Variation in Finnish Vowel Harmony: an OT Account Author(S): Catherine O

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Variation in Finnish Vowel Harmony: an OT Account Author(S): Catherine O Variation in Finnish Vowel Harmony: An OT Account Author(s): Catherine O. Ringen and Orvokki Heinämäki Source: Natural Language & Linguistic Theory, Vol. 17, No. 2 (May, 1999), pp. 303-337 Published by: Springer Stable URL: http://www.jstor.org/stable/4047991 Accessed: 18/05/2010 16:12 Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of JSTOR's Terms and Conditions of Use, available at http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp. JSTOR's Terms and Conditions of Use provides, in part, that unless you have obtained prior permission, you may not download an entire issue of a journal or multiple copies of articles, and you may use content in the JSTOR archive only for your personal, non-commercial use. Please contact the publisher regarding any further use of this work. Publisher contact information may be obtained at http://www.jstor.org/action/showPublisher?publisherCode=springer. Each copy of any part of a JSTOR transmission must contain the same copyright notice that appears on the screen or printed page of such transmission. JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide range of content in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new forms of scholarship. For more information about JSTOR, please contact [email protected]. Springer is collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and extend access to Natural Language & Linguistic Theory. http://www.jstor.org CATHERINE O. RINGEN AND ORVOKKI HEINAMAKI VARIATION IN FINNISH VOWEL HARMONY: AN OT ACCOUNT* This paper presentsdata on vowel harmonywith disharmonicroots in Finnishwhich show that when the last harmonicvowel in a disharmonicroot is back, in almost all cases the only possible harmonicsuffix vowel is back, but when the last harmonic vowel is front, there is usually variation in suffix vowel choice that seems to be influencedby severalfactors, includingsonority and stress. These data, which cannot easily be accountedfor in rule-basedtheories, can be accountedfor in Optimality Theory.A highlyranked alignment constraint accounts for harmonywith native roots and loans in which the last harmonicvowel is back. Unrankedconstraints, which tie suffix vowel choice to stress and sonority, as well as alignment requirements, determinesuffix vowel quality for the remainderof forms. Variationis seen to be a function of the relative frequencywith which a particularsuffix vowel is designated as optimalby the differentpossible rankingsof the unrankedconstraints. 0. INTRODUCTION Finnish vowel harmony has received considerable attention in the literatureof generative phonology because of its relevance for a variety of issues in phonologicaltheory. Different analysesof Finnishvowel har- mony, many of which include discussions of disharmonic roots, are presented in Campbell (1980, 1981), Halle and Vergnaud (1981), Ki- parsky (1973, 1981), Goldsmith (1985), Ringen (1988b), Steriade (1987), Valimaa-Blum(1987), and Vago (1988), among others. This paper reportson the results of our empiricalinvestigation of vowel harmonywith Finnish disharmonicroots.l Our studies were conductedin * Portions of this paper were presented at the 1988 and 1998 LSA meetings, at Nordic ProsodyV, 1989, in Turku,Finland; at the 81st SASS AnnualMeeting, Amherst,Massachu- setts, 1991; at Kielitieteen Paivat 1990 in Helsinki, Finland; at MCWOP 1996 at the Universityof Illinois, Urbana-Champaign,Illinois; at the DepartmentalColloquium, March 1997, Universityof Iowa; and the Universityof Warsaw,University of Stuttgart,University of Salzburg,and Eotvos L6rand University,Budapest, in October 1997. Thanksto Gregg Oden for help with the statisticalanalysis, to RosemaryPlapp for assistancein tabulating the data, and to Arto Anttila, Jill Beckman, Ellen Broselow, Chris Culy, Gary Dell, Gregg Oden, Aimo Hakanen. Bob Harms, Richard Hurtig, Anneli Pajunen, Jon Ringen, Jerzy Rubach, Szilard Szentgyorgyi, Cheryl Zoll, and two anonymous reviewers for helpful commentsand/or discussions.This, of course, does not mean that any of these individuals bears any responsibilityfor any errors or that they necessarilyagree with all of our claims or interpretations. 1 Practicallyall disharmonicroots are loanwords, althoughnot all loanwordsare dishar- monic. The statusof these words as loanwordsper se is not important.Our claimsare about disharmonicroots, regardlessof whether or not they are loanwordsor whether speakers recognizethem as loans. NaturalLanguage and LinguisticTheory 17: 303-337, 1999. (g) 1999 KluwerAcademic Publishers. Printed in the Netherlands. 304 CATHERINE 0. RINGEN AND ORVOKKI HEINAMAKI Helsinki with native speakersof Finnishat the Universityof Helsinki. We found that vowel harmony with Finnish disharmonicroots has not been accuratelyportrayed in the literature.2Although suffix vowel choice is categoricallyfront or back with some disharmonicforms, there is variation in suffix vowel choice in other cases. In almost all cases when the last harmonicvowel in a disharmonicroot is back, the only possible harmonic suffixvowel is back. When the last harmonicroot vowel is front, however, most forms exhibit variationthat seems to be influencedby the qualityof the vowel with primary stress, the quality of the vowel with secondary stress, the quality of the most sonorous vowel, as well as the quality of the last vowel in the root. These data, which cannot easily be accounted for in rule-based theories, can be accounted for in Optimality Theory (McCarthyand Prince 1993a, 1995; Prince and Smolensky 1993) if it is assumedthat constraintsare partiallyranked. A highly ranked constraint requiringthat the feature [+back] be aligned with the right edge of the word accounts for harmony with native roots and disharmonicroots in whichthe last harmonicvowel is back. Unrankedconstraints which require that suffix vowels agree with the backness of the vowel which is most sonorous, with the vowel with primarystress, with the vowel with secon- dary stress, and with the last root vowel determinesuffix vowel qualityfor the remainderof forms. For native roots, any rankingof these unranked constraints will give the correct result because all the vowels will be front. For disharmonicroots, however, differentresults are possible. The variation that occurs is seen to be a function of the relative frequency with which a particularsuffix vowel is designatedas optimal by the differ- ent possible rankingsof the unrankedconstraints. 1. FACTS OF VOWEL HARMONY The essential characteristicsof Finnish vowel harmony are well-known and uncontroversial.There are eight surface vowels which are listed in (1): (1) neutral harmonic front front back i [i] y [y] u [u] e [e] o[0] o [o] a[a] a [a] 2 See Kontra and Ringen (1986, 1987) and Ringen and Kontra (1989) for discussionsof empiricalinvestigations of Hungarianvowel harmonywhich show that certainwidely cited data are, in fact, not accurate. VARIATION IN FINNISH VOWEL HARMONY 305 In native Finnish noncompoundwords, front and back harmonicvowels do not co-occur. Neutral or transparentvowels, on the other hand, are found in words with either front vowels (harmonic or neutral) or with back vowels. Some examples are given in (2) :3 (2) a. poiytai 'table' b. pouta 'fine weather' c. hamara 'dusk' d. kasi 'hand' e. koti 'home' f. kesy 'tame' g. veli 'brother' h. vero 'tax' i. tie 'road' Harmonic suffix vowels usually agree in backness with harmonic root vowels, as illustratedin (3): (3) a. poyta-na 'table' essive b. pouta-na 'fine weather' essive c. hamara-na 'dusk' essive d. kade-lla 'hand' adess e. koti-na 'home' essive f. kesy-lla 'tame' adess g. vero-lla 'tax' adess If all root vowels are neutral, harmonicsuffix vowels are (usually) front, as illustratedby the examples in (4): 3 We give here orthographic representations: long vowels and consonants are represented by double letters. Primary and secondary stress are marked throughout. 306 CATHERINE 0. RINGEN AND ORVOKKI HEINAMAKI (4) a. velje-lla 'brother' adess. b. tie-lla 'road' adess. But loanwords often violate harmony restrictions, as illustrated by the disharmonicforms in (5): (5) disharmonicloans a. vuilgaari 'vulgar' b. tyranni 'tyrant' c. afaari 'affair' d. analy'ysi 'analysis' It is sometimes claimed that suffix vowels following such disharmonic roots agree with the last harmonicroot vowel, e.g., Kiparsky(1973). The situation is not this simple, however. While it is generally agreed that a disharmonicroot in which the last harmonicvowel is back requires back harmonicsuffix vowels, there is variationwith disharmonicroots in which the last harmonic vowel is front. Moreover, what the facts are has not been clear. Different Finnish (normative and descriptive) grammarsdo not agree with each other or with generativephonologists who have made claims about which suffix vowels occur with such disharmonicroots. The Language Board of the Finnish LiteratureSociety recommendedin 1945 (Sadeniemi 1946, pp. 79-80) that in additionto i and e, y should also be treated as neutral in loanwords whereas o and a should always be consi- dered harmonic. Saarimaa'snormative grammar (1971, p. 17) presents y as basically neutral, while o and d are said to be harmonic, especially when they are long. Penttila's grammar(1963, p. 17) also considersy to be neutral, but o and d are said to be always harmonic.Ikola (1986, pp. 134-135) considersall the front vowels y, o and d to be basicallyharmonic, but states that y may also be neutral when unstressed.
Recommended publications
  • Notes on Complete Consonantal Assimilations. PUB DATE Apr 73 NOTE 11P
    DOCUMENT RESUME ED 127 777 FL 006 759 AUTHOR Hutcheson, James W. TITLE Notes on Complete Consonantal Assimilations. PUB DATE Apr 73 NOTE 11p. JOURNAL CIT Working Papers in Linguistics; n14 p58-64 Apr 1973 EDRS PRICE MF-$0.83 HC-$1.67 Plus Postage. DESCRIPTORS Arabic; *Articulation (Speech); Consonants; Finnish; Generative Phonology; Latin; *Linguistic Theory; *Phonological Units; *Phonology; Suprasegmentals; Synchronic Linguistics; Vowels; Yakut IDENTIFIERS Assimilation (Language); Sandhi ABSTRACT This paper is one of a number of studies within the conceptual framevork of natEral phonology, according to which phonological processes are of two kinds, context-free aril context-sensitive. Context-free changes can be explained by the character of the sounds themselves; context-sensitive processesare explained largely by the function of the actual processes and by the character of the sounds affected by them. This paper investigates the operation of complete consonantal assimilations within this framework. Complete assimilation results in the complete identity of the two sounds involved. Evidence from English casual speech is offered supporting the principle claiming that complete assimilations normally occur only when the segments involved are already very similar. Sandhi phenomena in Arabic and Yakut are shown to support this principle. Latin assimilations and Finnish consonant gradation are considered. The following points are emphasized:(1) Complete assimilations normally affect sounds already very similar; ti2) If relatively different sounds assimilate completely, so will less different sounds - assimilations operate hierarchically; (3) Nonphonological conditions can play a role in triggering assimilatory processes.(CHK) *********************************************************************** Documents acquired by ERIC include many informal unpublished * materials not available from other sources. ERIC makes every effort * * to obtain the best copy available.
    [Show full text]
  • Top-Down Predictions of Familiarity and Congruency in Audio-Visual Speech Perception at Neural Level
    This is a self-archived version of an original article. This version may differ from the original in pagination and typographic details. Author(s): Kolozsvári, Orsolya B.; Xu, Weiyong; Leppänen, Paavo H. T.; Hämäläinen, Jarmo A. Title: Top-Down Predictions of Familiarity and Congruency in Audio-Visual Speech Perception at Neural Level Year: 2019 Version: Published version Copyright: © The Authors, 2019. Rights: CC BY 4.0 Rights url: https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ Please cite the original version: Kolozsvári, O. B., Xu, W., Leppänen, P. H. T., & Hämäläinen, J. A. (2019). Top-Down Predictions of Familiarity and Congruency in Audio-Visual Speech Perception at Neural Level. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 13, 243. 10.3389/fnhum.2019.00243 fnhum-13-00243 July 11, 2019 Time: 17:36 # 1 ORIGINAL RESEARCH published: 12 July 2019 doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2019.00243 Top-Down Predictions of Familiarity and Congruency in Audio-Visual Speech Perception at Neural Level Orsolya B. Kolozsvári1,2*, Weiyong Xu1,2, Paavo H. T. Leppänen1,2 and Jarmo A. Hämäläinen1,2 1 Department of Psychology, University of Jyväskylä, Jyväskylä, Finland, 2 Jyväskylä Centre for Interdisciplinary Brain Research (CIBR), University of Jyväskylä, Jyväskylä, Finland During speech perception, listeners rely on multimodal input and make use of both auditory and visual information. When presented with speech, for example syllables, the differences in brain responses to distinct stimuli are not, however, caused merely by the acoustic or visual features of the stimuli. The congruency of the auditory and visual information and the familiarity of a syllable, that is, whether it appears in the listener’s native language or not, also modulates brain responses.
    [Show full text]
  • TEEM NO ISBN-951-678-255-B PUB DATE 79 NOTE 228P
    DOCUMENT RESUME ED 192 584 FL 011 744 AUTHOR Lehtonen, Jaakko, Ed.; Sajavaara, Kari, Ed. TITLE Papers in Contrastive Phonetics, Jyvaskyla Cross-Language Studies, No. 7. INSTITUTION Jyvaskyla Univ. (Finland). Dept. of English. TEEM NO ISBN-951-678-255-B PUB DATE 79 NOTE 228p. AVAILABLE FECM Department of English, University yvaskyla, 40100 Jyvaskyla 10, Finland EDFS PRICE MF01/PC10 Plus Postage. DESCRIPTOF Auditory Perception: *Consonants: *Contrastive Linguistics: Cistinctive Features (Language): English (Second Language) :Finnish: Interference (Language) *Phonetics: Pronunciation: Second Language Learning: Stress (Phonology): *Vowels IDENTIFIERS Reduction (Phonology): *Voicing ABSTRACT Four papers report on phonetic differences between Finnish and English, with pedagogical implicationsfor teaching English as a second language. "The English/ptk/-/bdg/ Distinction: Data and Discussion" by Kari Suomi isa survey of recent work on the phonetic parameters of the fortis/lenis distinction.The distinction "voiced-voiceless" is preferred to "fortis-lenis."In "The Voiceless-Voiced Opposition of English Consonants: Difficultiesof Pronunciation and Perception in Communication between Nativeand Finnish Speakers," Risto Hanninen analyzes the learningdifficulties encountered by Finns in isolated words, in words included inword lists, and in words embedded in meaningfulsentences. Learning difficulties vary greatly according to context.In voiced-voiceless identification tests, Finnish students failed to identifymost of the tested oppositions. In "Vowel
    [Show full text]
  • Xerox University Microfilms 300 North Zmb Road Ann Arbor, Michigan 46106 74-3207
    INFORMATION TO USERS This material was produced from a microfilm copy of the original document. While the most advanced technological means to photograph and reproduce this document have been used, the quality is heavily dependent upon the quality of the original submitted. The following explanation of techniques is provided to help you understand markings or patterns which may appear on this reproduction. 1. The sign or "target" for pages apparently lacking from die document photographed is "Missing Page(s)". If it was possible to obtain the missing page(s) or section, they are spliced into the film along with adjacent pages. This may have necessitated cutting thru an image and duplicating adjacent pages to insure you complete continuity. 2. When an image on the film is obliterated with a large round black mark, it is an indication that the photographer suspected that the copy may have moved during exposure and thus cause a blurred image. You will find a good image of the page in the adjacent frame. 3. When a map, drawing or chart, etc., was part of the material being photographed the photographer followed a definite method in "sectioning" the material. It is customary to begin photoing at the upper left hand corner of a large sheet and to continue photoing from left to right in equal sections with a small overlap. If necessary, sectioning is continued again — beginning below the first row and continuing on until complete. 4. The majority of users indicate that the textual content is of greatest value, however, a somewhat higher quality reproduction could be made from "photographs" if essential to the understanding of the dissertation.
    [Show full text]
  • Handouts for Advanced Phonology: a Course Packet Steve Parker GIAL
    Handouts for Advanced Phonology: A Course Packet Steve Parker GIAL and SIL International Dallas, 2018 Copyright © 2018 by Steve Parker and other contributors Page 1 of 281 Preface This set of materials is designed to be used as handouts accompanying an advanced course in phonology, particularly at the graduate level. It is specifically intended to be used in conjunction with two textbooks: Phonology in generative grammar (Kenstowicz 1994), and Optimality theory (Kager 1999). However, this course packet could potentially also be adapted for use with other phonology textbooks. The materials included here have been developed by myself and others over many years, in conjunction with courses in phonology taught at SIL programs in North Dakota, Oregon, Dallas, and Norman, OK. Most recently I have used them at GIAL. Many of the special phonetic characters appearing in these materials use IPA fonts available as freeware from the SIL International website. Unless indicated to the contrary on specific individual handouts, all materials used in this packet are the copyright of Steve Parker. These documents are intended primarily for educational use. You may make copies of these works for research or instructional purposes (under fair use guidelines) free of charge and without further permission. However, republication or commercial use of these materials is expressly prohibited without my prior written consent. Steve Parker Graduate Institute of Applied Linguistics Dallas, 2018 Page 2 of 281 1 Table of contents: list of handouts included in this packet Day 1: Distinctive features — their definitions and uses -Pike’s premises for phonological analysis ......................................................................... 7 -Phonemics analysis flow chart ..........................................................................................
    [Show full text]
  • Dialectal Gemination in Finnish: Phonetics/Phonology Interplay in Moraic Theory
    Dialectal gemination in Finnish: Phonetics/phonology interplay in moraic theory Christopher Spahr Forum Paper Submitted in partial fulfillment of the degree of Master of Arts University of Toronto c 2011 Christopher Spahr Contents Contents i Acknowledgments iii 1 Introduction 1 1.1 FinnishPhonology ....................... 3 1.2 Gemination and Degemination . 7 2 Gemination: A Dialectal Phenomenon 11 2.1 “Standard Finnish” and Finnish Dialects . 12 2.2 An Informal Typology of Dialectal Gemination . 15 2.2.1 Phonetic Gemination . 16 2.2.2 Phonemic Gemination . 17 2.2.3 Morphological Gemination . 21 2.2.4 Summary ........................ 22 3 Moraic Structure 23 3.1 MoraCountandMoraicTheory . 24 i ii CONTENTS 3.2 SecondMoraLengthening . 30 3.3 DefiningtheMora: Whatdoesitdo? . 33 3.3.1 LengthandDuration . 33 3.3.2 Weight and Timing . 34 4 Second Mora Lengthening and Primary Gemination 37 4.1 EvidenceforaCorrelation . 38 4.2 Modeling Primary Gemination . 40 4.2.1 APreviousAccount .................. 42 4.2.2 TheSynchronicConnection . 45 4.2.3 Gemination Before Diphthongs . 49 4.2.4 GradientEffects: AModelforChange . 57 4.2.5 Summary ........................ 59 5 Additional Issues and Closing Remarks 61 5.1 FurtherTheoreticalIssues . 61 5.1.1 Underlying Representations . 62 5.1.2 Segmental Phonology and Syllabification . 63 5.1.3 PhoneticInterpretation. 65 5.2 Conclusions ........................... 66 5.3 DirectionsforFutureResearch. 67 References 69 Acknowledgments Writing a forum paper is not an easy task, or at least so the clich´egoes. Actually I found it quite manageable, though by no means trivial; the real challenge was making it to the forum meetings at 9 o’clock every Tuesday morning. Nonetheless, there are some people I should mention who made the process easier.
    [Show full text]
  • A MDEE Is “A Process That Takes Place Only When Its Conditions Are Crucially Met by Virtue of Material from Two Different Morphemes” [Mccarthy 2003] 2
    CLS 47 Sharon Inkelas Where did the Derived Environment Condition Go? 1. The Morphologically Derived Environment Condition (1) A MDEE is “a process that takes place only when its conditions are crucially met by virtue of material from two different morphemes” [McCarthy 2003] 2. Canonical examples Canonical MDEE: stem-final consonant undergoes alternation triggered by following suffix-initial vowel; comparable CV sequence within the base does not undergo the alternation (2) Finnish Assibilation: ti → si (e.g. Keyser & Kiparsky 1983; Kiparsky 1993). a. /halut-i/ → halusi ‘want-3P.SG.PRET’ /halut-a/ → haluta ‘want-INF’ b. /æiti/ → æiti ‘mother’ (*æisi) c. /tilat-i/ → tilasi ‘order-3P.SG.PRET’ (*silasi) /tilat-a/ → tilata ‘order-INF’ (*silata) (3) Polish Palatalization: k, x, g → č, š, ž before i, e (Lubowicz 2002). a. ‘to step’ kro[k]-i-ć → kro[č]ɨć ‘to frighten’ stra[x]-i-ć → stra[š]ɨć ‘to weigh’ va[g]-i-ć → va[ž]ɨć b. ‘kefir’ [k]ef’ir *[č]ef’ir ‘jelly’ [k’]iśel *[č’]iśel ‘agent’ a[g]ent *a[ž]ent c. ‘chemist-DIM’ [x]em’i[k]-ek → [x]emi’[č]ek *[š]emi’[č]ek (4) Korean Palatalization: t, tʰ → c, cʰ before i, y (T. Cho 1998, Y. Cho 2009) a. mat-i → maci ‘eldest-NML = eldest son’ putʰ-i → pucʰi ‘adhere-CAUS = to affix’ kyətʰ-i → kyəcʰi ‘side-NOM = side’ patʰ-ita → pacʰita ‘field-COP = to be the field’ b. mati ‘joint’ titi-ta ‘to tread’ nɨtʰi-namu ‘zelkova tree’ (5) Turkish Velar Deletion (Lewis 1967, Zimmer & Abbott 1978, Sezer 1981) a.
    [Show full text]
  • Variation and Opacity Abstract
    ARTO ANTTILA VARIATION AND OPACITY ABSTRACT: Phonological variation and phonological opacity have been extensively studied independently of each other. This paper examines two phonological processes that simultaneously exhibit both phenomena: Assibilation and Apocope in Finnish. The evidence converges on two main conclusions. First, variation results from the presence of MULTIPLE METRICAL SYSTEMS within Finnish. Assibilation and Apocope are metrically conditioned alternations and the segmental variation reflects metrical variation. The metrical analysis explains a number of apparently unrelated phenomena, including typological asymmetries across dialects, quantitative asymmetries within dialects, differences between nouns and verbs, differences among noun classes, and the loci of lexical frequency effects. Second, phonological opacity arises from MORPHOLOGICAL LEVEL ORDERING. By interleaving transparent phonologies with independently motivated morphosyntactic constituents (stems, words, phrases) we derive the transparent and opaque interactions of four phonological processes, including Assibilation and Apocope. ABBREVIATIONS: 1P/2P/3P – first/second/third person; ALL – allative; CAUS – causative; CLIT – clitic; COND – conditional; ESS – essive; GEN – genitive; ILL – illative; INE – inessive; PAR – partitive; PAST – past tense; PL – plural; PX – possessive suffix; SG – singular; SUP – superlative; TRA – translative; QUE – question 1. INTRODUCTION Phonological variation and phonological opacity have been extensively studied independently of each other. Variation arises when one input yields multiple outputs. Opacity arises when a phonological process applies even if its conditioning environment is not met on the surface (overapplication), or conversely, fails to apply even if its conditioning environment is met on the surface (underapplication). As an illustration, consider two phonological processes in Finnish: ASSIBILATION which fricativizes a short nongeminate t before i (t Æ s /_ i) and APOCOPE which deletes a short i in an unstressed syllable (i Æ ∅).
    [Show full text]
  • Consonant Gradation in Finnish Dialects
    Liisa Duncan Consonant Gradation in Finnish Dialects 1. Introduction Consonant gradation is a lenition process common to most of the Finnic languages, as well as several other Uralic languages. Whether it is inherited directly from Proto-Uralic or is the result of similar phonological structures and tendencies in the daughter languages is still under debate. In Finnish it is now only partly productive phonologically in that it rarely affects recent loans and neologisms. However, it is still pervasive in the lexicon and is very much a part of the Finnish phonology. Gradation in Finnish affects geminate and singleton voiceless stops preceding a short vowel in a closed syllable. The geminates shorten (quantitative gradation) and the singletons undergo various lenition processes (qualitative gradation), depending on the environment, the dialect, and a range of sociolinguistic factors. Because quantitative gradation in geminates and qualitative gradation in singletons exhibit many disparate behaviours, including degree of productivity, I argue in this paper that they are now separate phenomena, though diachronically likely a single process. Thus, only qualitative gradation is discussed in any detail herein. Finnish consonant gradation is a widely discussed phenomenon in the literature. However, rarely does an account attempt to determine which weak grade occurs for the singleton stops or to explain the variation that exists both within and between dialects. Several previous studies which do attempt to account for the weak grades are discussed in sections 3.1 and 4.1. Lainio (1989), as part of a larger study of variation in Finnish, presents the array of possible weak grades for /t/.
    [Show full text]
  • Finnish Noun Inflection / 111
    4 Finnish Noun Inflection PAUL KIPARSKY 1 Introduction Inflected words in Finnish show a range of interdependent stem and suffix alternations which are conditioned by syllable structure and stress. In a pen- etrating study, Anttila (1997) shows how the statistical preferences among optional alternants of the Genitive Plural can be derived from free constraint ranking. I propose an analysis which covers the rest of the nominal morphol- ogy and spells out the phonological constraints that interact to produce the alternations, and show how it supports a stratal version of OT phonology.1 In the model of stratal OT that I will be assuming, stems, words, and phrases are subject to distinct parallel constraint systems, which may dif- fer in the ranking of constraints. These levels interface serially: the output of the stem morphology and phonology is the input to the word morphology and phonology, and the output of the word morphology and phonology is the input to the syntax and postlexical phonology. 1I am deeply indebted to Arto Anttila for discussing this material with me over many years, and for commenting on this latest effort of mine to make sense of it. Generative Approaches to Finnic and Saami Linguistics. Diane Nelson and Satu Manninen (eds.). Copyright c 2003, CSLI Publications. 109 110 / PAUL KIPARSKY (1) Stem phonology Word phonology Postlexical Phonology I assume that the constraint system of level n+1 may differ in ranking from constraint system of level n by promotion of one or more constraints to undominated status. These may be faithfulness constraints as well as marked- ness constraints.
    [Show full text]
  • The Prosodic Structure of Finnish and the Theory Of
    THE PROSODIC STRUCTURE OF FINNISH AND THE THEORY OF PHONOLOGICAL GOVERNMENT by DAVID MILLARD BMus, The University of Victoria, 1982 Diploma in Applied Linguistics, The University of British Columbia, 1985 A THESIS SUBMITTED IN PARTIAL FULFILLMENT OF THE REQUIREMENTS FOR THE DEGREE OF MASTER OF ARTS in THE FACULTY OF GRADUATE STUDIES (Department of Linguistics) We accept this thesis as conforming to the required standard THE UNIVERSITY OF BRITISH COLUMBIA December 1991 © David Millard, 1991 In presenting this thesis in partial fulfilment of the requirements for an advanced degree at the University of British Columbia, I agree that the Library shall make it freely available for reference and study. I further agree that permission for extensive copying of this thesis for scholarly purposes may be granted by the head of my department or by his or her representatives. It is understood that copying or publication of this thesis for financial gain shall not be allowed without my written permission. Department of Linguistics The University of British Columbia Vancouver, Canada Date December 30, 1991 DE-6 (2/88) The Prosodic Structure of Finnish and the Theory of Phonological Government by David Millard Submitted September 1991 to the Department of Linguistics, UBC, in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Master of Arts. ABSTRACT The goal of this thesis is to examine same of the claims of the Theory of Phonological Government in the light of data taken from the Finnish language. The Theory of Phonological Government describes the oraganization of segments into constituent groupings on the basis of government relations established at the level of Underlying Represen- tation.
    [Show full text]
  • Notes on Complete Consonantal Assimilations James W. Hutcheson
    notes on Complete Consonantal Assimilations James W. Hutcheson O. Introduction This paper is one of a number of studies within the conceptual frwnework of natural phonology {Strunpe 1972b), according to which phonological processes are of two kinds, context-free and context- sensitive. Stud.ies have shown that context-free. changes in simple vot,els and diphthongs a.re to be explained by the character of' the sounds themselves (Miller 1972, Sta.mpe 1972a). Recent studies in context-sensitive processes--syncope (Zwicky 1972b), naso.l assimilation ( Zuckerman 1972), palatalization (1feeld 19'[2), and vowel nasalization (Schourup 1972a, 1972b)--havc demonstrated that they are also to be · explained largely by the function of the processes themselves a·nd by the character of the sounds which are affected by them. Much of this work has been concerned with the notion of hierarchy of application. The purpose of this paper (and the larger work from which it is excerpted) is to inYestigate the operation of complete consonantal assimilations within the same theoretical framework as the studies mentioned above. 1. pomplete assimilation: a definition The designation £_omulete assimilation has been used in a variety of ways, I use the tem to mean that the process of assimilation results in the complete identity of the two sounds involved, i.e. c1c2 ->- CiC1 (complete progressive assimilation), C1C2 + C2C2 {complete regressive assimilation}, or C1C2 + C3C3 (complete coalescence). Some writers use complete assimilation to describe the assimilation of one feature, such as complete assimilation of position. For such single- feature processes I use locutions like c~mplete, or ~otal assimilation of manner or position.
    [Show full text]