Sensory Biology of Aquatic Animals Conference Participants

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Sensory Biology of Aquatic Animals Conference Participants Sensory Biology of Aquatic Animals Conference participants. From left to right: Front, row 1: W. S. M. Hagedorn, J. Wyneken, M. Salmon, C. McCormick, J. Song, Wilcox, W. Saidel, W. Plassman, B. Fritsch, H. Zakon, M. Braford, M. Kreithen, M. Wullimann, J. Case, J. Gray, M. Powers, J. Levine, P. Borroni, B. U. Budelmann, C. Platt, H. Bleckmann, C. Hawr­ D. Hoekstra, T. Finger, P. Hamilton, D. Woodward, A. Kalmijn, wyshyn, J. Sivak. Middle, row 2: T. Waterman, B. Sokolowski, T. Cronin, T. Bullock, M. Laverack, H. Munz, J. Douglass, S. W. Heiligenberg, M. Swain, S. Coombs, E. Denton, R. G. Northcutt, Holderman. Last, row 4: C. Hopkins, W. Stachnik, T. Ream, P. W. Tavolga, J. Atema, R. Fay, A. Popper, J. Patton, P. Gomer, Gilbert, J. Kendall, B. Ache, A. Elepfandt, R. Barlow, R. Brill, P. Moller, J. Caprio, J. Crawford, J. Blaxter, R. Voigt, J. Webb, R. Fernald, R. Eaton, B. Zahuranec, W. Carr, J. Janssen, J. Lythgoe, R. Gleeson, C. Derby, I. Assip. Back, row 3: P. Rogers, M. Cox, J. R. Strickler, E. Hartwig, K. Wiese. lelle Atema Richard R. Fay Arthur N. Popper William N. Tavolga Editors Sensory Biology of Aquatic Animals Springer-Verlag New York Berlin Heidelberg London Paris Tokyo JELLE ATEMA, Boston University Marine Program, Marine Biological Laboratory, Woods Hole, Massachusetts 02543, USA Richard R. Fay, Parmly Hearing Institute, Loyola University, Chicago, Illinois 60626, USA ARTHUR N. POPPER, Department of Zoology, University of Maryland, College Park, MD 20742, USA WILLIAM N. TAVOLGA, Mote Marine Laboratory, Sarasota, Florida 33577, USA The cover illustration is a reproduction of Figure 13.3, p. 343 of this volume Library of Congress Cataloging-in-Publication Data Sensory biology of aquatic animals. Papers based on presentations given at an International Conference on the Sensory Biology of Aquatic Animals held, June 24-28, 1985, at the Mote Marine Laboratory in Sarasota, Fla. Bibliography: p. Includes indexes. 1. Aquatic animals--Physiology-Congresses. 2. Senses and sensation-Congresses. I. Atema, Je1le. II. International Conference on the Sensory Biology of Aquatic Animals (1985 : Sarasota, Fla.) QL120.S46 1987 591.92 87-9632 © 1988 by Springer-Verlag New York Inc. Softcover reprint of the hardcover 1st edition 1988 All rights reserved. This work may not be translated or copied in whole or in part without the written permission of the publisher (Springer-Verlag, 175 Fifth Avenue, New York 10010, U.S.A.), except for brief excerpts in connection with reviews or scholarly analysis. Use in connection with any form of information storage and retrieval, electronic adaptation, computer software, or by similar or dissimilar methodology now known or hereafter developed is forbidden. The use of general descriptive names, trade names, trademarks, etc. In this publication, even if the former are not especially identified, is not to be taken as a sign that such names, as understood by the Trade Marks and Merchandise Marks Act, may accordingly be used freely by anyone. Typeset by Arcata Graphics/Kingsport, Kingsport, Tennessee. 9 8 7 6 5 4 3 2 1 ISBN-13: 978-1-4612-8317-1 e-ISBN-13: 978-1-4612-3714-3 DOl: 10.1007/978-1-4612-3714-3 This volume is dedicated to Sven Dijkgraaf Karl von Frisch C. Judson Herrick Erich von Holst George H. Parker Jacob von Uexkull J. Z. Young Each of these investigators delved deeply and broadly into the sensory biology of aquatic animals. Their contributions to the study of sensory systems of aquatic animals have directly and indirectly influenced all of the work reported in this volume. Preface This volume constitutes a series of invited chapters based on presentations given at an International Conference on the Sensory Biology of Aquatic Animals held June 24-28, 1985 at the Mote Marine Laboratory in Sarasota, Florida. The immediate purpose of the conference was to spark an exchange of ideas, concepts, and techniques among investigators concerned with the different sensory modalities employed by a wide variety of animal species in extracting information from the aquatic environment. By necessity, most investigators of sensory biology are specialists in one sensory system: different stimulus modalities require different methods of stimulus control and, generally, different animal models. Yet, it is clear that all sensory systems have principles in common, such as stimulus filtering by peripheral structures, tuning of receptor cells, signal-to-noise ratios, adaption and disadaptation, and effective dynamic range. Other features, such as hormonal and efferent neural control, circadian reorganization, and receptor recycling are known in some and not in other senses. The conference afforded an increased awareness of new discoveries in other sensory systems that has effectively inspired a fresh look by the various participants at their own area of specialization to see whether or not similar principles apply. This inspiration was found not only in theoretical issues, but equally in techniques and methods of approach. The myopy of sensory specialization was broken in one unexpected way by showing limitations of individual sense organs and their integration within each organism. For instance, studying vision, one generally chooses a visual animal as a model. Subsequent intense focus on that animal's visual performance easily leads to ignoring the fact that in the real world this animal uses other senses as well. Thus, the demands the animal places on vision are tempered in the context of other sensory information. The behavioral demands on visual detection range and acuity for instance may not be as severe as one might think, if it were known that acoustic or chemical senses serve to locate and identify the source of interest for that animal at greater distances, whereas vision is used mainly at shorter range. This multisensory conference thus allowed the different senses to show not only their individual strengths, but also viii Preface their weaknesses and limitations, and their inherent interrelatedness as they appear in the aquatic environment. It is our hope that these issues will not be lost in book form. The long-range purpose of the conference was to create a book that could serve as a conceptual framework for further investigations of the aquatic "Umwelt" and the intricate, often unexpected, methods and processes used by animals to utilize the information contained in the acquatic stimulus world. This book and its organization reflect the central premise that sensory systems are constrained by the stimulus environ­ ment (Section I), by the behavioral requirements of the organism (Section II), and by phylogeny (various chapters in Section III). In addition, there is the tacit assumption of economy in sensory design. A major and unique effort was made to stimulate the development of conceptual models of the aquatic stimulus environment per se. This, we believe, is of paramount importance since only a detailed and comprehensive understanding of the natural distribution of stimuli and their noise backgrounds will allow the design of the proper experiments which, in tum, will lead to an understanding of sensory function in a natural context. This focus primarily serves to probe specific aquatic problems, but it also bears on terrestrial sensory problems by providing the aquatic contrast. The aquatic environment is a different medium than the terrestrial environment in nearly all aspects of sensory biology. Some differences are profound, such as the possibility of electroreception in the conductive aquatic medium. Other differences are less funda­ mental, such as the propagation speed of sound and the properties of water as a filter for light and a carrier for chemical stimuli. The sensory constraints imposed by the stimulus environment may emerge in clearest form when comparing aquatic and terrestrial solutions to similar sensory problems. Although a number of chapters refer explicitly to terrestrial animals and environments for contrast, a systematic compar­ ison falls beyond the scope of this volume. Intimately related to the direct constraints and opportunities presented by the sensory "Umwelt" are indirect sensory constraints imposed by animal behavior, the motoric "Umwelt." An animal's behavioral requirements invite, to various degrees, the devel­ opment of sensory capabilities. The reverse may be true as well: extended sensory horizons may allow the expansion of motoric virtuosity. Thus, to understand the physiology of receptor cells and organs it is instructive to know the animal's behavioral capabilities. In the aquatic environment these capabilities are influenced to a large degree by the physical density and viscosity of water, regardless of whether the animal moves through the medium or moves water through or along its body. An understanding of the constraints of behavior is a rich source of inspiration in the study of sensory function. Ultimately all sensory processing stands in the service of making behavioral decisions. These are often decisions of immediate life or death, and always decisions affecting competitive fitness. The central question concerns what an animal must know about its environment in order to survive and compete successfully. Behavioral ecology can show the knowledge that an animal must have had about its environment in order to have performed a certain behavior, or the information required to have followed a certain behavioral strategy. The acquisition of this knowledge is at least partially a matter of sensory information. Memory and expectation are also essential ingredients of this knowledge, and both memory and expectation require constant updating by sensory information. Preface ix In contrast to the acquisition of information, we recognize stimulus acquisition behavior, i.e., those behavioral actions and strategies that increase the animal's proba­ bilities of interfacing with the proper sensory stimuli. These actions include search patterns, choice of location, current generation, acoustic and visual scanning, flicking and sniffing, probing, and any other behavior that appears designed specifically or primarily to serve the function of encountering further sensory cues that, in tum, may provide information about the source of the stimulus.
Recommended publications
  • Masakazu Konishi
    Masakazu Konishi BORN: Kyoto, Japan February 17, 1933 EDUCATION: Hokkaido University, Sapporo, Japan, B.S. (1956) Hokkaido University, Sapporo, Japan, M.S. (1958) University of California, Berkeley, Ph.D. (1963) APPOINTMENTS: Postdoctoral Fellow, University of Tübingen, Germany (1963–1964) Postdoctoral Fellow, Division of Experimental Neurophysiology, Max-Planck Institut, Munich, Germany (1964–1965) Assistant Professor of Biology, University of Wisconsin, Madison (1965–1966) Assistant Professor of Biology, Princeton University (1966–1970) Associate Professor of Biology, Princeton University (1970–1975) Professor of Biology, California Institute of Technology (1975– 1980) Bing Professor of Behavioral Biology, California Institute of Technology (1980– ) HONORS AND AWARDS (SELECTED): Member, American Academy of Arts and Sciences (1979) Member, National Academy of Sciences (1985) President, International Society for Neuroethology (1986—1989) F. O. Schmitt Prize (1987) International Prize for Biology (1990) The Lewis S. Rosenstiel Award, Brandeis University (2004) Edward M. Scolnick Prize in Neuroscience, MIT (2004) Gerard Prize, the Society for Neuroscience (2004) Karl Spencer Lashley Award, The American Philosophical Society (2004) The Peter and Patricia Gruber Prize in Neuroscience, The Society for Neuroscience (2005) Masakazu (Mark) Konishi has been one of the leaders in avian neuroethology since the early 1960’s. He is known for his idea that young birds initially remember a tutor song and use the memory as a template to guide the development of their own song. He was the fi rst to show that estrogen prevents programmed cell death in female zebra fi nches. He also pioneered work on the brain mechanisms of sound localization by barn owls. He has trained many students and postdoctoral fellows who became leading neuroethologists.
    [Show full text]
  • Sensory Biology of Aquatic Animals
    Jelle Atema Richard R. Fay Arthur N. Popper William N. Tavolga Editors Sensory Biology of Aquatic Animals Springer-Verlag New York Berlin Heidelberg London Paris Tokyo JELLE ATEMA, Boston University Marine Program, Marine Biological Laboratory, Woods Hole, Massachusetts 02543, USA Richard R. Fay, Parmly Hearing Institute, Loyola University, Chicago, Illinois 60626, USA ARTHUR N. POPPER, Department of Zoology, University of Maryland, College Park, MD 20742, USA WILLIAM N. TAVOLGA, Mote Marine Laboratory, Sarasota, Florida 33577, USA The cover Illustration is a reproduction of Figure 13.3, p. 343 of this volume Library of Congress Cataloging-in-Publication Data Sensory biology of aquatic animals. Papers based on presentations given at an International Conference on the Sensory Biology of Aquatic Animals held, June 24-28, 1985, at the Mote Marine Laboratory in Sarasota, Fla. Bibliography: p. Includes indexes. 1. Aquatic animals—Physiology—Congresses. 2. Senses and Sensation—Congresses. I. Atema, Jelle. II. International Conference on the Sensory Biology - . of Aquatic Animals (1985 : Sarasota, Fla.) QL120.S46 1987 591.92 87-9632 © 1988 by Springer-Verlag New York Inc. x —• All rights reserved. This work may not be translated or copied in whole or in part without the written permission of the publisher (Springer-Verlag, 175 Fifth Avenue, New York 10010, U.S.A.), except for brief excerpts in connection with reviews or scholarly analysis. Use in connection with any form of Information storage and retrieval, electronic adaptation, Computer Software, or by similar or dissimilar methodology now known or hereafter developed is forbidden. The use of general descriptive names, trade names, trademarks, etc.
    [Show full text]
  • Perception, Perfusion & Posture
    Perception, Perfusion & Posture Billy L Luu Ph.D. Thesis University of New South Wales 2010 THE UNIVERSITY OF NEW SOUTH WALES Thesis/Dissertation Sheet Surname or Family name: LUU First name: BILLY Other name/s: LIANG Abbreviation for degree as given in the University calendar: PhD School: MEDICAL SCIENCES Faculty: MEDICINE Title: PERCEPTION, PERFUSION & POSTURE Upright posture is a fundamental and critical human behaviour that places unique demands on the brain for motor and cardiovascular control. This thesis examines broad aspects of sensorimotor and cardiovascular function to examine their interdependence, with particular emphasis on the physiological processes operating during standing. How we perceive the forces our muscles exert was investigated by contralateral weight matching in the upper limb. The muscles on one side were weakened to about half their strength by fatigue and by paralysis with curare. In two subjects with dense large-diameter neuropathy, fatigue made lifted objects feel twice as heavy but in normal subjects it made objects feel lighter. Complete paralysis and recovery to half strength also made lifted objects feel lighter. Taken together, these results show that although a signal within the brain is available, this is not how muscle force is normally perceived. Instead, we use a signal that is largely peripheral reafference, which includes a dominant contribution from muscle spindles. Unlike perception of force when lifting weights, it is shown that we perceive only about 15% of the force applied by the legs to balance the body when standing. A series of experiments show that half of the force is provided by passive mechanics and most of the active muscle force is produced by a sub- cortical motor drive that does not give rise to a sense of muscular force through central efference copy or reafference.
    [Show full text]
  • Zootechnologies
    Zootechnologies A Media History of Swarm Research SEBASTIAN VEHLKEN Amsterdam University Press Zootechnologies The book series RECURSIONS: THEORIES OF MEDIA, MATERIALITY, AND CULTURAL TECHNIQUES provides a platform for cuttingedge research in the field of media culture studies with a particular focus on the cultural impact of media technology and the materialities of communication. The series aims to be an internationally significant and exciting opening into emerging ideas in media theory ranging from media materialism and hardware-oriented studies to ecology, the post-human, the study of cultural techniques, and recent contributions to media archaeology. The series revolves around key themes: – The material underpinning of media theory – New advances in media archaeology and media philosophy – Studies in cultural techniques These themes resonate with some of the most interesting debates in international media studies, where non-representational thought, the technicity of knowledge formations and new materialities expressed through biological and technological developments are changing the vocabularies of cultural theory. The series is also interested in the mediatic conditions of such theoretical ideas and developing them as media theory. Editorial Board – Jussi Parikka (University of Southampton) – Anna Tuschling (Ruhr-Universität Bochum) – Geoffrey Winthrop-Young (University of British Columbia) Zootechnologies A Media History of Swarm Research Sebastian Vehlken Translated by Valentine A. Pakis Amsterdam University Press This publication is funded by MECS Institute for Advanced Study on Media Cultures of Computer Simulation, Leuphana University Lüneburg (German Research Foundation Project KFOR 1927). Already published as: Zootechnologien. Eine Mediengeschichte der Schwarmforschung, Sebastian Vehlken. Copyright 2012, Diaphanes, Zürich-Berlin. Cover design: Suzan Beijer Lay-out: Crius Group, Hulshout isbn 978 94 6298 620 6 e-isbn 978 90 4853 742 6 doi 10.5117/9789462986206 nur 670 © S.
    [Show full text]
  • The Scope of Neuroethology
    THE BEHAVIORAL AND BRAIN SCIENCES (1984) 7, 367-412 Printed in the United States of America The scope of neuroethology Graham Hoyle Institute of Neuroscience, University of Oregon, Eugene, Oreg. 97403 Abstract: Neuroethology, an interdisciplinary subdivision of neuroscience, has emerged in recent years. Since 1976 there has been a regular session under this heading at the annual meeting of the Society for Neuroscience. In 1980 two introductory texts in English were published on the subject (Ewert 1980; Guthrie 1980), and a third (Camhi 1984) was published recently. There is widespread interest in neural mechanisms underlying behavior, but they encompass such a vast array of often unrelated topics that proponents do not share common goals. This article describes the emergence of ethology as a discipline, pointing out that its practitioners were successful because they confined their research to stereotyped, complex, nonlearned, innate behavioral acts. A limited number of profoundly significant principles emerged. Each of these is redefined. The major concepts of earlier ethology were embodied in a simple hydraulic model used by Konrad Lorenz in 1949 (Lorenz 1950). It is pointed out that this model implies the existence of common neurophysiological mechanisms and neuronal circuitry. This model has now been made obsolete by neurophysiological progress, but with appropriate ~nodificationsan updated version may still be useful in focusing attention on possible principles. The initial aim of neuroethology should be to examine the neurophysiological events in a variety of behaviors, exhibited by diverse animals from different phyla, which meet the criteria of innate behavioral acts. The behaviors should be sufficiently complex to interest ethologists, yet they should be addressable with neurophysiological methods down to the cellular level.
    [Show full text]
  • Advances in the Study of Behavior, Volume 31.Pdf
    Advances in THE STUDY OF BEHAVIOR VOLUME 31 Advances in THE STUDY OF BEHAVIOR Edited by PETER J. B. S LATER JAY S. ROSENBLATT CHARLES T. S NOWDON TIMOTHY J. R OPER Advances in THE STUDY OF BEHAVIOR Edited by PETER J. B. S LATER School of Biology University of St. Andrews Fife, United Kingdom JAY S. ROSENBLATT Institute of Animal Behavior Rutgers University Newark, New Jersey CHARLES T. S NOWDON Department of Psychology University of Wisconsin Madison, Wisconsin TIMOTHY J. R OPER School of Biological Sciences University of Sussex Sussex, United Kingdom VOLUME 31 San Diego San Francisco New York Boston London Sydney Tokyo This book is printed on acid-free paper. ∞ Copyright C 2002 by ACADEMIC PRESS All Rights Reserved. No part of this publication may be reproduced or transmitted in any form or by any means, electronic or mechanical, including photocopy, recording, or any information storage and retrieval system, without permission in writing from the Publisher. The appearance of the code at the bottom of the first page of a chapter in this book indicates the Publisher’s consent that copies of the chapter may be made for personal or internal use of specific clients. This consent is given on the condition, however, that the copier pay the stated per copy fee through the Copyright Clearance Center, Inc. (222 Rosewood Drive, Danvers, Massachusetts 01923), for copying beyond that permitted by Sections 107 or 108 of the U.S. Copyright Law. This consent does not extend to other kinds of copying, such as copying for general distribution, for advertising or promotional purposes, for creating new collective works, or for resale.
    [Show full text]
  • Critical Neuroscience and Philosophy
    Critical Neuroscience and Philosophy A Scientific Re-Examination of the Mind-Body Problem David Låg Tomasi Critical Neuroscience and Philosophy “A ‘scientific re-examination of the mind-body problem’ is certainly a ‘difficult task’ and Tomasi seems to navigate the rough water with a safe methodological approach. The book provides the reader with a comprehensive overview, which exhibits a remarkable balance in the presentation of disputed topics. In addition, the author provides the necessary tools to have both people with science or phi- losophy backgrounds acquainted to the topic. Neuro-lovers will appreciate and learn from the presentation of the numerous neuroscience ‘sub-branches,’ together with details on the methodological approaches used in the neuroscience research. Philosophers will enjoy the freedom and degree of theoretical abstraction, unusual in neurobiology books. Tomasi does in fact analyse the ‘mind-body problem’ with a critical appraisal that combines the rigidness of the scientific method with the speculative insight and thoroughness of the philosophy. The combination of the two sources of knowledge makes this book a fundamental tool for those who share the need to bridge the (apparent) gap between science and philosophy. Another key adjective for describing the book is multidisciplinary. The author spans from logic to quantum mechanics, from medicine to informatics, from reli- gion to ethics, from theory to practice. In all the cases the rigor in defining critical words makes even a lay reader feel like taken by the hand during the journey.” —Francesco Orzi, Professor of Neurology, Sapienza University of Rome (retired), and member of the Accademia dei Fisiocritici, Siena, Italy “Critical Neuroscience and Philosophy is impressive in many ways—from the scope and variety of information analyzed to the inspiration that scientists, philosophers, and the wider public will find in it.
    [Show full text]
  • The Emulation Theory of Representation: Motor Control, Imagery, and Perception
    BEHAVIORAL AND BRAIN SCIENCES (2004) 27, 377–442 Printed in the United States of America The emulation theory of representation: Motor control, imagery, and perception Rick Grush Department of Philosophy, University of California, San Diego, La Jolla, CA 92093-0119 [email protected] http://mind.ucsd.edu Abstract: The emulation theory of representation is developed and explored as a framework that can revealingly synthesize a wide vari- ety of representational functions of the brain. The framework is based on constructs from control theory (forward models) and signal processing (Kalman filters). The idea is that in addition to simply engaging with the body and environment, the brain constructs neural circuits that act as models of the body and environment. During overt sensorimotor engagement, these models are driven by efference copies in parallel with the body and environment, in order to provide expectations of the sensory feedback, and to enhance and process sensory information. These models can also be run off-line in order to produce imagery, estimate outcomes of different actions, and eval- uate and develop motor plans. The framework is initially developed within the context of motor control, where it has been shown that inner models running in parallel with the body can reduce the effects of feedback delay problems. The same mechanisms can account for motor imagery as the off-line driving of the emulator via efference copies. The framework is extended to account for visual imagery as the off-line driving of an emulator of the motor-visual loop. I also show how such systems can provide for amodal spatial imagery.
    [Show full text]
  • Space Constancy: the Rise and Fall of Perceptual Compensation
    P1: JZP Trim: 174mm × 247mm Top: 0.581in Gutter: 0.747in CUUK929-07 cuuk929/Nijhawan ISBN: 978 0 521 86318 6 October 7, 2009 15:1 7 Space constancy: the rise and fall of perceptual compensation bruce bridgeman Summary Information about eye position comes from efference copy, a record of the innervation to the extraocular muscles that move the eye and proprioceptive signals from sensors in the extraocular muscles. Together they define extraretinal signals and indicate the position of the eye. By pressing on the eyelid of a viewing eye, the extraocular muscles can be activated to maintain a steady gaze position without rotation of the eye. This procedure decouples efference copy from gaze position, making it possible to measure the gain of the efference copy signal. The gain is 0.61; the gain of oculomotor proprioception, measured by a similar eye press technique, is 0.26. The two signals together sum to only 0.87, leading to the conclusion that humans underestimate the deviations of their own eyes and that extraretinal signals cannot be the mechanisms underlying space constancy (the perception that the world remains stable despite eye movements). The underregistration of eye deviation accounts quantitatively for a previously unexplained illusion of visual direction. Extraretinal signals are used in static conditions, especially for controlling motor behavior. The role of extraretinal signals during a saccade, if any, is not to compensate the previous retinal position but to destroy it. Then perception can begin with a clean slate during the next fixation interval. 7.1 Introduction All visual information arrives in the brain through the retinas, whose images are displaced with each eye movement.
    [Show full text]
  • There Is No Interface (Without a User). a Cybernetic Perspective on Interaction
    INTERFACE CRITIQUE JOURNAL – VOL. I – 2018 THERE IS NO INTERFACE (WITHOUT A USER). A CYBERNETIC PERSPECTIVE ON INTERACTION By Lasse Scherffig “Interaction is seen as a one-way street, conveying a design model to a user, who is acting by that model either because they adapted to it, or because the model replicates their given structure. This is the cognitivist heritage of the HCI discourse responsible for the idea that interfaces can actually be designed.” Suggested citation: Scherffig, Lasse (2018). “There is no Interface (without a User). A cybernetic Perspective on Interaction.” In: Interface Critique Journal Vol.1. Eds. Florian Hadler, Alice Soiné, Daniel Irrgang DOI: 10.11588/ic.2018.0.44739 This article is released under a Creative Commons license (CC BY 4.0). 58 SCHERFFIG: THERE IS NO INTERFACE The interface in itself does not exist. rected development from the compu- This is not to say that any phenome- ting machinery of the past towards a non must be perceived in order to ex- future of interaction. ist, but rather that interfaces quite This trajectory constitutes the literally only come into being if they standard account of the history of in- are used. They are effects of interac- teraction. Often the field is seen as tion and thus they are ultimately cre- following a teleological development ated by their users. of progress, during which computers Of course, academic and pro- became more and more interactive, fessional disciplines like human- and interaction became more intui- computer interaction and interaction tive, rich, and natural. This develop- design assume the opposite: namely ment is often explicated as a that interfaces are designed (and ex- genealogy.
    [Show full text]
  • Brainwaves: a Cultural History of Electroencephalography Cornelius Borck, Translated by Ann M
    Brainwaves In the history of brain research, the prospect of visualizing brain processes has continually awakened great expectations. In this study, Cornelius Borck focuses on a recording technique developed by the German physiologist Hans Berger to register electric brain currents; a technique that was expected to allow the brain to write in its own language, and which would reveal the way the brain worked. Borck traces the numerous contradictory interpretations of electroencephalography, from Berger’s experiments and his publication of the first human EEG in 1929, to its international proliferation and consolidation as a clinical diagnostic method in the mid-twentieth century. Borck’s thesis is that the language of the brain takes on specific contours depending on the local investigative cultures, from whose conflicting views emerged a new scientific object: the electric brain. Cornelius Borck is Professor of History, Theory and Ethics of Medicine and Science and Director of the Institute of History of Medicine and Science Studies at the University of Lübeck, Germany. Science, Technology and Culture, 1700–1945 Series Editors Robert M. Brain The University of British Columbia, Canada and Ernst Hamm York University, Canada Science, Technology and Culture, 1700–1945 focuses on the social, cultural, industrial and economic contexts of science and technology from the ‘scientific revolution’ up to the Second World War. Publishing lively, original, innovative research across a broad spectrum of subjects and genres by an international list of authors,
    [Show full text]
  • Descarga Y Online ISBN 978-987-42-8555-3 1
    teseopress.com EL CONCEPTO DE FUNCIÓN Y LA EXPLICACIÓN FUNCIONAL DE LA NEUROETOLOGÍA teseopress.com teseopress.com EL CONCEPTO DE FUNCIÓN Y LA EXPLICACIÓN FUNCIONAL DE LA NEUROETOLOGÍA Andrea Soledad Isabel Olmos teseopress.com Olmos, Andrea Soledad Isabel El concepto de función y la explicación funcional de la neuro- etología / Andrea Soledad Isabel Olmos. – 1a ed. – Ciudad Autónoma de Buenos Aires : Andrea Soledad Isabel Olmos, 2018. Libro digital, EPUB Archivo Digital: descarga y online ISBN 978-987-42-8555-3 1. Filosofía de la Ciencia. I. Título. CDD 501 ISBN: 9789874285553 Las opiniones y los contenidos incluidos en esta publicación son responsabilidad exclusiva del/los autor/es. El concepto de función Compaginado desde TeseoPress (www.teseopress.com) ExLibrisTeseoPress 31205. Sólo para uso personal teseopress.com Índice Comité Editor del Departamento de Filosofía .......................9 Agradecimientos........................................................................... 11 Dedicatoria..................................................................................... 13 1. Introducción.............................................................................. 15 2. ¿Qué es la neuroetología?. Una introducción a la neurobiología del comportamiento animal .......................... 21 3. Los enfoques filosóficos......................................................... 45 4. La comunicación acústica del grillo de campo. Un caso de estudio .............................................................................. 89 5.
    [Show full text]