A Phonological Analysis of Soft <G> in Şanlıurfa Dialect of Turkish: a True Consonant

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

A Phonological Analysis of Soft <G> in Şanlıurfa Dialect of Turkish: a True Consonant A phonological analysis of soft <g> in Şanlıurfa dialect of Turkish: a true consonant MUSTAFA DURMAZ INTRODUCTION The status of the phoneme corresponding to the Turkish grapheme soft <g> is ambiguous, therefore there have been different treatments regarding its nature and varying behaviors in morphologically and phonologically derived environments (Kabak 2007). ● It does not surface at the segmental level, therefore it is deleted (Göksel and Kerslake 2005). ● It is a velar fricative when it is preceded by a vowel or when it is in intervocalic position (Ergin 2012). ● It is not a consonant but a semi vowel, and it assimilates to the adjacent vowel (Demircan (2002) and Karaağaç (2008). ●It is considered to be a glide by some. (Selen 1979) • Clearly, much remains to be answered regarding the status of the phoneme this grapheme corresponds to in Turkish. INTRODUCTION • Research (Demircan, 2002; Inkelas, 2009; Karaağaç, 2008; Kornfilt,1997; Göksel & Kerslake, 2005) investigating the nature of soft <g> have usually focused on the Istanbul dialect without giving much attention to the dialects which are spoken throughout the country. • Other dialects could provide some novel insights into the formal status of this phoneme. • This study aims to examine the nature of the phoneme corresponding to soft <g> grapheme in Şanlıurfa dialect of Turkish. • Main question: Is there any difference between the consonantal features of the phoneme corresponding to soft <g> in Standard Turkish and Şanlıurfa dialect of Turkish? SOME INTERESTING FACTS ABOUT THE SANLIURFA DIALECT • The Şanlıurfa dialect is one of the many dialects of Turkish which are spoken in Turkey. Şanlıurfa is a city located in south-east part of Turkey (bordering with Syria) and it is predominantly populated by Kurdish people, as well as Turkish (and some speakers of Arabic). • While standard Turkish has both rounding harmony and backness harmony, the Şanlıurfa dialect only has backness harmony, as can be seen in table(1 and 2). Gloss Words in St. Turksih Sanliurfa Dative Locative Ablative dialect(nominative) 1)water melon /kar.puz/ [kɑr.pɯz] [kɑr.pɯz.dɑ] [kɑr.pɯz.dɑn] [kɑr.pɯz. ɑ] 2) tongue /dil/ [dil] [dil. ɛ] [dil.dɛ] [dil.dɛn] 3) grape /y.zym/ [y.zɯm] [y.zɯm. ɛ] [y.zɯm.dɛ] [y.zɯm.dɛn] 4) twin /i.kiz/ /i.kɯz/ /i.kɯz. ɛ/ /i.kɯz. dɛ/ /i.kɯz. dɛn/ 5) wood /o.dun/ [o.dɯn] [o.dɯn. ɑ] [o.dɯn.dɑ] [o.dɯn.dɑn] 6) light /ɯ.ʃɯk/ /ɯ.ʃɯx/ /ɯ.ʃɯ. ɣɑ/ /ɯ.ʃɯ. ɣdɑ/ /ɯ.ʃɯ. ɣdɑn/ ●/ɰ/ vowel remains neutral and does not participate in vowel harmony. However, the backness harmony started by initial vowel in the word spreads to the vowels in suffixes as in (3,4,5 and, 6) ●The accusative case /ɰ/ in Sanliurfa dialect does not even agree even with backness harmony, it remains neutral. Sanliurfa Gloss Words dialect(nominative) Accusative 1) ‘grape /y.zym/ /y.zɯm/ [y.zɯm. ɯ] 2)` tongue ` /dil/ /dil/ [dil. ɯ] 3)‘water melon’ /kɑr-puz/ /kɑr-pɯz/ [kɑr-pɯz. ɯ] METHODOLOGY • Participants: • One male and one female native speaker of Sanliurfa dialect • Male participant is 30 years old and female participant is 28 years old. • Stimuli: • Target words including the phoneme corresponding to soft <g> were presented in Sanliurfa dialect. ●The stimuli were inserted in a carrier sentence, along with some filler sentences. And participants were asked to read aloud these sentences twice. - `Ben sana……….. dedim. `I told you ……`. The Phonological Comparison of Soft <g> in Standard Turkish and in the Şanlıurfa Dialect of Turkish: ● In Standard Turkish soft <g> can appear in word final position, where it is not articulated but lengthens the pronunciation of preceding vowels (1,2). ● In Şanlıurfa dialect, soft <ɡ> can appear in word final position, and it is articulated as /χ/ when preceded by [+back] vowels and is articulated as a voiced velar-plosive /ɡ/ when preceded by a [-back] vowel either in word final position or elsewhere. Gloss word Nominative (St. Turkish) Şanlıurfa Dialect 1) garden/bound bağ [ba:] [bɑχ] 2) needle tığ [tɯ:] [tɯχ] 3) raw çiğ [t͡ʃy: ] [t͡ʃyɡ] 4) entartainment eğlence [ɛ:.lɛn.cɛ] or [ɛɣ.lɛn.cɛ]* [ɛɡ.lɛn.cɛ] *possible (Kornfilt, 1997), but not typical. ● In Ottoman Turkish, soft /g/ alternated between two variants, in different vowel contexts (Logacev ; Fuchs; Żygis 2014): In the environment of front vowels, it was /ɡ/ and in back vowel contexts, it was /χ/. ● Soft <g> is articulated as /χ/ in word final position when preceded by a [+back] vowel. When it is followed by a vowel it receives voicing and pronounced as a voiced velar fricative [ɣ]. Gloss word Nominative (St. Turkish) Şanlıurfa Dialect Dative case 1) garden/bound bağ [ba:] [bɑχ] [bɑ. ɣa] 2) needle tığ [tɯ:] [tɯχ] [tɯ. ɣa] ● Final devoicing: soft /g/ is a voiced velar fricative [ɣ], but it gets devoiced when it is in word-final position as in (1,2) above. / ɣ / --> [-voice] / ___ # 3) raw çiğ [t͡ʃy: ] [t͡ʃyɡ] [t͡ʃyɡ.ɛ] ● Soft /g/ becomes a voiced velar plosive/ɡ/ when followed by [-back] V as in (3). ●Soft is underlying a voiced velar fricative; it has two allophones: voiceless velar fricative and voiced velar fricative plosive /g/. ● Soft <g> and regular /ɡ/ constitute the minimal pairs seen in (4,5,6 and 7). 4) raw [xɑm] 5) sorrow [ɡam] 6) news [xɛbɛr] 7) you die [gɛbɛr] ● This data suggests that they are separate phonemes but they are partially overlapping. ● In standard Turkish, soft <g> can also appear in intervocalic position. When the preceding vowels and the following vowels are the same, soft <g> is not pronounced or becomes less audible, and it lengthens the pronunciation of the vowel as seen (1,2,and 5). ● If the following vowel is different from preceding one, then soft <g> triggers the articulation of a diphthong (Ergenç, 1989), as in (3 and 4). ●The (Ergenç, 1989), analysis raises another problem due to the phonotactic constraints of Turkish dictating that two vowels cannot be next to each other in native words (except loan words such as `saat & müracaat). Gloss word Nominative (St. Şanlıurfa Şanlıurfa Dialect Turkish) Dialect 1)tree ağaç [a:at͡ʃ] ağaç [ɑɣɑt͡ʃ] 2)vapor buğu [bu:u] buğı [bu.ɣɯ] 3)deaf sağır [sa. ɯr] sağır [sɑ.ɣɯr] 4)to smash (dough) yoğurmak [jo.ur.mak] yoğırmak [jo.ɣɯr.mɑk] 5) rasp eğe [ ɛ: ɛ] eğe [ ɛ.g ɛ] 6) if eğer [ ɛ: ɛɾ] eğer [ ɛ.g ɛɾ] ● In intervocalic position, the Şanlıurfa dialect does not allow the deletion of soft <g>. When it is preceded by a back vowel and followed by a vowel, it is pronounced as a voiced velar fricative [ɣ] as in [1,2,3, and 4]. If it is preceded by a front vowel and followed by a vowel, it is pronounced as [ɡ] as in [5]. ●In standard Turkish, soft <g> never occurs adjacent to voiceless stops, but instead occurs adjacent to sonorant consonants. When it is followed by sonorant consonant, it may be articulated, but it typically lengthens the preceding vowel. On the other hand, in Şanlıurfa dialect, soft <g> can appear both before a voiceless stop and a sonorant consonant as other consonants. Gloss word Nominati Plural suffix Locative Case Plural suffix Locative Case (St. Turkish) (St. Turkish) ve (St. Şanlıurfa Şanlıurfa Dialect Turkish) Dialect mountai dağ [da:] [da:.lar] or #[daɣ.lar] [ da :.da] or *[daɣ.da] [daɣ.lar] [daɣ.dɑ] n connecti bağ [ba:] [ba:.lar] or # [daɣ.lar] [ba:.lar] or *[baɣ.da] [baɣ. lar] [baɣ.da] on # possible (Kornfilt,1997), but not typical *Ungrammatical ●When <k> and <g> are in word final position and followed by a suffix which has a vowel in the onset positon as in (1), (2), and (3), velar deletion applies (Lewis, 1967; Göksel & Kerslake, 2005). Gloss Words Nominative St. Turkish St. Turkish Şanlıurfa Şanlıurfa Dialect Dialect Dative /- Dative /-A/ Genitive /-In/ Genitive /-In/ A/ 1) donkey eşek [e.ʃek] [e.ʃe.e] [e. ʃe.in] [e.ʃe. ɡe] [e. ʃe. ɡɰn] 2) stupid sɑlɑk [sɑ.lɑk] [sɑ.lɑ. ɑ] [sɑ.lɑ. ɰn] [sɑ.lɑ.ɣɑ] [sɑ.lɑ.ɣɰn] 3)sosyologist sosyoloɡ [sos. jo.lok] [sos. jo.lo.ɑ] [sos. jo.lo.un [sos. jo.loɣ.ɑ] [sos. jo.loɣ.ɰn 4) `league` /lig/ [li. ɡ] [li. ɡɛ] [li. ɡin] [li. ɡɛ] [li. ɡ ɰn] ● Unlike in Standard Turkish, the /k/ and /g/ phonemes do not trigger velar deletion in the Şanlıurfa dialect; rather, these two consonants preserve their phonetic properties. However, they demonstrate varying phonetic manifestations in different phonological environments. In word final position, a voiceless velar stop /k/ is articulated as a voiced velar stop [g] in an intervocalic position, when preceded by a front vowel as in (1) in the chart above. The /k/ consonant is pronounced as a voiceless uvular fricative [X] in word-final position when preceded by a back vowel, and when it is in intervocalic position and preceded by a back vowel, it is articulated as [ɣ], a voiced velar fricative, as in (2) above. The /g/ phoneme is pronounced as [X] in word final position, and when it is in intervocalic position and is preceded [+back] vowel, it is pronounced [ɣ] as in (3). When preceded by a [-back] vowel and followed by vowel, it is articulated as [ɡ] as in (6). DISCUSSION AND CONCLUSION ● Unlike in Standard Turkish, soft <g> in Sanliurfa dialect always surfaces at the segmental level. ● It demonstrates behaviors like other consonants in Turkish: such as being devoiced at word final position or occurring adjacent to non-sonorant consonant. ● It is a voiced velar fricative /ɣ/ and it is in complementary distribution with voiceless velar fricative /x/ (word-final & when preceded by a back vowel) and voiced velar plosive/g/ (when preceded by a front vowel).
Recommended publications
  • The Violability of Backness in Retroflex Consonants
    The violability of backness in retroflex consonants Paul Boersma University of Amsterdam Silke Hamann ZAS Berlin February 11, 2005 Abstract This paper addresses remarks made by Flemming (2003) to the effect that his analysis of the interaction between retroflexion and vowel backness is superior to that of Hamann (2003b). While Hamann maintained that retroflex articulations are always back, Flemming adduces phonological as well as phonetic evidence to prove that retroflex consonants can be non-back and even front (i.e. palatalised). The present paper, however, shows that the phonetic evidence fails under closer scrutiny. A closer consideration of the phonological evidence shows, by making a principled distinction between articulatory and perceptual drives, that a reanalysis of Flemming’s data in terms of unviolated retroflex backness is not only possible but also simpler with respect to the number of language-specific stipulations. 1 Introduction This paper is a reply to Flemming’s article “The relationship between coronal place and vowel backness” in Phonology 20.3 (2003). In a footnote (p. 342), Flemming states that “a key difference from the present proposal is that Hamann (2003b) employs inviolable articulatory constraints, whereas it is a central thesis of this paper that the constraints relating coronal place to tongue-body backness are violable”. The only such constraint that is violable for Flemming but inviolable for Hamann is the constraint that requires retroflex coronals to be articulated with a back tongue body. Flemming expresses this as the violable constraint RETRO!BACK, or RETRO!BACKCLO if it only requires that the closing phase of a retroflex consonant be articulated with a back tongue body.
    [Show full text]
  • A Different Look at the Lutsk Karaim Sound System (From the Second Half of the 19Th Century On)
    View metadata, citation and similar papers at core.ac.uk brought to you by CORE provided by Jagiellonian Univeristy Repository Studia Linguistica Universitatis Iagellonicae Cracoviensis 128 (2011) MICHAŁ NÉMETH Jagiellonian University, Cracow A DIFFERENT LOOK AT THE LUTSK KARAIM SOUND SYSTEM (FROM THE SECOND HALF OF THE 19TH CENTURY ON) Keywords: Lutsk Karaim, Karaim phonetics and phonology, Ukrainian dialects Abstract After endeavouring to examine the grammatical descriptions published in the literature to date and to reconstruct the sound system of the south-western dialect of Karaim as it was presented in the literature, it can certainly be concluded that the matter is far from clear. This is for the simple reason that these works contradict each other at various points. The reason for such discrepancies should be sought in the historical and linguistic backgrounds of the two main centres of the south-western Karaim population, i.e. Lutsk and Halich. Even though these two centres were always in close communication with one another, and the language that was spoken in them originates beyond any doubt from one common root, they remained for centuries under slightly different linguistic influences as a result of the Slavonic languages surrounding them. The present paper aims to present and, where possible, clarify the differences which follow from the studies on the Karaim sound system we have at our disposal. An attempt is also made to identify some differences between the Lutsk and Halich subdialects of south-western Karaim, and explain their origin. Since the grammatical descriptions we are dealing with here and the written sources we are able to work with concern the end of the first half of the 19th century at the earliest, the time scale of our interest is limited to the second half of the 19th and the first four decades of the 20th century.
    [Show full text]
  • Sociophonetic Variation in Bolivian Quechua Uvular Stops
    Title Page Sociophonetic Variation in Bolivian Quechua Uvular Stops by Eva Bacas University of Pittsburgh, 2019 Submitted to the Graduate Faculty of the Dietrich School of Arts and Sciences in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Bachelor of Philosophy University of Pittsburgh 2019 Committee Page UNIVERSITY OF PITTSBURGH DIETRICH SCHOOL OF ARTS AND SCIENCES This thesis was presented by Eva Bacas It was defended on November 8, 2019 and approved by Alana DeLoge, Quechua Instructor, Department of Linguistics, University of Pittsburgh Melinda Fricke, Assistant Professor, Department of Linguistics, University of Pittsburgh Gillian Gallagher, Associate Professor, Department of Linguistics, New York University Thesis Advisor/Dissertation Director: Claude Mauk, Senior Lecturer, Department of Linguistics, University of Pittsburgh ii Copyright © by Eva Bacas 2019 iii Abstract Sociophonetic Variation in Bolivian Quechua Uvular Stops Eva Bacas, BPhil University of Pittsburgh, 2019 Quechua is an indigenous language of the Andes region of South America. In Cochabamba, Bolivia, Quechua and Spanish have been in contact for over 500 years. In this thesis, I explore sociolinguistic variation among bilingual speakers of Cochabamba Quechua (CQ) and Spanish by investigating the relationship between the production of the voiceless uvular stop /q/ and speakers’ sociolinguistic backgrounds. I conducted a speech production study and sociolinguistic interview with seven bilingual CQ-Spanish speakers. I analyzed manner of articulation and place of articulation variation. Results indicate that manner of articulation varies primarily due to phonological factors, and place of articulation varies according to sociolinguistic factors. This reveals that among bilingual CQ-Spanish speakers, production of voiceless uvular stop /q/ does vary sociolinguistically.
    [Show full text]
  • Lecture 5 Sound Change
    An articulatory theory of sound change An articulatory theory of sound change Hypothesis: Most common initial motivation for sound change is the automation of production. Tokens reduced online, are perceived as reduced and represented in the exemplar cluster as reduced. Therefore we expect sound changes to reflect a decrease in gestural magnitude and an increase in gestural overlap. What are some ways to test the articulatory model? The theory makes predictions about what is a possible sound change. These predictions could be tested on a cross-linguistic database. Sound changes that take place in the languages of the world are very similar (Blevins 2004, Bateman 2000, Hajek 1997, Greenberg et al. 1978). We should consider both common and rare changes and try to explain both. Common and rare changes might have different characteristics. Among the properties we could look for are types of phonetic motivation, types of lexical diffusion, gradualness, conditioning environment and resulting segments. Common vs. rare sound change? We need a database that allows us to test hypotheses concerning what types of changes are common and what types are not. A database of sound changes? Most sound changes have occurred in undocumented periods so that we have no record of them. Even in cases with written records, the phonetic interpretation may be unclear. Only a small number of languages have historic records. So any sample of known sound changes would be biased towards those languages. A database of sound changes? Sound changes are known only for some languages of the world: Languages with written histories. Sound changes can be reconstructed by comparing related languages.
    [Show full text]
  • LT3212 Phonetics Assignment 4 Mavis, Wong Chak Yin
    LT3212 Phonetics Assignment 4 Mavis, Wong Chak Yin Essay Title: The sound system of Japanese This essay aims to introduce the sound system of Japanese, including the inventories of consonants, vowels, and diphthongs. The phonological variations of the sound segments in different phonetic environments are also included. For the illustration, word examples are given and they are presented in the following format: [IPA] (Romaji: “meaning”). Consonants In Japanese, there are 14 core consonants, and some of them have a lot of allophonic variations. The various types of consonants classified with respect to their manner of articulation are presented as follows. Stop Japanese has six oral stops or plosives, /p b t d k g/, which are classified into three place categories, bilabial, alveolar, and velar, as listed below. In each place category, there is a pair of plosives with the contrast in voicing. /p/ = a voiceless bilabial plosive [p]: [ippai] (ippai: “A cup of”) /b/ = a voiced bilabial plosive [b]: [baɴ] (ban: “Night”) /t/ = a voiceless alveolar plosive [t]: [oto̞ ːto̞ ] (ototo: “Brother”) /d/ = a voiced alveolar plosive [d]: [to̞ mo̞ datɕi] (tomodachi: “Friend”) /k/ = a voiceless velar plosive [k]: [kaiɰa] (kaiwa: “Conversation”) /g/ = a voiced velar plosive [g]: [ɡakɯβsai] (gakusai: “Student”) Phonetically, Japanese also has a glottal stop [ʔ] which is commonly produced to separate the neighboring vowels occurring in different syllables. This phonological phenomenon is known as ‘glottal stop insertion’. The glottal stop may be realized as a pause, which is used to indicate the beginning or the end of an utterance. For instance, the word “Japanese money” is actually pronounced as [ʔe̞ ɴ], instead of [je̞ ɴ], and the pronunciation of “¥15” is [dʑɯβːɡo̞ ʔe̞ ɴ].
    [Show full text]
  • The Sound Patterns of Camuno: Description and Explanation in Evolutionary Phonology
    City University of New York (CUNY) CUNY Academic Works All Dissertations, Theses, and Capstone Projects Dissertations, Theses, and Capstone Projects 6-2014 The Sound Patterns Of Camuno: Description And Explanation In Evolutionary Phonology Michela Cresci Graduate Center, City University of New York How does access to this work benefit ou?y Let us know! More information about this work at: https://academicworks.cuny.edu/gc_etds/191 Discover additional works at: https://academicworks.cuny.edu This work is made publicly available by the City University of New York (CUNY). Contact: [email protected] THE SOUND PATTERNS OF CAMUNO: DESCRIPTION AND EXPLANATION IN EVOLUTIONARY PHONOLOGY by MICHELA CRESCI A dissertation submitted to the Graduate Faculty in Linguistics in partial fulfillment of the requirement for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy, The City Universtiy of New York 2014 i 2014 MICHELA CRESCI All rights reserved ii This manuscript has been read and accepted for the Graduate Faculty in Linguistics in satisfaction of the dissertation requirement for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy. JULIETTE BLEVINS ____________________ __________________________________ Date Chair of Examining Committee GITA MARTOHARDJONO ____________________ ___________________________________ Date Executive Officer KATHLEEN CURRIE HALL DOUGLAS H. WHALEN GIOVANNI BONFADINI Supervisory Committee THE CITY UNIVERSITY OF NEW YORK iii Abstract THE SOUND PATTERNS OF CAMUNO: DESCRIPTION AND EXPLANATION IN EVOLUTIONARY PHONOLOGY By Michela Cresci Advisor: Professor Juliette Blevins This dissertation presents a linguistic study of the sound patterns of Camuno framed within Evolutionary Phonology (Blevins, 2004, 2006, to appear). Camuno is a variety of Eastern Lombard, a Romance language of northern Italy, spoken in Valcamonica. Camuno is not a local variety of Italian, but a sister of Italian, a local divergent development of the Latin originally spoken in Italy (Maiden & Perry, 1997, p.
    [Show full text]
  • Wagner 1 Dutch Fricatives Dutch Is an Indo-European Language of The
    Wagner 1 Dutch Fricatives Dutch is an Indo-European language of the West Germanic branch, closely related to Frisian and English. It is spoken by about 22 million people in the Netherlands, Belgium, Aruba, Suriname, and the Netherlands Antilles, nations in which the Dutch language also has an official status. Additionally, Dutch is still spoken in parts of Indonesia as a result of colonial rule, but it has been replaced by Afrikaans in South Africa. In the Netherlands, Algemeen Nederlands is the standard form of the language taught in schools and used by the government. It is generally spoken in the western part of the country, in the provinces of Nord and Zuid Holland. The Netherlands is divided into northern and southern regions by the Rijn river and these regions correspond to the areas where the main dialects of Dutch are distinguished from each other. My consultant, Annemarie Toebosch, is from the village of Bemmel in the Netherlands. Her dialect is called Betuws Dutch because Bemmel is located in the Betuwe region between two branches of the Rijn river. Toebosch lived in the Netherlands until the age of 25 when she relocated to the United States for graduate school. Today she is a professor of linguistics at the University of Michigan-Flint and speaks only English in her professional career. For the past 18 months she has been speaking Dutch more at home with her son, but in the 9 years prior to that, she only spoke Dutch once or twice a week with her parents and other relatives who remain in the Netherlands.
    [Show full text]
  • Palatals in Spanish and French: an Analysis Rachael Gray
    Florida State University Libraries Honors Theses The Division of Undergraduate Studies 2012 Palatals in Spanish and French: An Analysis Rachael Gray Follow this and additional works at the FSU Digital Library. For more information, please contact [email protected] Abstract (Palatal, Spanish, French) This thesis deals with palatals from Latin into Spanish and French. Specifically, it focuses on the diachronic history of each language with a focus on palatals. I also look at studies that have been conducted concerning palatals, and present a synchronic analysis of palatals in modern day Spanish and French. The final section of this paper focuses on my research design in second language acquisition of palatals for native French speakers learning Spanish. 2 THE FLORIDA STATE UNIVERSITY COLLEGE OF ARTS AND SCIENCES PALATALS IN SPANISH AND FRENCH: AN ANALYSIS BY: RACHAEL GRAY A Thesis submitted to the Department of Modern Languages in partial fulfillment of the requirements for graduation with Honors in the Major Degree Awarded: 3 Spring, 2012 The members of the Defense Committee approve the thesis of Rachael Gray defended on March 21, 2012 _____________________________________ Professor Carolina Gonzaléz Thesis Director _______________________________________ Professor Gretchen Sunderman Committee Member _______________________________________ Professor Eric Coleman Outside Committee Member 4 Contents Acknowledgements ......................................................................................................................... 5 0.
    [Show full text]
  • Building a Universal Phonetic Model for Zero-Resource Languages
    Building a Universal Phonetic Model for Zero-Resource Languages Paul Moore MInf Project (Part 2) Interim Report Master of Informatics School of Informatics University of Edinburgh 2020 3 Abstract Being able to predict phones from speech is a challenge in and of itself, but what about unseen phones from different languages? In this project, work was done towards building precisely this kind of universal phonetic model. Using the GlobalPhone language corpus, phones’ articulatory features, a recurrent neu- ral network, open-source libraries, and an innovative prediction system, a model was created to predict phones based on their features alone. The results show promise, especially for using these models on languages within the same family. 4 Acknowledgements Once again, a huge thank you to Steve Renals, my supervisor, for all his assistance. I greatly appreciated his practical advice and reasoning when I got stuck, or things seemed overwhelming, and I’m very thankful that he endorsed this project. I’m immensely grateful for the support my family and friends have provided in the good times and bad throughout my studies at university. A big shout-out to my flatmates Hamish, Mark, Stephen and Iain for the fun and laugh- ter they contributed this year. I’m especially grateful to Hamish for being around dur- ing the isolation from Coronavirus and for helping me out in so many practical ways when I needed time to work on this project. Lastly, I wish to thank Jesus Christ, my Saviour and my Lord, who keeps all these things in their proper perspective, and gives me strength each day.
    [Show full text]
  • Information to Users
    INFORMATION TO USERS This manuscript has been reproduced from the microfilm master. UMI films the text directly from the original or copy submitted. Thus, some thesis and dissertation copies are in typewriter face, while others may be from any type of computer printer. The quality of this reproduction is dependent upon the quality of the copy submitted. Broken or indistinct print, colored or poor quality illustrations and photographs, print bleedthrough, substandard margins, and improper alignment can adversely affect reproduction. In the unlikely event that the author did not send UMI a complete manuscript and there are missing pages, these will be noted. Also, if unauthorized copyright material had to be removed, a note will indicate the deletion. Oversize materials (e.g., maps, drawings, charts) are reproduced by sectioning the original, beginning at the upper left-hand comer and continuing from left to right in equal sections with small overlaps. Each original is also photographed in one exposure and is included in reduced form at the back of the book. Photographs included in the original manuscript have been reproduced xerographically in this copy. Higher quality 6" x 9" black and white photographic prints are available for any photographs or illustrations appearing in this copy for an additional charge. Contact UMI directly to order. UMI University Microfilms International A Bell & Howell Information C om pany 300 North Zeeb Road. Ann Arbor, Ml 48106-1346 USA 313/761-4700 800/521-0600 Order Number 9401204 Phonetics and phonology of Nantong Chinese Ac, Benjamin Xiaoping, Ph.D. The Ohio State University, 1993 Copyri^t ©1993 by Ao, Benjamin Xiaoping.
    [Show full text]
  • Velar Segments in Old English and Old Irish
    In: Jacek Fisiak (ed.) Proceedings of the Sixth International Conference on Historical Linguistics. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, 1985, 267-79. Velar segments in Old English and Old Irish Raymond Hickey University of Bonn The purpose of this paper is to look at a section of the phoneme inventories of the oldest attested stage of English and Irish, velar segments, to see how they are manifested phonetically and to consider how they relate to each other on the phonological level. The reason I have chosen to look at two languages is that it is precisely when one compares two language systems that one notices that structural differences between languages on one level will be correlated by differences on other levels demonstrating their interrelatedness. Furthermore it is necessary to view segments on a given level in relation to other segments. The group under consideration here is just one of several groups. Velar segments viewed within the phonological system of both Old English and Old Irish cor relate with three other major groups, defined by place of articulation: palatals, dentals, and labials. The relationship between these groups is not the same in each language for reasons which are morphological: in Old Irish changes in grammatical category are frequently indicated by palatalizing a final non-palatal segment (labial, dental, or velar). The same function in Old English is fulfilled by suffixes and /or prefixes. This has meant that for Old English the phonetically natural and lower-level alternation of velar elements with palatal elements in a palatal environ ment was to be found whereas in Old Irish this alternation had been denaturalized and had lost its automatic character.
    [Show full text]
  • Nasalization of the Initial Consonant (D-) of the Present Perfect Tense Marker (-Dẹ) After the Main Verb in Ịzọn: a Case Study of the Tarakiri Dialect
    US-China Foreign Language, ISSN 1539-8080 January 2014, Vol. 12, No. 1, 36-43 D DAVID PUBLISHING Nasalization of the Initial Consonant (d-) of the Present Perfect Tense Marker (-dẹ) After the Main Verb in Ịzọn: A Case Study of the Tarakiri Dialect God’spower Tamaraukuro Prezi Niger Delta University, Wilberforce Island, Nigeria In Ịzọn, as in Akan and Edo, nasalization is distinctive, because it brings about a change in meaning as in: /s/ “fight” and /s/ “to stretch”; /bu/ “soft” and /bu/ “honey”. /d/ and /n/ are both alveolar stops in Ịzọn: /d/ is an oral stop, while /n/ is a nasal stop. Also, while /d/ is a voiced alveolar plosive, /n/ is an (voiced) alveolar nasal. This means, in effect, that the two consonants (/d/ and /n/) are homorganic in Ịzọn, because they are made at the same place of articulation and with the same articulators. However, in many verbs, whose present perfect tenses are represented by -dẹ in the Tarakiri dialect of Ịzọn, the -dẹ is changed to -nẹ after the main verb. That is, in phonological terms, /d/ rewrites as /n/ in the environment after a syllable consisting of a nasal consonant or a nasalized vowel, i.e., [+ voice + alveolar + plosive] becomes [+ alveolar + nasal] after [+ nasal]. This paper looks at, describes, and examines this phonological process/phenomenon of the replacement of an oral stop by a nasal stop after a syllable consisting of a nasal consonant or a nasalized vowel. Keywords: Ịzọn, Tarakiri, homorganic, nasal, nasalization, -dẹ, -nẹ, assimilation Introduction A Brief Introduction to Ịzọn Ịzọn is the major one of the Ijoid group of languages spoken in the Niger Delta Region and the South-South geo-political zone of Nigeria.
    [Show full text]