Language and Society: How Social Pressures Shape Grammatical Structure

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Language and Society: How Social Pressures Shape Grammatical Structure Language and society: How social pressures shape grammatical structure © 2020, Limor Raviv ISBN: 978-94-92910-12-7 Cover picture: Drawing by Nick Lowndes Printed and bound by Ipskamp Drukkers b.v. This research was supported by the Max Planck Society for the Advancement of Science, Munich, Germany. The educational component of the doctoral training was provided by the International Max Planck Research School (IMPRS) for Language Sciences. The graduate school is a joint initiative between the Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics and two partner institutes at Radboud University – the Centre for Language Studies, and the Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour. The IMPRS curriculum, which is funded by the Max Planck Society for the Advancement of Science, ensures that each member receives interdisciplinary training in the language sciences and develops a well-rounded skill set in preparation for fulfilling careers in academia and beyond. Language and society: How social pressures shape grammatical structure Proefschrift ter verkrijging van de graad van doctor aan de Radboud Universiteit Nijmegen op gezag van de rector magnificus prof. dr. J.H.J.M. van Krieken, volgens besluit van het college van decanen in het openbaar te verdedigen op donderdag 7 mei 2020 om 12.30 uur precies door Limor Raviv geboren op 21 oktober 1985 te Petah Tikva (Israël) Promotor: Prof. dr. Antje S. Meyer Copromotor: Dr. Shiri Lev-Ari (Royal Holloway University of London, Verenigd Koninkrijk) Manuscriptcommissie: Prof. dr. Asli Ozyurek Prof. dr. Caroline F. Rowland Dr. Mark Dingemanse Language and society: How social pressures shape grammatical structure Doctoral Thesis to obtain the degree of doctor from Radboud University Nijmegen on the authority of the Rector Magnificus prof. dr. J.H.J.M. van Krieken, according to the decision of the Council of Deans to be defended in public on Thursday, May 7, 2020 at 12.30 hours by Limor Raviv born on October 21, 1985 in Petah Tikva (Israel) Supervisor: Prof. dr. Antje S. Meyer Co-supervisor: Dr. Shiri Lev-Ari (Royal Holloway University of London, UK) Doctoral Thesis Committee: Prof. dr. Asli Ozyurek Prof. dr. Caroline F. Rowland Dr. Mark Dingemanse To my amazing grandmother Rita Table of Contents 1 General Introduction 11 2 Compositional structure can emerge without generational 25 transmission 3 Larger communities create more systematic languages 69 4 The role of social network structure in the emergence of 105 linguistic structure 5 What makes a language easy to learn? A preregistered 157 study on how systematic structure and community size affect language learnability 6 General Discussion 215 References 257 English summary 281 283 )סיכום בעברית( Hebrew Summary Dutch Summary (Nederlandse samenvatting) 285 Acknowledgements 287 Curriculum vitae 295 Publications 297 MPI Series in Psycholinguistics 299 11 1 General Introduction “I find it fascinating that there are seven thousand different ways to do what we’re doing right now.” John McWhorter The evolution of language and the origin of linguistic diversity have been occupying people since the dawn of days. Multiple myths and folk tales attempt to explain how languages began, and why they became so different from each other. According to the biblical account, all humans once spoke the same language (Genesis 11:1-9). Having only one shared language allowed people to cooperate with one another and engage in large-scale endeavors to challenge God (i.e., The Tower of Babel). In return, God punished the people by “confusing”1 their languages and scattering them around the world, ensuring that humankind would no longer be able to globally understand each other. This biblical story is probably the most well-known origin myth regarding the evolution of language and the source of linguistic diversity today, but is far from being the only one: very similar tales have been documented in Sumerian mythology, Ancient Greek mythology, Central American religions and tribal legends in Africa (Boas, Teit, Farrand, Gould, & Spinden, 1917; Kramer, 1968; Maher, 2017; Teit, 1917). But was there really once a single language that all people could understand? The truth is that linguists just don’t know. The biggest challenge in the field of language evolution is the lack of direct evidence regarding the origin and development of the first human languages: we do not have access to the minds and languages of our ancestors who lived hundreds of thousands of years ago. We therefore have no information on how the very first language (or languages) looked like: did early humans use gestures or vocalizations? Did they imitate sounds they heard in their environment, or babbled randomly until certain sounds took on meaning? Without a time-travel device, none of these questions can be answered directly. Therefore, all evidence favoring one theory or another comes from indirect and analogous observations, ranging from babies’ language development trajectories, to the communication systems of primates and 1 The name of the city Babel is derived from the Hebrew verb root B.B.L, which means “confuse”. 12 other non-human animals, to computational models and experimental paradigms mimicking the transition from a state of no language or proto- language to a state of complex language. Although the origin of languages remains mysterious, the thought of everyone in the world speaking the same language is an interesting one. If we view language as evolving primarily for the sake of communication, it seems very reasonable that we should all be able to understand each other and use one common tongue. It is easy to imagine how the past and present world would benefit from speaking a universal language. Indeed, in the past centuries there have been multiple attempts to impose a Lingua Franca (e.g., Latin), and even several attempts to artificially create such a language (e.g., Esperanto). But despite the theoretical (though unlikely) possibility of all languages deriving from a single origin, and despite the potential appeal of having a universal language, that is obviously not the case: There are about 7,000 different languages around the world today, and these languages vary greatly from one another2. The origins of linguistic diversity Why are there so many different languages? What are the sources of this astonishing linguistic diversity? While some questions about the evolution of language are impossible to answer, it turns out that we can provide some answers to these two questions. People speculate that languages differ due to cultural differences, environmental differences, historical changes, etc. The main principle underlying these intuitions is that languages are constantly changing: even if we had a common language once, it would probably not be the same 200 years later. Moreover, if groups of people split and migrate to different areas and engage in different activities, these changes would probably not look the same across different groups. Languages develop under different conditions, and such differences may lead to the formation of different languages and to ongoing changes in their sounds, their lexicon and their grammar. In short, languages are dynamic and adapt to their environment. One explanation for why languages differ is that they exist in diverse physical environments: languages are used in mountains and in open 2 Of course, one can also focus on the similarities between languages (i.e., linguistic universals): all natural languages serve communicative goals and are constrained by humans’ cognitive capacities; therefore, they inevitably share some features (Hockett, 1960). 13 plains, in deserts and in forests, in tropical climates and in freezing weather conditions. Importantly, these environmental differences can shape the languages that evolve in each region. Specifically, several geographic–linguistic correlational studies have tested the association between environmental features (such as climate and altitude) and linguistic features (such as sound systems). These studies report significant differences between languages evolving under different physical conditions, and highlight the physical affordances of various climates and landscapes. Specifically, our bodies react differently to different temperatures and air pressures: organs that are involved in the process of language production and comprehension (e.g., lungs, lips, vocal cords, ears) may respond differently to different conditions, making some linguistic elements harder or easier to produce and comprehend. For example, languages that are spoken in dry and cold climates are less likely to develop a tonal distinction between words, like that found in Chinese (Everett, Blasi, & Roberts, 2015). This is because in such dry climates, it is slightly harder to control the vocal cords and, consequentially, the tone of our voice. As such, almost all languages that make use of complex tones (and therefore require precise manipulation of pitch) are found in more humid regions. Another example is the prevalence of ejective consonants almost uniquely in mountain areas (Everett, 2013). Ejective consonants, which are produced by compressing a pocket of air in the throat instead of the lungs, are very common in high elevation regions around the world, but are very rare in other landscapes. This geographic correlation is mainly attributed to the fact that ejectives are much easier to produce in high altitudes, since low air pressure dramatically reduces the physiological effort required for the compression of air in the
Recommended publications
  • The Phenomenon of Overregularization in Esperanto-Speaking Children
    interlinguistics / interlingüística / interlinguistik / interlingvistiko Regularizing the regular The phenomenon of overregularization in Esperanto-speaking children Renato Corsetti, Maria Antonietta Pinto and Maria Tolomeo Università di Roma La Sapienza This article deals with the phenomenon of overregularization in a language already extremely regular, i.e. Esperanto, in children who are learning it as their mother tongue together with one or two other national languages. It consists of an analysis of the diaries kept by Esperanto-speaking parents, tracing the development of five children who were brought up speaking Esperanto as one of their two or three mother-tongues. The children were all of European origin, and their ages ranged from one to five years. The dif- ferent forms of overregularization have been subdivided into three levels of complexity based on the number and type of morpheme compositions used, and the degree of semantic elaboration. Detailed comments are provided on the forms and meanings of the various examples representing each level, showing the correlation between the age of the children and the growing complexity of the forms. This study can be seen as a first step towards a more systematic analysis of the typologies of overregularization specific to this cat- egory of early bilingual children and a better understanding of their language development profile. Introduction Overregularization in a language already extremely regular, i.e. Esperanto, in children who are learning it as their mother tongue together with one or two other national languages, is a particularly stimulating area of study, as can be inferred from the apparent contradiction between the two terms “overregularization” and “regular.” It can be broken down into three distinct disciplinary areas.
    [Show full text]
  • Contrastive Linguistics; Determiners Language Classification
    DOCUMENT RESUME ED 430 401 FL 025 837 TITLE Notes on Linguistics, 1998. INSTITUTION Summer Inst. of Linguistics, Dallas, TX. ISSN ISSN-0736-0673 PUB DATE 1998-00-00 NOTE 242p.; For the 1997 "Notes on Linguistics," see ED 415 721. PUB TYPE Collected Works - Serials (022) JOURNAL CIT Notes on Linguistics; n80-83 1998 EDRS PRICE MF01/PC10 Plus Postage. DESCRIPTORS Book Reviews; Computer Software; Computer Software Development; Contrastive Linguistics; Determiners (Languages); Doctoral Dissertations; English; Greek; Language Classification; Language Patterns; *Language Research; *Linguistic Theory; Research Methodology; *Semantics; Technical Writing; *Uncommonly Taught Languages; Workshops IDENTIFIERS Alamblak; Bungku Tolaki Languages; Chamicuro; Kham ABSTRACT The four issues of the journal of language research and linguistic theory include these articles: "Notes on Determiners in Chamicuro" (Steve Parker); Lingualinks Field Manual Development" (Larry Hayashi); "Comments from an International Linguistics Consultant: Thumbnail Sketch" (Austin Hale); "Carlalinks Workshop" (Andy Black); "Implications of Agreement Languages for Linguistics" (W. P. Lehmann); and "The Semantics of Reconciliation in Three Languages" (Les Bruce) . Dissertation abstracts, book reviews, and professional notes are included in each issue.(MSE) ******************************************************************************** Reproductions supplied by EDRS are the best that can be made from the original document. ******************************************************************************** NOTES ON LINGUISTICS Number 80 February 1998 Number 81 May 1998 Number 82 August 1998 Number 83 November 1998 SUMMER INSTITUTE OF LINGUISTICS 7500 WEST CAMP WISDOM ROAD DALLAS, TEXAS 75236 USA U.S. DEPARTMENT OF EDUCATION PERMISSION TO REPRODUCE AND office ot Educatlonal Research and Improvement DISSEMINATE THIS MATERIAL HAS EDUCATIONAL RESOURCES INFORMATION BEEN GRANTED BY CENTER (ERIC) \This document has been reproduced as received from the person or organization originating it.
    [Show full text]
  • Does Linguistic Explanation Presuppose Linguistic Description?"SUBJECT "SL, Volume 28:3"KEYWORDS ""SIZE HEIGHT "240"WIDTH "160"VOFFSET "2">
    <LINK "has-n*"> <TARGET "has" DOCINFO AUTHOR "Martin Haspelmath"TITLE "Does linguistic explanation presuppose linguistic description?"SUBJECT "SL, Volume 28:3"KEYWORDS ""SIZE HEIGHT "240"WIDTH "160"VOFFSET "2"> Does linguistic explanation presuppose linguistic description?* Martin Haspelmath Max-Planck-Institut für evolutionäre Anthropologie, Leipzig I argue that the following two assumptions are incorrect: (i) The properties of the innate Universal Grammar can be discovered by comparing language systems, and (ii) functional explanation of language structure presupposes a “correct”, i.e. cognitively realistic, description. Thus, there are two ways in which linguistic explanation does not presuppose linguistic description. The generative program of building cross-linguistic generalizations into the hypothesized Universal Grammar cannot succeed because the actually observed generalizations are typically one-way implications or implicational scales, and because they typically have exceptions. The cross-linguistic gener- alizations are much more plausibly due to functional factors. I distinguish sharply between “phenomenological description” (which makes no claims about mental reality) and “cognitively realistic descrip- tion”, and I show that for functional explanation, phenomenological de- scription is sufficient. 1. Introduction Although it may seem obvious that linguistic explanation necessarily presupposes linguistic description, I will argue in this paper that there are two important respects in which this is not the case. Of course, some kind of description is an indispensable prerequisite for any kind of explanation, but there are different kinds of description and different kinds of explanation. My point here is that for two pairs of kinds of description and explanation, it is not the case, contrary to widespread assumptions among linguists, that the latter presupposes the former.
    " class="panel-rg color-a">[Show full text]
  • Advances Analysis Spanish Exclamatives
    ADVANCES in the ANALYSIS of SPANISH EXCLAMATIVES EDITED BY Ignacio Bosque THEORETICAL DEVELOPMENTS IN HISPANIC LINGUISTICS Javier Gutiérrez- Rexach, Series Editor Advances in the Analysis of Spanish Exclamatives Edited by Ignacio Bosque THE OHIO STATE UNIVERSITY PRESS • COLUMBUS Copyright © 2017 by The Ohio State University. All rights reserved. Library of Congress Cataloging-in- Publication Data Names: Bosque, Ignacio, editor. Title: Advances in the analysis of Spanish exclamatives / edited by Ignacio Bosque. Other titles: Theoretical developments in Hispanic linguistics. Description: Columbus : The Ohio State University Press, [2017] | Series: Theoretical develop- ments in Hispanic linguistics | Includes bibliographical references and index. Identifiers: LCCN 2016041476 | isbn 9780814213261 (cloth ; alk. paper) | isbn 081421326X (cloth ; alk. paper) Subjects: lcsh: Spanish language—Exclamations. | Spanish language—Syntax. | Spanish language—Semantics. Classification: lcc pc4395.a35 2017 | ddc 465—dc23 lc record available at https:// lccn .loc .gov /2016041476 Cover design by AuthorSupport.com Text design by Juliet Williams Type set in Minion and Formata The paper used in this publication meets the minimum requirements of the American National Standard for Information Sciences—Permanence of Paper for Printed Library Materi- als. ANSI z39.48–1992. 9 8 7 6 5 4 3 2 1 The series Theoretical Developments in Hispanic Linguistics was conceived and directed by Javier Gutiérrez- Rexach, whose sudden and unexpected passing has meant a tremendous loss for the international linguistics community. The extraordinary significance of Javier’s contributions to the field of Hispanic linguistics over the past twenty- five years will inspire the work of many researchers in the future. This book, which contains one of his last contributions, is dedicated to his memory.
    [Show full text]
  • The Austrian Problem of Language and Peter Handke: a Documentation
    Louisiana State University LSU Digital Commons LSU Historical Dissertations and Theses Graduate School 1981 The Austrian Problem of Language and Peter Handke: a Documentation. Donald Russell Bailey Louisiana State University and Agricultural & Mechanical College Follow this and additional works at: https://digitalcommons.lsu.edu/gradschool_disstheses Recommended Citation Bailey, Donald Russell, "The Austrian Problem of Language and Peter Handke: a Documentation." (1981). LSU Historical Dissertations and Theses. 3667. https://digitalcommons.lsu.edu/gradschool_disstheses/3667 This Dissertation is brought to you for free and open access by the Graduate School at LSU Digital Commons. It has been accepted for inclusion in LSU Historical Dissertations and Theses by an authorized administrator of LSU Digital Commons. For more information, please contact [email protected]. INFORMATION TO USERS This was produced from a copy of a document sent to us for microfilming. While the most advanced technological means to photograph and reproduce this document have been used, the quality is heavily dependent upon the quality of the material submitted. The following explanation of techniques is provided to help you understand markings or notations which may appear on this reproduction. 1. The sign or "target” for pages apparently lacking from the document photographed is "Missing Page(s)”. If it was possible to obtain the missing page(s) or section, they are spliced into the film along with adjacent pages. This may have necessitated cutting through an image and duplicating adjacent pages to assure you of complete continuity. 2. When an image on the film is obliterated with a round black mark it is an indication that the film inspector noticed either blurred copy because of movement during exposure, or duplicate copy.
    [Show full text]
  • Phonological Readjustment and Multimodular Interaction: Evidence from Kirundi Language Games*1
    Jeanine Ntihirageza 51 Journal of Universal Language 8 March 2007, 51-89 Phonological Readjustment and Multimodular Interaction: Evidence from Kirundi Language Games*1 Jeanine Ntihirageza Northeastern Illinois University Abstract This paper investigates language games (also called ludlings [Laycock 1969, 1972, Bagemihl 1988a, 1995]) as linguistic tools used to unveil and understand phonological phenomena such as tone, vowel length and voice dissimilation. Although this study is comparative in nature, the focus is on novel data produced by Kirundi speakers. Three types of affixation language game data are * This paper is an excerpt from my dissertation, Quantity Sensitivity in Bantu Languages, presented at the University of Chicago in 2001. I am grateful to James D. McCawley and John Goldsmith for their extensive comments on previous versions. An earlier version of this paper was presented the Annual Conference on African Linguistics 2006. For comments at this conference, I am also grateful to Larry Hyman and Sharon Rose. Additionally, colleagues at Northeastern Illinois University, namely Audrey Reynolds, Teddy Bofman and William Stone provided me with valuable feedback. Their comments, questions, and suggestions were particularly important for the development of this work. 52 Phonological Readjustment Kirundi Language Games analyzed. The goal of this investigation is twofold. First I show that the only length and tone that matter are the ones that come floating with the affixed segmental material. Second, this paper discusses the issue of source language, i.e., where in the phonology of the language the game rules apply. Following Bagemihl’s modular model (1988, 1995), couched in lexical phonology, I demonstrate that the source language of the language games belongs to different modules depending on the nature of the inserted material.
    [Show full text]
  • The Interslavic Language As a Tool for Supporting E-Democracy in Central and Eastern Europe
    260 Int. J. Electronic Governance, Vol. 11, Nos. 3/4, 2019 The Interslavic language as a tool for supporting e-democracy in Central and Eastern Europe Vojtěch Merunka* Department of Information Engineering, Czech University of Life Sciences Prague, Prague, 165 21, Czech Republic Email: [email protected] *Corresponding author Jan van Steenbergen Language Creation Society, GK IJmuiden, 1975, Netherlands Email: [email protected] Lina Yordanova Faculty of Economics, Trakia University, Stara Zagora, 6015, Bulgaria Email: [email protected] Maria Kocór Faculty of Pedagogy, Rzeszów University, Rzeszów, 35-959, Poland Email: [email protected] Abstract: The quality of information systems to support democracy and public administration in the Slavic countries between Western Europe and Russia can be improved through the use of Interslavic, a zonal constructed language that can successfully replace English as a regional lingua franca, enhance participation and improve the overall quality of ICT used for e-Democracy assignments. Its potential role in improving computer translation between fusional languages with free word order by means of graph-based translation is discussed as well. This paper gives an overview of the pros and cons of various language options and describes the results of public research in the form of surveys, as well as the practical experiences of the authors. Special emphasis is given to the crucial role played by education: it is assumed that language, e-democracy, and education form a triangle of three inseparable, interdependent entities. Finally, the paper describes how these ideas can be developed in the future. Copyright © 2019 Inderscience Enterprises Ltd. The Interslavic language as a tool for supporting e-democracy 261 Keywords: e-democracy; education; Interslavic language; lingua franca; zonal constructed language; receptive multilingualism; Slavic countries; Central and Eastern Europe; human-computer interaction.
    [Show full text]
  • (Non)Native Speakering: the (Dis)Invention of (Non)Native Speakered Subjectivities in a Graduate Teacher Education Program
    University of Pennsylvania ScholarlyCommons Publicly Accessible Penn Dissertations 2016 (non)native Speakering: The (dis)invention Of (non)native Speakered Subjectivities In A Graduate Teacher Education Program Geeta A. Aneja University of Pennsylvania, [email protected] Follow this and additional works at: https://repository.upenn.edu/edissertations Part of the Bilingual, Multilingual, and Multicultural Education Commons, and the Teacher Education and Professional Development Commons Recommended Citation Aneja, Geeta A., "(non)native Speakering: The (dis)invention Of (non)native Speakered Subjectivities In A Graduate Teacher Education Program" (2016). Publicly Accessible Penn Dissertations. 2167. https://repository.upenn.edu/edissertations/2167 This paper is posted at ScholarlyCommons. https://repository.upenn.edu/edissertations/2167 For more information, please contact [email protected]. (non)native Speakering: The (dis)invention Of (non)native Speakered Subjectivities In A Graduate Teacher Education Program Abstract Despite its imprecision, the native-nonnative dichotomy has become the dominant paradigm for categorizing language users, learners, and educators. The “NNEST Movement” has been instrumental in documenting the privilege of native speakers, the marginalization of their nonnative counterparts, and why an individual may be perceived as one or the other. Although these efforts have contributed significantly owart ds increasing awareness of NNEST-hood, they also risk reifying nativeness and nonnativeness as objectively distinct categories. In this dissertation, I adopt a poststructuralist lens to reconceptualize native and nonnative speakers as complex, negotiated social subjectivities that emerge through a discursive process that I term (non)native speakering. I first use this framework to analyze the historico-political milieu that made possible the emergence of (non)native speakered subjectivities.
    [Show full text]
  • Librarians' Favourite Books from Their Country
    the world through picture books Librarians’ favourite books from their country A programme of Section Libraries for Children and Young Adults, IFLA – International Federation and Library Associations in collaboration with IFLA section Literacy and Reading and IBBY – International Board on Books for Young People. Programme coordination Annie Everall [email protected] in collaboration with Viviana Quiñones [email protected] The World through Picture Books, 2012 Edited by Annie Everall and Viviana Quiñones • Design by Ursula Held the throughworld picture books Foreword By Viviana Quiñones We are very happy to publish the first results of this participative, international, ongoing Chair, IFLA Section Libraries programme, “The World through Picture Books”. It deals with something we children’s for Children and Young Adults librarians must never lose sight of, even if we are so busy with new technologies, budget [email protected] restrictions, everyday work…: read children’s books and choose the best ones for our readers. Of course, we could spend hours discussing what “best” means, but one thing it surely means is very good books from the readers’ own country and from as many other countries as possible…This is why, inspired by Kazuko Yoda’s request to our Committee for advice on the “top ten” picture books in Committee members’ countries, we launched “The World through Picture Books” programme, in 2011. Librarians from thirty countries have already made their choice which we publish here, and, thanks to publishers’ generosity, their selected titles will be exhibited in Finland, before circulating in Japan; another set of books is available for any library in any country wanting to exhibit them.
    [Show full text]
  • Frontiers of Multilingual Grammar Development Ramona Enache ISBN 978-91-628-8787-2
    View metadata, citation and similar papers at core.ac.uk brought to you by CORE provided by Göteborgs universitets publikationer - e-publicering och e-arkiv THESIS FOR THE DEGREE OF DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY Frontiers of Multilingual Grammar Development Ramona Enache Department of Computer Science and Engineering Chalmers University of Technology and Göteborg University SE-412 96 Göteborg Sweden Göteborg, 2013 Frontiers of Multilingual Grammar Development Ramona Enache ISBN 978-91-628-8787-2 c Ramona Enache, 2013 Technical Report no. 99D Department of Computer Science and Engineering Language Technology Research Group Department of Computer Science and Engineering Chalmers University of Technology and Göteborg University SE-412 96 Göteborg, Sweden Printed at Chalmers, Göteborg, 2013 Abstract The thesis explores a number of ways for developing multilingual grammars written in GF (Grammatical Framework). The goal is to enhance both the coverage of the grammars, in terms of content and number of languages, and to reduce the development effort by automating a larger part of the process. The first direction in grammar development targets the creation of general language resources. These are the starting point for building domain-specific grammars for the language. Developing resource grammars gives a good overview of the effort required and provides a solid base for subsequent experiments in automation. Our work resulted in building computational grammars for Romanian and Swedish. A further development step is multilingual domain-specific grammar creation. The technique we employed is converting structured models into grammars, which pre- serves the original structure of the model as a backbone of the grammar and uses the general GF resources for a smooth multilingual verbalization of the model.
    [Show full text]
  • Applying Lessons from 20 Years of African Language Technology
    Looking forward by looking back: Applying lessons from 20 years of African language technology Martin Benjamin École Polytechnique Fédérale de Lausanne Lausanne, Switzerland [email protected] Mohomodou Houssouba Songhay.org Basel, Switzerland [email protected] Abstract This paper takes a frank look at what has and has not been achieved in African language technology during the past two decades. Several questions are addressed: What was the status of technology for African languages 20 years ago? What were the major initiatives during that time? What were their successes and failures? What can we learn from these experiences? How does this inform the work that we are planning going forward? Examining in particular the history of Swahili, it is argued that technology projects have often achieved their expressed aims, but have collectively not significantly advanced the normalization of African languages as operable within the technical sphere, even while Africa has become blanketed with mobile technology. It is argued that future projects will succeed only by asserting the goal that technology of 2035 must be fully operational in users' primary languages, and gearing policy, funding, and individual project efforts toward gathering and deploying linguistic data for a large number of African languages to meet cutting-edge technologies as they emerge. Keywords: HLT, localization, African languages, digital content, lexicography, Wikipedia, Swahili, equity, policy 1. African languages and technology in discuss the obstacles and opportunities those 1995 experiences reveal, and make suggestions for building In 1995, when graphical browsers were first opening those lessons into work going forward. the Internet to dial-up home-computer users in some ICT in 1995 was dominated by English, a Northern countries, African languages had virtually consequence of the locations of the predominant early no representation in the technical sphere.
    [Show full text]
  • ADVERBIAL ADJECTIVES and NOMINAL SCALARITY Melania
    ADVERTIMENT. Lʼaccés als continguts dʼaquesta tesi queda condicionat a lʼacceptació de les condicions dʼús establertes per la següent llicència Creative Commons: http://cat.creativecommons.org/?page_id=184 ADVERTENCIA. El acceso a los contenidos de esta tesis queda condicionado a la aceptación de las condiciones de uso establecidas por la siguiente licencia Creative Commons: http://es.creativecommons.org/blog/licencias/ WARNING. The access to the contents of this doctoral thesis it is limited to the acceptance of the use conditions set by the following Creative Commons license: https://creativecommons.org/licenses/?lang=en ADVERBIAL ADJECTIVES AND NOMINAL SCALARITY Melania Sánchez Masià Supervised by Dr. Violeta Demonte Barreto Dr. M. Carme Picallo Soler A doctoral dissertation submitted for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy in Cognitive Science and Language Universitat Autònoma de Barcelona Facultat de Filosofia i Lletres Departament de Filologia Catalana February 2017 This work by Melania Sánchez Masià is licensed under a Cre- ative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives 4.0 International License. A mi abuela mero1 (¿del cat. «nero»? Epinephelus guaza) m. Pez perciforme de carne muy estimada, como lo demuestra el proverbio «de la mar el mero y de la tierra el carnero». María Moliner, Diccionario de uso del español Abstract This dissertation investigates scalarity in the nominal domain through the study of a subset of prenominal adverbial adjectives in Spanish, adjectives of veracity (AVs; verdadero ‘true’, auténtico ‘authentic’) and adjectives of completeness (ACs; completo ‘complete’, total ‘total’, absoluto ‘absolute’). Its aim is to contribute to the understanding of the values associated with prenominal position in Romance, the parallelism between adverbial and adjectival modification, and the manifestations of scalarity in nouns.
    [Show full text]