Cariban Languages
Total Page:16
File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb
Load more
Recommended publications
-
A First Comparison of Pronominal and Demonstrative Systems in the Cariban Language Family*
A FIRST COMPARISON OF PRONOMINAL AND DEMONSTRATIVE SYSTEMS IN THE CARIBAN LANGUAGE FAMILY* Sérgio Meira Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics, Nijmegen, The Netherlands 1. Introduction The Cariban language family is composed of approximately 25 languages (numbers ranging from 20 to 50, depending on different researchers’ opinions about which varieties are dialects and which are independent languages), spoken by approximately 100,000 people in lowland South America, from south-eastern Colombia (where Karihona is spoken) to the Oiapoque river in Brazil (Karinya), from the coast of the Guianas (Karinya) down to the southern Xingu area in central Brazil (Bakairí). The field of comparative Cariban studies was initiated more than two hundred years ago, when the relationship between a number of Cariban languages was first noticed by Filippo Salvadore Gilij (1782). Unfortunately, the historical-comparative method has been only very rarely applied to Cariban languages, for two main reasons: (1) most of the languages are, to this day, poorly known, which means that there is very little reliable material to compare; (2) most people who compared Cariban languages were not trained comparativists. Girard (1971) remains the only methodical attempt at reconstructing Proto-Cariban lexical items and proposing a classification (unfortunately based on a still very poor data base). In the area of morphosyntax, Gildea (1998) presents the first reconstruction of the person-marking http://www.etnolinguistica.org/illa and tense-aspect-mood (TAM) systems of Proto-Cariban and their syntactic properties. This file is freely available for download at The present work attempts to contribute to the development of historical studies in the Cariban family by presenting a first preliminary reconstruction of the pronominal system of Proto-Cariban (including non-third-person and third-person, i.e. -
Redalyc.A Summary Reconstruction of Proto-Maweti-Guarani Segmental
Boletim do Museu Paraense Emílio Goeldi. Ciências Humanas ISSN: 1981-8122 [email protected] Museu Paraense Emílio Goeldi Brasil Meira, Sérgio; Drude, Sebastian A summary reconstruction of proto-maweti-guarani segmental phonology Boletim do Museu Paraense Emílio Goeldi. Ciências Humanas, vol. 10, núm. 2, mayo- agosto, 2015, pp. 275-296 Museu Paraense Emílio Goeldi Belém, Brasil Available in: http://www.redalyc.org/articulo.oa?id=394051442005 How to cite Complete issue Scientific Information System More information about this article Network of Scientific Journals from Latin America, the Caribbean, Spain and Portugal Journal's homepage in redalyc.org Non-profit academic project, developed under the open access initiative Bol. Mus. Para. Emílio Goeldi. Cienc. Hum., Belém, v. 10, n. 2, p. 275-296, maio-ago. 2015 A summary reconstruction of proto-maweti-guarani segmental phonology Uma reconstrução resumida da fonologia segmental proto-mawetí-guaraní Sérgio MeiraI, Sebastian DrudeII IMuseu Paraense Emílio Goeldi. Belém, Pará, Brasil IIMax-Planck-Institute for Psycholinguistics. Nijmegen, The Netherlands Abstract: This paper presents a succinct reconstruction of the segmental phonology of Proto-Maweti-Guarani, the hypothetical protolanguage from which modern Mawe, Aweti and the Tupi-Guarani branches of the Tupi linguistic family have evolved. Based on about 300 cognate sets from the authors’ field data (for Mawe and Aweti) and from Mello’s reconstruction (2000) for Proto-Tupi-Guarani (with additional information from other works; and with a few changes concerning certain doubtful features, such as the status of stem-final lenis consonants *r and *ß, and the distinction of *c and *č ), the consonants and vowels of Proto-Maweti-Guarani were reconstructed with the help of the traditional historical-comparative method. -
UC Berkeley UC Berkeley Previously Published Works
UC Berkeley UC Berkeley Previously Published Works Title A Bayesian Phylogenetic Classification of Tupí-Guaraní Permalink https://escholarship.org/uc/item/1331v899 Journal LIAMES, 15(2) Authors Michael, Lev David Chousou-Polydouri, Natalia Bartolomei, Keith et al. Publication Date 2015 Peer reviewed eScholarship.org Powered by the California Digital Library University of California A Bayesian Phylogenetic Classification of Tup´ı-Guaran´ı Lev Michael, Natalia Chousou-Polydouri, Keith Bartolomei Erin Donnelly, Vivian Wauters, S´ergioMeira, Zachary O'Hagan∗ Abstract This paper presents an internal classification of Tup´ı-Guaran´ıbased on a Bayesian phylogenetic analysis of lexical data from 30 Tup´ı-Guaran´ılanguages and 2 non-Tup´ı- Guaran´ıTupian languages, Awet´ıand Maw´e.A Bayesian phylogenetic analysis using a generalized binary cognate gain and loss model was carried out on a character ta- ble based on the binary coding of cognate sets, which were formed with attention to semantic shift. The classification shows greater internal structure than previous ones, but is congruent with them in several ways.1 1 Introduction This paper proposes a new classification of the Tup´ı-Guaran´ı(TG) language family based on the application of computational phylogenetic methods to lexical data from 30 TG languages ∗Affiliations for the authors of this paper are: Chousou-Polydouri, Donnelly, Michael, O'Hagan| University of California, Berkeley; Meira|Museu Paraense Em´ılio Goeldi; Bartolomei, Wauters|Independent Scholar. 1We are indebted to Sebastian Drude, Fran¸coiseRose, Eva-Maria R¨oBler, and Rosa Vallejos, who kindly shared unpublished lexical data from Awet´ı,Emerillon, Ach´e,and Kokama-Kokamilla, respectively. -
On the Geographical Origins and Dispersion of Tupian Languages
On the geographical origins and dispersion of Tupian Language s Greg Urban University of Pennsylvania Where did the Tupian languages originate? How did they come to occupy the ir historical homelands? José Brochado (1984), filling in a n1ajor lacuna in Lathrap' s ( 1970) scheme, has added a distinctive voice to the long- standing debates surrounding these questions. Iam grateful to Francisco Noelli for bringing him to my attention. As Noelli indicates, Bro chado ' s work provides the foundations for dialog and coope ration between linguistic s, cultural anthropology, and archaeology. It is in the spirit of cooperation and dialogue that I'll make some friendly criticistns of his resea rch , as well as of the linguistic and cultural work pertinent to the question of Tupian origins. My purpo se will be to pinpoint areas for further research that might provide clue s for solv ing the continuing mystery surroundin g the Tupi. I' ll be conce rned prín1arily with two principal hypotheses put forth by Brochado , the first deriving from Donald Lathrap, the second the nove l contribution of Brochado hi1nse]f: GR EG URI3 AN. ÜN THE GEOCiRAPHICAL ORJGINS ANO DISPERSION OF T UPIAN L ANGUAGES 1 1- Displaced Pe rsons Hypot hes is : that the Tupian stock originated a1ong the 1nain co u rse of the Atna zo n ri ver. Th e hypoth es is is ba sed on the obse rvation that the fami lies of the Tupi an stock (other than the Tupí-Guaraní fan1i]y) occ upy tributarie s of "the upp er co ur se of the Xingu, Tapajós, and Ma deira . -
Native American Languages, Indigenous Languages of the Native Peoples of North, Middle, and South America
Native American Languages, indigenous languages of the native peoples of North, Middle, and South America. The precise number of languages originally spoken cannot be known, since many disappeared before they were documented. In North America, around 300 distinct, mutually unintelligible languages were spoken when Europeans arrived. Of those, 187 survive today, but few will continue far into the 21st century, since children are no longer learning the vast majority of these. In Middle America (Mexico and Central America) about 300 languages have been identified, of which about 140 are still spoken. South American languages have been the least studied. Around 1500 languages are known to have been spoken, but only about 350 are still in use. These, too are disappearing rapidly. Classification A major task facing scholars of Native American languages is their classification into language families. (A language family consists of all languages that have evolved from a single ancestral language, as English, German, French, Russian, Greek, Armenian, Hindi, and others have all evolved from Proto-Indo-European.) Because of the vast number of languages spoken in the Americas, and the gaps in our information about many of them, the task of classifying these languages is a challenging one. In 1891, Major John Wesley Powell proposed that the languages of North America constituted 58 independent families, mainly on the basis of superficial vocabulary resemblances. At the same time Daniel Brinton posited 80 families for South America. These two schemes form the basis of subsequent classifications. In 1929 Edward Sapir tentatively proposed grouping these families into superstocks, 6 in North America and 15 in Middle America. -
The Story of *Ô in the Cariban Family1, °
Language Documentation & Conservation Special Publication No. 2 (May 2010): Fieldwork and Linguistic Analysis in Indigenous Languages of the Americas, ed. by Andrea L. Berez, Jean Mulder, and Daisy Rosenblum, pp. 91-123 http://nflrc.hawaii.edu/ldc/ 5 http://hdl.handle.net/10125/4452 The Story of *ô in the Cariban Family1, ° Spike Gildeaa, B. J. Hoffb, and Sérgio Meirac aUniversity of Oregon bLeiden University cKoninklijke Nederlandse Akademie van Wetenschappen/Leiden University This paper argues for the reconstruction of an unrounded mid central/back vowel *ô to Proto-Cariban. Recent comparative studies of the Cariban family encounter a consistent correspondence of ə : o : ɨ : e, tentatively reconstructed as *o2 (considering only pronouns; Meira 2002) and *ô (considering only seven languages; Meira & Franchetto 2005). The first empirical contribution of this paper is to expand the comparative database to twenty- one modern and two extinct Cariban languages, where the robustness of the correspond- ence is confirmed. In ten languages, *ô merges with another vowel, either *o or *ɨ. The second empirical contribution of this paper is to more closely analyze one apparent case of attested change from *ô > o, as seen in cognate forms from Island Carib and dialectal variation in Kari’nja (Carib of Surinam). Kari’nja words borrowed into Island Carib/Garí- funa show a split between rounded and unrounded back vowels: rounded back vowels are reflexes of *o and *u, unrounded back vowels reflexes of *ô and *ɨ. Our analysis of Island Carib phonology was originally developed by Douglas Taylor in the 1960s, supplemented with unpublished Garifuna data collected by Taylor in the 1950s. -
Aweti in Relation with Kamayurá: the Two Tupian Languages of the Upper
O presente arquivo está disponível em http://etnolinguistica.org/xingu Alto Xingu UMA SOCIEDADE MULTILÍNGUE organizadora Bruna Franchetto Rio de Janeiro Museu do Índio - Funai 2011 COORDENAÇÃO EDITORIAL , EDIÇÃO E DIAGRAMAÇÃO André Aranha REVISÃO Bruna Franchetto CAPA Yan Molinos IMAGEM DA CAPA Desenho tradicional kuikuro Dados Internacionais de Catalogação na Publicação (CIP) (Câmara Brasileira do Livro, SP, Brasil) Alto Xingu : uma sociedade multilíngue / organizadora Bruna Franchetto. -- Rio de Janeiro : Museu do Indio - FUNAI, 2011. Vários autores. ISBN 978-85-85986-34-6 1. Etnologia 2. Povos indígenas - Alto Xingu 3. Sociolinguística I. Franchetto, Bruna. 11-02880 CDD-306.44 Índices para catálogo sistemático: 1. Línguas alto-xinguanas : Sociolinguística 306.44 EDIÇÃO DIGITAL DISPON Í VEL EM www.ppgasmuseu.etc.br/publicacoes/altoxingu.html MUSEU DO ÍNDIO - FUNAI PROGRAMA DE PÓS -GRADUAÇÃO EM ANTROPOLOGIA SOCIAL DO MUSEU NACIONAL UNIVERSIDADE FEDERAL DO RIO DE JANEIRO SEBAST I AN DRUDE AWETI IN ReLATION WITH KAMAYURÁ THE TWO TUP I AN LANGUAGES OF THE UPPER X I NGU S EBAST I AN DRUDE Johann Wolfgang Goethe-Universität Frankfurt/Main Museu Paraense Emílio Goeldi INTRODUCT I ON The Aweti and the Kamayurá are the two peoples speaking Tupian languages within the Upper Xingu system in focus in this volume. This article explores the relationship between the two groups and their languages at various levels, as far as space and our current kno- wledge allow. The global aim is to answer a question that frequently surfaces: how closely related are these two languages? This question has several answers depending on the kind and level of ‘relationship’ between the two languages one wishes to examine. -
Languages of the Middle Andes in Areal-Typological Perspective: Emphasis on Quechuan and Aymaran Adelaar, W.F.H.; Campbell L., Grondona V.M
Languages of the Middle Andes in areal-typological perspective: Emphasis on Quechuan and Aymaran Adelaar, W.F.H.; Campbell L., Grondona V.M. Citation Adelaar, W. F. H. (2012). Languages of the Middle Andes in areal-typological perspective: Emphasis on Quechuan and Aymaran. In G. V. M. Campbell L. (Ed.), The Indigenous Languages of South America. A Comprehensive Guide. (pp. 575-624). Berlin; Boston: De Gruyter Mouton. doi:10.1515/9783110258035 Version: Not Applicable (or Unknown) License: Leiden University Non-exclusive license Downloaded from: https://hdl.handle.net/1887/70116 Note: To cite this publication please use the final published version (if applicable). Languages of the Middle Andes in areal-typological perspective: Emphasis on Quechuan and Aymaran Willem F.H. Adelaar 1. Introduction1 Among the indigenous languages of the Andean region of Ecuador, Peru, Bolivia, northern Chile and northern Argentina, Quechuan and Aymaran have traditionally occupied a dominant position. Both Quechuan and Aymaran are language families of several million speakers each. Quechuan consists of a conglomerate of geographically defined varieties, traditionally referred to as Quechua “dialects”, notwithstanding the fact that mutual intelligibility is often lacking. Present-day Aymaran consists of two distinct languages that are not normally referred to as “dialects”. The absence of a demonstrable genetic relationship between the Quechuan and Aymaran language families, accompanied by a lack of recognizable external genetic connections, suggests a long period of independent development, which may hark back to a period of incipient subsistence agriculture roughly dated between 8000 and 5000 BP (Torero 2002: 123–124), long before the Andean civilization attained its highest stages of complexity. -
Adjectives, Adverbs, And/Or Nouns?
Property Concepts in the Cariban family: Adjectives, Adverbs, and/or Nouns? Sérgio Meira and Spike Gildea KNAW/Leiden University University of Oregon 1 Introduction1 It has been a staple of typology since Dixon (1977, 1982) that the adjective class is not universal, with “property concepts” (semantic adjectives) sometimes found in a lexical class of adjectives, but sometimes in other word classes, especially verbs and nouns. Much of the descriptive typological discussion since has focused on the question of whether a “missing” adjective class is a subcategory of nouns or a subcategory of verbs. The theoretical discussion, too, has focused on the ways in which adjectives are midway between nouns and verbs, e.g. 1 Data sources for this work: Hixkaryana: Derbyshire (1965, 1979, 1985); Makushi: Abbott (1991), Amodio and Pira (1996), Raposo (1997); Tiriyó: Meira (1999), field notes, Carlin (2003); Akawaio: Gildea (2005), Fox (2003). Abbreviations used in this work: 1 = first person; 1+2 = first person dual inclusive; 1+3 = first person plural exclusive; 2 = second person; 3 = third person; 3ANA = third person anaphoric; 3R = third-person reflexive possessive (coreferential with subject); A = subject of transitive verb; ADJ = adjective; ADV = adverb; AGT = agent; AN = animate; ATTR = attributivizer (essive marker); AZR = adverbializer; C.NZR = circumstance nominalizer; COL = collective (number); COP = copula; DETR = detransitivizer; DIR = directional; EMPH = emphatic; ERG = ergative; FRUST = frustrative; HAVE = ‘having’ (predicative possession) -
Redalyc.Subordination Strategies in Tupian Languages
Boletim do Museu Paraense Emílio Goeldi. Ciências Humanas ISSN: 1981-8122 [email protected] Museu Paraense Emílio Goeldi Brasil van Gijn, Rik; Vilacy Galucio, Ana; Nogueira, Antonia Fernanda Subordination strategies in Tupian languages Boletim do Museu Paraense Emílio Goeldi. Ciências Humanas, vol. 10, núm. 2, mayo- agosto, 2015, pp. 297-324 Museu Paraense Emílio Goeldi Belém, Brasil Available in: http://www.redalyc.org/articulo.oa?id=394051442006 How to cite Complete issue Scientific Information System More information about this article Network of Scientific Journals from Latin America, the Caribbean, Spain and Portugal Journal's homepage in redalyc.org Non-profit academic project, developed under the open access initiative Bol. Mus. Para. Emílio Goeldi. Cienc. Hum., Belém, v. 10, n. 2, p. 297-324, maio-ago. 2015 Subordination strategies in Tupian languages Estratégias de subordinação em línguas Tupi Rik van GijnI , Ana Vilacy GalucioII, Antonia Fernanda NogueiraIII IUniversity of Zürich, Zürich, Switzerland II Museu Paraense Emílio Goeldi, Belém, Pará, Brasil IIIUniversidade Federal do Pará, Breves, Pará, Brasil Abstract: Assessing the internal coherence and constituency of language families often centers either around comparing certain form-meaning correspondences, or around identifying the presence or absence of linguistic features across the members of the family. The former approach is generally restricted to the lexicon. The latter approach focuses mostly on structural characteristics of language. In this paper we present an alternative approach to comparing grammatical systems between languages within a language family, which aims at bringing these two approaches and their results closer to each other. We look at subordination strategies in a sample of Tupian languages, taking constructions as the basic unit of comparison, treating them as form-meaning correspondences. -
Demographic and Evolutionary Trajectories of the Guarani and Kaingang Natives of Brazil Andrea R
AMERICAN JOURNAL OF PHYSICAL ANTHROPOLOGY 132:301–310 (2007) Demographic and Evolutionary Trajectories of the Guarani and Kaingang Natives of Brazil Andrea R. Marrero,1 Wilson A. Silva-Junior,2 Cla´ udio M. Bravi,3 Mara H. Hutz,1 Maria L. Petzl-Erler,4 Andres Ruiz-Linares,5 Francisco M. Salzano,1 and Maria C. Bortolini1* 1Departamento de Gene´tica, Instituto de Biocieˆncias, Universidade Federal do Rio Grande do Sul, 91501-970 Porto Alegre, Rio Grande do Sul, Brazil 2Departamento de Gene´tica e Centro de Terapia Celular, Universidade de Sa˜o Paulo, 14049-900 Ribeira˜o Preto, Sa˜o Paulo, Brazil 3Laboratorio de Gene´tica Molecular Poblacional, Instituto Multidisciplinario de Biologı´a Celular (IMBICE), La Plata, Argentina 4Departamento de Gene´tica, Universidade Federal do Parana´, 81531-990 Curitiba, Parana´, Brazil 5The Galton Laboratory, University College, London, UK KEY WORDS mtDNA; Y-chromosome markers; Amerindians; asymmetrical interethnic matings ABSTRACT A total of 278 individuals from two Brazil- gesting a higher level of differentiation between the three ian Indian tribes (Guarani and Kaingang) living in five dif- Guarani partialities than between the two Kaingang ferent localities had their mitochondrial DNA sequenced villages. Non-Amerindian admixture varied with sex and for the first hypervariable segment (HVS-I), and a fraction in the Guarani was only observed through the paternal of them was also studied for seven biallelic Y-chromosome line. Using these data and those of other Tupian and polymorphisms. Nineteen HVS-I lineages were detected, Jeˆan tribes, it was possible to make inferences about past which showed distinct distributions in the two tribes. -
Language Contact and Change in the Multilingual Ecologies of the Guianas
Yakpo, Kofi & Pieter Muysken. 2017. Language contact and change in the multilingual ecologies of the Guianas. In Kofi Yakpo & Pieter Muysken (eds.), Boundaries and bridges: Language contact in multilingual ecologies (Language Contact and Bilingualism (LCB) 14), 3–19. Berlin: De Gruyter Mouton. doi:10.1515/9781614514886-001. AUTHOR MANUSCRIPT VERSION Kofi Yakpo and Pieter Muysken Language contact and change in the multilingual ecologies of the Guianas 1 Introduction This book deals with multilingualism, language contact, language change and convergence in the Guianas of South America, with a focus on Suriname. The Guianas are a very complex region. The national identity of the countries in the Guianas involves both a sense of common destiny and of multiple ethnic affilia- tions. In this sense it presents a condensed microcosm of the Latin American and Caribbean quest for identity that we find in works as apart geographically, if not intellectually, as José Vasconcelos’ La Raza Cósmica in Mexico (1925), José Carlos Mariátegui’s 7 Ensayos de Interpretación de la Realidad Peruana in Peru (1928), or Jean Price-Mars’ Ainsi parla l’oncle in Haiti (1928) (see also the overview of essays on ethnicity and boundaries gathered in Oostindie 1996). We have named our volume Boundaries and bridges because it reflects at the same time the maintenance of ethnic and linguistic boundaries, through the languages involved, but also the numerous instances of cross-linguistic influ- ence across these boundaries. It illustrates the point that in the complex multi- lingual and multi-ethnic area of the Guianas, the languages spoken have been part of an effort of groups to keep themselves apart, as boundaries, but have also undergone numerous changes in the presence of other languages, and thus form bridges.