the output of this

pro cess as b etter as more optimal than its input A rst reason seems to

b e the OCPYet there are other ways to avoid OCP violations that do not

leave the vo calic ro ot unparsed One instance of an alternative structure is

given b elow

 

cons son cons

Cplace

Cplace

vocalic

Vplace

coronal



See also section on why only coronals can b e palatalized in underived forms

The second p erson clitic

Variations on this structure are also logically p ossible For instance an

other geometric p ossibility is that the Vplace no de links to the consonant

j

creating a doubly linked structure t i

This form leaves the original vowel slot parsed while it solves the prob

lem with the OCP In this resp ect then it seems to do even b etter than

b There is however one resp ect in which it do es considerably worse

syllable structure This form has the structure of a consonantandahigh

vowelglide which share material

Given our ndings presented ab ove there is no legitimate way to parse

these ob jects into syllable structure We cannot parse the two segments

as a complex onset b ecause as wehave seen j cannot o ccur in a complex

j

onset We also cannot parse them as a onsetrhyme sequence t i either

b ecause as wehave seen onset segments may not share place features with

afollowing no de this was the constraint I called Contour

So the only way to satisfy b oth the OCP and conditions on syllable

structure is by creating b This means that at the level we are discussing

nowwhich I assume to b e p ostlexical since we are dealing with clitics

these constraints outrank whatever constraints are violated by the phonetic

j

output t I will consider one such constraint SecArt from Rosenthall

In I give an informal denition of this constraint

SecArt No segmentmay b e sp ecied b oth for Cplace and V

place features

Postlexical grammar

Contour C j

N N

SecArt

As a matter of fact SecArt in my view should b e derived from two

constraints which can b e written semiformally as follows see app endix A

for some more discussion

CF A cons ro ot do es not dominate a vo calic feature

V

VF Acons ro ot do es not dominate a consonantal feature

C

Since every ro ot is sp ecied as either cons or as cons these twocon

straints taken together have the same eect as SecArtFurthermore

the separation into two constraints can help us derive the eect that sec

ondary articulation is much more constrained on vo calic ro ots than it is on

consonantal ro ots We might assume that VF is ranked very high

C

 j

I found one verb starting with t i tjielpen cheep I consider this form to b e an

j

onomatop o eia Furthermorethis verb is often rendered as tjilpen t l



For secondary articulation on vowels in Swedish cf Clements

The second p erson clitic

The constraintCF plays the role of SecArt in the case of palatali

V

sation We will also see that there is evidence for some constraints which

are remotely related to these ones

We can now draw the following tableau for haat je

Candidates Contour CF C j

V

N N

j

hat j

p

j

hat

hatj

hati

j

hat i

It might b e an imp ortant assumption that this ordering denes the p ost

j

lexical grammar As noted ab ove lexical forms with t are not found

at all There seems to b e hardly any evidence for underlying complex seg

ments We could therefore assume that the structure preserving constraint

CF is ranked topmost in the lexical grammar An imaginary input

V

tirn will then surface as tirn

Candidates CF Contour NCNj

V

j

t jrn

j

t rn

tjrn

p

ti rn

j

t i

This would explain the absolute nonexistence of forms like tjeran I

return to the lexical grammar in section b elow where we will see that

the situation is slightly more complex there

For now I prop ose to concentrate on the p ostlexical grammar I have

to discuss one other prop erty of the pro cess sketched in the fact

that it applies only to coronal stops There is no palatalisation of any

other coronal consonantinDowntown Rotterdam Wend kenie kenje

wil lie wilje etc

Wehave to lo ok somewhat more closely at the representation of the

coronal segmentinventory to nd the reason why t and d are sp ecial

among coronals Ihave listed the relevant parts of the structure for all

Rotterdam coronals b elow



I once again p oint out that I do not consider r a coronal in Rotterdam Dutch

although this assumption do es not really aect the argument

The second p erson clitic

s t

 

cons son cons son

Cplace

Cplace continuant

coronal

coronal

z n

 

cons son cons son

Cplace Laryngeal continuant Cplace nasal

coronal voice coronal

d l

 

cons son cons son

Cplace

Cplace Laryngeal

coronal

coronal voice

lateral

t is the simplest coronal consonant since it do es not b ear any feature apart

from place assuming cont do es not exist or is the unmarked feature

value cf Lombardi The intuition therefore is that t is the only

consonant that can supp ort some extra complexity in the form of secondary

articulation

One way of formalizing this idea is as follows All coronal consonants

except t and d have some consonantal feature a feature that normally

only go es with consonants

It is not unnatural to assume there are constraints banning combinations

of vowels with lateral or less stronglyofvowels with nasal Although

there clearly are languages with nasal vowels it is wellknown that nasality

is less marked for consonants than for vowels For instance languages with

nasal vowels always have at least one while the opp osite

is not necessarily true More in particular Rotterdam Dutch only has

nasal consonants no nasal vowels Furthermore many languages have nasal

vowels only in the context adjacent to a nasal consonant which seems to

indicate in those cases the nasality feature is in some way licensed by the

consonant cf Human and Krakow for an overview of the literature

on nasality

The second p erson clitic

We therefore need to sp ecify the constraints in a through c

 

Vplace

a

lateral

 

Vplace

b

nasal

 

Vplace

c

cont

 

Vplace

d

voice

d is the only consonant apart from t that allows for a secondary articu

lation in Rotterdam The constraint d whichwould b e needed to rule

j

out d is not very deeply grounded in the phonetics Although vowels

are not usually contrastively sp ecied for voice d do es not seem a

constraintwewould allow under the Grounded Phonology Hyp othesis cf

section

j

On the other hand de consonantin hadje hadS ha seems to

be voiceless It cannot b e that Final Devoicing has applied rst b ecause

that is not the case in for instance hebbie haveS or any other form

Voiced palatalized consonants therefore really seem to b e excluded

If intro duction of d were the only way of establishing this wewould

have to abandon the Grounded Phonology Hyp othesis b ecause a more

reasonable grounded feature co o ccurrence constraint seems to b e one of

the following form

Vplace voice

The problem raised here has a much wider scop e than palatalisation It

is w ellknown that sonorants in general have to b e undersp ecie d for the

feature voice For instance the phonetic voicing of sonorants is invisible

for the working of Lymans Law in Japanese Ito and Mester

Ito Mester and Padgett have suggested that these and similar

facts should not b e analysed in terms of nongrounded constraints suchas



In the formulation of these constraints I havechosen to use the Vplace no de as the

no de that may not co o ccur with the consonantal features Mayb e I could just as well have

chosen the vo calic no de or each of the vo calic place features coronal labial velar

v v v

etc The reason that I have not chosen the latter option is that it would have tripled

the numb er of constraints The reason that I have not chosen the vo calic no de is that

height features seem to b e even stricter in their preference for vowels than vo calic place

features Dierences b etween high and low secondary articulations are rarely found if

at all cf Van de Weijer

The second p erson clitic

d but rather in terms of complemented by a theory of licensing

the core of which is given b elow

Licensing Cancellation If FG then F G

If the sp ecication F implies the sp ecication G then it is not the

case that F licenses G

Licensevoice voice must b e licensed

If the Licensing condition dominates the condition that Vplace voice we

get the required result that segments with a secondary articulation cannot

b ear the feature voice

dj

Candidates License Vplace voice

p

j

t

j

d

The same is true for c a constraint against the combination of con

tinuant and vo calic quality do es not seem to b e grounded in the phonetic

realityThecontinuant contrast as far as I know do es not play a role in

the vowel phonology of any language Again this might b e attributed to

the interaction of an implicational constraint and a requirement on licensing

of the feature continuant

Vplace continuant

Licensecontinuant continuant must b e licensed

If we assume that the constraints against the combination of vo calic place

features with any consonantal nonplace feature plus the licensing con

straints on voice and continuant are undominated we predict only t is

susceptible to palatalisation t do es not b ear any feature except for coro

nal In order to make the right prediction ab out d wehave to assume that



Cf Steriade for a similar prop osal in whichhowever the term licensing in

a dierent almost the opp osite way in her theory obstruents cannot license voice

whereas sonorants can



As I have noted ab ove there are dialects which allow palatalization of other coronal

consonants than t My impression is that a dialect has most often palatalization

of s and z then of n and nally of l If this is correct it suggests that the

constraints in have some inherentweight ranking the higher on the list the heavier

the constraint and Licensecontinuant is relatively low The interesting thing is that

vowels are and that they can b e nasal in some languages but not lateral

This reects exactly the same hierarchy

The second p erson clitic

d has a slightly dierent status than the other feature co o ccurrence re

strictions in While the other features simply blo ck palatalisation in

the case of d it is voice itself that loses the battle While in the other

cases the constraints forcing parsing lateral nasal and continuant are

ranked higher than the feature co o ccurrence restrictions Parsevoice is

ranked lower This low ranking of Parsevoice seems intuitively justied

b ecause Dutch has several devoicing pro cesses such as Final Devoicing and

ProgressiveVoice Assimilation Lombardi Bo oij a while it do es

not have delateralisation denasalization or decontinuantisation

Third p erson singular clitic Inow turn to the third p erson

singular masculine nominative clitic S whichisvery similar to S pho

netically This clitic is realized as i ti or di dep ending on the phono

logical context but never as j tjor dj

realisations verb stem gloss

si sti zdi sj sz is he

lsi lsti lst likes he lstj

mxi mxti mdi m mayhe mdj

kni knti kndi knt can he kndj

I follow Berendsen among others in assuming that di is the un

derlying form of this clitic Rules of voice assimilation degemination and

tdeletion apply to it partly optionally yielding the variation in I

will not discuss these pro cesses here

Nowif die has the underlying form di it will always b e syllabied as

in



The phonetic similaritybetween S and the i variantofSprovided the explanation

for the sp ecial b ehaviour of S after coronal stops according to Overdiep and De

WildeVan Buur If S would b e i after coronal stops one could no longer hear

the dierence b etween S and S uitie whistleS uittie whistleSyti

This functional account did not explain however why the i form of S is not found in

environments where no confusion with S is p ossible either likein Roept Piet j i

moeder niet do esnt Pete call your mother where the underlined element can only b e

a p ossessive pronoun b ecause S can only b e the nominative and the sub ject p osition

is already lled by Piet Neither do es it explain why the same phenomenon o ccurs

in the diminutivewherewe equally nd a distinction b etween coronal and noncoronal

consonants but where most of the time no confusion is p ossible



Bo oij a argues that what is optional here is phonological encliticisationIncor

p oration or Chomskyadjunction of the clitic into the proso dic word would b e optional

in the case of die and dat that All phonological rules mentioned would apply obliga

torily within the domain of the proso dic word however

The diminutivesux

N

N

d i

If the underlying representation of S is di palatalisation will not take

place b ecause it would pro duce a complex segment in the co da where such

segments are not allowed It is therefore absolutely essential for this analysis

that the structure of S is di not di

The diminutivesux

Ifollow the tradition established by the authors mentioned in the rst

section of this chapter and supp ose the underlying form of this sux is tje

ie ti

If the structure of S were di this sux would dier from the diminu

tiveinavery minimal way

diminutive

  

cons son cons cons

Cplace Cplace

coronal vocalic

Vplace Ap erture

coronal high

S

  

cons son cons cons

Cplace

Cplace Laryngeal

vo calic

coronal voice

Vplace Ap erture

coronal high

The two elements would only dier in the presence versus absence of the

feature voice Yet S always app ears as di whereas the diminutiveshows

the same variation as S The Rotterdam forms are raampie rampi but

The diminutivesux

j

katje k kti The dierence in voicing can not explain the dierent

b ehaviour b ecause wehave just seen voice is the only consonantal feature

apart from coronal that do es not blo ck palatalisation

Level ordering cannot provide us with an explanation either The i

j alternations discussed until now are p ostlexical Wewould then exp ect

S to pattern like S to the exclusion of the diminutive but the facts are

exactly the opp osite of this

Actually it is somewhat surprising that the diminutivebehaves likea

clitic While it is not unreasonable to assume that syntactic clitics and

inectional suxes app ear only at this level the same is not immediately

j

obvious for the diminutive sux The fact that wend not present

in derived lexical forms in diminutives is an indication that these suxes

have some semiinectional status

However this may b e it seems that the only way to represent the phono

logical dierence b etween the diminutive and S is by incorp orating it into

the lexical representation one ends in a schwa the other do es not Assum

ing the diminutiveenters the grammar at the same time as the clitics viz

p ostlexically its b ehaviour is as exp ected

Ineedtosaymorehowever ab out the t in this diminutive b ecause

its b ehaviour is somewhat dierent from that of other ts in other envi

ronments This consonant assimilates in all its features to the preceding

consonant This is not the usual pattern for suxes starting with t I

givesomemorerelevant facts b elow

diminutive te S Past tense

raampi ruimt komti verzuimd

little window space comes he neglected

koningki lengt zingti verlengd

little king length sings he lengthened

j

ko e auwt zeidi raadd

little cow faintingt says he guessed

j

kat breedt haatti haatt

kitten breadth hates he hated

The t of the diminutive sux is the only segment that shows this p eculiar

b ehaviour Normal derivational suxes liket in the second column of

do not assimilate at all Real p ostlexical elements like S and the

past tense sux show dierenttyp es of voicing assimilation but no place

assimilation The diminutive has place assimilation but no overt voicing

assimilation



See for instance Sp encer for some discussion of the hybrid status of diminu

tives in the distinction b etween derivation and inection in many languages

The diminutivesux

Wehave to formally express the fact that the obstruent in the diminutive

sux is extremely sensitive to place assimilation One waytoachievethis

is byhaving the segment totally unsp ecied for place features Van der

Hulst The rst segment of the diminutivethus is not a coronal stop

but simply a voiceless stop without a place no de Since it is voiceless it

presumably also do es not have a Laryngeal no de It therefore is an empty

consonantal ro ot I have argued that the empty consonantal ro ot also is

the representation of a glottal stop Yet in the case of the diminutive

the glottal stop app ears in a somewhat p eculiar phonological context viz

b efore a p otential onset and in some cases also after a consonant The

glottal stop in Dutch only surfaces as a last resort to satisfy the Onset

requirement

I assume therefore that the diminutive sux is sp ecial in the sense that

it is the only morpheme in Dutch where a glottal stop is underlying This

explains the exceptional b ehaviour of the rst segment of this sux in the

Dutch lexicon

Supp ose furthermore that there is a lter which states that all con

sonants have to b e sp ecied for a place feature Since glottal stops are

very limited in their distribution this do es not seem a very controversial

assumption I will return to glottal stops in hiatus contexts b elow

In order to satisfy the lter against empty consonantal ro ots a weakness

constraint stating that segments in the weak p osition of a syllable need to

have some place sp ecication the obstruent in the diminutive assimilates

to the preceding consonant if there is one cf

Weak N C A segment in the weak p osition of a syllable needs

to have a consonantal place feature CC fcoronal dorsal

c c

labial g

c

cons cons

Cplace

This accounts for the raampie and koninkie cases which end with the fol

lowing representation the os indicate ro ot no des

o o o o o o o o o o o o o o

r a m p i k o n k i

labial dorsal



To b e maximally accurate this constraint should b e replaced bya setofweakness

N C WeakN C g constraints fWeak

The diminutivesux

N V will Since the obstruent can form an onset the constraint Project

select the optimal candidate

Candidates Onset ProjectN V

rampi

ramp j

C j Notice that ProjectN V can replace the ad hoc constraint

N N

against Cj clusters in all the relevant cases It correctly do es not blo ck

Cj clusters in words with a full vowel suchasfjord

If no consonant is adjacent to the unsp ecied segmentlikeinkoe tj

little cow or tj the segment has to get its place feature in a

dierentway

I prop ose that in this case the place feature is assimilated from the

i instead This typ e of voweltoconsonant spreading is blo cked bya

constraintwe already encountered

CF Acons ro ot dominates a consonantal place feature

C

If Weak N C CF the b est thing we can do in koetje is assimilate

C

the place feature of the vowel

Weak N C CF

C

Candidates WeakN C Monolog CF

C

kubi

p

j

kut

j

kup

Ihave assumed the emptyrootreceived its place feature from the following

i not from the preceding u The reason for this is probably related

to the Crispness of Edges constraint prop osed by Ito and Mester

The exact ordering of this constraint with resp ect to WeakN C cannot

b e established I call this constraint Monolog

Monolog The edges of a morphological domain should b e crisp

no feature should b e linked b oth to an edge segment of that domain

and to an element outside of the domain

Weak N C Monolog CF

V



Conceptually this constraintisvery closely linked to the Alignment constraints but

cf Ito and Mester for arguments that it is not

The diminutivesux

The edges of are not crisp b ecause the feature labial crosses one of

them the edges of on the next page are

o o o o o

k u p i

labial

The obstruentthus gets the place feature of the following vowel This

feature happ ens to b e coronal This is the reason why the rst segment

of the diminutive shows up as a coronal palatalized obstruent

 

cons son cons

Cplace

vo calic

Vplace

coronal

Now the result of this assimilation pro cess is of course the same as the result

the palatalisation pro cess discussed earlier koetje will get the following

structure assigned to it

o o o o

j

k u

Inow turn to a somewhat more complicated case kattje inwhich the

diminutive is preceded by a coronal consonant Here we exp ect the placeless



Again the variantwherewe create a palatalized consonant and leavethevocalic

ro ot unparsed is preferred over the variant where the vowel ro ot p ersists

i cons cons

Cplace

vocalic

Vplace

The reason for this is syllabication as discussed ab ove The sequence in i cannot b e

syllabied in any reasonable way b ecause of Contour

Sieverss Law

obstruent to assimilate to the place of the preceding consonant just likein

baktje

The empty ro ot assimilates to the preceding obstruent If nothing

further would happ en this would make the form undistinguishable from

raampie and koninkie with resp ect to the ij alternation wewould

exp ect the form kti

o o o o o o

k t t i

coronal coronal

Dutch has an indep endently motivated degemination pro cess simplifying

to so that will b e preferred over

o o o o o

k t i

coronal coronal

But here t and are still adjacent to one another other This is there

fore a violation of the OCP It can b e lifted by assimilating t and i in

the now familiar way As a result we end up with the following structure

o o o o

j

k

This structure violates neither WeakN C nor the constraint against

geminates nor the OCPnorany of the syllable structure constraints We

j

get the pronunciation k as a result

Sieverss Law

The alternation b etween vowel and glide which is the topic of this chapter

is similar in some ways to Sieverss Law a phonological pro cess that has

b een op erativeinmany IndoEurop ean languages likeVedic and Gothic

Sievers Dresher and Lahiri Lahiri and Van der Hulst

Riad Rice Calabrese Although a full discussion of the



A complicationmight arise in the case of words like baantje little job and paaltje

little eel In these cases n l t and i are all predicted to assimilate to one

another but this would mean that also the n and l should undergo palatalisation

Ihave the impression that this is true for n but not for l

Sieverss Law

complicated facts p ertaining to Sieverss Lawwould take us to o far aeld

it is interesting to compare the Rotterdam phenomena to real instances of

Sieverss Law I will concentrate on Gothic which is particularily interest

ing b ecause it p ossessed real phonological long vowels My main source

is Riad who discusses the following examples among others

a so okjis so okiis you seek

mikiljis mikiliis you glorify

sip o onjis sip o oniis you are a disciple

b nasjis nasjis you save

sto o jis sto o jis you judge

Riad argues following Vennemann among others that

syllable structure is the determining factor in all of these cases He as

sumes that there is an archiphoneme I which surfaces as either i or j

dep ending on the surrounding syllable structure

Thus the analysis of Sieverss law reduces to the characteriza

tion of syl labication in Gothic and Germanic Given archiphonemes

Sieverss law synchronical ly becomes a question of derivation

where I whenever it ends up as onset is realizedas j and

whenever it is not part of the onset is realized as a vowel

Riad

This is almost the same characterisation as I gave for Rotterdam Dutch

If we assume that the archiphoneme I in our present approach stands

for a nonsyllabied i we can adopt Riads prop osals without

much further diculty

In the examples in a the optimal syllable structure is the following

I translate Riads structures to the formalism adopted here I will return

to the representation of long vowels b elow

so okiis



Riad also has an underlying j dierent from the archiphoneme for glides

which do not have a predictable p osition in the syllable Here I p osit no such

archiphonemes for the Rotterdam vowelglide alternation Whether or not this could

also apply to Gothic is an op en question cf Calabrese



I represent Gothic long vowels as two ro ot no des that are b oth linked to the nucleus

no de so that sup erheavy syllables are represented as single syllables I havedonethis

to change as little as p ossible of Riads representations Riad uses a mora theory even

though strictly sp eaking these representations are somewhat inconsistent with the theory

Ihavesketched in the previous chapters I am convinced however that with a few minor

adaptations these structures could b e adapted to the representations used here

Sieverss Law

N N

N N

N

N

s o o k i i s

mikiliis

N N N

N

N N

N

N

N

m i k i l i i s

The consonant b efore ii can b est b e analysed as part of the onset The

alternativewould b e to put this consonantinto the co da of the preceding

syllable but in general consonants prefer a p osition in an onset to a p osition

in a co da In this case the nucleus is the b est p ossible p osition for the ii

cluster if we assume that the complex consonant clusters of the form Cj

are marked in Gothic

The situation is somewhat more complicated in the case of nasjisOn

rst sight this form p erhaps do es not dier to o much from sookiisWe could

assume that the rst s in nasjis is in an onset We then get the following

structure

nasiis

N N

N N

N

N

n a s i i s

Sieverss Law

Yet Riad argues that in these cases wehavetotake Prokoschs Law

into account Prokosch According to this Law stressed syllables

should always contain at least two moras ie two p ositions in the rhyme

in the Germanic languages cf the discussion of Norwegian in chapter

Main stress in Gothic is on the rst syllable so this rst syllable should

conform to Prokoschs Law This is no problem in the word sookiis b ecause

the long vowel already do es o ccupytwo p ositions The same story do es not

hold for the rst syllable of nasjishowever For this reason the s should

be drawn into the rst syllable and we end up with the following structure

nasjis

N N

N N

N N

n a s i i s

In the form mikiliis whichwe discussed ab ove the syllable ki can stay light

b ecause it is not stressed

Disregarding the eects of Prokoschs Law which is no longer op erative

in Dutch dialects the pro cesses in Gothic and the Rotterdam dialect show

up a remarkable resemblance There is however one subtle dierence that

wehave to explain in Gothic the long i alternates with ji in Rotterdam

Dutch the same vowel alternates with j

I think the key to the solution lies in the observation wemadeabove

Wehave just seen that a long o o in Gothic is able to let a syllable satisfy

Prokoschs Law This means that in Gothic a CV V syllable counts as

i i

heavy A Dutch i is not phonologically long but has a structure as a

Yet in Gothic long i has the really long tworo ot structure of b

a o

i

b o o

i

If weinterpret the two no des in b an onset and a rhyme the rhymal

segment will b e linked to i and also b e phonetically interpreted as such

The sequence as a whole will b e interpreted as ji on the surface The

Other issues

lter Contour against shared structure in tautosyllabic onset and rhyme

is apparently was less strictly observed in Gothic than it is in Dutch

Indirectly this could give us another argument against length in Dutch

If high tense vowels were really long it would not b e explained whyDutch

do es not b ehave like Gothic On the other hand the dierent ranking

of Contour can b e another explanation for the dierence it could b e

that long iq cannot break to ji b ecause that syllable is not p ossible for

indep endent reasons

Other issues

The main p oint of this chapter a statement of the conditions under which

i glides to j b efore a schwa in Rotterdam Dutchhasnow b een made

In this section I want to discuss some problems which arise if we treat the

Rotterdam Dutch ij alternation in the waysketched here Most of

these problems have already b een noted in the preceding sections but they

still await further treatment

In the rst place wehave the facts of Standard Dutch where wedonot

nd these alternations In the second place wehave to study the relation

between i and j in lexical forms where we nd semiminimal pairs

like Hanny name hni hnjversus Sp anje Spain spnj spni

Subsequentlywe also have to discuss the reason why the ultimate form of

S is i not i In the fourth place wehavewords like Italiewhere

schwa follows i on the surface Finally in section I discuss the clitic

we which has almost the same structure as je viz it consists of a glide

followed byschwa but which do es not alternate with u or y and inversely

the p olite second p erson pronoun u which never alternates with w

Clitics and the diminutive in Standard Dutch The fact that

pakkie little b ox has a more optimal syllable structure than pakje in Rot

terdam Dutch raises the question why the sp eaker of Standard Dutchdoes

not cho ose this same optimal form Another way of putting the same ques

tion is why do es a Rotterdam sp eaker who starts sp eaking in a formal

style register all of a sudden prefer the less optimal syllable structure of

pakje ab ove pakkie

In previous chapters esp ecially in chapter I have argued that the

dierence b etw een style registers seems mainly to b e that Faithfulness con

straints are more observed in the more formal styles of sp eech

It is therefore crucial that S and the diminutive but also lexical words

like garage contain a schwa In informal Rotterdam Dutchthisschwa

the emptyvo calic ro ot no de remains unparsed This means that the con

Other issues

straint forcing the parsing of this ro ot is suciently low at least lower than

ProjectN V In more formal styles of sp eech and in the Standard lan

N guage ParseVowel and the equivalent constraint Projectcons

is ranked much higher at least higher than Project N V

For a form like stoktje little stick this has the following eect

stoktje formal style of sp eech

Candidates ParseVowel ProjectN V

p

stkj

stki

The dierence b etween Standard Dutch and Rotterdam Dutchisthus that

in the former dialect the constraint ParseVowel do es not get lower than

N V inanystyle register The Rotterdam child has evidence Project

for a relatively high ranking of the ProjectN V constraint when she

observes that a form which surfaces in other contexts as j surfaces as i

here A child growing up in an environment where p eople sp eak Standard

Dutchnever encounters such evidence

A complication in the analysis of S is that the ie versus je forms

b ehave dierently with resp ect to Final Devoicing

heb je hebbie

hbj hbi

hpj hpi

These facts are discussed in Berendsen according to whose

intuitions Final Devoicing is optional in the case of j According to the

intuitions of the present author however and according to Bo oij the

variant with a voiced obstruent is more marked and certainly not b etter

than for instance lob j for loop je with an underlying voiceless

I susp ect that in these cases the app earance of the feature voice is the

consequence of something like a minor phonetic assimilation pro cess

C j C j

j

voice

If we disregard this optional rule there are two p ossibilitie s hbi and

hpj In other words w e nd Final Devoicing in j forms but not in i

forms

In the theory prop osed here this do es not havetobetaken as an in

dication that the i form is more cliticli ke than the j form It can b e

Other issues

reduced to syllabication hebbie is naturally syllabied as hbi where

the b o ccurs in the onset of a full syllable On the other hand heb je

could b e syllabied as hpj It is true that this syllabication violates

some syllable structure constraints like the Syllable Contact Law On the

other hand it satises the constraint against Cj clusters in onsets which

wehave to assume outranks the SCL

ProjectN V SCL

heb je formal style register

Candidates ProjectN V SCL

p

hpj

hb j

I should add here that Rotterdam S is the only clitic that do es not normally

show the eect of Final Devoicing Other clitics b ehaveasifFinalDevoicing

applied b efore them hebem haveemhpm heb een boek havea

b o ok hpnbuk This might b e related to the fact that in these other

clitics the vowel is a schwa cf Gussenhoven Bo oij b for recent

discussion of phonological cliticizati on in Dutch

Lexical forms Wehave seen that it is not necessary to sp ecify a

lexical dierence b etween i and j in S S or in the diminutive sux In

all those environments the dierence follows directly from resyllabicati on

We can nowwonder whether there is any need to sp ecify this distinction

in any other part of the phonological system of Dutch Since wehave

lo oked at all relevant axes and clitics the only place left to lo ok are the

monomorphemic stems

If we study other p ossible p ositions in the monomorphemic words a few

p ossible cases are found

garage araz vs quasi kwazi

garage id

col lege kle z vs cohesie kohezi

class cohesion

schoelje sxulj vs koelie kuli

scamp co olie

Spanje spnj vs Hanny hni

Spain name

bonje bnj vs ponny pni

ructions id

Other issues

Although none of these pairs are p erfectly minimal the distinctions seem

to b e fairly trivial and irrelevant They are quasiminimal pairs In each

of these cases the phonological environment directly b efore i and jis

exactly the same It seems as if wehave found cases where weneedto

sp ecify a distinction In one way or another wehave to indicate that the

words in the lefthand column cannot b e pronounced as sxuli spni etc

even though the theory seems to predict otherwise

It has to b e noticed that in each of these cases it is exactly a coronal

consonant that app ears to the left of the j cluster I have found one class

of words with a noncoronal consonant preceding j in Standard Dutch

hachje liafje snufje ikje puikje The corresp onding Rotterdam forms

all haveie These words have to b e regarded as a kind of idiomatic

diminutives This hyp othesis is supp orted by the fact that these forms are

all nouns and all have the neuter gender they cho ose the determiner het

het hachje het snufje etc The diminutive sux also makes a noun into a

neuter de viool the violin het viooltje the small violin This is not true

for the forms where je is preceded by a coronal They are not always nouns

and if they are nouns they mayaswell have common gender de kastanje

de ratatoelje de plunje de garage etc

Wemay therefore assume that in these cases the feature has b een linked

lexical ly to the consonant and to i The lexical sp ecications of bonje and

ponny then are as follows in the case of pni it do es not matter whether

the underlying form ends in i or in i this is the reason whyIhave

put a nal emptyrootinbrackets

a bnj

o o o o o

b n i

coronal

b pni

o o o o o

p n i

cor cor



Van Haeringen p oints out that the word familie family has an interesting

history in Dutch in this resp ect In the oldest stages of Dutch the word was most

probably pronounced as familij In the nineteenth century the pronunciationwas

familj Since then famili has b ecome the standard form although famiqljis still used in some dialects

Other issues

We correctly predict that there are no lexical forms like herje I assume

r do es not have a coronal feature This form would get the following

representation

o o o o o

h r

coronal

The feature coronal is not doubly linked here Therefore this form can b e

syllabied as hri without problems This is exactly what happ ens we

do havewords like herrie but no words like herje

For the rest we only nd j forms after a long vowel as in ooievaar

stork o jvar i after a long vowel is also found but only in the words

naef naive naif cocan id kokainand run ruins rywin In

the case of naef even if the underlying form would b e naif the schwa

would b e followed by f which it do es not normally tolerate in its rhyme

as wehaveseeninchapter

In the case of rune and cocane if the underlying forms would b e

ryin and kokain wewould get twoschwa syllables next to each

other in a monomorphemic word Again apparently the prohibition against

this typ e of conguration ProjectFt V is lexically higher than On

set in Standard Dutch

In the immediately preceding discussion I have assumed that the obstruent

j sequences all are clusters in the literal sense Logically sp eaking it is also

p ossible that the jelement is underlyingly sp ecied as a secondary articu

lation on the consonant As a matter of fact the segments in and also

j j

for instance in words like jury id z yri and jam id z m seem single

segments on the surface with multiple articulation rather than sequences of

consonants

j

Why then are there no monomorphemic forms that surface as pk

j

or hf

Since only coronals allow a coronal secondary articulation the deciding

factor should b e the place feature The constraint which is resp onsible for

this is closely related to some constraints wehave already seen

ConsPlace CF A place feature dominated by a cons ro ot

C

no de should b e directly dominated by a Cplace no de



Notice the last word is a loan word of which the rst segment has b een adapted to

j

the Dutch phonology I assume this is an argument for considering z as an underlying

Dutch consonant

Other issues

This constraint if dominating the relevant Parsefeature constraints

has as an eect that secondary articulation is only allowed if primary and

secondary articulation are homorganic Consider for example the pruned

j

representations of x and z

j

a x

cons

Cplace

dorsal Vplace

C

coronal

V

b z

cons

Cplace

vo calic

Vplace

C

coronal

V

Representation a is ruled out by b ecause the coronal feature is

dominated bycons but not directly dominated by a Cplace no de In

b the only place feature coronal is dominated by cons and a place

no de The fact that it is also dominated by a Vplace is irrelevantfor

this constraint This is the reason why only coronals can have a coronal

secondary articulation

Of course this do es not exclude labial consonants with a labial secondary

articulation I do not discuss dorsal secondary articulation since its theo

w w w

retical status seems so marginal Yet segments likep b orf donot

seem to o ccur in Dutch For one reason or another noncoronal secondary

articulations seem more marked than coronal secondary articulations

At the end of this section I have to briey touch on three remaining

j

observations In the rst place we only nd tense vowels b efore z

j j

araz arz this might b e related to the observation made on page

that vowels b efore fricatives are almost always tense In the second

place we nd only lax or high vowels b efore lj and nj sxulj sxolj

spnj spanj indicating that in these cases wehave real clusters nj

Other issues

or lj the sonorant closes the preceding syllable while the yo d is syllabied

in the onset It seems that palatalized fricatives and nasalj are allowed

but palatalized nasals and j sequences are not Finally exactly in

j

the case of lexical forms we nd no t formsthismaygive an indication

that we still need to refer to the notion Derived Environment cf Polgardi

for discussion of issues relating to this in Optimality Theory The

segments ti fuse to a consonant with secondary articulation only when

they are separated by a morpheme b oundaryIhave not b een able to nd

a satisfying analysis for any of these three observations

The underlying form of S is not i Secondary articulation

formation in my analysis leaves an unparsed vowel ro ot viz the ro ot of

the underlying i The output of secondary articulation formation on

j

for instance the sequence si s without a following schwa will b e as

follows

cons cons

Cplace

vo calic

Vplace

coronal

j

In isolation this sequence would b e pronounced as s This raises the

question whywe need to assume si is the underlying form at all Why

couldnt the underlying form of S b e i rather than i

Ihopetohave made it suciently clear in the preceding discussion that

it is essential to distinguish b etween underlying i and i b ecause other

wise we cannot explain the dierence b etween jenever and idee orbetween

the diminutive and S Single i should not b e p ermitted to surface as j

otherwise wewould incorrectly predict idee to surface as jde

I therefore assume that if a place no de is parsed and its corresp onding

ro ot no de is also parsed the place no de should b e linked to the ro ot no de

On the one hand this assumption seems somewhat ad hoc since it rules

out exactly the conguration that is discussed here On the other hand

it do es not seem to b e very unreasonable to assume a constraintlikethis

If two elements are linked underlyingly and if b oth elements are parsed

they should b e parsed together Naturalness is a notion that is dicult

to formalize or to measure but I do not think the assumption just made is particularily unnatural

Other issues

A problem connected to the underlying representation of S is whyonly

underlying high vowels are sub ject to gliding It seems to me that there is

evidence for a weakness constraint of the following form

WeakN high A vowel V is in the weak p osition of a syllable

V dominates high

In chapter wehave seen evidence for another weakness constraint refer

ring to the feature consonantal There is sucient evidence for weakness

constraints referring to high at dierentlevels of representation For in

stance in many languages high vowels are the only ones that can o ccur in

the weak p osition of a diphthong and Kenstowicz b has shown that

there are languages in which high vowels preferrably o ccur in the weak

p osition of the fo ot

Of course in a fullyworked out theory we will exp ect constraints for

mally similar to WeakN high but referring to purely consonantal fea

tures which also do not o ccur in the head p osition of a syllable or fo ot

Weak N high expresses the intuition that high vowels are the most

consonantal the least sonorant among vowels see chapter for a fuller

discussion of this issue

High vowel followed byschwa One other fact that seems prob

lematic for the analysis prop osed here is formed bywords containing ij

like Belgie and patricier patrician

The problem is that for these forms the most logical underlying rep

resentation is i with the j inserted as a homorganic glide However

we cannot select this representation b ecause it would surface in the system

given here as i so that wewould incorrectly predict pronunciations such

as blxi patrisiqr etc at least in the Rotterdam dialect

Wehave seen in chapter however that exactly these same words b ehave

as derived rather than monomorphemic forms with resp ect to stress If

they were really monomorphemic wewould predict stress on a p enultimate

syllable rather than on the antep enultimate where it actually resides My

The solution for this problem was that these words really are derived

schwaapp endix in these cases constitutes a sux

This prop osal also helps us to understand the b ehaviour of the i

cluster in this case In chapter I prop osed a constraint MorPa rep eated

here

MorPaAt least one element the most sonorous element of a

morpheme is incorp orated into a proso dic word



The form blxi exists as the diminutiveof Belg Belgian

Other issues

If the schwain Belgie is a sux of its own it should b e parsed into a

Proso dic Word according to MorPa This means that the parse blxi

is unacceptable b ecause it leaves the whole sux unparsed The schwa

should b e parsed and in order to satisfy the onset requirement the gram

mar intro duces a glide that is homorganic with the preceding high vowel

blxij

st p erson plural clitic Je is not the only clitic consisting of

a glide schwa in Dutch The other one is the rst p erson plural clitic

P we w The rst segment of this clitic is the labio dental glide w

but this clitic still do es not showany alternation with a labial high vowel

u or y at all dat we that we is never pronounced as dtu or

dty but always as dtw or dtm with the allomorph m As

farasIknow no dialect of Dutch shows alternation of w with a high

vowel although j i is found in some restricted form in manyDutch and

Frisian dialects

On the other hand the p olite second p erson singular or plural pronoun

is u y There is no reduced or clitic form of this pronoun In particular it

do es not alternate with w Tomyknowledge there is no dialect in which

na u naby is ever pronounced as naw Although I call u a p olite

pronoun I feel this gap cannot b e attributed to the fact that u is only

used in formal sp eech In some idiolects u can b e used to address p ersons

with whom the sp eaker has reached a high level of intimacylike parents or

grandparents Nevertheless a pronunciation wisnever found Of course

a linguistic explanation for this gap is more easily found If the underlying

form of the p olite pronoun is y rather than y we predict the surface

to b e y This leaves the problem of P untouched however

It seems that wehaveanasymmetrybetween the two glides j and

w There are more reasons to consider the relation b etween i and j

amuch closer one than that b etween u or y and w j seems to b e a

more real glide than w in the sense that there are many arguments for

considering j really a variant of i in consonantal p osition while w seems

to b e slightly more consonantal

While j can never b e in a bip ositional onset at all jraak kjaad

w can act as an obstruentlike element in onsets such as the one in wraak

revenge wrak or as a sonorantsuchasinkwaad angry kwat



I use the term glide in a lo osely descriptiveway Most of the asymmetries noted here

have b een observed byTrommelen who prop osed to assign w to the class of

liquids She also observed that intervo calic Cw clusters are always syllabied as onsets

etui id etwi aquarium id akwarijym



The distribution of wC clusters in onsets in Dutchisvery limited in twoways In

the rst place the number of words containing such clusters is very limited and in the

Other issues

On the other hand w and j do pattern together in certain ways In

particular this is true if we assume w is the glide version of y For

instance wehaveseeninchapter that a constraint against homorganic

glidevowel sequences aects b oth ji and wy while the clusters b ecome

p erfectly regular if we replace the glides by normal consonants lier lyra

liqr dier animal diqr vuur re vyqr etc

The same typ e of restriction holds on glides after vowels While word

nal sequences like ej ij uw are excluded in contrast to ew iw

uj sequences like et it up are not

Although going into the matter in much detail would lead us to issues of

segment representation that fall to o far outside of the scop e of this thesis

I think one of the reasons for this o dd b ehaviour may b e the labiodental

character of w My preliminary prop osal is that this segment has a

coronal dental secondary and a labial primary place of articulation while

it still has a vo calic ro ot no de

cons

Cplace

labial Vplace

coronal

This representation would distinguish w from vowels and from j imme

diately b ecause the latter segments only have a secondary vo calic

place of articulation Clements Given an appropriate theory of p os

sible onset sequences which I will not try to develop here this gives us the

p ossibility to distinguish b etween w and j in onset clusters

On the other hand w is still a glide in the sense that it is a vo calic

no de dominating a vo calic place no de This makes it p ossible to formulate

the constraints on glidevowelvowelglide sequences just outlined

I nd it dicult to bring empirical evidence to b ear up on the represen

tation in I havechosen to make coronal the vo calic place feature

b ecause it allows me to formulate a pro jection constraint against gliding in

a fairly straightforward manner

second place in many dialects the w in these clusters hardens to the labio dental v



It might b e that the coronal feature is even absent phonologically and only the

result of phonetic enhancement The representation given here however might b e seen

as the result of a constraint to the eect that having more than one place feature under

any place no de is dispreferred Ben Hermans pc

Furthermore some dialects for instance in Flanders and in Dutch Limburg havea

bilabial glide For these dialects I would b e forced to assume a representation where the

coronal feature has disapp eared and the labial feature is linked to b oth tiers

Other issues

N all vo calic features should b e in the head of an N ProjectV

except the feature coronal

V

Constraint excludes all vo calic ro ots in the onset except for the front

unrounded vowels and w Interestingly it can b e seen as a weaker version

of OnsetProjectF N

It seems furthermore justied to put the labial feature in Cplace p o

sition b ecause in its interaction with other consonants it b ehaves as labial

rather than coronal pw clusters are excluded b ecause of the OCP and

Contour but tw clusters do o ccur in Dutch as the examples twee two

twe twist dispute twst and twijg twig twix may attest

Another prediction wemake is that w can lexically o ccur in the nu

cleus of a syllable just like nasals and liquids cf chapter Postlexically

wheaded syllables would b e sub ject to schwaep enthesis just like sylla

bles headed by sonorant consonants Now there are no syllables ending

in w in Dutch Yet Trommelen has shown that wordnal yw

clusters in forms like schaduw shadow sxadyw b ehavelikeschwaheaded

syllables in many resp ects for instance syllable structure and stress It

could therefore b e that in this case vowel ep enthesis is accompanied by

a total assimilation of the place features from w to the sup ercial y

head

FinallyIwant to p oint out that this analysis do es not imply that there

are no homorganic glides after u or y After u or u we usually

nd a bilabial u After y and y we nd either j or bilabial u

dep ending on the dialect and some sp eakerdep endent factors which are

hard to formalize

a douane customs duuan

b duo id dyjodyuo

c buien showers byjbyu



The except clause makes us susp ect that this constraint conates some more basic

constraints for instance a general constraint against vo calic features in consonantal

p osition interacting with a constraint on licensing of coronal Aninteresting asp ect

V

of this is that here wehave another instance of the sp ecial status of coronals in the

syllable in the sense that coronal can o ccur in a p osition in which other place features

are not licensed See chapter for some additional discussion



There is a constrast b etween forms with a tense vowel and forms with a lax vowel

In the former case ep enthesis is obligatory schaduw sxadyw sxadw in the latter

case it is prohibited murw mellow mrw mryw erwt p ea rwt rywt

The former case can b e explained but I have no explanation why there is no ep enthesis in the latter case

Conclusion

Conclusion

In this chapter wehave studied typical vo calic elements o ccuring in extra

cephalic p ositions I think the analysis of the vowelglide alternation has

given us the clearest example of an advantage of theories which use the

idea of relative strength of constraints over other views of grammar If we

put twovowels next to each other these get into comp etition for b eing the

head of a nucleus If one of the twovowels is a schwa and the other is a full

vowel the latter normally wins This is something wehave already seen in

chapter In this chapter I have shown that certain external circumstances

mayfavour the surfacing of schwaover that of the full vowel Twosuch

circumstances have b een shown to exist in Dutch These were the Onset

constraint and the OCP These two constraints seem to overrule the normal

desire of full vowels to b ecome syllable nuclei

With resp ect to the Pro jection theory the main result of this chapter

h are com was that wehave to p osit a class of Weakness constraints whic

plementary in their formulation to the Pro jection constraints The latter

say that a strong p osition in the proso dy is connected to a certain feature

the former that certain other features are connected to weak p ositions

It app eared that vowels which o ccur in such a p osition are typically

high vowels and most typically i Dutch w has b een argued to b e more

consonantal than j Furthermore wehave seen some evidence for the

hyp othesis put forward by Clements that consonants and vowels

have the same place of articulation features This hyp othesis can help us

explain palatalisation The reason furthermore why the voiceless stop t

palatalizes more readily than any other coronal consonantisthatitisina

sense the simplest consonantwhichprovides us with evidence either for a

theory of feature markedness or for monovalent features

The Pro jection ConstraintFamily

Intro duction

In this thesis I have shown in what way features on the head segmentof

some proso dic constituent and the structure which is built on top of that

segment can inuence each other It app eared that in particular aperture

features like high lax and low are relevant for proso dic structure

This nding in itself is not new On the one hand theories that work

with sonority scales often assign separate p ositions on the scale to high

mid and lowvowels where high vowels get a lower sonority and lowvow

els a higher sonority Most often these sonority scales are then used to

explain facts that are related to syllable structure such as gliding and

diphthongization On the other hand rules of changing

vowel height in stressed or unstressed p osition have b een prop osed for in

stance in the literature on various Italian dialects

Yet this insight never seems to have received a systematic treatment I

think that one of the advantages of the approach defended here is that it

oers a more uniform formal means to express the relation b etween vo calic

features and proso dic structure This allowed us to develop for instance a

more interesting view on the relation b etween laxness and syllable closed

ness or on the degenerate syllable which is built on top of the empty vowel

The theory presented here is couched within a framework of constraint

based phonologyIfwe do not constrain the notion of p ossible constraint

such a theory is not particularily restrictive

This statementobviously needs some qualication Since in standard

Optimality Theory the set of constraints is supp osed to b e universal and

nite it is not strictly necessary for the individual constraints to b e for

mally related to one another in any sense In principle it is p ossible that

Universal Grammar consists of a numb er of totally unrelated constraints

See Kenstowicz for the application of the sonority hierarchy to stress

phenomena

Pro jection and weakness

However given the ndings of phonological theory it seems more plausible

that the set of constraints is mo dular sub divided into subsets of tightly

connected constraints It also seems metho dologically b etter to assume

such a clustering of constraints As long as linguists do not agree on the

exact content of UG and all other things b eing equal the use of a constraint

A in a particular analysis seems to b e preferable over the use of a constraint

B when A is formally related to other constraints C and D whereas B is

not

The most imp ortant new constraint families whichIhave prop osed in

this thesis are the pro jection family and the weakness family These fam

ilies relating feature content to proso dic structure form the core of the

prop osal made here

Pro jection and weakness

I will start out by listing all the pro jection and weakness constraints that

Ihave prop osed in this thesis

 

Connect N lax p N dominates lax N is the head of a

branching N



Pro jectV Ft p N dominates a vo calic no de N is the head



of a branching Fo ot



Pro jectV Ft p N dominates a vo calic no de N is the head

of a Foot



N is the head of a branching Foot N dom Pro jectFt V p



inates a vo calic no de

 

Pro ject N V p N is the head of a branching N N dominates

avo calic no de

 

Pro jectN cons p N is the head of a branching N N

dominates cons

 

N F p N is the head of a branching N N dominates Pro ject

some feature F F

 

Pro jectF N Onset p N dominates some feature N is the

head of a branching N A syllable with some feature in its head

should have an onset

  

Pro jectN low p N branches N dominates low

 

N p N dominates a vo calic feature N is in the Pro jectV

head of an N

 

Pro jectcons N p N dominates cons N is the head

of a branching N All vowels are heads of syllables

Weak N high p A vowelVisinthedependent p osition of a

N V dominates high syllable

Pro jection and weakness

Weak cons N p A segment S has the feature cons Sis

in the dep endent p osition of a syllable N

WeakN C p A segmentintheweak p osition of a syllable

needs to have a consonantal place feature CC fcoronal dorsal

c c

labial g

c

This family of constraints forms the core of the prop osal made in this

thesis It is a consequence of the syllable headedness hyp othesis that the

feature structure on the syllable head determines to a large extent the

structure of the syllable The general form of most of the constraints the

pro jection constraints listed here is as follows

 x

a b ottom up N dominates a feature F N branches

x 

where N f N N N Ft gF some feature set S

x 

b top down N branches N dominates a feature F

x 

where N f N N N Ft gF some feature set S

In some cases there is no reason to distinguish b etween a top down and

a b ottom up version of the pro jection constraints In those cases I called

the relevant constraints ConnectX Y Yet in other cases ranking con

siderations led us to treat b ottom up and top down as constraints with

indep endent lo cations in the hierarchyFor instance in French tense mid

vowels do not o ccur in closed syllables which I to ok as an indication for a

sucient high ranking of ProjectN lax but in the same language lax

mid vowels can freely o ccur in op en syllables I assumed that this shows

that Projectlax N is ranked lower

Strictly sp eaking this is not necessarily true We could also assume that

while in Dutch Connect N lax dominates all Faithfulness constraints in

Frenchwe draw a distinction b etween Parselax on the one hand and

lax on the other

a Dutch Connect N laxParselax lax

N lax lax b French Parselax Connect

ConnectN lax dominates the constraint against insertion of lax so

that tense vowels in closed syllables get laxed without problem Yet ConnectN

lax is dominated by Parselax so that underlyingly lax vowels staylax

even if they o ccur in op en syllables

Given this p ossibility of dierently ranking the twotyp es of Faithfulness

it might turn out to b e unnecessary to dierentiate b etween b ottom up and

top down pro jection It would therefore b e interesting to try to recast the

analyses given in this thesis using only ConnectX Y constraints Yet

Pro jection and weakness

since this would complicate the ranking arguments considerablyIhave

decided to only use ProjectX Y constraints wherever there is no real

evidence for an active ProjectY X

Normally pro jection constraints refer to heads of branching constituents

Only at the level of the fo ot wehave seen constraints referring also to heads

of nonbranching constituents

Furthermore in a few cases wehave seen evidence for constraints of the

following form the weakness constraints

 x

a b ottom up N dominates a feature F N is in the weak

x

p osition of an N constituent

x 

where N f N N N gF some feature set S

x x

b top down N is in the weak p osition of an N constituent



N dominates a feature F

x 

where N f N N N gF some feature set S

We need to draw a distinction b etween pro jection constraints and weakness

constraints only in a theory that uses monovalent features



For binary features the weakness constraintN dominates a feature

x x

F N is in the weak p osition of an N constituent equals the

x 

pro jection constraintN branches N dominates a feature F and

x x 

N is in the weak p osition of an N constituent N dominates a

 x

feature F equals N dominates a feature F N branches It might

b e relevant that the weakness constraints used here refer either to cons

or to high and that high and consonantal are two features for

which it is notoriously hard to design a unary feature analysis

Again it is p ossible that there is no real need to distinguish b etween

b ottom up and top down weakness but for ease of exp osition I have

decided to distinguish b etween the two

Several remarks hav e to b e made ab out these constraintschemes First

it seems that they are b est interpreted in a theory of proso dic structure in

whichevery pro jection no de is a projection of its head in the most literal

i i i

sense of the word for every N a pro jection of N N has exactly the

i

same features as N

There are several theories of syllable structure whichhave the required

prop erties In this thesis I havechosen to adopt an Xbar like structure

which is similar in some resp ects to the theory of Bare Phrase Structure

prop osed by Chomsky In this theory the Xbar lab els are seen as

mere mnemonic terms A structure like a is a mere way of representing

the structure in b in an insightful way

Pro jection and weakness

a N b

N

N



N

b l k b l k

We could also assume that feet are pro jections of syllables to higher levels

By transitivity feet and proso dic words are therefore headed by syllable



heads in the typical case byvowels

There are other ways of formalising the same idea that heads pro ject to

higher levels at which they enter into lo cal relations with other elements

Examples of such formalisations are Dep endency Phonology Anderson and

Jones Anderson and Ewen and bracketed grid theory Halle and

Vergnaud The equivalence b etween these formalisms and the typ e of

Xbar trees used here has b een established in Van Oostendorp In

this thesis I havechosen to use Xbar theory It will b e more dicult

to incorp orate these constraints into a theory using moras b ecause the

notion head seems foreign to this theory and b ecause the structures used

in ortho dox moraic phonology are not binary branching

There are two parameters in the denitions and the level of

n

pro jection X and the feature F In this chapter I briey discuss the range of

p ossibiliti es that arise if wecombine the dierent settings for the pro jection

level parameter and for the feature parameter Not every logically p ossible

combination is discussed and I have to admit that I havenoteven found

all logically p ossible combinations of parameter settings attested In the

discussion of the previous chapters it has turned out to b e convenientto

refer to the following sets of features

V fhigh low lax coronal labial dorsal cons song the

v v v

set of vo calic features

F the set of all segmental features



Avery interesting alternativewould b e to assume that feet are built on top of N



or N no des The reason for this could b e that by denition no further pro jection is

N This would help us explain why onset segments often do not count p ossible on a

for stress and it would also provide us with an interesting view of fo ot structure when

combined with the ideas sketched in app endix A

The fo ot level

For more discussion see section b elow I will now discuss the particular



instantiations of a and b for eachlevel of the proso dic hierarchy

The fo ot level

The rst level I want to consider is the fo ot level On this level we exp ect

the following pro jection constraints



Nheads a ProjectF Ft N dominates a feature F F V



a branching fo ot



b ProjectFt F N heads a branching fo ot N dominates a



feature F F V

One version of a is the constraint ProjectV Ft which I used in



the analysis of Dutch and an instance of b is ProjectFt V again



applying transp osition As wehave seen in the case of the ConnectFt



V constraints any vo calic feature under the vo calic no de is able to attract

in the case of a or license in the case of b a fo ot

Normally pro jection constraints refer to heads of branching constituents

Only at the level of the fo ot wehave seen constraints referring also to heads

of nonbranching constituents In the analysis of Dutchwehave seen evi

dence for a nonbranching counterpart of a ProjectV Ft



In chapter wehave seen evidence for a version of the constraint



ProjectFt in Frenchwhich refers to the feature lax





ProjectFt lax French Nheadsafoot N dominates a



feature lax

The reverse constraintmight b e resp onsible for mid vowel neutralisation

eects in other languages and dialects For instance in



It is dicult to nd similar evidence at the word level I have not b een able to

nd any evidence that pro jection constraints mightbeatwork at levels higher than the

proso dic word such as the phonological phrase the Intonation Phrase or the Utterance

Mayb e one might think of the assignmentofFo cus tones as the result of such a constraint

x 

N is the head of an Intonation Phrase N dominates a H tone but there do not

seem to b e languages in which the o ccurrence of for instance low tones is restricted to



the head of any phrasal category and furthermore instances of N dominates a H tone

x

N is the head of an Intonation Phrase are unknown to me



It is usually assumed that Dutchisquantity sensitiveasI have discussed in chapter

The analysis presented here raises another p ossibility which is not explored here viz

that Dutch stress is sensitive to the feature lax Again here wehave an instance of

the close connection b etween laxness quantity and closedness I nd it hard to nd

go o d empirical evidence for or against stress theories based on the closed versus op en

distinction of syllables on the one hand and stress theories based on the tensedness versus

laxness of vowels on the other

The fo ot level

lax vowels can only o ccur in stressed p osition In weak p ositions suchvowels

might b e neutralized towards their tense counterparts Wetzels

a mromoramos Iwe reside

b mvemovei move singplur

In the case of Portuguese rhyme structure seems unimp ortant for the fea

ture lax what matters is rather fo ot structure

We might assume this pattern is the result of the interaction b etween

constraints on fo ot structure and all dominating Parselax in Brazil

ian Portuguese



Projectlax Ft Portuguese N dominates a feature lax



N heads a branching fo ot

Metrical Wellformedness MWF ProjectFt Parselax



mro

Candidates MWF ProjectFt Parselax



p

mro

mro

moro

mramos

Candidates MWF ProjectFt Parselax



mramos

mramos

p

moramos

In this thesis wehave not come across any evidence for a weakness con

straint at the fo ot level Yet there are generalisations which could b e stated

in these terms In all relevant cases I have found the feature to which

Weakness refers is high or cons

WeakFt high A vowelVisintheweak p osition of a fo ot V

dominates high

In the discussion of Chamorro on page wehave seen that mid vowels in

that language surface as high in unstressed p osition This might b e seen as

an eect of

Another eect of the same constraint can b e seen in Northern Salentino

Italian Calabrese Exactly as in Chamorro mid vowels can only

surface as such in stressed p osition otherwise they are high

The fo ot level

sg kre u s ntu kanosku

pl kri tiamu si ntmu kanussmu

b elieve feel recognize

When we follow Burzio wemighteven have evidence for weakness

constraints p ertaining to high vowels and consonantal syllable heads in

English Burzio distinguishes b etween three classes of syllables which are

relevant for English word stress HeavyLightandWeak Weak syllables

are in a sense the lightest syllables for stress in manywords they are not

metried at all and when they are incorp orated into a fo ot this fo ot itself

b ecomes weak ie it cannot get primary word stress Whether or not a

syllable is weak is dep endent on its head Burzio claims that Weak

syllables either haveanempty head in the case of degenerate syllables a

consonantal head ble cle etc or a high vowel as its head ly ry

A particularly wellknown example of a feature which stands in a sp ecic

relation to stress is of course tone In many languages tones are aligned

with the most prominent syllable in a fo ot a word or a phonological phrase

This is expressed for Serb oCroatian in the following ways by Hermans

a and Zec resp ectively

a Hermans a Tone Blo cking A tone maynotbe

governed ie it may not o ccur in the dep endent p osition of

either a syllable or a fo ot

b Zec Align High tone with the head of a fo ot

Tomethesetwo conditions seem to b e equivalent We could formalize b oth



constraints in the pro jection framework as follows



Projecthigh tone Ft Serb oCroatian N dominates a high

tone N heads a fo ot

Actually the situation is a little more complex since high tones maynot

occurintheweak mora of a stressed syllable either so that wemay

assume that Serb oCroatian also has a tonerelated constraint at the level

N of

Finally Serb oCroatian also requires all feet to b e accompanied bya

tone at some level of representation This means that we also nd a tonal

counterpart of ProjectFt in this langugae



ProjectFt high tone Serb oCroatian N heads a fo ot N

dominates a high tone



See also for instance Goldsmith Peterson and Progo and Peterson for similar principles in Nguni

The fo ot level

The theory of pro jection and weakness constraints predicts that only fea

tures of the head of a syllable may aect the p osition of that syllable in a

fo ot One very wellknown counterexample to this claim is Piraha Everett

and Everett In this language syllables with a voiceless consonantin

their onset count as heavier than similar syllables with voiced onsets which

in turn are heavier than onsetless syllables Stress falls on the heaviest of

the nal three syllables I underlined stressed syllables

kaa gai word biisai red kaibai monkey etc

The fact that it is the feature voice which counts as relevantisinteresting

for at least two reasons In the rst place I assume that vowels which

apparently are the only syllable heads in Piraha do not p ossess a sp ec

ication for voice Now some work in morphology has concentrated on

the concept of a RelativizedHead Selkirk Di Sciullo and Williams

According to this work features of nonheads may p ercolate up to

the pro jection level but only provided that these features are not present

in the head The Piraha stress facts may provide us with a phonological

example of this

The second reason whyitisinteresting that it is the feature voice that

seems to count as relevant is that there is a strong correlation b etween



laryngeal features and tone Esp ecially the voicing of onset consonants

seems to b e the most common source of tonogenesis Kingston and Solnit

Authors like Halle and Stevens Yip Bao

Duanmu Van der Hulst c have suggested to treat these two

classes of features as one gesture For instance Yip prop oses the

following partial feature geometry

Laryngeal

Register HL voice Glottal Ap erture

cg sg

In this mo del High and Low tone on the one hand and voice are basically

the same thing More sp ecically one could saythatvoice High

upp er and voice Lowupp er Other mo dels are p ossibly also



See Everett for a discussion of the interaction b etween stress and vo calic tone

in Piraha



Yip assumes Pitch no des dep endent on Register in order to account for four

way heightcontrasts These I have omitted b ecause they do not seem to b e particularily

relevant to the present discussion Furthermore cg constricted glottis and sg spread glottis

The N level

feasible but in any case the relation b etween voicing and tone seems to b e

rather well established

Wethus can reduce the relevant stress constraint to the version of

ProjectN suggested for Serb oCroatian in ab ove

The N level

The level b elow the fo ot is the syllable level Here we nd the following

constraints

 

N N dominates a feature F N heads a a ProjectF

branching N

 

b ProjectN F N heads a branching N N dominates a

feature F

All pro jection constraints at this level seem to refer to branching Nnodes

I therefore leave out the subscript

An instance of constraint b is Project N V inchapters and

the constraint a has an instantiation that has come to b e known in

the literature as the Onset constraint ProjectN V can b e satised by

anyvo calic feature just like the constraints on the fo ot level The Onset

constraint on the other hand seems much stronger in the sense that any

syllable needs an onset even when it is headed by a sonorant consonant

or an emptyvowel

At the level of the syllable wealsohave evidence for a weakness con

straint

N high A vowelVisintheweak p osition of a syllable Weak

no de N V dominates high

This is part of the constraint HighWeak the constraint that states that

only high vowels can o ccur in extracephalic p osition A similar constraint

Weak N cons mightsay that only consonants may o ccur in onsets and

After this reduction only one problematic case remains to b e solved This is Madi

Madi Davis Go edemans pc The stress rule in this language seems to b e of

approximately the following form stress is on the rst syllable except when the second

syllable contains a coronal obstruent in its onset in which case stress is on that syllable

If this is true it is problematic for the accountpresented here in twoways In the rst

place if vo calic and consonantal place features are the same we cannot invoke the notion

of a Relativized Head In the second place place features do not seem to play a role in

prominence relations in any other system I know

Nlevel

N that consonants o ccur only in the dep endent p ositions of Weak cons

syllables

In order to capture the observation that consonants typically o ccur in

onsets we could also add the following weakness constraint

Weak cons N A segment S dominates F F a consonantal fea

N ture S is in the weak p osition of an

The Onset constraintProjectF N is among the strongest pro jection

constraints in one imp ortant resp ect any phonological feature is sucient

to force a syllable to have an onset It is unclear whythe Onset constraint

should b e so forceful Wehave encountered a weaker version of this con

straintinchapter viz ProjectV N al l vo calic features should bein

the head of an NWehave also seen a constraint Projectcons N a

constraint forcing all vowels even schwa to b e heads of syllables

In general the pro jection theory is b etter in explaining the symmetries

between the Nlevel on the one hand and the lower and higher pro jection

levels on the other than in explaining the asymmetries between those cat

egories Some of these asymmetries are i ProjectN never seems to

refer to lax or low whereas pro jection at lower or higher levels do es

ii onsets do not or hardly ever contribute to weight iii the relation

between voice on onset obstruents and tone on nuclei do es not seem to b e

mirrorred in the relation b etween nucleus and co da iv in many languages

co da p ositions allow only a subset of the consonants that are allowed in

onset p osition v the Contour constraint also seems quite typical for

onsets constraints against the homorganicity of co da segments and the

preceding nucleus seems far less widespread The theory would probably

have to b e supplemented by a theory on consonantal p ositions The pro

jection theory is intended to b e part of a mo dular phonological system of

which a theory of consonantal p ositions should b e another part

Nlevel

The most imp ortant instance of a pro jection constraintonelevel b elow the

syllable level is what I have called ConnectN lax

Connect N lax

 

a Projectlax N N dominates lax N heads a branching

N

 

b Project NlaxN heads a branching N N dominates lax

Nuclear level

Again all instances of ProjectN seem to refer to branching rhymes In

Dutch the two parts of ConnectN lax seem inseparable visa vis their

overall p osition in the hierarchybutwehave seen that in Frenchonly

b is visibly activeininteraction with constraints on place In b oth

languages the constraint refers to the feature lax and the same is true for

Andalusian Spanish and for Polynesian languages like Eastern Javanese and

Tagalog In many languages the only vowels that are allowed in the weak

constituent partofadiphthong are the high vowels If falling diphthongs

form a branching N this might b e describ ed byaWeakness constraintof

the following form

WeakN high A vowel V is in the weak p osition of an N no de

V dominates high

As we will see in the following section it can also b e that diphthongs are

represented as branching nuclei In that case the N in this denition should



b e replaced byN In chapter wehave seen evidence for a weakness

constraint Weak cons N This constraint said that consonants should

o ccur in a dependent p osition of the rhyme not in its head and it banned

syllabic nuclei in the p ostlexical phonology

In the literature we sometimes nd a constraint against co das the

NoCoda constraint There are in myviewtwo p ossible ways to view

the relation b etween this constraint and the pro jection constraints at the

N level Either we can see NoCoda as a formulation of ConnectN lax

in combination with a constraint against the insertion of lax or wecan

see b as the result of the constraint ranking aNoCoda

Nuclear level

The lowest level of pro jection is the nucleus If we assume that this level

can branch just as well as any other level and that we need to distinguish

between a nucleus and a rhyme we might supp ose that also here wehave

pro jection constraints of the following typ e



a Projectlow N a segment S dominates low Sheadsa



branching N

 

b ProjectN low A segment S heads a branching N S

dominates low

There would however b e one p otential problem for this reformulation we could

not explain why there are langugaes whichdohave the feature lax and for instance

laxharmony while at the same time only allowing for op en syllables

Features

It is very likely that the nucleus is the domain for long vowels and diph

thongs cf for instance Harris for a discussion of English long vowels

If this holds true the constraint NoLongVowel can b e reduced to b

In Antwerp Dutch all vowels are lax if and only if they are long Un

der a branchingnucleus analysis of long vowels this could b e analysed by

assuming a and b with reference to lax instead of low are very

strong in this dialect

What is interesting ab out the branching nucleus analysis is that falling



diphthongs show up the prominence asymmetries that wehavecometo

exp ect the rst comp onentvowel is generally low andor lax and the sec

ond comp onent is high and tense For instance Philips lists the

feature changing rules needed to account for all falling diphthong forma

tions mentioned in Hayes

Middle English iq uq ei ou Add high left

New High German iq uq ai au Add low left

Icelandic eq oq q e o Add ATR right

Central Swedish iq yqTq uq ij yw T u Add cons right

Czech eq oq ei ou Add high right

In all cases the dep endent part on the right gets either high cons or

ATR and the head to the left gets high or low

Atechnical problem that has to b e solved under an analysis assuming

branching nuclei for long vowelsisthatabranching nucleus has to count

N in all resp ects but not vice versa the branching nucleus as a branching

apparently is enough to satisfy ConnectN lax it contributes to syllable



weight in the same way as a branching N and furthermore N and N cannot

branch at the same time otherwise wewould exp ect sup erheavy syllables

suchas kilk in Standard Dutch As always there are ways of solving



this problem for instance by making the nonco o ccurrence of branching N

Ninto a separate constraint dominating all others Another and branching

option would b e to intro duce a government relation into the theory and

derive the nonmultiple branchingness eect from a stipulated adjacency

and directionality requirement of this relation Kaye Lowenstamm and

Vergnaud

Features

The following features or sets of features are referred to in the pro jection

and weakness constraints used in this thesis



Rising diphthongs need to have a separate treatment cf Sluyters for a

p ossible approach in which a subnuclear constituent is prop osed the peak

Features

def

a F the set of all phonological features

def

b VF the set of vo calic features f high low lax coronal

dorsal labial g

c lax low cons son upp ervoice V

d high V

It is imp ortant to constrain the p ossible value for the F parameter to these

values since probably we do not exp ect for instance the place feature of

avowel to aect its pro jection capacities in whichcasewewould predict

languages in which rounded vowels can only o ccur in stressed p osition or

in a closed syllable

The fact that ap erture features and more sp ecicall y high and low

are relevant for syllabication has b een implicitly accepted in most of the

literature Many theorists who employ a sonority scale to describ e the

facts of a language and who distinguished among vowels on this scale as

signed a lower sonorityvalue to high vowels than to mid vowels or low

vowels

The sonority dierence is captured in the approach defended here by

assuming the metatheoretic generalisations in

a The features lax and low cons son play a role in

pro jection constraints not in weakness constraints

b The feature high and cons playsaroleonlyinweakness

constraints

This expresses the intuition that high vowels are in a sense more consonantal

than the nonhigh vowels while lowandlaxvowels are in a sense the most

ideal heads of syllables and feet

In purely treegeometric terms it is not very clear why ap erture features

playsuch an imp ortant role in the pro jection of syllables It is relatively

easy to show that the ap erture no de and the place no de should b e sisters

since some languages show a pro cess of total vowel assimilation spreading

all vo calic features to the exclusion of the nonvo calic features and there

is no evidence that the ap erture features are for instance linked higher in

the feature tree than the vo calic features Harry van der Hulst pc has

suggested that mayb e ap erture features play the role of a head in the

structure of a vowel Since the pro jection of an elementby denition is

In order to go again one step further and explain we could either refer to pho

netic grounding or try to explore the hyp othesis put forward in Van der Hulst c

that phonological features haveaninternal structure which in the case of high would in

clude more consonantal elements and in the case of low and lax more vo calic elements

Features

the pro jection of the head of that element this would explain the asymme

try b etween ap erture features and place features If such a theory can b e

worked out in the appropriate way so that for instance we can still distin

guish the pro jection prop erties of e which has at least one vo calic feature



but no ap erture feature and which has neither an ap erture feature

nor a place feature it would b e an interesting extension of the pro jection

theory defended here Possibly such a theory would refer to the notion of

a Relativized Head

In section I have argued that also tone and voice play a role in

pro jection constraints In all wehave the following feature geometrywith

the no des which are relevant for pro jection of metrical structure in b old

face

cons son

Laryngeal Cplace

vo calic

Ap erture Vplace

All features except for the Place features seem to b e able to playarole

in pro jection constraints From a phonetic p oint of view we could mayb e

say that the relevant features except the ma jor class features all havea

primary inuence on the F levelbuthightoneandvowel heighthave



the opp osite eects high tone attaches to head p ositions whereas high

vowels tend to o ccur in dep endent p ositions even though the twotyp es of

heighthave comparable eects on F The question therefore has to remain





op en

A further sp eculation in chapter has b een that the feature parameter

might b e set for the pro jection constraints at every dierentlevel For

instance in Frenchwehave found evidence for ProjectFt lax but not



for ProjectFt V whereas in Dutchwehave found evidence for the





This is true under the assumption that mid vowels are undersp ecied for ap erture

The same typ e of problem holds for a particle approachtovo calic features with resp ect

to the comparison of high vowels whichbearnoAelement or op en feature and therefore

are presumably undersp ecied for op enness and schwa



One p ossibility suggested in Van der Hulst c is that approximately the

b oldfaced feature sets here act in a sense as heads of the segment Head features would

then b e allowed to p ercolate up to higher proso dic levels of pro jection whereas this

would b e much more diculty for nonhead features In this resp ect it mightalso

b e relevant that segments with a place sp ecication but without ap erture ie full

mid vowels such as e and o are much more common than vowels without a place

sp ecication but with ap erture instances of the latter might b e

Constraints conicting with pro jection and weakness

latter constraint but not for the former No cases were found of conicting

or otherwise interacting ProjectFt constraint It might therefore b e



more plausible that languages have at most one typ e of ProjectFt



constraint and that a parameter is set

Constraints conicting with pro jection and weak

ness

The relation b etween vowel quality and syllable structure crosslinguisti cal ly

seems to b e suciently weaksoastohave gone undetected in most of the

literature In many languages the qualityofvowels seems to have hardly

any relation to proso dic structure at all Under the assumptions presented

here all the constraints presented are supp osed to have a universal force

They are present in the grammar of every language b e it that in some

grammars they can b e forced to b e violated

It is therefore imp ortant to detect the constraints that crucially con

ict with pro jection and weakness The most reasonable candidates are

the Faithfulness constraints In order to satisfy a pro jection or weakness

constraintwe often have to insert or unparse features Take for example the

constraint Connect N lax and let us supp ose this constraintisranked

very high and wehave an input domp with a tense vowel and a sequence

of consonants which can only b e syllabied as a co da plus an onset All

candidate outputs for this input which satisfy ConnectN lax violate

some faithfulness constraint

domp

Candidates ConnectN lax Faithfulness

domp

dmp lax

do m  p Parsep

do m p  Parsem

Parsep and Parsem are shorthands for parse constraints on all of the

features of these two segments In a language in which all of these faith

fulness conditions dominate ConnectN lax the output domp will b e

selected

In some cases pro jection constraints can also work against each other

I will givetwo examples from the analysis of Dutch to illustrate this p oint

In the Dutch lexicon wehave ConnectV FtProject N V The



latter constraint among others disallows schwaheaded syllables but the

former requires exactly such syllables in unstressed p osition Thus the head

Conclusion

N V in order to satisfy of an unstressed syllable can violate Project

ConnectV Ft



The second example is from the p ostlexical phonology In that stratum

wehave to insert schwa in a syllable with a consonantal head suchas

vergiet strainer vrit in order to satisfy Projectcons N Wecan

N V do this in twoways vrorvr The former violates Project

the latter Connect N lax We select the latter b ecause ProjectN

V ConnectN lax

Conclusion

In this chapter I have discussed the notion p ossible pro jection or weakness

constraint Some of the eects that fall under this notion have already

b een known for a long time For instance it is wellknown that high vowels

are the most likely among all full vowels to o ccur as the weak part of a

diphthong as a glide in an onset or in the weak syllable of a fo ot Similarly

consonants are very marked syllable nuclei and if they do o ccur as such

the syllable in most languages has to b e unstressed Generalisations such

as these can b e captured in a theory of weakness constraints

Lax and lowvowels on the other hand in most languages do not partic

ipate in glide formation do not o ccur as the weak part of a diphthong and

get stressed wherever they can Suchvowels are the most typical syllable

heads They also require a lot of extra structure for instance in Dutch

where they only o ccur as the heads of branching rhymes Generalisations

such as these can b e captured in a theory of pro jection constraints

Mid tense vowels are in b etween high vowels and lowvowels They

can o ccur in head p ositions but mostly they do not require anybranching

in many languages mid vowels can also b e in a glide p osition or in the

unstressed syllable of a fo ot There is go o d evidence for assuming they lack

anyvowel height sp ecication since they alternate quite freely with schwa

This is what the theory presented here predicts

A Constraints and Families of Constraints

In this app endix I list all the constraints used in this thesis with the ex

ception of the pro jection constraints that have already b een dicussed in

the previous chapter I have adapted most of the constraints listed here

from the literature they serve asabackground to the pro jection theory

Their interest for this thesis lies in their interaction with the pro jection

constraints not so much in their own sp ecic formulation

Here I show that some of these constraints can b e organized into fami

lies sets of constraints that are derived from one common ancestor by the

instantiation of certain parameters I will discuss each of these families in a

separate section The constraints which do not participate in any family are

group ed according to the phonological mo dule to which they b elong such

as autosegmental theory It is not my goal to give an extensive metatheory

of constraints in this app endix see McCarthy and Prince but I want

to giveanoverview of the typ es of constraints that seem to b e needed in

the study of a larger fragment of a grammar

For each constraint listed in this app endix I rst give the name I used in

the previous chapters plus the denition of the constraint and a reference

to the rst time the constraintwas mentioned

A Proso dic wellformedness

In this section I discuss those constraints on proso dic structure that I needed

in my analysis and that are not pro jection or weakness constraints I dis

cuss such constraints on syllable structure on fo ot structure and on word

structure each in their own section

A Syllable wellformedness Apart from pro jection and weak

ness constraints w e turned out to need the following constraints on well

formed syllables

Syllable Contact Law SCL p In any sequence C C C ex

a b a

ceeds C in sonority

b

A Proso dic wellformedness

Contour pIf a segment in the onset is  a following segmentmay

not b e 

Contiguity p Given a string X  Y if there is a syllable such

that   are dominated by should b e dominated by

Co dah p h must b e in the onset

FinalDevoice p voiced except in the environment



sonor sonor

Laryngeal

These constraints are memb ers of dierent families The SCL is probably

one of a family of constraints on consonant clusters Similar constraints not

used in this thesis might b e one that demands a rising sonority in syllable

onsets and the government and binding constraints of Kaye Lowenstamm

and Vergnaud Rice and Harris whichhave

as an eect for instance that clusters of consonants share a place feature

or otherwise that one of the two consonants is coronal It mightbethat

a theory of consonantal phonology in the syllable if worked out in the

appropriate way can also accomo date Contour within this family

The constraint Contiguity seems related to the Autosegmental No

Skipping constraint cf section A It might also b e p ossible to reduce

this constraint to an alignment constraint but this will not b e attempted

here Ab out FinalDevoice see Lombardi ab out Codah Ihave

already suggested that it might b e the result of some interaction of weakness

constraints

Although this thesis deals mainly with asp ects of syllable structure

there are quite a lot of syllable structure phenomena ab out which I had

nothing to sayFor instance apart from the SCL I have not intro duced

any constraint dealing with the interaction of sonority place and voicing

features of consonants in clusters Clearly such constraints will b e needed

in a fulledged theory of syllable structure that also wants to explain why

the consonants in an onset have a rising sonority why in clusters of two

consonants normally the consonants agree in place or else one of the twois

coronal etc Another asp ect of syllable structure ab out whichIhave had

very little to say are the asymmetries b etween onset and co da It is unclear

in this theory why some segments can o ccur in onset but not in co das As

I already p ointed out in chapter the pro jection theory should b e seen as

a mo dule in a broader theory of proso dic structure dealing also with these and other asp ects of syllable structure

A Proso dic wellformedness

A Foot wellformedness In addition to constraints on syllable

wellformedness wehave also used three general constraints on the well

formedness of feet

Fo otBin p Fo ot no des are binary branching

Tro chee p Feet are leftheaded

Prokosch Law p The head syllable of a fo ot should b e heavy

Prokosch Law describ es an asp ect of metrical structure that is not other

wise studied in this thesis although it is recurrent in the literature the

fact that heads of proso dic structure tend to b e more complex and longer

than nonheads I refer to the work of Dresher and Van der Hulst for a

more complete discussion

With resp ect to FootBin one could wonder whether this constraint

plays the role of a violable constraint or not All proso dic trees except

mayb e the word tree used in this thesis are binary branching at most also

at the syllable level The same seems to b e true under most recent analyses

for morphological and for syntactic structures suchasKayne and

Chomsky One might therefore supp ose that the binarybranching

nature of Xbar structure is a general prop erty of the linguistic calculus

the Gen function of Optimality theory rather than a violable constraint



The same might b e true for the constraint Trochee According to

Kayne and Chomsky structures of one pro jection are univer

sally leftheaded a while structures of two pro jections are leftheaded

at the lowest level and rightheaded one level up b

a X b X



X Y Y X



X Z

The syllable structures used in this thesis have exactly the same general

form According to the ndings of Hayes tro chaic systems

always consist of one simple pro jection only they either dominate two

moras or two syllables Iambs on the other hand have a complex structure

b ecause their head needs to b e heavy in other words to dominate two

moras

In the analysis on Frenchwehave used the violabilityof FootBin as one of our

analytical to ols weallowed for the creation of degenerate feet



Alternatively one might try to derivethetrochee from a metrical principle requiring

all stressed syllables to b e followed by an unstressed syllable following Hung

A Proso dic wellformedness

a Moraic trochee

F

b Syl labic trochee

F

 

A Proso dic wellformedness

c Iamb

Ft

 

Mo dulo the lab els these structures corresp ond exactly to the general Xbar

structures used for syllable structure here and prop osed for morphological



and syntactic structure in Kayne and Chomsky If this is

true there also might b e no violable tro chee constraint

A Word wellformedness Wehave prop osed the following con

straints to deal with the syntaxphonology and morphologyphonology

interfaces and with proso dic wordwellformedness



AlignWord p AlignPrWd X Y X

Align p Align XYPX

h

AlignFoot p AlignF R PrWd R

FinalC p A Proso dic Word or phonological phrase has to end in

a consonant AlignPrWd R C x

Sux p The elements of a sux should o ccur maximally close to

the right edge of a proso dic word AlignSuffix R PrWd R

Monolog p The edges of a morphological domain should b e crisp

no feature should b e linked b oth to an edge segment of that domain

and to an element outside of the domain

MorPa p At least one element the most sonorous element of a

morpheme is incorp orated into a proso dic word

The rst ve constraints can b e captured within the formal framework

of Generalized Alignment as prop osed by McCarthy and Prince

According to these authors the basic scheme for Alignment constraints is

as follows

def

C E C C suchthatE of C and E AlignC E

   

of C coincide



where C C MCat PCat



E E fLeft Right g



 

MCat the set of morphosyntactic categories fP A N AgrS DP

g

PCat the set of proso dic categories f  Ft PrWd g



In the Xbar theory used here moraic tro chees and iambs are built on top of the

segments dominated by N and syllabic tro chees on top of N no des See Hermans a

for a full development of this kind of theory

A Proso dic wellformedness

Elsewhere Van Oostendorp I have argued that it only makes sense

to refer to left versus right edges in the case of proso dic categories such

as feet and syllables In the case of morphosyntactic categories it do es not

make sense to drawsuch a distinction Although this argument is in fact

immaterial to the discussion in this thesis I nevertheless have omitted the

reference to word edges in the case of AlignWord

Although the constraint Monolog was also supp osed to b e a sp ecial

instance of Alignmentby McCarthy and Prince Ito and Mester

haveshown that this is not in fact the case and that this constraint

which they call Crispness should instead b e allowed to play a separate

role in the grammar

A problem for the denition of Alignmentgiven here is that it refers to

the following structure as wellaligned

A B

a b c

This is true b ecause the right Edge of A can b e b and the left Edge of B

can also b e b In most cases however what wereallywanttohaveisthat

only the following structure counts as wellaligned

A B

a b c d

In order to get this result McCarthy and Prince intro duce the notion

of an optional null string which can b e p osited b etween b and c and which

forms the formal Edge of A and B Yet this still do es not solve the problem

of since null strings are never obligatory Ito and Mesters

notion of Crispness solves the problem in the sense that is assumed to

satisfy Alignment indeed but to violate Crispness I havegiven the name

Monolog to this constraint but actually it is known in graph theory as

the Single Mother Condition

The MorPa constraint nally expresses the intuition that all mor

phemes should participate at least partly in the core proso dic makeup of

aword Wehave seen that this constraintmight b e parametrized for dif

ferent suxes in the sense that the incorp oration of some suxes seems to

b e more urgent than that of others

A Autosegmental Representations

A Autosegmental Representations

Autosegmental phonology is not a topic which is discussed in any detail

in this thesis Still we needed to use several constraints on Autosegmental

representations These I have listed b elow

No Skipping p a feature that is asso ciated to the segments  and

 in the segment string  should also b e asso ciated to 

CF p A cons ro ot do es not dominate a vo calic feature

V

VF p A cons ro ot do es not dominate a consonantal fea

C

ture

CF p A place feature dominated by a cons ro ot no de should

C

b e directly dominated by a Cplace no de

NoLongVowel p Vo calic no des maynotbelinked to more than

one root node

Harmony p Avowel V should have the same sp ecication for

RTR as the vowel W that o ccurs on the left of V if V and W are

in the same proso dic word

We nd one small family of constraints following the general scheme in

a An cons ro ot dominates an  consonantal place feature

b An  consonan tal place feature is dominated byancons ro ot

CF and VF are instantiations of b after transp osition and

V C

under the assumption that cons are mutually exclusive feature values

ConsPlace is an instantiation of a the only other p ossible constraint

would have the following form

VF Avo calic ro ot dominates a vo calic place no de

V

Because all ro ots are either vo calic or consonantal I assume the ma jor

class feature cons cannot b e undersp ecied following Clements

the constraints VF and CF together imply EmptyRootA

V C

vo calic ro ot needs to dominate a vo calic place feature and a consonantal

ro ot a consonantal place no de Empty ro ots are therefore dispreferred we

do not need to rule out emptyrootsby an indep endent constraint

Apart from these constraints relating ro ots with place features wehave

used two other autosegmental constraints the OCP and the No Skipping

constraint Both constraints haveplayed an imp ortant role in autosegmen

tal theory at least since Goldsmith The No Skipping constraint

furthermore seems to b e formally related to the syllable wellformedness

constraint Contiguity

A Feature Co o ccurrence and Licensing

The constraint NoLongVowel althoughusedinchapter do es not

seem to b e strictly necessary in the analysis of Dutch Standard Dutch

do es have long vowels but these are all low We already have a constraint



which has this same eect viz ProjectN low a memb er of the family

of pro jection constraints If this constraintisranked higher than the con

straint against insertion of the feature low it app ears that we do not need

to p osit a separate constraint against long vowels Consider for instance

the tableau for an input q

q



Candidates ProjectN low low

q

p

q

Although this is suggested in the main text of chapter it is not true for

all Dutch idiolects that the long rounded vowels are also lowered This

might b e due to a slightly dierent ranking of the constraint against the

combination lowlabial in the twovariants of Dutch

q DutchA



labial



Candidates ProjectN low low

low

q

p

q

q DutchB



labial



low Candidates ProjectN low

low

p

q

q

This argument do es not imply that crosslinguisti cal ly wewillnever need

a constraint against long vowels cf Rosenthall but the evidence for

such a constraint in the phonology of Dutch is scarce if presentatall

A Feature Co o ccurrence and Licensing

In order to derivethevowel set of Dutchwe had to p osit the following set

of constraints all place features refer to features under the vocalic no de

A Parsing

   

dorsal high high low

dorsallow highcoronal

coronal lax low labial

highlabial labialdorsal lax dorsal lax low lowlax

Vplacevoice Vplacecontinuant

License

All of these constraints are grounded in the sense of Archangeli and Pul

leyblank they are p osited as primitives in phonological theory and

should b e derived from phonetics although of course it might b e p ossible

that in a more fullyworked out phonological theory for instance one

that treats the internal structure of features along the lines of Van der

Hulst c they can b e seen as phonological theorems instead

The status of the constraint against the co o ccurrence of dorsal and

coronal is dep endent on ones assumptions regarding the status of dorsal

in the phonology of vowels It is sometimes claimed that there are reasons

to assume that dorsal is always presentonvowels If this is true this

constraintwould play no role at all

In chapter I referred to the theory of Licensing as put forward by Ito

Mester and Padgett I refer to that pap er for a fuller explication of

this theory

A Parsing

I assume there are twotyp es of faithfulness constraints constraints which

require input prop erties to b e visibly present in the output ParseF

and constraints which require output prop erties to b e arising from input

prop erties The latter will b e discussed in the next section the former will

b e discussed here

The following parse constraints have b een used in this thesis

ParseConsonant A consonantmust b e parsed into syllable structure

ParseVowel Avowel must b e parsed into syllable structure

ParseRo ot A ro ot must b e parsed into syllable structure

ParseHigh ParseTense ParseLabial

ParseLow ParseCoronal ParseDorsal A feature has to b e parsed

into a segment

Faithful Underlying syllable structure may not b e aected

Most of these constraints are plain straightforward parsing constraints I

have assigned the related Fill constraints of Prince and Smolensky

to the family of feature co o ccurence restricti ons highcoronal etc to

the set of constraints on autosegmental structure Emptyroot and to

the family of pro jection constraints

A Constraints against unnecessary structure

Finally there is a constraint Faithful which demands that all input

syllabication is resp ected Assuming that underlying forms do not have

any syllable structure as wehave done throughout this thesis this means

that this particular constraint Faithful can only b e seen to workinthe

p ostlexical phonology

Ihave shown that it is dicult to distinguish b etween ParseV and

the pro jection constraint Projectcons N if vo calic ro ots are parsed

they are usually parsed in the head of a syllable and if they are parsed

outside of such a p osition this could b e attributed to some Parsefeature

constraint The test here would b e gliding of schwa ie wewould need to

nd a language where schwa needs to b e parsed and therefore it surfaces as a

glide for instance h This would provide us with evidence for ParseV

indep endentofProjectcons N

A Constraints against unnecessary structure

One of the rst constraint families prop osed by Prince and Smolensky

was the family of constraints that militate against structure struc

The only memb ers of this family that have b een used here are the con

straints against features and against proso dic no des

low lax velar coronal labial cons cons

These constraints serve to blo ck the unnecessary insertion of features in

this thesis The formulation given here blo cking all o ccurrences of a given

feature in the output is very much dep endent on the assumption that

linguistic derivations satisfy the principle of Containment monotonicity

all elements of the input are present in all candidate outputs This means

that for instance an underlying low feature will always have a violation of

the constraint against low in all candidates This violation will therefore

b e irrelevant The same is not true however for a feature low that is

inserted in the course of the derivation b ecause there will always b e an

alternative derivation in which this feature is not inserted

McCarthy and Prince have prop osed to abandon the principle of

Containment and to allow constraints that explicitly compare inputs and

outputs instead If such a theory turns out to b e more fruitful than the one

based on Containment the formulation of these constraints will havetobe

adapted in suchaway that they ban inserted instances of a given feature

features that are presentinagiven output candidate but not in the input

A Ad Ho c Constraint

A Ad Ho c Constraint

In the course of the analyses we had to have recourse to one constraint

that could not b e motivated in any principled way In the ideal theory of

phonology this constraint will b e replaced by more principled explanations

RColour p CVr inside the fo ot

rColour says that r cannot o ccur after a short vowel inside a fo ot pre

sumably this should b e related to the high sonorityofr in a certain way

but it is not clear to me how this can b e accomplished

B Arguments for ranking

In this app endix I list all the arguments for the constraint ranking in Stan

dard Dutch that have b een collected in this thesis I have limited myself

to this language b ecause it is the most extensively discussed grammar I

assume that it is relatively easy for the interested reader to reconstruct

the arguments for French Norwegian Tilburg Dutchorany of the other

languages discussed here

Ihave limited myself to strictly lo cal ranking arguments That is to say

that if I have found evidence that i AB ii BC and iii AC I

have only listed the arguments for the rankings i and ii Evidence that

AC is often discussed in the main text of this thesis

In App endix C I give the top ologies of the fragments of Standard Dutch

whichhave b een discussed here as well as those for Rotterdam Dutch and

French The items in this app endix have b een listed in alphab etical order

For every ranking I mention a the ranking itself and b an example

comparison of two candidates for some input which is crucially established

by the ranking In this comparison I used the symbol AB meaning is

less harmonic than

In most cases I have only listed the ranking arguments that are abso

lutely clear from the main text This means the ranking argued for here is

only partial The top ologies argued for here will havetobeemb edded in the

complete constraint ranking in the eventual complete Dutch phonological

grammar Where there was no reason to distinguish b etween ProjectX

Y and ProjectY X I hav etaken them together under the name Con

nectX Y

B Top ology of the Dutch lexicon

Align Onset

rodoxrotox ro do

Co dahParselax

mhmah mh

Pro jectFt V Align



katrkkatr katrk

B Top ology of the Dutch lexicon

ConnectFt V Sux



vadrvdr vadrumlaut

ConnectN lax Align

mma m

ConnectN lax Harmony

mbmba mba

ConnectN lax Parselax

mma m

ContourAlign

dmdm dm

ContiguityPro ject N V

r m  prmp rmp

dorsal

Parsedorsal Parsecoronal

coronal

lax dorsal labial dorsalhigh coronal lowdorsal

Dorsal coronal vowels do not exist

FinalDevoice Align

hbhp hb

Fo otBin Align

katrkkatr katrk

GeminateParseC

ttt t

high

low

lax

   

high high

lax lax

i

coronal coronal



high

Parsehigh or Parselow

lo w

 

high high

or

low low

  

high high high

low low low

highcoronal coronal

high i high

highlabial labial



High vowels tend to surface as round

labial

highlabial

coronal

yi i

labial dorsal dorsal

Rounded vowels are normally dorsal

lax MorPa

slptslapt slapt

lax Pro ject N V

mnman man

lax dorsal dorsal

Lax vowels are normally dorsal

low lax low

B Top ology of the Dutch p ostlexical phonology

low rColour



No lexical colouring of vowels b efore r

low

ParseloworParselabial

labial

Low rounded vowels do not surface

lowdorsal dorsal

Lowvowels are normally dorsal

lowlax lax

lowvowels are always lax

N ParseV Onset Pro ject cons

elitqrelitqr

Onset Pro jectN V tt

ParseC MorPa



wr k twrkt wrk t

labial

Parsecoronal Parselabial

coronal

i u y y

ParseF F where Fflax low high coronal dorsal labial g

v v v

All these features normally surface

Parsehighhigh labial labial

 

high high

high

labial labial



labial

Parselabial Parselax

lax

o

low Parselax Parsehigh

   

high high

lax i lax

coronal coronal

   

high high

lax lax

i

coronal coronal



Pro jectN low low

 

high

ii lax

 coronal

low



DutchA Pro jectN low

labial



low



Pro jectN lowDutchB

l abial

qq DutchA

qq DutchB

Pro ject N V ParseC

matkama t ka matka

trltl tl

B Top ology of the Dutch p ostlexical phonology

Align Pro jectV Ft

B Top ology of the Dutch p ostlexical phonology

tftfe tfe

N lax lax Connect

eqrqr er

ContourPro jectV Ft

ltelit elit

 hlashelas

high

Connect Nlax

lax

iqriqr ir

lax dorsal Connect N lax

krlaxkkrk krk



low Pro jectN low

qrqr er

N V Onset Pro ject

aiajai

ParseF F where Fflax low high coronal dorsal labial g

v v v

All these features normally surface

Parsehigh ParselowParselax

Parselabial Parsedorsal Parsecoronal

Reduction hierarchy

Parsehigh Pro jectV Ft

High vowels do not reduce

Pro jectcons NContourOnset

mm m

N cons cons formal style register Pro ject

hlphlp hlp

Pro ject N consSCL

heb jhpj hbj

N V ConnectNlax Pro ject

trtr tr

Pro jectV Ft Pro jectV Ft



Weak syllables reduce more easily than unfo oted syllables

Pro jectV Ft Parsecoronal



Unstressed e is almost always reduced

rColourConnectN lax

iriqr ir

Weak cons N cons Pro jectN cons informal style

OK

tejatr teatr vs hlp hlp

C Ranking schemes

In this app endix I give four graphs representing the four most extensively

discussed grammar fragments of this thesis those of the Standard Dutch

lexical and p ostlexical phonologyofFrench and of Rotterdam Dutch

Ihave not included all undominated constraints or very trivial rankings

The intention of this app endix is merely to lay out the more complicated

transitive ranking arguments

C Top ology of the Standard Dutch lexicon

C Top ology of the Standard Dutch lexicon

C Top ology of the Standard Dutch p ostlexical phonology

C Top ology of the Standard Dutch p ostlexical phonol

ogy

C Top ology of the French phonology

C Top ology of the French phonology

C Top ology of the Rotterdam Dutch phonology

C Top ology of the Rotterdam Dutch phonology

References

Akinlabi A Featural Alignment Manuscript Rutgers University

Anderson J and C Ewen Principles of Dependency Phonology Cam

bridge University Press Cambridge UK

Anderson J and C Jones Three Theses Concerning Phonological Rep

resentations Journal of Linguistics

Anderson S The Analysis of FrenchSchwa or How to Get Something

from Nothing Language

Anderson S AMorphous Morphology Cambridge University Press

Cambridge UK

Angoujard JP and M Denais Le Pluriel Brise en Tigrigna Langues

orientales anciennes Philologie et Linguistique Not consulted

cited in Buckley

Aoun J Is the Syllable or the Sup ersyllabl e a Constituent MIT Work

ing Papers in Linguistics

Archangeli D Underspecication in Yawelmany Phonology and Mor

phology PhD Dissertation MIT Published by Garland Press Outstanding

Dissertations in Linguistics Series

Archangeli D and D Pulleyblan k GroundedPhonology MIT Press

Cambridge Mass

Arono M WordFormation in Generative Grammar MIT Press Cam

bridge Mass

Arono M Morphology by itself Stems and Inectional ClassesMIT

Press Cam bridge Mass Linguistic Inquiry Monograph

Bao Z On the NatureofTone PhD Dissertation MIT

Bagemihl B Syllable Structure in Bella Co ola Linguistic Inquiry

Baker M Incorporation A Theory of Grammatical Function Changing

University of Chicago Press Chicago

Bakker D and G Dibb ets eds GeschiedenisvandeNederlandse

Taalkunde Malmb erg Den Bosch

References

Basbll H Metrical Theory and the FrenchFo ot in W Dressler

and O Pfeier eds Phonologica Akten der Vierten Internationalen

Phonologietagung pp

Basbll H Sur lIdentite Phonologiq ue du SchwaFrancais et son Role

dans lAccentuation et dans la Syllabation in Verluyten

Basbll H DistinctiveFeatures Syllable Structure and Vowel Space

in PM Bertinetto M Kenstowicz and M Loparco eds Certamen Phono

logicum II Papers from the Cortona Phonology MeetingRosenberg

and Sellier Torino pp

BatEl O Onset Violation in Tib erian Hebrew in J Merchant J Pad

gett R Walker eds Phonology in Santa Cruz Distributed by Linguistic

ResearchCenter UCSC pp

Beard R Morpheme Order in a LexemeMorpheme Based Morphology

Lingua

Bennink C in prep Harmony and Disharmony in Hungarian and Turkish

PhD Dissertation Tilburg University

Berendsen E Final Devoicing Assimilation and Sub ject Clitics in

Dutch in H Bennis and WUS van Lessen Klo eke eds Linguistics in

the Netherlands Foris Dordrecht pp

Berendsen E The Phonology of Cliticization PhD Dissertation Univer

sityofUtrecht Also app eared with Foris Dordrech t

Berendsen E and W Zonneveld Nederlandse Schwainvo eging op zn

Deens Spektator

Berg B van den Foniek van het NederlandsVan Go or Zonen Den Haag

Berg B van den Een Fonologische Reductieregel in de Randstad De

Nieuwe Taalgids Also in H Hulshof red Transformationeel

generatieve grammatica in artikelen Tjeenk Willink Groningen

Berhane G Issues in the Phonology and Morphology of TigrinyaPhD

Dissertation UniversiteduQueb ec a Montreal Not consulted cited in

Buckley

Berwick R The Acquisition of Syntactic Know ledge MIT Press Cam

bridge Mass

Bethin C Polish Syl lables Slavica Columbus Ohio

Bo oij G Klinkerrreductie in het Nederlands Leuvense Bijdragen

Bo oij G Dutch Morphology A Study of WordFormation in Generative

GrammarForis Dordrecht

Bo oij G Generatieve Fonologie van het Nederlands Sp ectrum Utrecht

Antwerp en

References

Bo oij G Fonologische en Fonetische Asp ecten van Klinkerreducti e

Spektator

Bo oij G Lexical Phonology Final Devoicing and Sub ject Pronouns in

Dutch in H Bennis and F Beukema eds Linguistics in the Netherlands

Foris Dordrecht pp

Bo oij G On the Representation of Diphthongs in Frisian Journal of

Linguistics

Bo oij G a The Phonology of Dutch Oxford University Press Oxford

Bo oij G b Cliticization as Proso dic Integration the Case of Dutch

Manuscript Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam Toappearin The Linguistic Re

view

Bo oij G and R Lieb er On the Simultaneity of Morphological and

Proso dic Structure in S Hargus and E Kaisse eds Studies in Lexical

Phonology Academic Press San Diego pp Phonetics and Phonol

ogy

Bo oij G and J Rubach Morphological and Proso dic Domains in Lexical

Phonology Phonology

Bo oij G and J Rubach Postcyclic versus Postlexical Rules in Lexical

Phonology Linguistic Inquiry

Borowsky T Topics in the Lexical Phonology of English PhD Disserta

tion University of Massachussetts

Borowski T On the Word Level in S Hargus and E Kaisse eds

pp

Borowsky T J It o A Mester The Formal Representation of Ambisyl

labicity Evidence from Danish NELS

Boutkan D Klinkerwisseli ng in het Tilburgs Taal en Tongval

Boutkan D and M Kossmann Het Stadsdialekt van Tilburg Klank en

Vormleer Manuscript Leiden University

Brink D Problems in Phonology A Generative Phonology of DutchPhD

Dissertation University of Wisconsin

Browman C and L Goldstein Targetless schwa an Articulatory

Analysis in G Do cherty and R Ladd eds Papers in Laboratory Phonol

ogy II Gesture Segment ProsodyCambridge University Press Cam

bridge UK pp

Buckley E On the Status of Vowel Length in Tigrinya Manuscript

UniversityofPennsylvani a

Burzio L Principles of English Stress Cambridge University Press Cam

bridge UK

Calabrese A Metaphony in Salentino Rivista di GrammaticaGenera

tiva

References

Calabrese A Towards a Theory of Phonological PhD Disser

tation MIT

Calabrese A Sievers Law in Gothic A Synchronic Analysis with some

Notes on its Diachronic Development The Linguistic Review

Carrier J The Interaction of Morphological and PhonologicalRules in

Tagalog A Study in the Relationship between Rule Components in Gram

mar PhD Dissertation MIT

Charette M Conditions on phonological government Cambridge Uni

versity Press Cambridge UK

Chierchia G Length Syllabi cati on and the Phonological Cycle in Ital

ian Journal of Italian Linguistics

Chomsky N Remarks on Nominalizati on in R Jacobs and P Rosen

baum eds on Transformational Grammar Ginn Waltham

Mass

Chomsky N A Minimalist Program for Linguistic Theory in K Hale

and SJ Keyser eds The View from Building MIT Press Cambridge

Mass

Chomsky N Bare Phrase Structure Manuscript MIT Cambridge

Mass

Chomsky N The Minimalist Program MIT Press Cambridge Mass

Chomsky N and M Halle The Sound Pattern of English MIT Press

Cambridge Mass

Chung S Transderivational Relationshi ps in Chamorro Language

Cinque G A Null Theory of Phrase and Comp ound Stress Linguistic

Inquiry

Clements N Principles of Tone Asso ciation in Kikuyu in N Clements

and J Goldsmith eds Autosegmental Studies in Bantu ToneForis Dor

drecht pp

Clements N The Geometry of Phonological Features Phonology Year

book

Clements N Towards a Substantive Theory of Feature Sp ecication

NELS GLSA Amherst Mass

Clements N On the Representation of Vowel Height Manuscript Cor

nell University Ithaca New York

Clements N The Role of the Sonority Cycle in Core Syllabica tion

in J Kingston and M Beckman eds Papers in Laboratory Phonology

Between the Grammar and the Physics of Speech Cambridge University

Press Cambridge UK pp

References

Clements N a Vowel Height Assimilatio n in Bantu Languages Working

papers of the Cornel l Phonetics Laboratory Cornell University

Ithaca New York

Clements N b Place of Articulation in Consonants and Vowels A Uni

ed Approach Working Papers of the Cornel l Phonetics Laboratory

Cornell University Ithaca New York

Clements N The Sonority Cycle and Syllable Organization in W

Dressler H Luschutzky O Pfeier and J Rennison eds Phonologica

Proceedings of the th International Phonology Meeting Cambridge

University Press Cambridge UK

Clements N and E Hume The Internal Organization of Sp eech Sounds

in J Goldsmith ed The Handbook of PhonologicalTheory Basil Blackwell

London

Clements N and J Keyser CV Phonology A Generative Theoryofthe

Syl lable The MIT Press Cambridge Mass Linguistic Inquiry Monographs

Clements N and E Sezer Vowel and Consonan t DisharmonyinTurkish

in H van der Hulst and N Smith eds The Structure of Phonological

RepresentationsVolume I I Foris Dordrecht pp

Cohen A Het Nederlandse Diminutiefsux een Morfonologische

Pro eve Nieuwe Taalgids

Cohen A C Eb eling K Fokkema A van Holk Fonologie van het

Nederlands en het Fries Inleiding in de Moderne Klankleer Nijho Den

Haag

Cohn A Stress in Indonesian and Bracketing Paradoxes Natural Lan

guage and Linguistic Theory

Cohn A and J McCarthy Fo ot Alignment and Apparent Cyclicityin

Indonesian Manuscript Cornell Universty and University of Massachus

setts

Davis S Topics in Syl lable Geometry PhD Dissertation Universityof

Arizona Tucson

Davis S Syllable Onsets as a Factor in Stress Rules Phonology

Dechaine RM Stress in Queb ecois Evidence from High Vowels in L

Dobrin L Nichols and R Ro drigues eds Papers from the th Regional

Me eting of the Chicago Linguistic Society CLS Chicago pp

Dell F Les Regles et les Sons Herrmann Paris First edition

second edition

Dell F Consonant Clusters and Phonologica l Syllables in French Lin

gua

Dell F and M Elmedlaoui Syllabic Consonants and Syllabicati on in

Imdlawn Tashliyt Berb er JALL

References

Di Sciullo AM and E Williams On the Denition of Word MIT Press

Cambridge Mass

y

Dixon R Some Phonological Rules of Yidin Linguistic Inquiry

Dogil G La Phonologie p eutelle Renoncer aux Traits Distinctifs de

Classe Superieur in B Laks and A Rialland eds Architecturedes

Representations Phonologiques Editions du CNRS Paris

Donegan J J Miller On the Natural Phonology of Vowels PhD Dis

sertation Ohio State University Published by Garland Press

Dresher E en A Lahiri The Germanic Fo ot Metrical Coherence in Old

English Linguistic Inquiry

Dresher E and H van der Hulst HeadDep endent Asymmetries in

Phonology in H van der Hulst and J van de Weijer eds Leiden in

Last HIL Phonology Papers I Holland Academic Graphics The Hague

pp

Dressler W Allegroregeln Rechtfertigen Lentoregeln Sekundare

des Bretonischen Innsbrucker Beitrage zur Sprachwissenschaft Inns

bruck

Dressler W Metho disches zu AllegroRegeln in W Dressler and F

Mares eds Phonologica pp FinkVerlag Munich

Duanmu S AFormal Study of Syl lable Tone Stress and Domain in

Chinese Languages PhD Dissertation MIT

Dumas D Queb ec French High Vowel Harmony Proccedings of the

Chicago Linguistics Society

Dunlap E Tensing in Lexical Phonology in E Dunlap and J Pad

gett eds UMOP Papers in Phonology GLSA Amherst Mass pp

Dunlap E Issues in the Moraic StructureofSpanish PhD Dissertation

University of Massachussetts

Durand J Generative Phonology Dep endency Phonology and Southern

French Langue et Stile

Durand J French Liaison Floating Segments and Other Matters in a

Dep endency Framework in J Durand ed Dependency and Nonlinear

Phonology London Cro om Helm pp

Durand J Generative and Nonlinear Phonology Longman London

Durand J C Slater and H Wise Observations on Schwa in Southern

French in C Slater J Durand and X Bate eds French Sound Patterns

Changing Perspectives Asso ciation for French Language Studies no

University of Essex Not consulted cited in Durand and Charette

Edgerton F Sieverss La w and IE Weakgrade Vo calism Language

References

Encreve P La Liaison Avec et Sans Enchanement Phonologie Tridi

mensionel le et Usages du Francais Editions du Seuil Paris

Everett D and K Everett On the Relevance of Syllable Onsets to Stress

Placement Linguistic Inquiry

Evers A and R Huybregts The Cycle in Dutch Its Relevance to the

Theory of Grammar in A Kraak ed Linguistics in the Netherlands

Van Gorcum Assen pp

Ewen C The Phonology of the DiminutiveinDutch A Dep endency

Account Lingua

Ferguson C Short a in Philadelphi a English in M Smith ed Studies

in Linguistics in Honour of GL Trager Mouton The Hague pp

Not consulted cited in Harris

Fidelholtz J Word Frequency and Vowel Reduction in English Pro

ceedings of the Chicago Linguistics Society

Fouche P TraitedePrononciation Francaise e Edition Klinksieck

Paris

Fudge E and D Gilb ers On Underlying Vowels Manuscript Groningen

Univ ersity

Giegerich H Metrical Phonology and Phonological Structure Cambridge

University Press Cambridge UK

Giegerich H English Phonology An Introduction Cambridge University

Press Cambridge UK

Gilb ers D Phonological Networks A TheoryofSegment Representation

PhD Dissertation University of Groningen

Ginneken J van De Phonologie van het Algemeen Nederlands Onze

Taaltuin

Goldsmith J Autosegmental and Metrical Phonology Basil Blackwell

Oxford UK and Cambridge Mass

Goldsmith J ed The Last Reections in Constraints

and DerivationsUniversity of Chicago Press Chicago and London Studies

in Contemporary Linguistics

Goldsmith J ed The Handbook of PhonologicalTheory Blackwell

Oxford and Cambridge

Goldsmith J K Peterson and J Progo Tone and Accent in the Xhosa

oaches to Verbal System in P Newman and R Botner eds Current Appr

African Lingusitics Foris Dordrecht pp

Grimshaw J Extended Functional Pro jections Pap er presented at

Utrecht Lexicon Workshop

Gro ot AW de De Wetten der Phonologie en hun Betekenis voor de

Studie van het Nederlands Nieuwe Taalgids

References

Gussenhoven C Het Nederlandse Diminutiefsux Schwainsertie An

ders Bekeken De Nieuwe Taalgids

Gussenhoven C Cliticization in Dutch as Phonological Word Forma

tion Journal of Linguistics

Gussmann E Resyllabicati on and Delinking Linguistic Inquiry

Haas W de Partial Syllabica tion and SchwaEpenthesis in Dutch

Gramma

Haas W de and M Trommelen Morfologisch Handboek van het Ned

erlands Een Studie over Verschil lende Vormen van WoordformatieSDU

Den Haag Aan het Woord

Haeringen C van Iets over het Vo orvo egsel in het Verleden Deelwoord

Driemaandelijkse Bladen

Haeringen C van Herverfransing Mededelingen der Koninklijke

Akademie van Wetenschappen Nieuwe reeks KNAW Amster

dam Also app eared in Gramarie

Haeringen C van De Plaats van ie o e en uu in het Nederlandse Klink

erstelsel in GramarieVan Gorkum Assen pp

Hall T German Syllabi catio n the Velar Nasal and the Representation

of Schwa Linguistics

Hall T Syl lable Structure and Syl lablerelatedProcesses in German

Niemeyer Tubingen

Halle M On DistinctiveFeatures and their Articulatory Implementa

tion Natural Language and Linguistic Theory

Halle M Resp ecting Metrical Structure Natural Language and Lin

guistic Theory

Halle M Rules or Constraints The Case of Eastern Massachussetts

r Manuscript MIT

Halle M and N Clements AProblem Book in Phonology MIT Press Cam

bridge Mass

Halle M and M Kenstowicz The Free Element Condition and Cyclic

versus Noncyclic Stress Linguistic Inquiry

Halle M and KP Mohanan Segmental Phonology of Mo dern English

Linguistic Inquiry

Halle M and K Stevens On the Feature Advanced Tongue Ro ot

Quarterly Progress Report Research Lab oratory of Electronics

Cambridge Mass

Halle M and K Stevens A Note on Laryngeal Features Quarterly

Progress Reports Research Lab of Electronics MIT pp

References

Halle M and JR Vergnaud An Essay on Stress MIT Press Cambridge

Mass

Hargus S and E Kaisse eds Studies in Lexical Phonology Academic

Press New York etc Phonetics and Phonology series editors S An

derson and P Keating

Harris J James MIT Press Cambridge Mass

Harris J James Integrity of Proso dic Constituents and the Domain of

Syllabicati on in Spanish and Catalan in K Hale and SJ Keyser eds

The View from Building MIT Press Cambridge Mass pp

Harris J John Towards a Lexical Analysis of a in

Progress Journal of Linguistics

Harris J John Segmental Complexity and Phonological Government

Phonology

Harris J John English Sound Structure Blackwell Oxford

Hart J t Fonetische Steunpunten De Nieuwe Taalgids

Hayes B a The Proso dic HierarchyinMeterinP Kiparsky and G

Youmans eds Rhythm and Meter Academic Press Orlando

Hayes B b Comp ensatory Lengthening in Moraic Phonology Linguistic

Inquiry

Hayes B Diphthongizatio n and Coindexing Phonology

Hayes B Metrical Stress Theory Principles and Case Studies Manu

script UCLA

Haugen E NorwegianEnglish Dictionary a Pronouncing and

Translation Dictionary of Bokmaal and

Bergen etc Universitetsforlaget First Edition Consulted d Edi

tion

Hawkins W Patterns of Vowel loss in Macushi Carib International

Journal of American Linguistics

Hawkins J and A Cutler Psycholinguis tic Factors in Morphological

Asymmetry in J Hawkins ed Explaining Language Universals Basil

Blackwell London

Heeroma K De Plaats van Ie Uu Oe in het Nederlandse Klinkersys

teem Nieuwe Taalgids

Helten W van Middelnederlandsche Spraakkunst Groningen

Helten W van Die Altniederfrankischen Psalmenfragmente die Lip

siusschen Glossen und die Altsudmittelfrankischen Psalmenfragmente

Groningen New York

Hermans B On the Representation of Quasilong Vowels in Dutch and

Limburgian in R BokBennema en R van Hout eds Linguistics in the

Netherlands John Benjamins Amsterdam

References

Hermans B a DutchSchwa Manuscript Tilburg University

Hermans B b Het Onderscheid tussen Zware en Lichte Lettergrep en in

het Nederlands Spektator

Hermans B a The Composite NatureofAccent With Case Studies of

the Limburgian and SerboCroatian Pitch Accent PhD Dissertation Free

University Amsterdam

Hermans B b Palatalisatie in een Limburgs Dialect Dissimilati e als

Gevolg van de OCP in G Bo oij and J van Marle eds Dialectfonologie

PJ Meertensinstituut Amsterdam

Hewitt M Vertical Maximization and Metrical Theory PhD Dissertation

Brandeis University

Ho eksema J Wortels Stammen en de Schwa TABU

Ho op er J J Byb ee An Introduction to Natural Generative Phonology

Academic Press New York

Hout R van De Structuur van Taalvariatie Een Sociolingustisch On

derzoek naar het Stadssialect van Nijmegen PhD dissertation Tilburg Uni

versity

Human M and R Krakow eds Nasals and the Velum

Academic Press San Diego etc pp Phonetics and Phonology

Series editors S Anderson and P Keating

Hulshof H ed TransformationeelgeneratieveGrammaticainArtikelen

TjeenkWillink Groningen

Hulst H van der Syl lable Structure and Stress in DutchForis Dordrecht

Hulst H van der Ambisyllabici ty in Dutch in H Bennis and F

Beukema eds Linguistics in the Netherlands Foris Dordrecht pp

Hulst H van der a Radical CV Phonology Manuscript Leiden Univer

sity

Hulst H van der b Core Syllables in Dutch handout of talk presented

in Tilburg

Hulst H van der c An Intro duction to Radical CV Phonology in

S Shore and M Vilkuna eds Suomen Kielitieteel lisen YhdistykenVu

oskirja Yearb o ok of the Linguistic Asso ciation of Finland SKY

Helsinki

Hulst H van der and J van Lit Lettergreepstructuur GLOT

Hulst H van der and N Smith On Neutral Vowels in K Bogers

H van der Hulst and M Mous eds The Phonological Representationof

ht pp SuprasegmentalsForis Dordrec

References

Hulst H van der and N Smith The Varieties of Pitch Accent Systems

in H van der Hulst and N Smith eds Autosegmental Studies on Pitch

AccentForis Dordrecht pp

Hulst H van der and J van de Weijer Vowel Harmony in J Goldsmith

ed pp

Hume E Front Vowels Coronal Consonants and their Interaction in

Nonlinear Phonology PhD Dissertation Cornell University

Hume E and D Odden Against consonantal Talk presented at IWPS

Durham

Hung H The Rhythmic and Prosodic Organisation of Edge Constituents

PhD Dissertation Brandeis University

Hyman L A Theory of PhonologicalWeightForis Dordrecht

Inkelas S and D Zec eds The PhonologySyntax ConnectionUniversity

of Chicago Press Chicago

Ito J Syl lable Theory in Prosodic Phonology PhD dissertation Uni

versity of Massachussetts at Amherst App eared in Garland Outstanding

Dissertations in Linguistics Series

Ito J and A Mester The Phonology of Voicing in Japanese Linguistic

Inquiry

Ito J and A Mester Alignment and Crisp Edges Talk presented in

OTS Workshop on Proso dic Morphology

Ito J A Mester and J Padgett NC Licensing and Undersp ecicatio n

in Optimality Theory Manuscript University of California Santa Cruz

Jacobs H Nonlinear Studies in the Historical Phonology of FrenchPhD

Dissertation Katholieke Universiteit Nijmegen

Jakobson R Observations sur le Classement Phonologiqu e des Con

sonnes in N Ruwet ed Essais de LinguistiqueGeneralevol Editions

de Minuit Paris

Jakobson R Kindersprache Aphasie und Allgemeine Lautgesetze in

Selected writings volume I Mouton The Hague pp

Johnsen L J KayeandJRVergnaud Raddoppiamento Sintattico

Norwegian Style Handout AVH Seminar Trondheim

Jong D de The Sociolinguistic Aspects of French Liaison PhD Disser

tation Free University of Amsterdam

Jong D de and M Hietbrink The Morphonology of the French Prex

RE in R BokBennema and C Cremers eds Linguistics in the Nether

lands Amsterdam John Benjamins pp

Kaisse E ConnectedSpeech Academic Press Orlando

Kager R A Metrical Theory of Stress and Destressing in English and

DutchForis Dordrecht

References

Kager R DutchSchwa in Moraic Phonology in M Ziolkowski M

Noske and K Deaton eds Papers from the th Regional Meeting of the

Chicago Linguistic Society Chicago CLS Volume I I The parasesssion on

the syl lable in phonetics and phonology pp

Kager R a German Quantity Class notes Universitat Salzburg Oc

tob er

Kager R b Generalized Alignment and Morphological Parsing Talk

given at the OTS Robustness Conference Utrecht Octob er

Kager R Consequences of Catalexis in H van der Hulst and J van de

Weijer eds Leiden in Last HIL Phonology Papers I Holland Academic

Graphics The Hague pp

Kager R E Visch and W Zonneveld Nederlandse Wo ordklemto on

Ho ofdklemto on Bijklemto on Reductie Voeten GLOT

Kager R and W Zonneveld Schwa Syllables and Extrametricalityin

Dutch The Linguistic Review

Kaisse E ConnectedSpeech Orlando Press New York

Katz D AGrammar of the Yiddish Language Duckworth London

Kaye J Lexical Phonology and Phonological Phenomena In W

Dressler H Luschutzky O Pfeier and J Rennison eds Phonologica

Cambridge University Press Cambridge pp

Kaye J and J Lowenstamm De la Syllabici te in F Dell D Hirst JR

Vergnaud eds Forme SonoreduLangage Hermann Paris pp

Kaye J J Lowenstamm and JR Vergnaud The Internal Structure of

Phonological Elements A Theory of Charm and Government Phonology

Yearbook

Kaye J J Lowenstamm and JR Vergnaud Constituent Structure and

Government in Phonology Phonology

Kayne R The Antisymmetry of Syntax Manuscript CUNY

Keating P Undersp ecication in Phonetics Phonology

Kenstowicz M a Phonology in Generative Grammar Basil Blackwell

London

Kenstowicz M b SonorityDriven Stress Manuscript MIT

Kenstowicz M and Ch Kisseb erth Generative Phonology Description

and Theory Academic Press New York

Keymeulen L and J Taeldeman Tussen Fonologie en Morfologie de

TaalenTongval Vokaalverkorting in een Brabants Dialekt

KiparskyP Remarks on the Metrical Structure of the Syllable in W

Dressler O Pfeier and J Rennison eds Phonologica Akten der

Vierten Internationalen Phonologietagung pp

References

KiparskyP From Cyclic Phonology to Lexical Phonology in H van der

Hulst and N Smith eds The Structure of PhonologicalRepresentations

Part I Foris Dordrecht pp

KiparskyP Some Consequences of Lexical Phonology Phonology Year

book

KiparskyP Catalexis Manuscript Stanford University

Ko opmansVan Beinum F Vowel Contrast Reduction AnAcoustic and

Perceptual Study of Dutch Vowels in Various Speech Conditions Academis

che Pers Amsterdam

Ko opmansVan Beinum F Ako estische en Perceptieve Asp ecten van

Klinkercontrastreducti e en de Rol van de Fonologie Spektator

Ko opmansvan Beinum FJ Whats in a Schwa IFAProceedings

Also app eared in Phonetics

Krejnovich E F onetika Nivskogo Jazyka Leningrad

Kristoerson G The Syl lable Structure of Norwegian Do ctoral Disser

tation Troms University

Kruisinga E De Vorm van de Verkleinwo orden Nieuwe Taalgids

Lab ov W Principles of Linguistic Change Volume I Internal Factors

Blackwell Oxford UK and Cambridge Mass Language in Society

Lahiri A and V Evers Palatalisati on and Coronality in C Paradis

and JF Prunet eds The Special Status of Coronals Academic Press

San Diego pp

Lahiri A en H van der Hulst On Foot Typ ology NELS

Lahiri A and J Koreman Syllable Weight and QuantityinDutch

Proceedings of the West Coast ConferenceonFormal Linguistics

Langeweg S The Stress System of Dutch PhD dissertation University

of Leiden

Lass R English Phonology and PhonologicalTheory Synchronic and

Diachronic StudiesCambridge University Press Cambridge Cambridge

studies in linguistics

Lev elt C On the Acquisition of Place PhD Dissertation Leiden

Levin J A Metrical Theory of Syl labicity PhD Dissertation MIT

Lieb er R An IntegratedTheoryofAutosegmental ProcessesSUNY

Press Albany SUNY Series in Linguistics Mark Arono Editor

Lindau M The Feature Expanded Journal of Phonetics

Lit J van A Structural Condition on Autosegmental Licensing in P

Branigan J Gaulding M Kub o and K Murasuji eds SCIL

MIT Working Pap ers in Linguistics Cambridge Mass

References

Lo dge K French Phonology Again Deletion Phenomena Syllable

Structure and Other Matters in J Anderson and J Durand eds Ex

plorations in Dependency PhonologyForis Dordrecht pp

Lombardi L Laryngeal Features and Laryngeal Neutralization PhD Dis

sertation University of Massachussetts at Amherst

Martin A Klinker reductie Een Casus Manuscript Universiteit

Utrecht

Mascaro J Vowel Reduction as Deletion Handout Going Romance

Conference Utrecht

McCarthy J Formal Problems in Semitic Phonology and Morphology

Do ctoral Dissertation MIT Published by Garland Press New York

McCarthyJFeature Geometry and Dep endency A Review Phonetica

McCarthy a LInxation Reduplicative dans les Langages Secrets Lan

gages Sp ecial issue on Les javanais edited byMPlenat

McCarthy J b The Phonology of Pharyngeal Articulations in Semitic

Pap er presented at the rd Conference on Lab oratory Phonology UCLA

Los Angeles

McCarthy J c Synchronic Rule Inversion BLS

McCarthy J and A Prince Proso dic Morphology Manuscript Amherst

Brandeis

McCarthy J and A Prince QuantitativeTransfer in Reduplicati ve and

Templatic Morphology in SeukDik Kim ed Linguistics in the morning

calm Hanshin Seoul pp

McCarthy J and A Prince Foot and Word in Proso dic Morphology

The Case of the Arabic Broken Plural NLLT

McCarthy J and A Prince a Proso dic Morphology I Manuscript

Amherst Rutgers

McCarthy J and A Prince b Generalized Alignment in G Bo oij and

J van Marle eds Yearbook of Morphology Kluwer Dordrecht

McCarthy J and A Prince a The Emergence of the Unmarked Opti

mality in Proso dic Morphology NELS

McCarthy J and A Prince b Proso dic Morphology An Overview Talk

presented at Proso dic Morphology Workshop

McCarthy J and A Taub Review of C Paradis and JF Prunet eds

The Sp ecial Status of Coronals Phonology

Melvold J Structure and Stress in the Phonology of Russian PhD Dis

sertation MIT

Mester A and J Ito Feature Predictability and Undersp ecication

Palatal Proso dy in Japanese Mimetics Language

References

Michaels D Natural and Unnatural Phonology in W Dresher H

Luschutzky O Pfeier and J Rennison eds Phonologica Cambridge

University Press Cambridge pp

Mohanan KP The TheoryofLexical Phonology Reidel Dordrecht

Mohanan KP On the Bases of Radical Undersp ecicatio n NLLT

Mohanan KP Fields of Attraction in Phonology in J Goldsmith

Mohanan T Syllable Structure in Malayalam Linguistic Inquiry

Mo ortgat M and H van der Hulst Leeft het Nederlands op Grote Vo et

Manuscript Leiden University

Morin Y The Status of Mute e Studies in French Linguistics

Morin Y Crosssyllabic Constraints and the French emuet Journal

of Linguistic Research

Morin Y De lAjustementduChevaenSyllabeFerme dans la Phonolo

gie du Francais in Verluyten

Mors C ter Een Vo caalinsertieparameter Of Waarom Nederlanders

Geen Indianen Zijn TABU

Moulton W The Vowels of Dutch Phonetic and Distributiona l Classes

Lingua

ctionsForis Muysken P and H van Riemsdijk eds Features and Proje

Dordrecht Studies in Generative Grammar

Myers S Vowel Shortening in English NLLT

Nesp or M and I Vogel Prosodic PhonologyForis Dordrecht

Neyt A and W Zonneveld Metrische Fonologie De Representatie van

Klemto on in Nederlandse Monomorfematische Wo orden De Nieuwe Taal

gids

Nijen Twilhaar J Generatieve Fonologie en de Studie van Oostneder

landse Dialecten Generative Phonology and the Study of Eastern Dutch

Dialects PJ MeertensInstituut Amsterdam

Nijen Twilhaar J Genus en Fonologisch Gedrag van Nomina op Schwa

in G Bo oij and J van Marle eds DialectfonologiePJ Meertensinstituut

Amsterdam

No oteb o om S Some Regularities in Phonemic Sp eech Errors IPO An

nual progress report

No oteb o om S Production and Perception of Vowel Duration A Study

of Durational Properties of Vowels in Dutch PhD Dissertation Rijksuni

versiteit Utrecht

References

Noske R Syllabica tion and Syllabl e Changing Rules in French in H

van der Hulst and N Smith eds The Structure of Phonological Represen

tations Part IIForis Dordrecht

Noske R La Syllabi catio n et les Regles de Changement de Syllab e en

Francais in Verluyten

Noske R ATheory of Syl labication and Segmental Alternation With

Studies on the Phonology of French German Tonkawa and Yawelmani

PhD Dissertation Tilburg University

Nouveau D Language Acquisition Metrical Theory and Optimality A

Study of Dutch WordStress PhD Dissertation OTS Utrecht

Noyer R Mobile Axes in Huave Optimality and Morphologica l Well

formedness in E Duncan M Hart and P Spaelti eds Proceedings of

WCCFL University of California Santa Cruz

Noyer R Optimal Words Towards a Declarative Theory of Word

Formation Manuscript Princeton

Nuyts J Het Antwerpse Vokaalsysteem Een Synchronische en Diac hro

nische Schets Taal en Tongval pp

Odden D Vowel Geometry Phonology

Odden D Tone African Languages in J Goldsmith pp

Odden D and M Odden Ordered Reduplicati on in Khehe Linguistic

Inquiry pp

Oostendorp M van Formal Prop erties of Metrical Structure in S

Krauwer M Mo ortgat and L des Tomb e eds Proceedings of the Sixth

ConferenceoftheEuropean Chapter of the Association for Computational

Linguistics OTS Utrecht pp

Oostendorp M van Axation and Syllable Integrity in Dutch in R

BokBennema and C Cremers eds Linguistics in the Netherlands

John Benjamins Amsterdam

Oudenaarden J Wat Zeggie Azzie Val dan Leggie Een Speurtocht

naar het Dialect van RotterdamContact Utrecht

Overdiep G Moderne NederlandscheGrammatica TjeenkWillink

Zwolle

Padgett J StrictureinFeatureGeometry PhD Dissertation University

of Massachussetts at Amherst

adgett J Stricture and Nasal Place Assimilation Natural Language P

and Linguistic Theory

Padgett J Feature Classes Manuscript to app ear in University of Mas

sachussetts Working Papers

References

Painter C Cineradiographi c Data on the Feature Covered in Twi

Vowel Harmony Phonetica

Paradis C and JF Prunet eds The Special Status of CoronalsAca

demic Press San Diego

Paradis C and JF Prunet A Note on Velar Nasals The Case of

Uradhi Canadian Journal of LinguisticsRevue canadienne de linguistique

Peterson K A Comparative Lo ok at Nguni Verbal Tone in I Hak

and L Tuller eds Current Approaches to African Linguistics Foris

Dordrecht pp

Philips C Are Feature Hierarchies Autosegmental Hierarchies in A

Carnie H Harley T Bures eds Papers on Phonology and Morphology

MIT Working Papers in Linguistics MITWPCambridge Mass

Polgardi K Derived Environment Eects in Optimality Theory

Manuscript Leiden University

Pollo ck JY Verb Movement Universal Grammar and the Structure

of IP Linguistic Inquiry

Prince A Improving Tree Theory Proce edings of the Eleventh Annual

Meeting of the Berkeley Linguistcs SocietyBerkeley BLS Universityof

California pp

Prince A Quantitative Consequences of Rhythmic Organisation in K

Deaton M Noske and M ZiolkowskyedsCLS Volume I I Papers from

the Parasession on the Syl lable in Phonetics and Phonology CLS Chicago

Prince A and P Smolensky Optimality TheoryManuscript Rutgers and

Colorado

Prokosch E AComparative Germanic Grammar Linguistic so cietyof

America UniversityofPennsylvania Philadel phi a

Prunet JF Spreading and Locality Domains in Phonology PhD Dis

sertation McGill University

Pulgram E Syl lable Word Nexus Cursus Mouton The HagueParis

Janua Linguarum Studia Memoria Nicolai van Wijk Dedicata Series

Minor

Pulleyblank D Vo calic Undersp ecication in Yoruba Linguistic In

quiry

Ramers KH Vokalquantitat and qualitat im Deutschen Niemeyer

Tubingen

Redenb erger W Lusitian Portuguese A is ATR and CP in

Studies in Romance Linguistics Proceedings of the MPHawigara ed

Fifth Symposium on Romance Linguistics Newbury House Rowley Mass

Not consulted cited by Donegan

References

Riad Tomas Structures in Germanic Prosody A Diachronic Study with

Special Reference to the Nordic Languages PhD Dissertation Sto ckholm

University

Rialland A Schwa et Syllab es en Francais in L Wetzels and E Sezer

eds Studies in Foris Dordrecht

Rice C Stress AssignmentintheChugach Dialect of Alutiiq CLS

Papers from the General Session pp

Rice C Proso dic Trapping and Sievers Law Manuscript Universityof

Troms

Rice K On Deriving Sonority A Structural Account of SonorityRela

tionships Phonology

Rijen H van Men Tilborgs Woordeboek Gianotten Tilburg

Rijk R de Aprop os of the DutchVowel System Manuscript MIT

Robinson O Synchronic Reexes of Diachronic PhonologicalRulesPhD

dissertation Cornell University

Ro ca I Generative Phonology Routledge London

Rogier LJ Aantekeningen en Correcties op de Rotterdamse Dialect

pro even Tijdschrift vor TaalenLetteren

Rosenthall S VowelGlide Interaction in a Theory of Constraint Inter

action PhD Dissertation University of Massachussetts at Amherst

Rotenb erg J The Syntax of Phonology PhD Dissertation MIT

Rowicka G Polish Palatal Assimilation in Prexed Words in R Bok

Bennema and C Cremers eds Linguistics in the Netherlands John

Benjamins Amsterdam

Rowicka G and J van de Weijer Proso dic Minimality Constraints in

the Lexicon of Polish The Case of Derived Imp erfectives The Linguistic

Review

Rubach J Abstract Vowels in Threedimensional Phonology the Yers

The Linguistic Review

Sagey E The Representation of Features and Relations in Nonlinear

Phonology Garland New York

Schane S The Phonological and Morphological StructureofFrenchPhD

Dissertation MIT

Schane S On the Abstract Character of FrenchEmuet Glossa

Schane S There is no FrenchTruncation Rule in R Campb ell et al

eds Linguistic Studies in R omanceLanguages Georgetown University

Press Washington DC pp

Schlindwein D The PhonologicalGeometry of Morpheme Concatenation

PhD dissertation University of Southern California

References

Schultink H Boundaries Word Classes and the Accentuation of Derived

Words in Dutch in W Zonneveld et aledsStudies in Dutch Phonology

Martinus Nijho The Hague pp

Schutter G de Asp ekten van de Nederlandse Klankstruktuur Antwerp

Papers in Linguistics

Selkirk E The Phrase Phonology of English and French PhD Disserta

tion MIT Published by Garland Press

Selkirk E Comments on Morins Pap er The FrenchFo ot On the

Status of Mute E Studies in French Linguistics

Selkirk E The Role of Proso dic Categories in English Word Stress

Linguistic Inquiry

Selkirk E a The Syntax of Words MIT Press Cambridge Mass

Selkirk E b The Syllable in H van der Hulst and N Smith eds The

Structure of Phonological RepresentationsForis Dordrecht

Selkirk E Phonology and Syntax MIT Press Cambridge Mass

Selkirk E On Derived Domains in Sentence Phonology Phonology

Yearbook

Selkirk E Dep endency Place and the Notion Tier Manuscript Uni

versity of Massachussetts

Selkirk E On the Inalterabili ty of Geminates in PM Bertinetto M

Kenstowicz and M Loparco eds Certamen Phonologicum II Papers from

the Cortona Phonology Meeting Rosenb erg and Sellier Torino pp

Selkirk E A Tworo ot Theory of Length Manuscript Universityof

Massachussetts at Amherst

Siegel D Topics in English Morphology PhD dissertation MIT

Siegel D The Adjacency Condition and the theory of Morphology

NELS

Sievers E Zur Accent und Lautlehre der Germanischen Sprachen

Beitrage zur Geschichte der Deutschen Sprache und Literatur

Sievers E Grundzuge der Phonetik Breitkopf und Kartel Wiesbaden

th edition

Slis I and M van Heugten Voicedvoiceless Distinction in DutchFrica

tives in H Bennis and A van Kemenade eds Linguistics in the Nether

lands Foris Dordrecht pp

Slo otweg AM and J Wester Nederlandse Wo ordklemto on Cijfers en

Structuren Gramma

Sluyters W Representing Diphthongs PhD Dissertation Nijmegen Uni

versity

References

Smith N R Bolognesi F van der Leeuw J Rutten and H de Leeuw

Aprop os of the DutchVowel System Years On in H Bennis and A

van Kemenade eds Linguistics in the Netherlands Foris Dordrecht

Spa J GeneratieveFonologie Levende talen Also in Hul

shof pp

Sp encer A A Nonlinear Analysis of VowelZero Alternations in Polish

Journal of Linguistics

Sproat R On Deriving the Lexicon PhD dissertation MIT

Stanley R Redundancy Rules in Phonology Language

Stemb erger J Glottal Transparancy Phonology

Steriade D RedundantValues in A Bosch B Need and E Schiller

eds CLS Parasession on Autosegmental and Metrical Phonology

Chicago CLS pp

Steriade D Reduplication and Syllabl e Transfer in Sanskrit and Else

where Phonology

Steriade D Closure Release and Nasal Contours in M Human and

R Krakow eds

Steriade D Undersp ecication and Markedness in J Goldsmith ed

pp

Stewart J Tongue Ro ot Positions in Akan Vowel Harmony Phonetica

Stro op J Metathesis van s en p Spektator

Stro op J Afgedwongen Nasalisering Tijdschrift voor Nederlandse Taal

en Letterkunde

Szpyra J Ghost Segments in Nonlinear Phonology Polish Yers Lan

guage

Takahashi T ConstraintInteraction in Aranda Stress in J Harris ed

UCL Working Papers in Linguistics

Trager GL and HL Smith An Outline of English Structure Battenburg

Press Norman Oklahoma Studies in linguisti cs Occasional pap ers

Tranel B Concreteness in Generative PhonologyUniversity of California

Press Berkeley

Tranel B Floating Schwas and Closed Syllable AdjustmentinFrench

in W Dressler et al eds Phonologica Cambridge University Press

Cambridge

Tranel B FrenchSchwa and Nonlinear Phonology Linguistics

Tranel B A Prop os de lAjustementdeE enFrancais in Verluyten pp

References

Tranel B French Liaison and Elision Revisited A Unied Account

within Optimality Theory Manuscript University of California Irvine

Trigo Ferre R L On the Phonological Derivation and Behavior of Nasal

Glides PhD Dissertation MIT

Trommelen M The Syl lable in Dutch With Special Reference to Diminu

tive FormationForis Dordrecht

Trommelen M On ie uu and oe DutchVowel Length revisited in

G de Haan and W Zonneveld eds Formal Parameters of Generative

GrammarOTS Yearbook III OTS Utrecht pp

Trommelen M Lexical WordPro cesses in Dutch Linguistic Review

Trommelen M and W Zonneveld Stress Diphthongs r in Dutch in

H Bennis and A van Kemenade eds Linguistics in the Netherlands

Foris Dordrecht

Trommelen M and W Zonneveld Klemtoon en Metrische Fonologie

Coutinho Muiderb erg

Trub etzkoy N Grundzuge der Phonologie Travaux du Cercle Linguis

tique de Prague Praag

Uriagereka J GovernmentFunctions and the Verb

Paradigm Handout of the th GLOW Collo quium

Vago R The TreatmentofLongVowels in Word Games Phonology

Yearbook

Vagra M Phonetic Explanations in Phonology the Case for Glottal

Stops in Italian Wordnal Stressed Syllables Manuscript Scuola Normale

Sup eriore Pisa Not consulted cited in Burzio

Vater H Zum Silb enNukl eus im Deutschen in P Eisenberg K

Ramers H Vater eds Silbenphonologie des DeutschenGunter Narr Ver

lag Tubingen pp

Velde H Van der A Trend Study of a Trendy Change Manuscript

Nijmegen University

Vennemann T The Phonology of Gothic Vowels Language

Vennemann T PreferenceLaws for Syl lable Structure De Gruyter

Berlin

Vennemann T Syllable Structure and Syllable Cut Proso dies in Mo dern

Standard German in P M Bertinetto and M Lop orcaro eds Certamen

PhonologicumProceedings of the Cortona Phonology Conference Rosen

b erg and Sellier Torino

Verdam J Uit de Geschiedenis der NederlandscheTaal Thieme Zut phen

References

Vergnaud JR On the Foundations of Phonology Pap er presented at

the th GLOW Collo quium Not consulted cited in Charette

Verluyten S La Structure Proso dique des Mots Francais Hierarchique

ou Lineaire in K van den Eynde M Dominicy and S Verluyten eds

Linguistics in Belgium Univeristeit van Antwerp en Antwerp en Antwerp

Papers in Linguistics

Verluyten S ed La Phonologie du Schwa Francais John Benjamins

Amsterdam

Visch E A Metrical Theory of Rhythmic Stress Phenomena PhD Dis

sertation Utrecht University

Visser W Schwaapp endixen in het Fries in G Bo oij and J van Marle

eds DialectfonologiePJ Meertensinstituut Amsterdam

Vogel I Phonological interfaces in Italian in M Mazzola ed Proceed

ings of Linguistic Studies on RomanceLanguages XXIII

Vlugt M van der and S No oteb o om Auditory Word Recognition is not

more SensitivetoWordInitial than to WordFinal Stimulus Information

IPO Annual Progress R eport

Vries J de et al De Slott in de Consonantclusters te Leiden een So ci

olingusti sch Onderzo ek Forum der Letteren

Wennerstrom A Fo cus on the Prex Evidence for WordInternal

Proso dic Words Phonology

Weijer J van de Basque Aricates and the MannerPlace dep endency

Lingua

Weijer J van de Segmental Structure and Complex Segments PhD Dis

sertation Leiden University

Weijnen A Verklaren van Dialectverschijnsel en Text of lecture Ni

jmegen University

Weijnen A Vergelijkende Klankleer der Nederlandse Dialecten Staats

drukkerijuitgeveri j Den Haag Aan het Woord

Wetzels L The Timing of Latent Consonants in Mo dern French in C

Neidle and R Nunez Cedeno eds Studies in Romance LinguisticsForis

Dordrecht pp

Wetzels L Umlaut en Verkleinwo ordvorming in het Schinnens Gramma

W etzels L Possible and Imp ossible Assimilation Pro cesses The Place of

the Feature continuant in the Feature Tree Manuscript Free University

of Amsterdam

Wiese R Schwa and the Structure of Words in German Linguistics

References

Wiese R Silbische und Lexikalische Phonologie Studien zum Chinesis

chen und Deutschen Niemeyer Tubingen

Wijk N van De Nederlandsche Taal TjeenkWillin k Zwolle

Wijk N van Phonologie Een Hoofdstuk uit de Structurele Taalweten

schap Nijho Den Haag

WildeVan Buul G de Het Enclitisch Pronomen Personale van de e en

e Perso on Singulari s in het Rotterdams Tijdschrift voor Nederlandsche

taal en letterkunde

Williams E On the Notions Lexically Related and Head of a Word

Linguistic Inquiry

Winteler J C Die Kerenzer Mundart des Kantons Glarus in ihren

Grundzugen Dargestel lt Carl Winter Heidelb erg

Yip M English Vowel Ep enthesis Natural Language and Linguistic

Theory

Yip M The Tonal Phonology of Chinese PhD Dissertation MIT

Yip M Tone in East Asian Languages in J Goldsmith pp

Zonneveld R van Axgrammatika Een Onderzoek naar Woordvorming

in het Nederlands PhD Dissertation Rijksuniversitei t Groningen

Zonneveld R van Word Rhythm and the Janus Syllable in H van der

Hulst and N Smith eds The Structure of Phonological Representations

II Foris Dordrecht

Zonneveld W Fonologische Polariteit en de Vlaams Brusselse Vokaalver

schuiving Leuvense Bijdragen

Zonneveld W AFormal TheoryofExceptions in Generative Phonology

The Peter de Ridder Press Lisse

Zonneveld W The DescriptivePower of the Dutch ThemeVowel Spek

tator

Zonneveld W Schwa Sup erheavies Stress and Syllabl es in Dutch The

Linguistic Review

Zonneveld W and M Trommelen Inleiding tot de Generatieve Fonologie

Coutinho Muiderb erg

Zubizaretta ML Vowel Harmony in Andalusian Spanish MIT Work

ing Papers in Linguistics

Zwart CJW Dutch Syntax A Minimalist Approach PhD Dissertation

University of Groningen

Summary in Dutch

Klinkerkwali tei t en

lettergreeppro jectie

In dit pro efschrift wordt de ho ofdigheidshyp othese vo or lettergrep e n nader

uitgewerkt Met name wordt b estudeerd in ho everre fonologische ken

merken van prototypische lettergreepho ofden dat wil zeggen klinkers van

invlo ed zijn op de structuur van de proso die rondom het ho ofd Centraal

in de discussie staan het StandaardNederlands en een aantal Nederlandse

dialecten zoals het Tilburgs en het Rotterdams maar o ok enkele meer ex

otische talen zoals het Indonesisch het Frans het Gotisch en het No ors

worden b esproken Na een algemeen inleidend ho ofdstuk komen de vol

gende onderwerp en aan b o d

In ho ofdstukken en wordt een analyse gemaakt van het contrast

tussen gespannen versus ongespannen of lange versus korte klinkers in het

Standaard Nederlands Er wordt b eargumenteerd dat dit verschil alleen

b evredigend b eschreven kan worden met een kenmerk Retracted Tongue

Ro ot RTR Vervolgens is er een conditie die dit kenmerk relateert aan

het op en versus gesloten karakter van de lettergreep Zeer sterkeevidentie

vo or deze analyse komt uit talen die een op engesloten contrast combineren

met klinkerharmonie op het kenmerk RTR Brabantse dialecten Tilburgs

Antwerps Hofstaads zijn in dit verband o ok van b elang omdat ze de pa

rameters lengte en gespannenheid onafhankelijk com bineren en omdat ze

laten zien dat klinkerlengte over het algemeen corresp ondeert met RTR

niet met RTR zoals tot nu to e op basis van het Standaard Nederlands werd

aangenomen

In ho ofdstukken en wordt de Nederlandse sjwa b esproken De

stomme e mo et worden geanalyseerd als fonetisch en fonologischnage

no eg leeg Met deze kenmerklo osheid corresp onderen een aantal bijzondere

eigenschapp en van deze klinker hij is de reductiekli nker de ep enthetische

klinker en als onderliggende klinker komt hij alleen vo or in lettergrep en

Summary in Dutch

met een zeer eenvoudige structuur een enkelvoudige onset en een co da

waarin ho oguit een sonorant mag staan Er wordt aangeto ond dat al deze

eigenschapp en o ok inderdaad direct samenhangen met de leegheid van de

sjwa

In ho ofdstuk wordt de analyse over de Nederlandse sjwa to egepast op

een aantal andere talen met name het Frans en het No ors Het Frans staat

anders dan het Nederlands juist wel een complexe onset en geen co da to e in

sjwaho ofdige lettergrep en Het No ors heeft zowel een Franse sjwa eple

als een Nederlandse sjwa tiger deze sjwas zijn echter o ok in het No ors

gescheiden wo orden als katrel komen niet vo or De verschillen tussen de

drie talen wordenzoveel mogelijk b eschreven in termen van onafhankelijk

gemotiveerde andere verschillen en b ovendien wordt gewezen op enkele zeer

opvallende overeenkomsten

In ho ofdstuk wordt tenslotte ingegaan op het voorkomen van klinkers

in niet ho ofdige p ositie Dit geb eurt aan de hand van een uitgebreide anal

yse van de Rotterdamse ie je alternantie in monomorfemische wo orden het

clitisch pronomen van de tweede p erso on singularis en het diminutiefsuf

x In al deze vormen zijn de klinkers i en sjwa met elkaar in comp etitie

Deze comp etitie kan uitstekend b eschreven worden in termen van de Opti

maliteitstheorie Het blijkt dat de i zich inderdaad gedraagt als een b eter

lettergreepsho ofd en dat de sjwa alleen aan de opp ervlakte komt als real

isatie van de volle klinker als nucleair ho ofd geblokkeerd wordt Vergelijking

van dit Rotterdamse pro ces met de Wet van Sievers over het Gotisch levert

bovendien evidentie tegen de lengteanalyse van het Standaard Nederlands

in talen met echte lengte zoals het Gotisch breekt i tot ji Het blijkt zo te

zijn dat vo oral het kenmerk ho og een voorkeur heeft vo or de extracefalis

che p ositie op allerlei niveaus

Ho ofdstuk geeft een overzichtvan de familie van b ep erkingen die de

kern vormen van de meeste analyses in dit pro efschrift de familie van

pro jectieb ep er kingen Deze verbinden allemaal het voorkomen of niet

vo orkomen van een b epaald kenmerk of van een gro ep kenmerken aan

het voorkomen of nietvoorkomen van een b epaald typ e proso dische struc

tuur Het wordt aangeto ond dat het b egrip familie ons een elegante en

b evredigende metatheorie van fonologische pro jectie verschaen

Tenslotte wordt in een app endix een complete klinkergrammatica van

het Nederlands uitgewerkt en er worden enige b eschouwingen gewijd aan

het in het pro efschrift gebruikte formalisme

Summary in English Vowel

quality and phonological

pro jection

In this thesis we try to work out some consequences of the hyp othesis that

syllables are headed constituents In particular we examine to what extent

phonological features of prototypical heads that is to say vowels have

an inuence on the structure of the syllable around the head Central to

the discussion are Standard Dutch and some Dutch dialects likeTilburg

Dutch and Rotterdam Dutch but also other languages like Indonesian

French Gothic and Norwegian receive some treatment After a general

intro ductory chapter the following topics are discussed

In the chapters and we analyse the contrast b etween tense and

lax or long versus short vowels in Standard Dutch It is claimed that

this dierence can only b e describ ed in a satisfactory way when using the

feature Retracted Tongue Ro ot RTR We also p osit a constraintwhich

relates this feature to the op en versus closed nature of a syllable Very

strong evidence for this relation is drawn from languages such as Andalusian

Spanish and Eastern Javanese whichcombine the op enclosed contrast with

vowel harmony on the feature RTR Also a dialect like Tilburg Dutchis

of interest b ecause it has b oth a tenselax and a longshort contrast In

this dialect we can also see that vowel length correpsonds to RTR not to

RTR as is usually assumed on the basis of Standard Dutch

In chapters and we discuss Dutchschwa This vowel has to b e

analysed as almost empty b oth from a phonetic and phonological p ointof

view This featurelessnes s corresp onds to several sp ecial prop erties of this

vowel it is the reduction vowel the ep enthetic vowel and as an underlying

vowel it only supp orts syllables with a very simple structure viz a simple

onset and a co da in whichwe only nd a sonorant consonant It is shown

that all of these prop erties are indeed directly dep endent on the emptiness

of schwa

Summary in English

In chapter the analysis of Dutchschwaisappliedtoanumber of

other languages esp ecially French and Norwegian Frenchschwaallows a

complex onset but no co da and therefore seems markedly dierent from its

Dutchcounterpart Norwegian has b oth a French eple and a Dutchschwa

tiger These schwas however still have a dierent status in the language

we do not nd words suchaskatrel These dierences b etween the three

languages are describ ed in terms of indep endently motivated constraints en

on top of this it is shown that the three languages still show a few very

remarkable similarities

In chapter we discuss the o ccurrence of vowels in extracephalic p osi

tion The main topic of discussion here is a striking alternation b etween the

high vowel i and the syllable jwe nd in Rotterdam Dutch monomor

phemic forms second p erson singular clitics and diminutive forms The

comp etition b etween these two forms can b e describ ed in a theory of con

straint conict such as Optimality Theory It app ears that i acts as a b etter

nuclear head and that the form with schwa only surfaces as a realisation

of the full vowel is blo cked A comparison of the Rotterdam pro cess to

Sievers Law in Gothic additionally provides us with extra evidence against

a length analysis of Standard Dutch

Chapter gives an overview of the family of constraints whichform

the backb one of most analyses in the thesis the family of pro jection con

straints All of these constraint connect the o ccurrence or nono ccurrence

of a certain feature to the o ccurrence or non o ccurrence of certain typ es

of proso dic structure It is shown that the notion of a constraint fam

ily provides us with an elegant and satisfying metatheory of phonological pro jection