7. Dakota Pronouns.2

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

7. Dakota Pronouns.2 uŋkitawapi ma ni iś uŋ Pronouns in Dakota: miye A Brief Look wića wa ya iye ići nitawa REFERENCES u Boas, F. & Deloria, E.C. (1939). Dakota Grammar. OUR ROADMAP Washington, DC: National Academy of Science. v What information do pronouns in u Corbett, G. G. (2000). Number. Cambridge: Cambridge Dakota convey University Press. v The distribution of different kinds u Pinson, T. M. (1990). Possessor Ascension in Dakota Sioux. Work Papers of the Summer Institute of of pronouns Linguistics 34, 75-96. v Bound Pronouns u Riggs, S. R. (1893). Dakota Grammar: with Texts and v FreePronouns Ethnography. Department of the Interior. [Reprinted in 2004. St. Paul, MN: Minnesota Historical Society Press.] v Combined Pronouns u Shaw, P. A. (1980). Theoretical Issues in Generative v Possessive pronouns Phonology and Morphology: a case study from Dakota (Doctoral Dissertation). University of Toronto: Toronto. v Alienable vs inalienable [Published 1980. New York, NY: Garland.] possession u Dakota speakers at the Dakota Workshop at Carleton, held August 26-28, 2016. Ling 222 ~ Fall 2016 ~ C. Ussery 2 Dakota is a Siouan language – and so is the closely related Lakota, and this has led to some confusion. Ling 222 ~ Fall 2016 ~ C. Ussery 3 u Stephen Riggs (1812-1883) was a missionary who was part of the Some Dakota Mission, organized by the General Association of Context Congregational Churches of Massachusetts. u His grammar of Dakota was published in 1893 and is “the” grammar of the language. u Franz Boas and Ella Deloria’s grammar was published in 1939 by the National Academy of Sciences. u But, this grammar is primarily of Lakota u Some more contemporary linguists have spent time on reservations gathering data but haven’t given anything back to the community - and sometimes profit from the work that members of a tribe put into various projects. Ling 222 ~ Fall 2016 ~ C. Ussery 4 u The Linguistics and Computer Science Departments at Carleton have a nascent partnership with the Sisseton Wahpeton Oyate, of the Lake Traverse Reservation in South Dakota. u The goal is to contribute to the documentation and revitalization of Dakota and to produce a pedagogical grammar which could be used by teachers, learners, and linguistics. u Between the US and Canada, there are appr. 300,000 Dakota people. u But, for the Sisseton, the language is projected to be extinct on their reservation by 2028. Ling 222 ~ Fall 2016 ~ C. Ussery 5 1st: The person(s) speaking Person 2nd: The person(s) being addressed The Information 3rd: The person(s)/thing(s) being spoken about that Pronouns singular: One person/thing Convey in English Number plural: More than one person/thing masculine: Male gendered thing (usually an animal) or person Gender feminine: Female gendered thing or person neutral (neuter): Some animals, inanimate things, or a non- specific gender for a person subject: The doer of the action Role in object of the verb: The person/thing that the action is being done to Sentence object of the preposition: The noun that follows a preposition 6 owner/possessor: The person/thing that possesses Ling 222 ~ Fall 2016 ~ C. Ussery someone/something else 1st: The person(s) speaking The Information Person 2nd: The person(s) being addressed that Pronouns 3rd: The person(s)/thing(s) being spoken about Convey in Dakota singular: One person/thing Interestingly, gender is encoded in the dual: The person speaking and the person being addressed language in lots of Number Ø The dual has pronouns only in the 1st person in Dakota ways, but not in pronouns plural: More than one person/thing subject: The doer of the action object of the verb: The person/thing that the action is being done to Role in Sentence object of the preposition: The noun that follows a preposition Ø prepositions are sometimes built into verbs in Dakota – e.g. kíćikaġa = to make for owner/possessor: The person/thing that possesses someone or something else7 Ling 222 ~ Fall 2016 ~ C. Ussery Bound Pronouns u Most pronouns in Dakota are attached to the verb. ti = to dwell wati = I dwell u wa is a prefix here. itoŋśni = to tell a lie iwatoŋśni = I tell a lie u wa is an infix here. (examples from Riggs 1893, p. 13) We’ll return to when a pronoun is a prefix and when it is an infix. Ling 222 ~ Fall 2016 ~ C. Ussery 8 • In Teton, the only third person bound pronoun is for plural objects that are human or animate. Bound Pronouns in Dakota (1) (Boas and Deloria 1939, p. 76) Subject Object Ø True for Dakota also. 1st person -singular wa we ma mi kaġa = to make -dual uŋ uŋki -- -- wakaġa = I make -plural uŋ-pi uŋki-pi uŋ-pi uŋki-pi yakaġa = you (singular) make uŋkaġapi = we make 2nd person Ø “pi” is a plural marker that is attached to the end of a word or phrase -singular ya ye ni ni -plural ya-pi ye-pi ni-pi ni-pi kaġa = he made makaġa = he made me nićaġapi = he made you (plural) or 3rd person they made you -singular -- -- -- -- waśtedaka = to love -plural -- -- wića waśtewićadaka = he loves them (examples from Riggs 1893, p. 13-14) 9 (table adapted from Riggs 1893, p. 16) Ling 222 ~ Fall 2016 ~ C. Ussery Interesting Facts: Bound Pronouns in Dakota (2) • There are different pronouns when the prepositions ki ‘to’ and kíći ‘for’ are part of Subject Object the verb. 1st person ki/kíći kíćićaġa = to make for -singular wa we ma mi yećićaġa = you make for -dual uŋ uŋki -- -- -plural uŋ-pi uŋki-pi uŋ-pi uŋki-pi Ø Question: Do the object pronouns also differ based on ki and kíći? Is: nd niyećićaġa = you make for me 2 person Ø The speakers aren’t sure about this one -singular ya ye ni ni • If a verb begins with yu or ya, the y is -plural ya-pi ye-pi ni-pi ni-pi dropped. The 1st person pronoun changes to md or bd and the 2nd person changes to d. 3rd person yawa = to read -singular -- -- -- -- bdawa = I read -plural -- -- wića dawa = you read 10 (Riggs 1893, p. 13) (table adapted from Riggs 1893, p. 16) Ling 222 ~ Fall 2016 ~ C. Ussery Interesting Fact: A NOTE ABOUT THE DUAL NUMBER u In Dakota, the dual includes the speaker. u The dual pronouns are only in 1st person. English does not have the dual, but some other u The pronoun for the dual is the same as for the languages do. plural, but without the plural marker –pi. u Dual: uŋ/uŋki Plural: uŋ-pi/uŋki-pi u In Upper Sorbian, the dual does not have to include The dual in Upper Sorbian the speaker. • Upper Sorbian is spoken in eastern Germany u The dual means “two” and can be in the 2nd and by appr. 40,000 – 45,000 people. 3rd person. singular dual plural ja ‘I’ mój ‘we two’ my ‘we’ dźěłam ‘I work’ dźěłamoj ‘we two work’ dźěłamy ‘we work’ ty ‘you’ wój ‘you two’ wy ‘you all’ hród ‘palace, castle’ hrodaj ‘two hrody ‘palaces/castles’ palaces/castles’ Ling 222 ~ Fall 2016 ~ C. Ussery 11 (examples from Corbett 2000, p. 20) § According to Riggs (1893), Dakota does Subject Object not have an object pronoun for the dual number. 1st person ki/kíći § According to Boas and Deloria (1939), -singular wa we ma mi the dual object pronoun is the same as -dual uŋ uŋki -- -- the dual subject pronoun in some -plural uŋ-pi uŋki-pi uŋ-pi uŋki-pi dialects. nd Ø Question: 2 person -singular ya ye ni ni § Is “uŋkaġa” OK for “he made you and me”? -plural ya-pi ye-pi ni-pi ni-pi § NO: “You and I made something.” kaġa = he made 3rd person makaġa = he made me -singular -- -- -- -- -plural -- -- wića nićaġapi = he made you (plural) or they made you Ling 222 ~ Fall 2016 ~ C. Ussery 12 Ø wićaśta = man Sometimes pronouns are Ø wimaćaśta = I am a man. bound to other nouns… Ø ma is the first person singular object pronoun Ø A man is me. Ø Speakers say that ma is not an object pronoun. “X is happening to me./X is my state of being. Ø But, speakers said that no one would ever say wimaćaśta. Ø waoŋśida = merciful …and sometimes pronouns Ø waoŋsiwada = I am merciful. are bound to adjectives. Ø Here, we have the first person singular subject pronoun Ø Speakers agree: I am the doer of X (examples from Riggs 1893, p. 13-14) Ling 222 ~ Fall 2016 ~ C. Ussery 13 Speakers agree that there is a There are other times when the object pronoun distinction between “I am is used for a subject. doing” pronouns and “Something is happening to me” pronouns Subject Pronoun Object Pronoun So, there is an active/stative or unaccusative/unergative split. wanowan = I was swimming. miśtima = I was sleeping. u The vowel in the pronoun is dropped. u Swimming is a more active verb. The swimmer is physically doing something. u Sleeping is a more passive verb. “I” is more like the object of sleeping and not the agent of sleeping. (examples from Pinson 1990, page 79) Ling 222 ~ Fall 2016 ~ C. Ussery 14 Adjectival verbs in Dakota also use object pronouns as subjects. asní: to get well, be well, Subject Object recover from sickness 1st person ki/kíći -singular wa we ma mi amásni: I am well. -dual uŋ uŋki -- -- uŋkásni: You and I are well. -plural uŋ-pi uŋki-pi uŋ-pi uŋki-pi uŋkásnipi: We are well. 2nd person anísni: You (singular) are well.
Recommended publications
  • Animacy and Alienability: a Reconsideration of English
    Running head: ANIMACY AND ALIENABILITY 1 Animacy and Alienability A Reconsideration of English Possession Jaimee Jones A Senior Thesis submitted in partial fulfillment of the requirements for graduation in the Honors Program Liberty University Spring 2016 ANIMACY AND ALIENABILITY 2 Acceptance of Senior Honors Thesis This Senior Honors Thesis is accepted in partial fulfillment of the requirements for graduation from the Honors Program of Liberty University. ______________________________ Jaeshil Kim, Ph.D. Thesis Chair ______________________________ Paul Müller, Ph.D. Committee Member ______________________________ Jeffrey Ritchey, Ph.D. Committee Member ______________________________ Brenda Ayres, Ph.D. Honors Director ______________________________ Date ANIMACY AND ALIENABILITY 3 Abstract Current scholarship on English possessive constructions, the s-genitive and the of- construction, largely ignores the possessive relationships inherent in certain English compound nouns. Scholars agree that, in general, an animate possessor predicts the s- genitive while an inanimate possessor predicts the of-construction. However, the current literature rarely discusses noun compounds, such as the table leg, which also express possessive relationships. However, pragmatically and syntactically, a compound cannot be considered as a true possessive construction. Thus, this paper will examine why some compounds still display possessive semantics epiphenomenally. The noun compounds that imply possession seem to exhibit relationships prototypical of inalienable possession such as body part, part whole, and spatial relationships. Additionally, the juxtaposition of the possessor and possessum in the compound construction is reminiscent of inalienable possession in other languages. Therefore, this paper proposes that inalienability, a phenomenon not thought to be relevant in English, actually imbues noun compounds whose components exhibit an inalienable relationship with possessive semantics.
    [Show full text]
  • The Term Declension, the Three Basic Qualities of Latin Nouns, That
    Chapter 2: First Declension Chapter 2 covers the following: the term declension, the three basic qualities of Latin nouns, that is, case, number and gender, basic sentence structure, subject, verb, direct object and so on, the six cases of Latin nouns and the uses of those cases, the formation of the different cases in Latin, and the way adjectives agree with nouns. At the end of this lesson we’ll review the vocabulary you should memorize in this chapter. Declension. As with conjugation, the term declension has two meanings in Latin. It means, first, the process of joining a case ending onto a noun base. Second, it is a term used to refer to one of the five categories of nouns distinguished by the sound ending the noun base: /a/, /ŏ/ or /ŭ/, a consonant or /ĭ/, /ū/, /ē/. First, let’s look at the three basic characteristics of every Latin noun: case, number and gender. All Latin nouns and adjectives have these three grammatical qualities. First, case: how the noun functions in a sentence, that is, is it the subject, the direct object, the object of a preposition or any of many other uses? Second, number: singular or plural. And third, gender: masculine, feminine or neuter. Every noun in Latin will have one case, one number and one gender, and only one of each of these qualities. In other words, a noun in a sentence cannot be both singular and plural, or masculine and feminine. Whenever asked ─ and I will ask ─ you should be able to give the correct answer for all three qualities.
    [Show full text]
  • Serial Verb Constructions Revisited: a Case Study from Koro
    Serial Verb Constructions Revisited: A Case Study from Koro By Jessica Cleary-Kemp A dissertation submitted in partial satisfaction of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy in Linguistics in the Graduate Division of the University of California, Berkeley Committee in charge: Associate Professor Lev D. Michael, Chair Assistant Professor Peter S. Jenks Professor William F. Hanks Summer 2015 © Copyright by Jessica Cleary-Kemp All Rights Reserved Abstract Serial Verb Constructions Revisited: A Case Study from Koro by Jessica Cleary-Kemp Doctor of Philosophy in Linguistics University of California, Berkeley Associate Professor Lev D. Michael, Chair In this dissertation a methodology for identifying and analyzing serial verb constructions (SVCs) is developed, and its application is exemplified through an analysis of SVCs in Koro, an Oceanic language of Papua New Guinea. SVCs involve two main verbs that form a single predicate and share at least one of their arguments. In addition, they have shared values for tense, aspect, and mood, and they denote a single event. The unique syntactic and semantic properties of SVCs present a number of theoretical challenges, and thus they have invited great interest from syntacticians and typologists alike. But characterizing the nature of SVCs and making generalizations about the typology of serializing languages has proven difficult. There is still debate about both the surface properties of SVCs and their underlying syntactic structure. The current work addresses some of these issues by approaching serialization from two angles: the typological and the language-specific. On the typological front, it refines the definition of ‘SVC’ and develops a principled set of cross-linguistically applicable diagnostics.
    [Show full text]
  • Finding and Fixing Apostrophe Errors 425.640.1750 |
    Edmonds CC Writing Center | MUK 113 Finding and Fixing Apostrophe Errors 425.640.1750 | www.edcc.edu/lsc FINDING AND FIXING APOSTROPHE ERRORS What is an Apostrophe Error? Apostrophes are used to show ownership and are used to show that one or more letters are missing in a contraction. An apostrophe error occurs when an apostrophe is needed to show possession or contraction, but it is not there or is in the wrong place. Apostrophes have two main uses: contractions and possession. If you can learn these rules, you can master apostrophe usage! Contractions: Contractions are shortened forms of words which add an informal tone to academic writing. An apostrophe indicates that one or more letters have been left out. To assess whether you need an apostrophe or not, re-read the sentence to see if you can expand the shortened contraction words by filling in the missing letters. Place an apostrophe where letters are missing. Thao wasn’t in class today. (The apostrophe takes the place of the missing letter “o”). Thao was not in class today. (Note that you can expand the shortened word “wasn’t” by just filling in the missing letter “o”). These are some of the most common contractions: I am = I’m It is = It’s Who is = Who’s She is = She’s She would = She’d She would have = She would’ve He is = He’s He would = He’d He would have = He would’ve Let us = Let’s You are = You’re They had = They’d When to Use It’s vs.
    [Show full text]
  • Chapter 1 Negation in a Cross-Linguistic Perspective
    Chapter 1 Negation in a cross-linguistic perspective 0. Chapter summary This chapter introduces the empirical scope of our study on the expression and interpretation of negation in natural language. We start with some background notions on negation in logic and language, and continue with a discussion of more linguistic issues concerning negation at the syntax-semantics interface. We zoom in on cross- linguistic variation, both in a synchronic perspective (typology) and in a diachronic perspective (language change). Besides expressions of propositional negation, this book analyzes the form and interpretation of indefinites in the scope of negation. This raises the issue of negative polarity and its relation to negative concord. We present the main facts, criteria, and proposals developed in the literature on this topic. The chapter closes with an overview of the book. We use Optimality Theory to account for the syntax and semantics of negation in a cross-linguistic perspective. This theoretical framework is introduced in Chapter 2. 1 Negation in logic and language The main aim of this book is to provide an account of the patterns of negation we find in natural language. The expression and interpretation of negation in natural language has long fascinated philosophers, logicians, and linguists. Horn’s (1989) Natural history of negation opens with the following statement: “All human systems of communication contain a representation of negation. No animal communication system includes negative utterances, and consequently, none possesses a means for assigning truth value, for lying, for irony, or for coping with false or contradictory statements.” A bit further on the first page, Horn states: “Despite the simplicity of the one-place connective of propositional logic ( ¬p is true if and only if p is not true) and of the laws of inference in which it participate (e.g.
    [Show full text]
  • Patience Luke 21:19 in Your Patience Possess Ye Your Souls. (KJV)
    Patience Luke 21:19 In your patience possess ye your souls. (KJV) Luke 21:5-18 5 And as some spake of the temple, how it was adorned with goodly stones and gifts, he said, 6 As for these things which ye behold, the days will come, in the which there shall not be left one stone upon another, that shall not be thrown down. 7 And they asked him, saying, Master, but when shall these things be? and what sign will there be when these things shall come to pass? 8 And he said, Take heed that ye be not deceived: for many shall come in my name, saying, I am Christ; and the time draweth near: go ye not therefore after them. 9 But when ye shall hear of wars and commotions, be not terrified: for these things must first come to pass; but the end is not by and by. 10 Then said he unto them, Nation shall rise against nation, and kingdom against kingdom: 11 And great earthquakes shall be in divers places, and famines, and pestilences; and fearful sights and great signs shall there be from heaven. 12 But before all these, they shall lay their hands on you, and persecute you, delivering you up to the synagogues, and into prisons, being brought before kings and rulers for my name's sake. 13 And it shall turn to you for a testimony. 14 Settle it therefore in your hearts, not to meditate before what ye shall answer: 15 For I will give you a mouth and wisdom, which all your adversaries shall not be able to gainsay nor resist.
    [Show full text]
  • An Investigation of Possession in Moroccan Arabic
    Family Agreement: An Investigation of Possession in Moroccan Arabic Aidan Kaplan Advisor: Jim Wood Submitted to the faculty of the Department of Linguistics in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Bachelor of Arts Yale University May 2017 Abstract This essay takes up the phenomenon of apparently redundant possession in Moroccan Arabic.In particular, kinship terms are often marked with possessive pronominal suffixes in constructions which would not require this in other languages, including Modern Standard Arabic. In the following example ‘sister’ is marked with the possessive suffix hā ‘her,’ even though the person in question has no sister. ﻣﺎ ﻋﻨﺪﻫﺎش ُﺧﺘﻬﺎ (1) mā ʿend-hā-sh khut-hā not at-her-neg sister-her ‘She doesn’t have a sister’ This phenomenon shows both intra- and inter-speaker variation. For some speakers, thepos- sessive suffix is obligatory in clausal possession expressing kinship relations, while forother speakers it is optional. Accounting for the presence of the ‘extra’ pronoun in (1) will lead to an account of possessive suffixes as the spell-out of agreement between aPoss◦ head and a higher element that contains phi features, using Reverse Agree (Wurmbrand, 2014, 2017). In regular pronominal possessive constructions, Poss◦ agrees with a silent possessor pro, while in sentences like (1), Poss◦ agrees with the PP at the beginning of the sentence that expresses clausal posses- sion. The obligatoriness of the possessive suffix for some speakers and its optionality forothers is explained by positing that the selectional properties of the D◦ head differ between speakers. In building up an analysis, this essay draws on the proposal for the construct state in Fassi Fehri (1993), the proposal that clitics are really agreement markers in Shlonsky (1997), and the account of clausal possession in Boneh & Sichel (2010).
    [Show full text]
  • Determining Gender Markedness in Wari' Joshua Birchall University Of
    Determining Gender Markedness in Wari’ Joshua Birchall University of Montana 1. Introduction Grammatical gender is rare across Amazonian languages. The language families that possess grammatical gender properties are Arawak, Chapakuran and Arawá. Wari’, a Chapakuran (Txapakura) language of Brazil, is unusual among these languages in that it possesses three gender categories: what we can refer to as masculine, feminine and neuter. While efforts have been made to posit the functionally unmarked gender in Arawak and Arawá languages, no such analysis has been given for any Chapakuran language. In this paper I propose that neuter is the functionally unmarked grammatical gender of Wari’. Aikhenvald (1999:84) states that masculine is the functionally unmarked gender for non-Caribbean Arawak languages, while Dixon (1999:298) claims that feminine is the unmarked gender in Arawá languages. Functionally unmarked gender refers to the gender that is obligatorily assigned when its designation is otherwise opaque. With attempts, as in Greenberg (1987), to claim a genetic relationship between Chapakuran languages and the Arawak and Arawá families, the presence of gender is a major grammatical similarity and possible motivating factor for such a hypothesis and thus should be examined. In order to investigate this claim, I first present an outline of the Wari’ gender distinctions and then proceed to describe how gender is manifested within the clause. A case is then presented for neuter as the functionally unmarked gender. This claim is further examined for its historical implications, especially regarding genetic affiliation. All of the data1 used in this paper come from Everett and Kern (1997). 2. Gender Distinctions The three gender distinctions we find in Wari’ are largely determined by the semantic domain of the noun.
    [Show full text]
  • Adnominal Possession: Combining Typological and Second Language Perspectives
    Adnominal possession: combining typological and second language perspectives Björn Hammarberg and Maria Koptjevskaja-Tamm 1. Introduction The notion of possession and its linguistic manifestations have been a popular topic in linguistic literature for a long time. What we will attempt here is to explore the domain of adnominal possession - pos- sessive relations and their manifestations within the noun phrase - in one particular language from two combined points of view: the ty- pological characteristics of the system, and the picture that emerges from second language learners' attempts to handle adnominal pos- session in production. We are focusing on Swedish, a language with a distinctive and rather elaborate system of adnominal possession. Our aim is twofold: to present an overview of how the Swedish sys- tem of adnominal possessive constructions works, and to show how acquisitional aspects connect with typological aspects in this domain. The English phrases Peter's hat, my son and a boy's leg all exem- plify prototypical cases of adnominal possession whereby one entity, the possessee, referred to by the head of the possessive noun phrase, is represented as possessed in one way or another by another entity, the possessor, referred to by the attribute. It has become a common- place in linguistics that possession is a difficult, if not impossible, notion to grasp (for a survey of adnominal possession in a cross- linguistic perspective cf. Koptjevskaja-Tamm 2001). However, even though it seems impossible to give a reasonable general definition for all the meanings covered by possessive constructions across lan- guages and even in one language, we can still provide criteria for identifying a possessive construction, or possessive constructions in a language.
    [Show full text]
  • 'YE ARE MY FRIENDS' by John Macmurray
    'YE ARE MY FRIENDS' by John Macmurray Ye are my friends, if you do whatsoever I command you. Henceforth I call you not servants but friends. These things I command you that ye love one another. John xv. 1-17. THE purpose of God in the life of the world is a web of purposes which has a single centre, from which all the threads go out and to which they all return. Only from the centre can we begin to trace the plan of it. From any other point it will seem a meaningless tangle. Copernicus made a revolution in human knowledge merely by shifting the centre of the solar system from the earth to the sun. The world- revolution of the Christians came when Jesus discovered the true centre of human life. 'Not servants but friends' is the proclamation of the revolution. The key- word of the Christian gospel is not service but friendship. Of late, I believe, we have been thinking too much in terms of service—service of God and of the world. There is nothing distinctively Christian about that. It is the natural way of religious thought when it becomes practical. Socrates called himself the servant of Apollo. Christ's revolution consisted, like that of Copernicus, precisely in denying the 'Natural' point of view and substituting friendship for service. 'But surely,' you will say, 'we are called as Christians to serve Christ and to serve the world.' No, we are called to be the friends of Christ and the friends of men. That is not at all the same thing.
    [Show full text]
  • Serial Verb Constructions: Argument Structural Uniformity and Event Structural Diversity
    SERIAL VERB CONSTRUCTIONS: ARGUMENT STRUCTURAL UNIFORMITY AND EVENT STRUCTURAL DIVERSITY A DISSERTATION SUBMITTED TO THE DEPARTMENT OF LINGUISTICS AND THE COMMITTEE ON GRADUATE STUDIES OF STANFORD UNIVERSITY IN PARTIAL FULFILLMENT OF THE REQUIREMENTS FOR THE DEGREE OF DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY Melanie Owens November 2011 © 2011 by Melanie Rachel Owens. All Rights Reserved. Re-distributed by Stanford University under license with the author. This work is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution- Noncommercial 3.0 United States License. http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc/3.0/us/ This dissertation is online at: http://purl.stanford.edu/db406jt2949 ii I certify that I have read this dissertation and that, in my opinion, it is fully adequate in scope and quality as a dissertation for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy. Beth Levin, Primary Adviser I certify that I have read this dissertation and that, in my opinion, it is fully adequate in scope and quality as a dissertation for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy. Joan Bresnan I certify that I have read this dissertation and that, in my opinion, it is fully adequate in scope and quality as a dissertation for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy. Vera Gribanov Approved for the Stanford University Committee on Graduate Studies. Patricia J. Gumport, Vice Provost Graduate Education This signature page was generated electronically upon submission of this dissertation in electronic format. An original signed hard copy of the signature page is on file in University Archives. iii Abstract Serial Verb Constructions (SVCs) are constructions which contain two or more verbs yet behave in every grammatical respect as if they contain only one.
    [Show full text]
  • Declension of Nouns
    DECLENSION OF NOUNS In English, the relationship between words in a sentence depends primarily on word order. The difference between the god desires the girl and the girl desires the god is immediately apparent to us. Latin does not depend on word order for basic meaning, but on inflections (changes in the endings of words) to indicate the function of words within a sentence. Thus the god desires the girl can be expressed in Latin deus puellam desiderat, puellam deus desiderat, or desiderat puellam deus without any change in basic meaning. The accusative ending of puellam shows that the girl is being acted upon (i.e., is the object of the verb) and is not the actor (i.e., the subject of the verb). Similarly, the nominative form of deus shows that the god is the actor (agent) in the sentence, not the object of the verb. The inflection of nouns is called declension. The individual declensions are called cases, and together they form the case system. Nouns, pronouns, adjectives and participles are declined in six Cases: nominative, genitive, dative, accusative, ablative, and vocative and two Numbers (singular and plural). (The locative, an archaic case, existed in the classical period only for a few words). Nominative Indicates the subject of a sentence. (The boy loves the book). Genitive Indicates possession. (The boy loves the girl’s book). Dative Indicates indirect object. (The boy gave the book to the girl). Accusative Indicates direct object. (The boy loves the book). Ablative Answers the questions from where? by what means? how? from what cause? in what manner? when? or where? The ablative is used to show separation (from), instrumentality or means (by, with), accompaniment (with), or locality (at).
    [Show full text]