Association Learning in the Acute Confusional State

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Association Learning in the Acute Confusional State J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry: first published as 10.1136/jnnp.65.3.390 on 1 September 1998. Downloaded from 390 J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 1998;65:390–392 SHORT REPORT Association learning in the acute confusional state Radek Ptak, Klemens Gutbrod, Armin Schnider Abstract and global disturbance of attention in these The usefulness of cognitive rehabilitative conditions.1 Patients in acute confusional state treatment in the acute stages after brain fail to focus on relevant stimuli, react slowly, injury seems questionable because pa- and fail to sustain their attention in tients in severe acute confusional state conversation.2 Alertness and the sleep-wake early after coma clinically seem unable to cycle are usually disturbed, resulting in leth- learn and store new information. There- argy or agitation.3 Some features of the acute fore, the capability of patients in acute confusional state—for example, disorientation, confusional state to learn and retain asso- hallucinations, language, and thought ciative information was assessed. On two disorders—may sometimes be secondary to occasions pairs of simple nouns were pre- attentional diYculties.4 sented to six patients in severe acute con- Clinical evidence suggests that patients in fusional state. Stimuli were presented acute confusional state fail to store ongoing repeatedly either in written form only or events and that after resolution of the acute with additional pictorial representations. confusional state only “islands of memory” Immediate and 20 minutes delayed recall remain.5 However, a systematic investigation of was measured. Patients in acute confu- explicit memory capabilities of patients with sional state were able to learn progres- head injury in severe acute confusional state sively more word pairs across several has never been done. presentations. They retained some infor- Despite this lack of knowledge and the very mation over an interval of 20 minutes. In severe memory disturbance of acutely con- addition, they learned and remembered fused patients, there has been growing demand pictorially supported associations better to start rehabilitative treatment early in the than pure verbal associations. Patients in course of illness.6 However, the ability to severe acute confusional state may retain acquire declarative knowledge is important for some explicit information and may profit rehabilitative eVorts and may even enhance or from an imagery mnemonic aid. These facilitate procedural learning. results were not expected on the basis of The present study investigated declarative clinical findings alone and they have memory in severe acute confusional state. The http://jnnp.bmj.com/ potential implications for the care of goals of this study were to test whether patients patients in acute confusional state. in acute confusional state are able to retain (J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 1998;65:390–392) simple associations after several presentations of the material and whether they benefit from a Keywords: confusional state; traumatic brain injury; visual mnemonic. post-traumatic amnesia Division of Subjects and methods Neuropsychological on October 2, 2021 by guest. Protected copyright. Rehabilitation, The acute confusional state is a common con- Patients in severe acute confusional state, Department of sequence of head trauma, stroke, infections, or several of them just gaining consciousness after Neurology, University other diseases of the brain, and reflects severe coma, were selected. Waxing and waning of Hospital, Bern, Switzerland Table 1 Demographic and clinical characteristics of the patients with acute confusional state R Ptak K Gutbrod Time after A Schnider Education injury Orientation Age Sex (y) (days) Aetiology Initial CT findings score* Correspondence to: 27 f 12 40 TBI Bilateral frontotemporal, right 11 Dr Radek Ptak, Division of medial temporal, and thalamic Neuropsychological contusion Rehabilitation, Department 27 m 13 14 TBI Right medial temporal contusion 10 of Neurology, University 19 m 13 5 TBI Right anterior medial temporal 13 Hospital, CH-3010 Bern, lobe, basal ganglia, and basal Switzerland. Telephone 0041 forebrain contusion 31 6324729; fax 0041 31 35 f 17 44 TBI Left frontotemporal contusion 8 6329770. 32 f 11 21 TBI Left parietotemporal contusion 14 55 m 16 25 Aneurysm rupture (right A Subarachnoidal haemorrhage in 13 Received 3 November 1997 cerebri media) right sylvian fissure and in revised form 16 February 1998 TBI=traumatic brain injury. Accepted 24 February 1998 *A score <15 indicates disorientation.14 J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry: first published as 10.1136/jnnp.65.3.390 on 1 September 1998. Downloaded from Association learning in the acute confusional state 391 attention was mandatory for the diagnosis. Day 1 Day 2 Patients showed marked disturbances in con- sciousness and attention, diurnal fluctuations, 4 and disorientation. Reduced vigilance, drowsi- ness, and lethargy were common features of all patients even during the short phases of relative 3 wakefulness. Consensus of three independent clinicians on the diagnosis of acute confusional 2 state was required to include patients. In- formed consent was obtained from the caregivers of all subjects. From 11 patients, five 1 were unable to complete experimental sessions due to their impaired arousal. The table Correctly recalled words (n) presents demographic characteristics of the six 0 Trial 1 Trial 5 Delayed Trial 1 Trial 5 Delayed patients included in the study. recall recall For the severe attentional failure of our Mean performance of patients in acute confusional state patients, two simple associate learning para- learning associations between simple nouns, which were digms were used. Sixteen concrete and image- presented either in written form only (solid line) or with able words were used to prepare two lists of additional pictorial representation (dashed line). Error bars four semantically unrelated word pairs. This show SEM. limited number of items was considered to be by the significant interaction of condition×trial relevant because patients may lose motivation ( (2,10)=5.4, p<0.05). This interaction was and score at bottom if the amount of F due to the fact that only in the WORD condi- information surpasses their capacities. In one tion performance of trial 5 was better than trial condition (WORD), word pairs were printed 1 and dropped significantly after 20 minutes. on white sheet (for example, BUTTER- Recall of pictorial material was generally LANDSCAPE). The other condition (PICT) better than recall of words alone ( (1,5)=8.9, consisted of both words and line drawings rep- F p<0.05). Finally, patients’ performance was resenting the concepts (for example, the better on day 2 compared with day 1 picture of an APPLE on the head of a NUN). ( (1,5)=6.3, p<0.05). On two consecutive days, patients partici- F These results are representative of the whole pated in a total of four experimental sessions. study group. Five patients performed better in Each session consisted of five learning trials of the PICT condition than the WORD condi- either the WORD or PICT condition followed tion, whereas one patient scored equally on by a 20 minute delayed recall. In each of the both conditions. All six patients were better on five learning trials, patients first saw every item trial 5 than trial 1. Finally, five patients were for 5 seconds and attempted to read it aloud. If better on day 2 than day 1, and one patient a patient did not succeed at reading, words scored equally on both days. This pattern were read aloud by the investigator. Immedi- implies that the eVects found were stable and ately after presentation of the entire list, in both not biased by extreme distribution of scores conditions the first word of the word pair was across patients. presented in written form only and subjects were asked to recall the second word. Presenta- tion of items was random in every trial. Twenty Discussion http://jnnp.bmj.com/ minutes later without further learning a Patients in this study showed a residual ability delayed recall followed. Beginning with the to learn simple associations and to retain some WORD condition, WORD and PICT were information for 20 minutes. Performance in the learned in two separate sessions on day 1. This immediate recall during five learning trials procedure was repeated on day 2, using the increased significantly only in the WORD con- same word lists. dition. This was due to the relatively good recall in the PICT condition already in the first STATISTICAL ANALYSIS learning trial. A ceiling eVect precluded further on October 2, 2021 by guest. Protected copyright. Because considerable variation of the data was increase in performance. A second finding was expected, the raw scores were square root that overall performance was better on the sec- transformed. A three way repeated measures ond day. Finally, there was an eVect of experi- analysis of variance (ANOVA) with main mental condition with pictorial material lead- factors of experimental condition ing to better retention than verbal material. (WORD×PICT), learning trial (trial 1×trial Cognitive functions in the acute stages after 5×delayed recall), and day (day 1×day2)was coma are rarely the subject of experimental performed. Significant interactions were ana- investigation. This lack of research is due to the lysed using post hoc t tests with Bonferroni fact that confused or disoriented patients are corrected significance levels. often unable to cooperate. For the same reason, five of our 11 patients had to be excluded from Results the study. Several studies investigated memory The figure shows that patients were able to impairment in post-traumatic amnesia. Post- learn progressively more word pairs across five traumatic amnesia is defined as the period after learning trials and that they recalled some traumatic brain injury during which patients information even after 20 minutes (trials are unable to store ongoing events.7 It is a F(2,10)=12.0, p<0.01). This eVect diVered in related but not identical concept to acute con- the two experimental conditions, as reflected fusional state, and some patients with post- J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry: first published as 10.1136/jnnp.65.3.390 on 1 September 1998.
Recommended publications
  • How Trauma Impacts Four Different Types of Memory
    How Trauma Impacts Four Different Types of Memory EXPLICIT MEMORY IMPLICIT MEMORY SEMANTIC MEMORY EPISODIC MEMORY EMOTIONAL MEMORY PROCEDURAL MEMORY What It Is What It Is What It Is What It Is The memory of general knowledge and The autobiographical memory of an event The memory of the emotions you felt The memory of how to perform a facts. or experience – including the who, what, during an experience. common task without actively thinking and where. Example Example Example Example You remember what a bicycle is. You remember who was there and what When a wave of shame or anxiety grabs You can ride a bicycle automatically, with- street you were on when you fell off your you the next time you see your bicycle out having to stop and recall how it’s bicycle in front of a crowd. after the big fall. done. How Trauma Can Affect It How Trauma Can Affect It How Trauma Can Affect It How Trauma Can Affect It Trauma can prevent information (like Trauma can shutdown episodic memory After trauma, a person may get triggered Trauma can change patterns of words, images, sounds, etc.) from differ- and fragment the sequence of events. and experience painful emotions, often procedural memory. For example, a ent parts of the brain from combining to without context. person might tense up and unconsciously make a semantic memory. alter their posture, which could lead to pain or even numbness. Related Brain Area Related Brain Area Related Brain Area Related Brain Area The temporal lobe and inferior parietal The hippocampus is responsible for The amygdala plays a key role in The striatum is associated with producing cortex collect information from different creating and recalling episodic memory.
    [Show full text]
  • Explicit Memory Bias for Body-Related Stimuli in Eating Disorders
    Louisiana State University LSU Digital Commons LSU Historical Dissertations and Theses Graduate School 1994 Explicit Memory Bias for Body-Related Stimuli in Eating Disorders. Shannon Buckles Sebastian Louisiana State University and Agricultural & Mechanical College Follow this and additional works at: https://digitalcommons.lsu.edu/gradschool_disstheses Recommended Citation Sebastian, Shannon Buckles, "Explicit Memory Bias for Body-Related Stimuli in Eating Disorders." (1994). LSU Historical Dissertations and Theses. 5826. https://digitalcommons.lsu.edu/gradschool_disstheses/5826 This Dissertation is brought to you for free and open access by the Graduate School at LSU Digital Commons. It has been accepted for inclusion in LSU Historical Dissertations and Theses by an authorized administrator of LSU Digital Commons. For more information, please contact [email protected]. INFORMATION TO USERS This manuscript has been reproduced from the microfilm master. UMI films the text directly from the original or copy submitted. Thus, some thesis and dissertation copies are in typewriter face, while others may be from any type of computer printer. The quality of this reproduction is dependent upon the quality of the copy submitted. Broken or indistinct print, colored or poor quality illustrations and photographs, print bleedthrough, substandard margins, and improper alignment can adversely affect reproduction. In the unlikely event that the author did not send UMI a complete manuscript and there are missing pages, these will be noted. Also, if unauthorized copyright material had to be removed, a note will indicate the deletion. Oversize materials (e.g., maps, drawings, charts) are reproduced by sectioning the original, beginning at the upper left-hand corner and continuing from left to right in equal sections with small overlaps.
    [Show full text]
  • Effect of the Time-Of-Day of Training on Explicit Memory
    Time-of-dayBrazilian Journal of training of Medical and memory and Biological Research (2008) 41: 477-481 477 ISSN 0100-879X Effect of the time-of-day of training on explicit memory F.F. Barbosa and F.S. Albuquerque Departamento de Fisiologia, Universidade Federal do Rio Grande do Norte, Natal, RN, Brasil Correspondence to: F.F. Barbosa, Departamento de Fisiologia, Universidade Federal do Rio Grande do Norte, Lagoa Nova, Caixa Postal 1511, 59072-970 Natal, RN, Brasil Fax: +55-84-211-9206. E-mail: [email protected] Studies have shown a time-of-day of training effect on long-term explicit memory with a greater effect being shown in the afternoon than in the morning. However, these studies did not control the chronotype variable. Therefore, the purpose of this study was to assess if the time-of-day effect on explicit memory would continue if this variable were controlled, in addition to identifying the occurrence of a possible synchronic effect. A total of 68 undergraduates were classified as morning, intermediate, or afternoon types. The subjects listened to a list of 10 words during the training phase and immediately performed a recognition task, a procedure which they repeated twice. One week later, they underwent an unannounced recognition test. The target list and the distractor words were the same in all series. The subjects were allocated to two groups according to acquisition time: a morning group (N = 32), and an afternoon group (N = 36). One week later, some of the subjects in each of these groups were subjected to a test in the morning (N = 35) or in the afternoon (N = 33).
    [Show full text]
  • TRAUMATIC AMNESIA a Dissociative Survival Mechanism
    TRAUMATIC AMNESIA a dissociative survival mechanism Dr. Muriel Salmona, psychiatrist, Paris, January 19, 2018 I - Presentation Complete or fragmented traumatic amnesia is a common memory disorder found in victims of violence. Numerous clinical studies have described this well- known phenomenon since the beginning of the twentieth century, when the first systematic studies explored amnesia among traumatized combat soldiers, and later among victims of sexual violence. Studies found almost 40 % complete amnesia among these participants, and 60% partial amnesia when childhood sexual abuse was concerned (Brière 1993, Williams 1994, IVSEA 2015). These amnesias are psychotraumatic consequences of violence. They are neuro- psychological survival mechanisms based on dissociation (Van der Kolk, 1995, 2001). Since 2015, dissociative traumatic amnesias have been recognized as a defining criterium for Post-Traumatic Stress Disorder (DSM-5, 2015). They can last several decades and lead to amnesia for entire sections of childhood, with almost no retrievable memory, which results in a painful impression of having no past, nor landmarks. When amnesia fades away, traumatic memories most often return brutally, invasively, as uncontrolled, unintegrated, fragmented traumatic memories (flashbacks, nightmares) and trigger re-experiences of violence with identitical distress and sensations, and a vividness as if the trauma was happening all over again. Care professionals should be better acquainted with these phenomena in order to improve how they care for the victims, by treating such reminiscences without confusing them with hallucinations, identifying violence, and its psychotraumatic consequences for the victims. Similarly, justice professionals, when faced with sexual violence complaints after recovered memories, should be more aware of the reality of such frequent memory disorder.
    [Show full text]
  • Rethinking Trauma
    How to Use Brain Science to Help Patients Accelerate Healing after Trauma Daniel Siegel, MD - Main Session - pg. 1 Rethinking Trauma How to Use Brain Science to Help Patients Accelerate Healing after Trauma the Main Session with Daniel Siegel, MD and Ruth Buczynski, PhD National Institute for the Clinical Application of Behavioral Medicine How to Use Brain Science to Help Patients Accelerate Healing after Trauma Daniel Siegel, MD - Main Session - pg. 2 Rethinking Trauma: Daniel Siegel, MD How to Use Brain Science to Help Patients Accelerate Healing after Trauma Table of Contents (click to go to a page) How To Define What Happens in the Brain During Trauma ................................... 3 The Optimally Functioning vs. Traumatized Brain as Defined by FACES ................. 4 How Age Affects the Impact of Trauma on the Brain .............................................. 7 Trauma Leads to the Chemical Release of Cortisol and Adrenaline ....................... 8 How Trauma Impairs the Brain’s Memory Systems ............................................... 10 Why the Brain’s Developmental Period Is Crucial to Working with Trauma ............ 11 The Layers of Memory and Encoding Implicit Memory .......................................... 13 The Retrieval of Implicit Memory and the Experience of Flashback ...................... 15 Putting the Science into Clinical Practice ............................................................... 17 What Happens in the Brain during Dissociation? ................................................... 18
    [Show full text]
  • Interactions Between Attention and Memory Marvin M Chun and Nicholas B Turk-Browne
    Interactions between attention and memory Marvin M Chun and Nicholas B Turk-Browne Attention and memory cannot operate without each other. In form of selective attention to internal representations this review, we discuss two lines of recent evidence that [1,2]. support this interdependence. First, memory has a limited capacity, and thus attention determines what will be Classic psychologists such as William James stated long encoded. Division of attention during encoding prevents the ago that ‘we cannot deny that an object once attended to formation of conscious memories, although the role of attention will remain in the memory, while one inattentively in formation of unconscious memories is more complex. Such allowed to pass will leave no traces behind’ [3]. More memories can be encoded even when there is another recently, leading neuroscientists such as Eric Kandel have concurrent task, but the stimuli that are to be encoded must be stated that one of the most important problems for 21st selected from among other competing stimuli. Second, century neuroscience is to understand how attention memory from past experience guides what should be attended. regulates the processes that stabilize experiential mem- Brain areas that are important for memory, such as the ories [4]. Here, we review studies from the past two years hippocampus and medial temporal lobe structures, are that reveal progress towards understanding the inter- recruited in attention tasks, and memory directly affects frontal- actions between attention and memory in neural systems. parietal networks involved in spatial orienting. Thus, exploring the interactions between attention and memory can provide Attention at encoding new insights into these fundamental topics of cognitive A major question in many people’s minds is how to neuroscience.
    [Show full text]
  • How Do We Learn from Experience?
    TSI Graphics: Worth—Kolb/Whishaw: An Introduction to Brain and Behavior C H A P T E R 13 How Do We Learn from Experience? What Are Learning and Memory? The Structural Basis of Brain Studying Memory in the Laboratory Plasticity Two Types of Memory Measuring Synaptic Change What Makes Explicit and Implicit Enriched Experience and Plasticity Memory Different? Sensory or Motor Training and Plasticity Plasticity, Hormones, Trophic Factors, and Drugs Dissociating Memory Circuits Focus on Disorders: Patient Boswell’s Amnesia Recovery from Brain Injury The Three-Legged Cat Solution Neural Systems Underlying The New-Circuit Solution Explicit and Implicit Memories The Lost-Neuron-Replacement Solution A Neural Circuit for Explicit Memories Focus on Disorders: Alzheimer’s Disease A Neural Circuit for Implicit Memories Focus on Disorders: Korsakoff’s Syndrome A Neural Circuit for Emotional Memories Dimitri Messinis/AP Photo Micrograph: Oliver Meckes/Ottawa/Photo Researchers 488 ■ D TSI Graphics: Worth—Kolb/Whishaw: An Introduction to Brain and Behavior onna was born on June 14, 1933. Her memory of Over the ensuing 10 months, Donna regained most of the period from 1933 to 1937 is sketchy, but her motor abilities and language skills, and her spatial abil- D those who knew her as a baby report that she was ities improved significantly. Nonetheless, she was short- like most other infants. At first, she could not talk, walk, or tempered and easily frustrated by the slowness of her use a toilet. Indeed, she did not even seem to recognize her recovery, symptoms that are typical of people with closed father, although her mother seemed more familiar to her.
    [Show full text]
  • CHILDREN's MEMORY, TRAUMATIC MEMORIES and the CHILD WITNESS
    CHILD MEMORY, TRAUMATIC MEMORY and the CHILD WITNESS George E. Davis, MD 2017 Children’s Law Institute INTERPRETING THE RESEARCH • The complications of memory study • Test conditions and questions don’t always match real life situations and motivations • Divergent interests • Although broad rules can be proposed for different ages and conditions, reliability depends on the individual case REFERENCES 1. Memory and Suggestibility in the Forensic Interview (Eisen, Quas and Goodman, 2002) 2. Stress, Trauma and Children’s Memory Development (Howe, Goodman and Cicchetti, 2008) 3. Children as Victims, Witnesses, and Offenders (Bottoms, Najdowski and Goodman, 2009) MEMORY PROCESS • MEMORY COMPONENTS – Encoding • PerceptionConstruct • Child age and knowledge – Storage • Combination, sorting, comparison in the hippocampus • Short term, long term and working memory – Retrieval • Reconstruction from associations and neural networks that are frontal and temporal TYPES of MEMORY • TYPES OF MEMORY – Explicit / Declarative / Conscious • Episodic—events • Semantic—facts • Intentional, whether learned or recalled • Organized and encoded by hippocampus and medial temporal – Implicit / Contextual / Unconscious • Procedural—riding a bike, driving to work, tying shoes, etc • Encoded and stored in motor control and brain stem – Conditioned responses—eg, fear of certain people – Autobiographical—the continuous sense of self over time DISSOCIATION • Dissociation is a breakdown in memory, consciousness and sense of self provoked by extreme fear, pain or psychological
    [Show full text]
  • Explicit and Implicit Memory by Richard H
    Explicit and Implicit Memory by Richard H. Hall, 1998 Overview As you might suspect, researchers and theorists have extended the basic multi-store model in many ways, among these is the differentiation of different types of long term memories. Perhaps the most fundamental such differentiation is the distinction between explicit and implicit memories (Figure 1). Implicit memories are sometimes referred to as "non-declarative" because an individual is unable to verbally "declare" these memories. Implicit memories are nonconscious, and often involve memories for specific step-by-step procedures, or specific feelings/emotions. For example, your ability to carry out the various processes involved in driving a car are largely nonconscious, they have been "proceduralized". Further, your conditioned emotional response to a "scary" situation is not something that you consciously or verbally recall. On the other hand, explicit memories are conscious memories that can easily be verbalized. They are more complex types of memories because they are often holistic, in that they involve the recall of many different aspects of a situation. In order to consciously recall something, such as your memory of events that occurred yesterday, you not only recall the event, but you also recall the context in which the event occurred, that is, the time of day, the place, and other objects and people that were present, etc. Figure 1. Comparison of Implicit and Explicit Memory Explicit Memory Storage Research, in particular with brain damaged patients such as those suffering from Korsakoff's and the case of HM, indicates that these two fundamental types of long term memory are more than theoretical abstractions.
    [Show full text]
  • Memory Reactivation and Consolidation During Sleep Ken A
    Sleep & Memory/Review Memory reactivation and consolidation during sleep Ken A. Paller1 and Joel L. Voss Institute for Neuroscience and Department of Psychology, Northwestern University, Evanston, Illinois 60208-2710, USA Do our memories remain static during sleep, or do they change? We argue here that memory change is not only a natural result of sleep cognition, but further, that such change constitutes a fundamental characteristic of declarative memories. In general, declarative memories change due to retrieval events at various times after initial learning and due to the formation and elaboration of associations with other memories, including memories formed after the initial learning episode. We propose that declarative memories change both during waking and during sleep, and that such change contributes to enhancing binding of the distinct representational components of some memories, and thus to a gradual process of cross-cortical consolidation. As a result of this special form of consolidation, declarative memories can become more cohesive and also more thoroughly integrated with other stored information. Further benefits of this memory reprocessing can include developing complex networks of interrelated memories, aligning memories with long-term strategies and goals, and generating insights based on novel combinations of memory fragments. A variety of research findings are consistent with the hypothesis that cross-cortical consolidation can progress during sleep, although further support is needed, and we suggest some potentially fruitful research directions. Determining how processing during sleep can facilitate memory storage will be an exciting focus of research in the coming years. The idea that memory storage is supported by events that take otherwise intellectually intact.
    [Show full text]
  • Pervasive Influences of Memory Without Awareness of Retrieval Joel L
    This article was downloaded by: [Northwestern University] On: 28 July 2012, At: 08:25 Publisher: Psychology Press Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer House, 37-41 Mortimer Street, London W1T 3JH, UK Cognitive Neuroscience Publication details, including instructions for authors and subscription information: http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/pcns20 More than a feeling: Pervasive influences of memory without awareness of retrieval Joel L. Voss a , Heather D. Lucas b & Ken A. Paller b a Department of Medical Social Sciences, Northwestern University Feinberg School of Medicine, Chicago, IL, USA b Department of Psychology, Northwestern University, Evanston, IL, USA Accepted author version posted online: 15 Mar 2012. Version of record first published: 30 Mar 2012 To cite this article: Joel L. Voss, Heather D. Lucas & Ken A. Paller (2012): More than a feeling: Pervasive influences of memory without awareness of retrieval, Cognitive Neuroscience, 3:3-4, 193-207 To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/17588928.2012.674935 PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE Full terms and conditions of use: http://www.tandfonline.com/page/terms-and-conditions This article may be used for research, teaching, and private study purposes. Any substantial or systematic reproduction, redistribution, reselling, loan, sub-licensing, systematic supply, or distribution in any form to anyone is expressly forbidden. The publisher does not give any warranty express or implied or make any representation that the contents will be complete or accurate or up to date. The accuracy of any instructions, formulae, and drug doses should be independently verified with primary sources.
    [Show full text]
  • Implicit and Explicit Memory for Novel Visual Objects in Older and \Bunger Adults
    Psychology and Aging Copyright 1992 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 1992, Vol. 7, No. 2, 299-308 0882-7974/92/53.00 Implicit and Explicit Memory for Novel Visual Objects in Older and \bunger Adults Daniel L. Schacter Lynn A. Cooper Harvard University Columbia University Michael Valdiserri University of Arizona Two experiments examined effects of aging on implicit and explicit memory for novel visual ob- jects. Implicit memory was assessed with an object decision task in which subjects indicated whether briefly exposed drawings represented structurally possible or impossible objects. Explicit memory was assessed with a yes-no recognition task. On the object decision task, old and young subjects both showed priming for previously studied possible objects and no priming for impossible objects; the magnitude of the priming effect did not differ as a function of age. By contrast, the elderly were impaired on the recognition task. Results suggest that the ability to form and retain structural descriptions of novel objects may be spared in older adults. Explicit memory refers to conscious recollection of previous 1988; Davis et al, 1990; Hultsch, Masson, & Small, 1991). These experiences, as expressed on recall and recognition tests, latter findings are difficult to interpret, however, because they whereas implicit memory refers to facilitations of performance, may well reflect "contamination" of implicit task performance often referred to as priming effects, on tests that do not require by explicit retrieval strategies, strategies that are more benefi- any conscious or intentional retrieval of previous experiences cial to young than to old subjects (cf. Graf, 1990; Howard, 1991; (Graf & Schacter, 1985; Schacter, 1987).
    [Show full text]