There Is No Interface (Without a User). a Cybernetic Perspective on Interaction

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

There Is No Interface (Without a User). a Cybernetic Perspective on Interaction INTERFACE CRITIQUE JOURNAL – VOL. I – 2018 THERE IS NO INTERFACE (WITHOUT A USER). A CYBERNETIC PERSPECTIVE ON INTERACTION By Lasse Scherffig “Interaction is seen as a one-way street, conveying a design model to a user, who is acting by that model either because they adapted to it, or because the model replicates their given structure. This is the cognitivist heritage of the HCI discourse responsible for the idea that interfaces can actually be designed.” Suggested citation: Scherffig, Lasse (2018). “There is no Interface (without a User). A cybernetic Perspective on Interaction.” In: Interface Critique Journal Vol.1. Eds. Florian Hadler, Alice Soiné, Daniel Irrgang DOI: 10.11588/ic.2018.0.44739 This article is released under a Creative Commons license (CC BY 4.0). 58 SCHERFFIG: THERE IS NO INTERFACE The interface in itself does not exist. rected development from the compu- This is not to say that any phenome- ting machinery of the past towards a non must be perceived in order to ex- future of interaction. ist, but rather that interfaces quite This trajectory constitutes the literally only come into being if they standard account of the history of in- are used. They are effects of interac- teraction. Often the field is seen as tion and thus they are ultimately cre- following a teleological development ated by their users. of progress, during which computers Of course, academic and pro- became more and more interactive, fessional disciplines like human- and interaction became more intui- computer interaction and interaction tive, rich, and natural. This develop- design assume the opposite: namely ment is often explicated as a that interfaces are designed (and ex- genealogy. Depending on the focus ist) before they are used, possibly and goals of their narrators, there are even creating their users. This article1 genealogies of interaction focusing traces the development of this view, on a succession of hardware genera- as well as offering an alternative to it tions, interaction paradigms, theo- that fundamentally understands any retic frameworks, or visionaries interface as “cybernetic interface.”2 pushing the field to the next level. An early and paradigmatic ac- count that focuses on hardware is John Walker’s genealogy of five “User GENEALOGIES OF Interaction Generations”4 published in the early 1990s. This account starts INTERACTION with the “plug boards” and “dedicated setups” of early computing.5 These When during the late 1990s, the new were followed by the (in)famous era millennium prompted countless ret- of batch processing – a time when rospectives and outlooks, Terry Wino- programming meant punching holes grad contributed a chapter to a book into cards, handing batches of these about the next fifty years of computer cards to a mainframe operator and science titled, “From Computing Ma- 3 waiting for hours to be handed back a chinery to Interaction Design.” Fol- printed result. lowing an idea of evolutionary It is only the third of these progress, this title described a goal di- generations that was interactive. As –––––––––––––––––––––––––––––––––––––––––––––––––––––––––––––––––– 1 A German language discussion of similar 3 Terry Winograd, “From Computing questions, that is much broader in scope, can be Machinery to Interaction Design,” in Beyond found in Lasse Scherffig, “Feedbackmaschinen. Calculation: The Next Fifty Years of Computing, ed. Kybernetik und Interaktion” (Dissertation, Peter Denning and Robert Metcalfe (Berlin and New Kunsthochschule für Medien Köln, 2017). York: Springer, 1997). 2 Søren Bro Pold, “Interface Perception: The 4 John Walker, “Through the looking glass,” in Cybernetic Mentality and Its Critics: The Art of Human-Computer Interface Design, ed. Ubermorgen.com,” in Interface Criticism: Aesthetics Brenda Laurel (Redding, MA: Addison-Wesley, 1990), Beyond Buttons, ed. Christian Ulrik Andersen and 439. Søren Bro Pold (Aarhus: Aarhus University Press, 5 Ibid. 439-440. 2011), 91. 59 INTERFACE CRITIQUE JOURNAL – VOL. I – 2018 to be expected from a proper genera- defined by “electrical interaction” (us- tion, it was a child of the previous one ing cables, plugs, and the soldering and generated by it: When, this ac- iron) with Walker’s dedicated set-ups. count goes, the algorithms allocating This was followed by the era of “sym- a mainframe’s computing time to bolic interaction” that was marked by several batch jobs got more and more the use of punch cards and batch pro- advanced, it became clear that it cessing – which were often pro- would be possible to divide the com- grammed using the symbols of puting time of a mainframe even fur- assembler languages instead of the ther. Divided into small enough raw zeros and ones of machine code. pieces, that follow each other in rapid This generation, in turn, led to the succession, it would seem to several “textual interaction” with the termi- people that they would have exclu- nals of time sharing systems. “Graph- sive control over the whole machine. ical interaction” here again marks the This idea, named “time sharing,” did final step in an evolution starting not divide a computer’s resources be- with machinery and ending with to- tween several batch jobs but between day’s interactive surfaces. several humans (re-defining these, as Apart from their implicit as- we will see below, as “users”) who sumption that interactivity progres- could now engage in “conversational sively increases, these, and most interactivity” with the machine.6 other histories of human-computer While the conversations of the time interaction (HCI) have one thing in sharing generation happened as ex- common: They all assume an origin changes of written text, the fourth of of interaction. While operating a com- Walker’s generation of interaction in- puter during Walker’s second genera- troduced graphical displays that con- tion meant batch processing, the centrated textual commands into third generation introduced time visual menus. The fifth and final gen- sharing and with it, interaction. In eration then spawned the graphical Dourish’s terminology, this transition user interfaces of personal compu- corresponds to the shift from “sym- ting that, in various iterations, keep bolic” (based on assembler language accompanying us on our desktops, and punchcards) to “textual interac- laptops, and phones until today.7 tion” (based on conversational inter- Paul Dourish’s “History of In- activity via command line). Only teraction,”8 which is much more con- when computers, after time-sharing temporary in style, follows a very was introduced, started to react similar path “from soldering to (seemingly) exclusively and directly mouse.” Focusing on the mode of in- to human input, did they become in- teraction instead of hardware gener- teractive: “Arguably, this is the origin ations, the first generation here was of »interactive« computing.”9 –––––––––––––––––––––––––––––––––––––––––––––––––––––––––––––––––– 6 Ibid. 441. 8 Paul Dourish, Where the action is. The 7 Ibid. 441-442. foundations of embodied interaction (Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 2001), 1-17. 9 Ibid. 10. 60 SCHERFFIG: THERE IS NO INTERFACE Of course, history is not that ing with analog (i.e. continuous) val- simple. A closer look behind the nar- ues, it rather implied an entirely dif- ratives postulating a teleological de- ferent approach toward calculation: It velopment of interaction instead relied on building electrical and me- reveals contradictory and asynchro- chanic systems, that, as analogues or nous developments, as well as chron- analogies, could stand in for the sys- ological overlaps.10 Especially the tems they were built to simulate. Be- origin of interactive computing itself cause building such analog can be described differently, for in- computers entailed accurately fol- stance, by looking at the first interac- lowing and amplifying changing tive computer ever built – which physical signals, it largely depended happens to be one of the first comput- on another development: the rise of ers at all. It is, as this look reveals, the the use of negative feedback as the de very first generation of computing facto standard method for handling machinery that defined interactivity electro-mechanical systems. In fact, up to this day, including its problems. during the early twentieth century, negative feedback became so im- portant in both control and communi- cation engineering that both INTERACTIVE disciplines merged into one feedback COMPUTERS AS based control theory – in a paradigm shift that yielded the era of “classi- FEEDBACK cal”12 control. This development, in turn, constituted the nucleus of what MACHINES Norbert Wiener would later call cyber- netics – a science of “control and During the 1940s, the MIT Servomech- communication in the animal and the anisms Lab started to build a flight machine”13 that would become think- simulator. As the leading paradigm able mainly because the application for automatic computation at that of negative feedback and the associ- time was analog computing, the flight ated mathematical formalisms simulator was planned to be based on seemed to be powerful enough to that: “a cockpit or control cabin con- tackle any form of “behavior” – of liv- nected, somehow, to an analog com- ing and non-living systems.14 Be- puter.”11 “Analog computing” in this cause feedback implies using the context did not only imply calculat- output of a system as its own input, the systems of cybernetics exhibited –––––––––––––––––––––––––––––––––––––––––––––––––––––––––––––––––– 10 Hans Dieter Hellige, “Krisen- und Innovati- 12 Stuart Bennett, A History of Control onsphasen in der Mensch-Computer-Interaktion,” in Engineering 1930–1955 (Hitchin: Peter Peregrinus Mensch-Computer-Interface. Zur Geschichte und Zu- Ltd., 1993), 17. kunft der Computerbedienung, ed. Hans Dieter Hellige 13 Norbert Wiener, Cybernetics or: Control and (Bielefeld: Transcript, 2008), 15-20. Communication in the Animal and the Machine 11 Kent C. Redmond and Thomas M. Smith, (Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 1961). Project Whirlwind: the history of a pioneer computer 14 Arturo Rosenblueth, Norbert Wiener, and (Bedford, MA: Digital Press, 1980), 32. Julian Bigelow, “Behavior, Purpose and Teleology,” Philosophy of Science 10 (1943): 18–24.
Recommended publications
  • Sensory Biology of Aquatic Animals
    Jelle Atema Richard R. Fay Arthur N. Popper William N. Tavolga Editors Sensory Biology of Aquatic Animals Springer-Verlag New York Berlin Heidelberg London Paris Tokyo JELLE ATEMA, Boston University Marine Program, Marine Biological Laboratory, Woods Hole, Massachusetts 02543, USA Richard R. Fay, Parmly Hearing Institute, Loyola University, Chicago, Illinois 60626, USA ARTHUR N. POPPER, Department of Zoology, University of Maryland, College Park, MD 20742, USA WILLIAM N. TAVOLGA, Mote Marine Laboratory, Sarasota, Florida 33577, USA The cover Illustration is a reproduction of Figure 13.3, p. 343 of this volume Library of Congress Cataloging-in-Publication Data Sensory biology of aquatic animals. Papers based on presentations given at an International Conference on the Sensory Biology of Aquatic Animals held, June 24-28, 1985, at the Mote Marine Laboratory in Sarasota, Fla. Bibliography: p. Includes indexes. 1. Aquatic animals—Physiology—Congresses. 2. Senses and Sensation—Congresses. I. Atema, Jelle. II. International Conference on the Sensory Biology - . of Aquatic Animals (1985 : Sarasota, Fla.) QL120.S46 1987 591.92 87-9632 © 1988 by Springer-Verlag New York Inc. x —• All rights reserved. This work may not be translated or copied in whole or in part without the written permission of the publisher (Springer-Verlag, 175 Fifth Avenue, New York 10010, U.S.A.), except for brief excerpts in connection with reviews or scholarly analysis. Use in connection with any form of Information storage and retrieval, electronic adaptation, Computer Software, or by similar or dissimilar methodology now known or hereafter developed is forbidden. The use of general descriptive names, trade names, trademarks, etc.
    [Show full text]
  • Perception, Perfusion & Posture
    Perception, Perfusion & Posture Billy L Luu Ph.D. Thesis University of New South Wales 2010 THE UNIVERSITY OF NEW SOUTH WALES Thesis/Dissertation Sheet Surname or Family name: LUU First name: BILLY Other name/s: LIANG Abbreviation for degree as given in the University calendar: PhD School: MEDICAL SCIENCES Faculty: MEDICINE Title: PERCEPTION, PERFUSION & POSTURE Upright posture is a fundamental and critical human behaviour that places unique demands on the brain for motor and cardiovascular control. This thesis examines broad aspects of sensorimotor and cardiovascular function to examine their interdependence, with particular emphasis on the physiological processes operating during standing. How we perceive the forces our muscles exert was investigated by contralateral weight matching in the upper limb. The muscles on one side were weakened to about half their strength by fatigue and by paralysis with curare. In two subjects with dense large-diameter neuropathy, fatigue made lifted objects feel twice as heavy but in normal subjects it made objects feel lighter. Complete paralysis and recovery to half strength also made lifted objects feel lighter. Taken together, these results show that although a signal within the brain is available, this is not how muscle force is normally perceived. Instead, we use a signal that is largely peripheral reafference, which includes a dominant contribution from muscle spindles. Unlike perception of force when lifting weights, it is shown that we perceive only about 15% of the force applied by the legs to balance the body when standing. A series of experiments show that half of the force is provided by passive mechanics and most of the active muscle force is produced by a sub- cortical motor drive that does not give rise to a sense of muscular force through central efference copy or reafference.
    [Show full text]
  • Zootechnologies
    Zootechnologies A Media History of Swarm Research SEBASTIAN VEHLKEN Amsterdam University Press Zootechnologies The book series RECURSIONS: THEORIES OF MEDIA, MATERIALITY, AND CULTURAL TECHNIQUES provides a platform for cuttingedge research in the field of media culture studies with a particular focus on the cultural impact of media technology and the materialities of communication. The series aims to be an internationally significant and exciting opening into emerging ideas in media theory ranging from media materialism and hardware-oriented studies to ecology, the post-human, the study of cultural techniques, and recent contributions to media archaeology. The series revolves around key themes: – The material underpinning of media theory – New advances in media archaeology and media philosophy – Studies in cultural techniques These themes resonate with some of the most interesting debates in international media studies, where non-representational thought, the technicity of knowledge formations and new materialities expressed through biological and technological developments are changing the vocabularies of cultural theory. The series is also interested in the mediatic conditions of such theoretical ideas and developing them as media theory. Editorial Board – Jussi Parikka (University of Southampton) – Anna Tuschling (Ruhr-Universität Bochum) – Geoffrey Winthrop-Young (University of British Columbia) Zootechnologies A Media History of Swarm Research Sebastian Vehlken Translated by Valentine A. Pakis Amsterdam University Press This publication is funded by MECS Institute for Advanced Study on Media Cultures of Computer Simulation, Leuphana University Lüneburg (German Research Foundation Project KFOR 1927). Already published as: Zootechnologien. Eine Mediengeschichte der Schwarmforschung, Sebastian Vehlken. Copyright 2012, Diaphanes, Zürich-Berlin. Cover design: Suzan Beijer Lay-out: Crius Group, Hulshout isbn 978 94 6298 620 6 e-isbn 978 90 4853 742 6 doi 10.5117/9789462986206 nur 670 © S.
    [Show full text]
  • The Scope of Neuroethology
    THE BEHAVIORAL AND BRAIN SCIENCES (1984) 7, 367-412 Printed in the United States of America The scope of neuroethology Graham Hoyle Institute of Neuroscience, University of Oregon, Eugene, Oreg. 97403 Abstract: Neuroethology, an interdisciplinary subdivision of neuroscience, has emerged in recent years. Since 1976 there has been a regular session under this heading at the annual meeting of the Society for Neuroscience. In 1980 two introductory texts in English were published on the subject (Ewert 1980; Guthrie 1980), and a third (Camhi 1984) was published recently. There is widespread interest in neural mechanisms underlying behavior, but they encompass such a vast array of often unrelated topics that proponents do not share common goals. This article describes the emergence of ethology as a discipline, pointing out that its practitioners were successful because they confined their research to stereotyped, complex, nonlearned, innate behavioral acts. A limited number of profoundly significant principles emerged. Each of these is redefined. The major concepts of earlier ethology were embodied in a simple hydraulic model used by Konrad Lorenz in 1949 (Lorenz 1950). It is pointed out that this model implies the existence of common neurophysiological mechanisms and neuronal circuitry. This model has now been made obsolete by neurophysiological progress, but with appropriate ~nodificationsan updated version may still be useful in focusing attention on possible principles. The initial aim of neuroethology should be to examine the neurophysiological events in a variety of behaviors, exhibited by diverse animals from different phyla, which meet the criteria of innate behavioral acts. The behaviors should be sufficiently complex to interest ethologists, yet they should be addressable with neurophysiological methods down to the cellular level.
    [Show full text]
  • Cybernetique D'une Theorie De La Connaissance
    Trivium Revue franco-allemande de sciences humaines et sociales - Deutsch-französische Zeitschrift für Geistes- und Sozialwissenschaften 20 | 2015 Réflexivité et Système. Le débat sur l'ordre et l'auto- organisation dans les années 1970 Cybernetique d’une theorie de la connaissance Heinz von Foerster Traducteur : Didier Renault Édition électronique URL : http://journals.openedition.org/trivium/5178 ISSN : 1963-1820 Éditeur Les éditions de la Maison des sciences de l’Homme Référence électronique Heinz von Foerster, « Cybernetique d’une theorie de la connaissance », Trivium [En ligne], 20 | 2015, mis en ligne le 11 juin 2015, consulté le 08 septembre 2020. URL : http://journals.openedition.org/ trivium/5178 Ce document a été généré automatiquement le 8 septembre 2020. Les contenus des la revue Trivium sont mis à disposition selon les termes de la Licence Creative Commons Attribution - Pas d'Utilisation Commerciale - Pas de Modification 4.0 International. Cybernetique d’une theorie de la connaissance 1 Cybernetique d’une theorie de la connaissance Heinz von Foerster Traduction : Didier Renault NOTE DE L’ÉDITEUR Nous remercions la maison d’édition Suhrkamp de nous avoir accordé l’autorisation de traduire ce texte pour le présent numéro. Wir danken dem Suhrkamp Verlag für die freundliche Genehmigung, diesen Artikel in französischer Übersetzung zu publizieren. 1 Lorsque j’ai accepté de prononcer ici ma conférence1 en allemand, je ne soupçonnais pas les difficultés auxquelles je me verrais confronté. Au cours des vingt dernières années, dans mon activité scientifique, j’ai exclusivement pensé et parlé en anglais. Nombre de concepts et de résultats de recherches ont été baptisés en anglais lors de leur naissance, et se montrent rétifs à toute traduction.
    [Show full text]
  • The Emulation Theory of Representation: Motor Control, Imagery, and Perception
    BEHAVIORAL AND BRAIN SCIENCES (2004) 27, 377–442 Printed in the United States of America The emulation theory of representation: Motor control, imagery, and perception Rick Grush Department of Philosophy, University of California, San Diego, La Jolla, CA 92093-0119 [email protected] http://mind.ucsd.edu Abstract: The emulation theory of representation is developed and explored as a framework that can revealingly synthesize a wide vari- ety of representational functions of the brain. The framework is based on constructs from control theory (forward models) and signal processing (Kalman filters). The idea is that in addition to simply engaging with the body and environment, the brain constructs neural circuits that act as models of the body and environment. During overt sensorimotor engagement, these models are driven by efference copies in parallel with the body and environment, in order to provide expectations of the sensory feedback, and to enhance and process sensory information. These models can also be run off-line in order to produce imagery, estimate outcomes of different actions, and eval- uate and develop motor plans. The framework is initially developed within the context of motor control, where it has been shown that inner models running in parallel with the body can reduce the effects of feedback delay problems. The same mechanisms can account for motor imagery as the off-line driving of the emulator via efference copies. The framework is extended to account for visual imagery as the off-line driving of an emulator of the motor-visual loop. I also show how such systems can provide for amodal spatial imagery.
    [Show full text]
  • Space Constancy: the Rise and Fall of Perceptual Compensation
    P1: JZP Trim: 174mm × 247mm Top: 0.581in Gutter: 0.747in CUUK929-07 cuuk929/Nijhawan ISBN: 978 0 521 86318 6 October 7, 2009 15:1 7 Space constancy: the rise and fall of perceptual compensation bruce bridgeman Summary Information about eye position comes from efference copy, a record of the innervation to the extraocular muscles that move the eye and proprioceptive signals from sensors in the extraocular muscles. Together they define extraretinal signals and indicate the position of the eye. By pressing on the eyelid of a viewing eye, the extraocular muscles can be activated to maintain a steady gaze position without rotation of the eye. This procedure decouples efference copy from gaze position, making it possible to measure the gain of the efference copy signal. The gain is 0.61; the gain of oculomotor proprioception, measured by a similar eye press technique, is 0.26. The two signals together sum to only 0.87, leading to the conclusion that humans underestimate the deviations of their own eyes and that extraretinal signals cannot be the mechanisms underlying space constancy (the perception that the world remains stable despite eye movements). The underregistration of eye deviation accounts quantitatively for a previously unexplained illusion of visual direction. Extraretinal signals are used in static conditions, especially for controlling motor behavior. The role of extraretinal signals during a saccade, if any, is not to compensate the previous retinal position but to destroy it. Then perception can begin with a clean slate during the next fixation interval. 7.1 Introduction All visual information arrives in the brain through the retinas, whose images are displaced with each eye movement.
    [Show full text]
  • Connecting the Work of Gregory Bateson, Deleuze And
    INTERFACEINTERFACE:: CONNECTINGCONNECTING THETHE WORKWORK OOFF GREGORYGREGORY BATESON,BATESON, DELEUZDELEUZEE ANANDD GUATTARI,GUATTARI, ANANDD ALAIALAINN BADIOUBADIOU bbyy AdeleAdele HavertyHaverty BealerBealer A thesithesiss submittedsubmitted ttoo ththee facultyfaculty ofof TheThe UniversitUniversityy ooff UtahUtah iinn partialpartial fulfillmentfulfillment ofof thethe requirementrequirementss forfor ththee degreedegree ofof MasteMasterr ooff ArtsArts IIIin EnvironmentaEnvironmentall HumanitiesHumanities DepartmenDepartmentt ooff CommunicationCommunication TheThe UniversitUniversityy ooff UtahUtah AugustAugust 20082008 CopyrighCopyrightt © AdelAdelee HavertHavertyy BealeBealerr 20082008 AlAlll RightRightss ReservedReserved THE UNIVERSITY OF UTAH GRADUATE SCHOOL SUPERVISORY COMMITTEE APPROVAL of a thesis submitted by Adele Haverty Bealer This thesis has been read by each member of the following supervisory committee and by majority vote has been found to be satisfactory. inJbw&& --------:=:---.... '"'" ceHeonard C. Hawes r 7 �__ __-7) THE UNIVERSITY OF UTAH GRADUATE SCHOOL FINAL READING APPROVAL To the GraduateCouncil of the University of Utah: [ have read the thesis of Adele Haverty Bealer in its final form and have found that (1) its format, citations, and bibliographic style are consistent and acceptable; (2) its illustrative materials including figures, tables, and charts are in place; and (3) the final manuscript is satisfactory to the supervisory committee and is ready for submission toThe Graduate School. �--- pate , Chair: SupervisoryCommittee
    [Show full text]
  • Brainwaves: a Cultural History of Electroencephalography Cornelius Borck, Translated by Ann M
    Brainwaves In the history of brain research, the prospect of visualizing brain processes has continually awakened great expectations. In this study, Cornelius Borck focuses on a recording technique developed by the German physiologist Hans Berger to register electric brain currents; a technique that was expected to allow the brain to write in its own language, and which would reveal the way the brain worked. Borck traces the numerous contradictory interpretations of electroencephalography, from Berger’s experiments and his publication of the first human EEG in 1929, to its international proliferation and consolidation as a clinical diagnostic method in the mid-twentieth century. Borck’s thesis is that the language of the brain takes on specific contours depending on the local investigative cultures, from whose conflicting views emerged a new scientific object: the electric brain. Cornelius Borck is Professor of History, Theory and Ethics of Medicine and Science and Director of the Institute of History of Medicine and Science Studies at the University of Lübeck, Germany. Science, Technology and Culture, 1700–1945 Series Editors Robert M. Brain The University of British Columbia, Canada and Ernst Hamm York University, Canada Science, Technology and Culture, 1700–1945 focuses on the social, cultural, industrial and economic contexts of science and technology from the ‘scientific revolution’ up to the Second World War. Publishing lively, original, innovative research across a broad spectrum of subjects and genres by an international list of authors,
    [Show full text]
  • Varela 1996 the Specious Pres
    From: Naturalizing Phenomenology: Issues in Contemporary Phenomenology and Cognitive Science Edited by Jean, Petitot, Francisco J. Varela, Bernard Pachoud abd Jean-Michel Roy Stanford University Press, Stanford Chapter 9, pp.266-329 The Specious Present: A Neurophenomenology of Time Consciousness FRANCISCO J. VARELA I. SETTING THE STAGE My purpose in this essay is to propose an explicitly naturalized account of the experience of present nowness based on two complementary approaches: phenomenological analysis and cognitive neuroscience. What 1 mean by naturalization, and the role cognitive neuroscience plays, will become clear as this essay unfolds.1 It would be foolish to claim that one can tackle this topic and expect to be satisfied. The experience of temporality addresses head-on the fundamental fact that we exist within a transparent web of time. The elucidation of the experience of temporality occupies a central place in the history of thought altogether, and most certainly in the phenomenological tradition. Nlany have referred to St. Augustine's Confessions (specifically book I I) on the paradoxes of a past-containing nowness (distentio animi), or more recently to the pragmatic psychology of William james as found in the famous Principles of Psycbology, where the apt expression “specious present,” which gives this article its title, first appeared in print.2 Edmund Husserl considered temporality a foundational axis of his phenomenological research: all other forms of mental activity depend on temporality, but it depends on none of them. He worked on these questions until death (see Appendix A). Unlike his illustrious predecessors, who never pursued their descriptions far enough, Husserl did manage to make real progress in the formulation of the basic structures of intimate time.
    [Show full text]
  • Metatemas Libros Para Pensar La Ciencia Colección Dirigida Por Jorge Wagensberg
    Metatemas Libros para pensar la ciencia Colección dirigida por Jorge Wagensberg Al cuidado del equipo científico del Museu de la Ciencia de la Fundació "la Caixa" * Alef, símbolo de los números transfinitos de Cantor Richard Dawkins DESTEJIENDO EL ARCO IRIS Ciencia, ilusión, y el deseo de asombro Traducción de Joandoménec Ros Título original: Unweaving the Rainbow 1." edición: enero 2000 © Richard Dawkins, 1998 © de la traducción: Joandoménec Ros, 2000 Diseño de la cubierta: BM Reservados todos los derechos de esta edición para Tusquets Editores, S.A. - Cesare Cantü, 8 - 08023 Barcelona ISBN: 84-8310-669-8 Depósito legal: B. 710-2000 Fotocomposición: David Pablo Impreso sobre papel Offset-F Crudo de Papelera del Leizarán, S.A. Liberdúplex, S.L. - Constitución, 19 - 08014 Barcelona Impreso en España 9 Prefacio 17 1. La anestesia de la familiaridad 31 2. El salón de los duques 55 3. Códigos de barras en las estrellas 83 4. Códigos de barras en el aire 99 5. Códigos de barras en el estrado 131 6. Embaucados por la fantasía de las hadas 163 7. Destejiendo lo sobrenatural 197 8. Enormes símbolos nebulosos de un romance elevado 227 9. El cooperador egoísta 251 10. El libro genético de los muertos 273 11. Volviendo a tejer el mundo 303 12. El globo de la mente Apéndices 333 Bibliografía 343 índice onomástico y de materias Para Lalla Prefacio Un editor extranjero de mi primer libro {El gen egoísta, 1976) me confesó que después de leerlo no pudo dormir durante tres noches; hasta tal punto llegó a perturbarlo su, para él, frío y desolado mensaje.
    [Show full text]
  • From Reflexes and Motor Programs to the Equilibrium-Point Hypothesis
    Journal of Human Kinetics volume 19 2008, 3-24 DOI 10.2478/v10078-008-0001-2 3 © Editorial Committee of Journal of Human Kinetics Evolution of Motor Control: From Reflexes and Motor Programs to the Equilibrium-Point Hypothesis by Mark L. Latash1 This brief review analyzes the evolution of motor control theories along two lines that emphasize active (motor programs) and reactive (reflexes) features of voluntary movements. It suggests that the only contemporary hypothesis that integrates both approaches in a fruitful way is the equilib- rium-point hypothesis. Physical, physiological, and behavioral founda- tions of the EP-hypothesis are considered as well as relations between the EP-hypothesis and the recent developments of the notion of motor syner- gies. The paper ends with a brief review of the criticisms of the EP-hy- pothesis and challenges that the hypothesis faces at this time. Key words:motor control, reflexes, equilibrium-point hypothesis. 1 - The Pennsylvania State University, University Park, PA 16801, USA 4 Evolution of Motor Control: From Reflexes and Motor Programs to the … An Introduction: The History of the Active-Reactive Argument The origins of voluntary movement and of the relations between human movements and their controller, the central nervous system (CNS), have been fascinating scientists at least since the times of the great Greek philosophers of the past. At that time, the problem of movement-CNS relation was more com- monly formulated as that of the relation between the moving body and the controlling soul. For example, Plato viewed self-motion as a sign of immortal soul, which was apparently inherent to all animals capable of voluntary move- ments.
    [Show full text]