TEEM NO ISBN-951-678-255-B PUB DATE 79 NOTE 228P

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

TEEM NO ISBN-951-678-255-B PUB DATE 79 NOTE 228P DOCUMENT RESUME ED 192 584 FL 011 744 AUTHOR Lehtonen, Jaakko, Ed.; Sajavaara, Kari, Ed. TITLE Papers in Contrastive Phonetics, Jyvaskyla Cross-Language Studies, No. 7. INSTITUTION Jyvaskyla Univ. (Finland). Dept. of English. TEEM NO ISBN-951-678-255-B PUB DATE 79 NOTE 228p. AVAILABLE FECM Department of English, University yvaskyla, 40100 Jyvaskyla 10, Finland EDFS PRICE MF01/PC10 Plus Postage. DESCRIPTOF Auditory Perception: *Consonants: *Contrastive Linguistics: Cistinctive Features (Language): English (Second Language) :Finnish: Interference (Language) *Phonetics: Pronunciation: Second Language Learning: Stress (Phonology): *Vowels IDENTIFIERS Reduction (Phonology): *Voicing ABSTRACT Four papers report on phonetic differences between Finnish and English, with pedagogical implicationsfor teaching English as a second language. "The English/ptk/-/bdg/ Distinction: Data and Discussion" by Kari Suomi isa survey of recent work on the phonetic parameters of the fortis/lenis distinction.The distinction "voiced-voiceless" is preferred to "fortis-lenis."In "The Voiceless-Voiced Opposition of English Consonants: Difficultiesof Pronunciation and Perception in Communication between Nativeand Finnish Speakers," Risto Hanninen analyzes the learningdifficulties encountered by Finns in isolated words, in words included inword lists, and in words embedded in meaningfulsentences. Learning difficulties vary greatly according to context.In voiced-voiceless identification tests, Finnish students failed to identifymost of the tested oppositions. In "Vowel Reduction in the Englishof Finnish Learners" by Hannele Heikkinen, the quality of the final reduced vowel in English, when used by Finnish speakers,was found to be influenced by lip rounding reflective of Finnish "oe." Finally,in "The Discrimination and Identification of EnglishVowels, Consonants, Junctures and Sentence Stress by Finnish Comprehensive School Pupils," Reijo Lamminmaki deals with problems oflanguage testing as related to identification problems, and questions the usefulnessof existing tests in interlanguage research. (JB) ********************* ********* ******* ********************** reproductions supplied by EDRS are the best thatcan be made * from the original document. * ********************************* **************************** Jyvaskyla Cross-Language Studies No 7 US DEPARTMEN 'OF HEALTH- PERMISSION TO REPRODUCE THIS EDUCATION&VVELFARE MATERIAL HAS BEEN GRANTED BY NATIONALINSTITUTEOF EDUCATION THIS DOCUMENT HAS BEEN REPRO- KLSitArCka-altiv-A,_ DUCED EXACTLY AS RECEIVED FROM THE PERSON OR ORGANIZATION OR IOIN- ATiNG IT POINTS OF %dig* DR OPINIONS STATED DO NOT NECESSARILY REPRE- SENTOFFIC.ALNATIONALINSTITUTEOF EDUCATION POSITION OR POLICY TO THE EDUCATIONAL RESOURCES INFORMATION CENTER PAPERS IN COON RAST1vE PHONETICS edited by Jaakko Lehtonen and Kari Sajavaara JyvaskylaCross-Language Studies Department of English,University ofJyvaskyl'a edited by Karl Sajavaara and JaakkoLehtonen ++1. Assistant to the editors Hannele Heikkinen Jyvaskyld 1979 ISBN 951-67B-255-8 ISSN 0357-654x PREFACE The Finnish-English Contrastive Projectwas started at the University of JyvaskyU in the spring of 1974. The project has been carried out in two parts: the Department of Phonetics and General Linguistics isrespon- sible for the work on oronunciation problems, and the Department of English is concerned with other aspects of the research. Several reports have omen published previously on phonetically orientatedtopics (eg. Moisio and Valenta 1976; Suomi 1976; Lehtonen and Koponen 1977; Sajavaara and Lehtonen 1978a; Lehtonen 1978) and the results have also been applied to language teaching purposes in a textbook of spoken English (Lehtonen, Sajavaara and May 1977). The methodology and problems of contrastive phonetics have been discussed by eg. Lehtonen (1977; 1979). In co- operation with the Error Analysis Project at Abo Akademi, the Contrastive Project has also carried out some tests; the results have been summarized in Palmberg (1979). In addition, the problems concerning the influence of English language and culture on present-day Finnish have been studied in co-operation with the Anglicism Project at the University of4yvaskyla (see eg. Sajavaara et al- 1978). In traditional contrastive analysis an analysis of the similarities and differences of two sound systems it was supposed that the difficult- ies of foreign language students could be described and predicted. The 'strong hypothesis' of contrastive analysis has been much criticized(see eg. Sajavaara 1977). It obviously failed to recognize the importance of actual problems in learning situations which is exactly the kind of knowledge that the FL teacher needs in his classroom practice.Moreover, the focus was on foreign language production: the role of the listener, which is equally important, was grossly underestimated. The listener's impressions of speech derive from internalized categories of rules and structures usually called phonology.Thus, he does not hear the actual physical utterance but a chain of segments corresponding to the expec- tations about the utterance. This is typical of human perceptual mechan- isms in general: we observe structures of whose existence1.-m have discovered and which we choose to observe.For a speaker-hearer, language is an earphone through which he can hear only language, not speech. Thus, many of the acoustic cues which actually carry the information necessary 4 for speech perception are subconscious '(eg., formant: patterns of vowels. consonantal loci and transitions, temporal patterns, of utterance, etc.). Although a speaker can pay conscious attention to certain postures and movements of his speech, organs, both the way in which they actually operate during speech and the resulting acoustic message are beyond the scope of conscious observation. All this has important consequences: the contrastive analyst and the foreign language teacher must realize that the. way in which they 'hear' a certain pattern may be totally different to the way in which the student perceives the same thing.. The problems of the foreign language student can be under -:nly if we knave how he 'feels', what he attempts to hear, what he ac- -eers, what the structures are that ;')e actually perceives, and how 2iffer from the target, ie. the way the native speaker's perception works in similar situations. Therefore, the focus of studies should not be on 'pronunciation' only but on a complex of problems which extend far beyond thearea of what is traditionally called pronunciation teaching. This ccmol.ex includes the differences in the learner's Ll and L2 communication chains as a whole, as well as the problems and confusions arising from the fact that the learner unconsciously resorts to Li phenomena not only when producing L2 but also when trying to understand L2. The majority of the contrastive phonetic studies carried out within the Project were, however, limited to specific phonetic phenomena: the fortis/lenis distinction, vowel reduction, word and sentence stress, consonant clusters, word boundaries, fluency, pauses, the acceptability of the Finnish variety of pronunciation, the reliabYity of the marking of pronunciation errors etc. (for closer discussion, see eg. Sajavaara and Lehtonen 1978b)- The present volume is a final report on the strictly phonetic part of the project. Some of the topics included in the origtnal research programme cannot be reported here, either because they have been delayed or the methods applied have proved to he inadequate and the results are therefore unreliable. That is why studies on such important topics as word and sentence stress or rhythm and intonation are not found in this volume. In addition, some of the later studies have a wider scope: they deal with various aspects of discourse and not with specific pronunciation problems. The reports of these studies, which also include non-verbal communication analysis, are not included here, since a separate issue ,sated to discourse dnalysis will be bublishedin this series. Two of the present papers are concerned withthe fortis/lenis stinction. The paper by Suomi is a version ofa paper read at the h Meeting of Finnish Phoneticians at Jyvaskyld in 1978. It is a survey of the recent worx on the phonetic parameters of the fortis/ lenis distinction and a discussion of the theoreticalproblems involved In his discussion of the problems of labelling thedistinction the euthor prefers the terms "voiced-voiceless" to 'fortis-lenis",which were recommended for pedanogical purposes by eq. Lehtonen (1971',and Gimson (1960. In a related study, Hanninen has analysed the learning difficulties encountered by Finns in three differentcontextual environments: in isolated words, in words included in wordlists and in words embedded in meaningful sentences. The duratioral pattern of words embedded in meaningful sentences appeared to be clearly differentfrom the other environment. Acoustical data on the fricatives and affricates also turned out to be different from that of the plosives. The learning difficulties of Finns vary a great deal according to thecontext. In identification tests, Finnish students failedto identify most of the tested oppositions. English listeners had considerable difficulty in identifying the opposition as produced by Finnishspeakers. Reduction of the final vowel in English VCV-structures isdiscussed by Heikkinen. As regards the learning problems of Finnish students, shediscovered that the duration used by Finns was either influencedby the mother
Recommended publications
  • Notes on Complete Consonantal Assimilations. PUB DATE Apr 73 NOTE 11P
    DOCUMENT RESUME ED 127 777 FL 006 759 AUTHOR Hutcheson, James W. TITLE Notes on Complete Consonantal Assimilations. PUB DATE Apr 73 NOTE 11p. JOURNAL CIT Working Papers in Linguistics; n14 p58-64 Apr 1973 EDRS PRICE MF-$0.83 HC-$1.67 Plus Postage. DESCRIPTORS Arabic; *Articulation (Speech); Consonants; Finnish; Generative Phonology; Latin; *Linguistic Theory; *Phonological Units; *Phonology; Suprasegmentals; Synchronic Linguistics; Vowels; Yakut IDENTIFIERS Assimilation (Language); Sandhi ABSTRACT This paper is one of a number of studies within the conceptual framevork of natEral phonology, according to which phonological processes are of two kinds, context-free aril context-sensitive. Context-free changes can be explained by the character of the sounds themselves; context-sensitive processesare explained largely by the function of the actual processes and by the character of the sounds affected by them. This paper investigates the operation of complete consonantal assimilations within this framework. Complete assimilation results in the complete identity of the two sounds involved. Evidence from English casual speech is offered supporting the principle claiming that complete assimilations normally occur only when the segments involved are already very similar. Sandhi phenomena in Arabic and Yakut are shown to support this principle. Latin assimilations and Finnish consonant gradation are considered. The following points are emphasized:(1) Complete assimilations normally affect sounds already very similar; ti2) If relatively different sounds assimilate completely, so will less different sounds - assimilations operate hierarchically; (3) Nonphonological conditions can play a role in triggering assimilatory processes.(CHK) *********************************************************************** Documents acquired by ERIC include many informal unpublished * materials not available from other sources. ERIC makes every effort * * to obtain the best copy available.
    [Show full text]
  • Top-Down Predictions of Familiarity and Congruency in Audio-Visual Speech Perception at Neural Level
    This is a self-archived version of an original article. This version may differ from the original in pagination and typographic details. Author(s): Kolozsvári, Orsolya B.; Xu, Weiyong; Leppänen, Paavo H. T.; Hämäläinen, Jarmo A. Title: Top-Down Predictions of Familiarity and Congruency in Audio-Visual Speech Perception at Neural Level Year: 2019 Version: Published version Copyright: © The Authors, 2019. Rights: CC BY 4.0 Rights url: https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ Please cite the original version: Kolozsvári, O. B., Xu, W., Leppänen, P. H. T., & Hämäläinen, J. A. (2019). Top-Down Predictions of Familiarity and Congruency in Audio-Visual Speech Perception at Neural Level. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 13, 243. 10.3389/fnhum.2019.00243 fnhum-13-00243 July 11, 2019 Time: 17:36 # 1 ORIGINAL RESEARCH published: 12 July 2019 doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2019.00243 Top-Down Predictions of Familiarity and Congruency in Audio-Visual Speech Perception at Neural Level Orsolya B. Kolozsvári1,2*, Weiyong Xu1,2, Paavo H. T. Leppänen1,2 and Jarmo A. Hämäläinen1,2 1 Department of Psychology, University of Jyväskylä, Jyväskylä, Finland, 2 Jyväskylä Centre for Interdisciplinary Brain Research (CIBR), University of Jyväskylä, Jyväskylä, Finland During speech perception, listeners rely on multimodal input and make use of both auditory and visual information. When presented with speech, for example syllables, the differences in brain responses to distinct stimuli are not, however, caused merely by the acoustic or visual features of the stimuli. The congruency of the auditory and visual information and the familiarity of a syllable, that is, whether it appears in the listener’s native language or not, also modulates brain responses.
    [Show full text]
  • Xerox University Microfilms 300 North Zmb Road Ann Arbor, Michigan 46106 74-3207
    INFORMATION TO USERS This material was produced from a microfilm copy of the original document. While the most advanced technological means to photograph and reproduce this document have been used, the quality is heavily dependent upon the quality of the original submitted. The following explanation of techniques is provided to help you understand markings or patterns which may appear on this reproduction. 1. The sign or "target" for pages apparently lacking from die document photographed is "Missing Page(s)". If it was possible to obtain the missing page(s) or section, they are spliced into the film along with adjacent pages. This may have necessitated cutting thru an image and duplicating adjacent pages to insure you complete continuity. 2. When an image on the film is obliterated with a large round black mark, it is an indication that the photographer suspected that the copy may have moved during exposure and thus cause a blurred image. You will find a good image of the page in the adjacent frame. 3. When a map, drawing or chart, etc., was part of the material being photographed the photographer followed a definite method in "sectioning" the material. It is customary to begin photoing at the upper left hand corner of a large sheet and to continue photoing from left to right in equal sections with a small overlap. If necessary, sectioning is continued again — beginning below the first row and continuing on until complete. 4. The majority of users indicate that the textual content is of greatest value, however, a somewhat higher quality reproduction could be made from "photographs" if essential to the understanding of the dissertation.
    [Show full text]
  • Handouts for Advanced Phonology: a Course Packet Steve Parker GIAL
    Handouts for Advanced Phonology: A Course Packet Steve Parker GIAL and SIL International Dallas, 2018 Copyright © 2018 by Steve Parker and other contributors Page 1 of 281 Preface This set of materials is designed to be used as handouts accompanying an advanced course in phonology, particularly at the graduate level. It is specifically intended to be used in conjunction with two textbooks: Phonology in generative grammar (Kenstowicz 1994), and Optimality theory (Kager 1999). However, this course packet could potentially also be adapted for use with other phonology textbooks. The materials included here have been developed by myself and others over many years, in conjunction with courses in phonology taught at SIL programs in North Dakota, Oregon, Dallas, and Norman, OK. Most recently I have used them at GIAL. Many of the special phonetic characters appearing in these materials use IPA fonts available as freeware from the SIL International website. Unless indicated to the contrary on specific individual handouts, all materials used in this packet are the copyright of Steve Parker. These documents are intended primarily for educational use. You may make copies of these works for research or instructional purposes (under fair use guidelines) free of charge and without further permission. However, republication or commercial use of these materials is expressly prohibited without my prior written consent. Steve Parker Graduate Institute of Applied Linguistics Dallas, 2018 Page 2 of 281 1 Table of contents: list of handouts included in this packet Day 1: Distinctive features — their definitions and uses -Pike’s premises for phonological analysis ......................................................................... 7 -Phonemics analysis flow chart ..........................................................................................
    [Show full text]
  • Dialectal Gemination in Finnish: Phonetics/Phonology Interplay in Moraic Theory
    Dialectal gemination in Finnish: Phonetics/phonology interplay in moraic theory Christopher Spahr Forum Paper Submitted in partial fulfillment of the degree of Master of Arts University of Toronto c 2011 Christopher Spahr Contents Contents i Acknowledgments iii 1 Introduction 1 1.1 FinnishPhonology ....................... 3 1.2 Gemination and Degemination . 7 2 Gemination: A Dialectal Phenomenon 11 2.1 “Standard Finnish” and Finnish Dialects . 12 2.2 An Informal Typology of Dialectal Gemination . 15 2.2.1 Phonetic Gemination . 16 2.2.2 Phonemic Gemination . 17 2.2.3 Morphological Gemination . 21 2.2.4 Summary ........................ 22 3 Moraic Structure 23 3.1 MoraCountandMoraicTheory . 24 i ii CONTENTS 3.2 SecondMoraLengthening . 30 3.3 DefiningtheMora: Whatdoesitdo? . 33 3.3.1 LengthandDuration . 33 3.3.2 Weight and Timing . 34 4 Second Mora Lengthening and Primary Gemination 37 4.1 EvidenceforaCorrelation . 38 4.2 Modeling Primary Gemination . 40 4.2.1 APreviousAccount .................. 42 4.2.2 TheSynchronicConnection . 45 4.2.3 Gemination Before Diphthongs . 49 4.2.4 GradientEffects: AModelforChange . 57 4.2.5 Summary ........................ 59 5 Additional Issues and Closing Remarks 61 5.1 FurtherTheoreticalIssues . 61 5.1.1 Underlying Representations . 62 5.1.2 Segmental Phonology and Syllabification . 63 5.1.3 PhoneticInterpretation. 65 5.2 Conclusions ........................... 66 5.3 DirectionsforFutureResearch. 67 References 69 Acknowledgments Writing a forum paper is not an easy task, or at least so the clich´egoes. Actually I found it quite manageable, though by no means trivial; the real challenge was making it to the forum meetings at 9 o’clock every Tuesday morning. Nonetheless, there are some people I should mention who made the process easier.
    [Show full text]
  • A MDEE Is “A Process That Takes Place Only When Its Conditions Are Crucially Met by Virtue of Material from Two Different Morphemes” [Mccarthy 2003] 2
    CLS 47 Sharon Inkelas Where did the Derived Environment Condition Go? 1. The Morphologically Derived Environment Condition (1) A MDEE is “a process that takes place only when its conditions are crucially met by virtue of material from two different morphemes” [McCarthy 2003] 2. Canonical examples Canonical MDEE: stem-final consonant undergoes alternation triggered by following suffix-initial vowel; comparable CV sequence within the base does not undergo the alternation (2) Finnish Assibilation: ti → si (e.g. Keyser & Kiparsky 1983; Kiparsky 1993). a. /halut-i/ → halusi ‘want-3P.SG.PRET’ /halut-a/ → haluta ‘want-INF’ b. /æiti/ → æiti ‘mother’ (*æisi) c. /tilat-i/ → tilasi ‘order-3P.SG.PRET’ (*silasi) /tilat-a/ → tilata ‘order-INF’ (*silata) (3) Polish Palatalization: k, x, g → č, š, ž before i, e (Lubowicz 2002). a. ‘to step’ kro[k]-i-ć → kro[č]ɨć ‘to frighten’ stra[x]-i-ć → stra[š]ɨć ‘to weigh’ va[g]-i-ć → va[ž]ɨć b. ‘kefir’ [k]ef’ir *[č]ef’ir ‘jelly’ [k’]iśel *[č’]iśel ‘agent’ a[g]ent *a[ž]ent c. ‘chemist-DIM’ [x]em’i[k]-ek → [x]emi’[č]ek *[š]emi’[č]ek (4) Korean Palatalization: t, tʰ → c, cʰ before i, y (T. Cho 1998, Y. Cho 2009) a. mat-i → maci ‘eldest-NML = eldest son’ putʰ-i → pucʰi ‘adhere-CAUS = to affix’ kyətʰ-i → kyəcʰi ‘side-NOM = side’ patʰ-ita → pacʰita ‘field-COP = to be the field’ b. mati ‘joint’ titi-ta ‘to tread’ nɨtʰi-namu ‘zelkova tree’ (5) Turkish Velar Deletion (Lewis 1967, Zimmer & Abbott 1978, Sezer 1981) a.
    [Show full text]
  • Variation and Opacity Abstract
    ARTO ANTTILA VARIATION AND OPACITY ABSTRACT: Phonological variation and phonological opacity have been extensively studied independently of each other. This paper examines two phonological processes that simultaneously exhibit both phenomena: Assibilation and Apocope in Finnish. The evidence converges on two main conclusions. First, variation results from the presence of MULTIPLE METRICAL SYSTEMS within Finnish. Assibilation and Apocope are metrically conditioned alternations and the segmental variation reflects metrical variation. The metrical analysis explains a number of apparently unrelated phenomena, including typological asymmetries across dialects, quantitative asymmetries within dialects, differences between nouns and verbs, differences among noun classes, and the loci of lexical frequency effects. Second, phonological opacity arises from MORPHOLOGICAL LEVEL ORDERING. By interleaving transparent phonologies with independently motivated morphosyntactic constituents (stems, words, phrases) we derive the transparent and opaque interactions of four phonological processes, including Assibilation and Apocope. ABBREVIATIONS: 1P/2P/3P – first/second/third person; ALL – allative; CAUS – causative; CLIT – clitic; COND – conditional; ESS – essive; GEN – genitive; ILL – illative; INE – inessive; PAR – partitive; PAST – past tense; PL – plural; PX – possessive suffix; SG – singular; SUP – superlative; TRA – translative; QUE – question 1. INTRODUCTION Phonological variation and phonological opacity have been extensively studied independently of each other. Variation arises when one input yields multiple outputs. Opacity arises when a phonological process applies even if its conditioning environment is not met on the surface (overapplication), or conversely, fails to apply even if its conditioning environment is met on the surface (underapplication). As an illustration, consider two phonological processes in Finnish: ASSIBILATION which fricativizes a short nongeminate t before i (t Æ s /_ i) and APOCOPE which deletes a short i in an unstressed syllable (i Æ ∅).
    [Show full text]
  • Consonant Gradation in Finnish Dialects
    Liisa Duncan Consonant Gradation in Finnish Dialects 1. Introduction Consonant gradation is a lenition process common to most of the Finnic languages, as well as several other Uralic languages. Whether it is inherited directly from Proto-Uralic or is the result of similar phonological structures and tendencies in the daughter languages is still under debate. In Finnish it is now only partly productive phonologically in that it rarely affects recent loans and neologisms. However, it is still pervasive in the lexicon and is very much a part of the Finnish phonology. Gradation in Finnish affects geminate and singleton voiceless stops preceding a short vowel in a closed syllable. The geminates shorten (quantitative gradation) and the singletons undergo various lenition processes (qualitative gradation), depending on the environment, the dialect, and a range of sociolinguistic factors. Because quantitative gradation in geminates and qualitative gradation in singletons exhibit many disparate behaviours, including degree of productivity, I argue in this paper that they are now separate phenomena, though diachronically likely a single process. Thus, only qualitative gradation is discussed in any detail herein. Finnish consonant gradation is a widely discussed phenomenon in the literature. However, rarely does an account attempt to determine which weak grade occurs for the singleton stops or to explain the variation that exists both within and between dialects. Several previous studies which do attempt to account for the weak grades are discussed in sections 3.1 and 4.1. Lainio (1989), as part of a larger study of variation in Finnish, presents the array of possible weak grades for /t/.
    [Show full text]
  • Finnish Noun Inflection / 111
    4 Finnish Noun Inflection PAUL KIPARSKY 1 Introduction Inflected words in Finnish show a range of interdependent stem and suffix alternations which are conditioned by syllable structure and stress. In a pen- etrating study, Anttila (1997) shows how the statistical preferences among optional alternants of the Genitive Plural can be derived from free constraint ranking. I propose an analysis which covers the rest of the nominal morphol- ogy and spells out the phonological constraints that interact to produce the alternations, and show how it supports a stratal version of OT phonology.1 In the model of stratal OT that I will be assuming, stems, words, and phrases are subject to distinct parallel constraint systems, which may dif- fer in the ranking of constraints. These levels interface serially: the output of the stem morphology and phonology is the input to the word morphology and phonology, and the output of the word morphology and phonology is the input to the syntax and postlexical phonology. 1I am deeply indebted to Arto Anttila for discussing this material with me over many years, and for commenting on this latest effort of mine to make sense of it. Generative Approaches to Finnic and Saami Linguistics. Diane Nelson and Satu Manninen (eds.). Copyright c 2003, CSLI Publications. 109 110 / PAUL KIPARSKY (1) Stem phonology Word phonology Postlexical Phonology I assume that the constraint system of level n+1 may differ in ranking from constraint system of level n by promotion of one or more constraints to undominated status. These may be faithfulness constraints as well as marked- ness constraints.
    [Show full text]
  • The Prosodic Structure of Finnish and the Theory Of
    THE PROSODIC STRUCTURE OF FINNISH AND THE THEORY OF PHONOLOGICAL GOVERNMENT by DAVID MILLARD BMus, The University of Victoria, 1982 Diploma in Applied Linguistics, The University of British Columbia, 1985 A THESIS SUBMITTED IN PARTIAL FULFILLMENT OF THE REQUIREMENTS FOR THE DEGREE OF MASTER OF ARTS in THE FACULTY OF GRADUATE STUDIES (Department of Linguistics) We accept this thesis as conforming to the required standard THE UNIVERSITY OF BRITISH COLUMBIA December 1991 © David Millard, 1991 In presenting this thesis in partial fulfilment of the requirements for an advanced degree at the University of British Columbia, I agree that the Library shall make it freely available for reference and study. I further agree that permission for extensive copying of this thesis for scholarly purposes may be granted by the head of my department or by his or her representatives. It is understood that copying or publication of this thesis for financial gain shall not be allowed without my written permission. Department of Linguistics The University of British Columbia Vancouver, Canada Date December 30, 1991 DE-6 (2/88) The Prosodic Structure of Finnish and the Theory of Phonological Government by David Millard Submitted September 1991 to the Department of Linguistics, UBC, in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Master of Arts. ABSTRACT The goal of this thesis is to examine same of the claims of the Theory of Phonological Government in the light of data taken from the Finnish language. The Theory of Phonological Government describes the oraganization of segments into constituent groupings on the basis of government relations established at the level of Underlying Represen- tation.
    [Show full text]
  • Notes on Complete Consonantal Assimilations James W. Hutcheson
    notes on Complete Consonantal Assimilations James W. Hutcheson O. Introduction This paper is one of a number of studies within the conceptual frwnework of natural phonology {Strunpe 1972b), according to which phonological processes are of two kinds, context-free and context- sensitive. Stud.ies have shown that context-free. changes in simple vot,els and diphthongs a.re to be explained by the character of' the sounds themselves (Miller 1972, Sta.mpe 1972a). Recent studies in context-sensitive processes--syncope (Zwicky 1972b), naso.l assimilation ( Zuckerman 1972), palatalization (1feeld 19'[2), and vowel nasalization (Schourup 1972a, 1972b)--havc demonstrated that they are also to be · explained largely by the function of the processes themselves a·nd by the character of the sounds which are affected by them. Much of this work has been concerned with the notion of hierarchy of application. The purpose of this paper (and the larger work from which it is excerpted) is to inYestigate the operation of complete consonantal assimilations within the same theoretical framework as the studies mentioned above. 1. pomplete assimilation: a definition The designation £_omulete assimilation has been used in a variety of ways, I use the tem to mean that the process of assimilation results in the complete identity of the two sounds involved, i.e. c1c2 ->- CiC1 (complete progressive assimilation), C1C2 + C2C2 {complete regressive assimilation}, or C1C2 + C3C3 (complete coalescence). Some writers use complete assimilation to describe the assimilation of one feature, such as complete assimilation of position. For such single- feature processes I use locutions like c~mplete, or ~otal assimilation of manner or position.
    [Show full text]
  • Remarks on the Nature of Complete Consonantal Assimilations
    215 Remarks on the Nature of Complete Consonantal Assimilations James W. Hutcheson Ohio State University 0. Introduction The purpose of this paper is to clarify the nature of complete consonantal assimilations. By complete consonantal assimilations I mean those processes which convert a sequence C1C2 to C2C2 (complete regressive assimilation), CiCi (complete progressive assimilation), or C3C3 (complete coalescence). Correspondences which illustrate such assimilations are cited from both historical change and synchronic derivations, the latter including casual speech sandhi, derivational and inflectional processes. The nature of the data makes it necessary to distinguish care- fully between correspondences of either historical or synchronic derivations which arise from a single process of complete assimilation and those which arise in some other fashion. Of the latter, two different situations obtain. Either the complete assimilations arise fortuitously as a subset of some larger, single feature, assimilatory process, e.g. a voicing assimilation rule; or they are the result of a number of processes whose cumulative effect is a complete assimilation. Such processes may be all assimilatory in nature or they may include assimilatory and non-assimilatory processes. 1. Fortuitous complete assimilations: English casual speech sandhil In two notes C.-J. Bailey (1969, 1970) has offered evidence that consonant clusters of alveolars followed by nonalveolars appear to be marked sequences and for this reason are unstable, showing a tendency to become unmarked. Such marked clusters can be eliminated in a variety of ways. Bailey cites unmarking by metathesis in the case of ancient Greek and by assimilation in contemporary English. It is the latter case that is of interest here, because the assimi- lations which do occur--and, perhaps more important, those which do not--offer important insights into the nature of complete consonantal assimilations.
    [Show full text]