<<

Journal of Neurotherapy: Investigations in , and Applied Connectivity Theory of : Use of Connectivity Measures in Assessing and Treating Autistic Disorders a a Robert Coben PhD & Thomas E. Myers MS a Neurorehabilitation & Neuropsychological Services , Massapequa Park, New York Published online: 12 Dec 2008.

To cite this article: Robert Coben PhD & Thomas E. Myers MS (2008) Connectivity Theory of Autism: Use of Connectivity Measures in Assessing and Treating Autistic Disorders, Journal of Neurotherapy: Investigations in Neuromodulation, Neurofeedback and Applied Neuroscience, 12:2-3, 161-179, DOI: 10.1080/10874200802398824

To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10874200802398824

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

© International Society for Neurofeedback and Research (ISNR), all rights reserved. This article (the “Article”) may be accessed online from ISNR at no charge. The Article may be viewed online, stored in electronic or physical form, or archived for research, teaching, and private study purposes. The Article may be archived in public libraries or university libraries at the direction of said public library or university library. Any other reproduction of the Article for redistribution, sale, resale, loan, sublicensing, systematic supply, or other distribution, including both physical and electronic reproduction for such purposes, is expressly forbidden. Preparing or reproducing derivative works of this article is expressly forbidden. ISNR makes no representation or warranty as to the accuracy or completeness of any content in the Article. From 1995 to 2013 the Journal of Neurotherapy was the official publication of ISNR (www. Isnr.org); on April 27, 2016 ISNR acquired the journal from Taylor & Francis Group, LLC. In 2014, ISNR established its official open-access journal NeuroRegulation (ISSN: 2373-0587; www.neuroregulation.org).

THIS OPEN-ACCESS CONTENT MADE POSSIBLE BY THESE GENEROUS SPONSORS

Connectivity Theory of Autism: Use of Connectivity Measures in Assessing and Treating Autistic Disorders

Robert Coben, PhD Thomas E. Myers, MS

ABSTRACT. Background. Autism is a disorder characterized by deficits in communication, social interaction, a limited range of interests, and repetitive stereotypical behavior. Although it is believed that changes in the leading to Autism occur early on in prenatal and early postnatal development, there is no definitive test for a diagnosis of Autism. The diagnosis is made on the basis of behavioral signs and symptoms alone and is usually not made until age 2 or later. There have been numerous neuroanatomical abnormalities noted in Autism, some of which can be linked to neuropsychological dysfunction. Recently a new theory has become prominent which suggests the disorder may be due to aberrant neural connectivity patterns. Evi- dence in support of this theory has come from anatomical studies of as well as functional neuroimaging studies. Methods. Most studies have employed functional magnetic resonance imaging to investigate connectivity, or (EEG) coherence studies. The high temporal resolu- tion of EEG lends itself well to the investigation of cerebral connectivity. Research suggests there may be patterns of both hyper- and hypoconnectivity between various brain regions. Seven different patterns of abnormal connectivity which can be analyzed with EEG are proposed. Results. Patterns of hyperconnectivity may be found in frontotemporal and left hemispheric regions, whereas patterns of hypoconnectivity are often seen in frontal (orbitofrontal), right posterior (occipital=parietal-temporal), frontal-posterior, and left hemispheric regions. In addition to these patterns of hypo- and hyperconnectivity, a mu rhythm complex has been iden- tified. Treatment goals may be based on coherence anomalies identified by quantitative EEG analysis. Increased coherence between brain regions may be downtrained, whereas decreased coherence between brain regions may be uptrained. Clinical examples of each pattern and a discussion of their neurofeedback treatment are provided. Conclusion. A theory of autistic disorders is presented that has at its’ core neural connectivity disturbances. Multivariate EEG connectivity indices are utilized to formulate a typology of connectivity anomalies or patterns that have been observed over a series of autistic patients.

Robert Coben is Director, Neurorehabilitation & Neuropsychological Services, Massapequa Park, New York. Thomas E. Myers is affiliated with Neurorehabilitation & Neuropsychological Services, Massapequa Park, New York. Address correspondence to: Robert Coben, PhD, Director, Neurorehabilitation & Neuropsychological Services, 1035 Park Boulevard, Suite 2B, Massapequa Park, NY 11762 (E-mail: [email protected]). Journal of Neurotherapy, Vol. 12(2–3) 2008 Copyright © 2008 ISNR. All rights reserved. doi: 10.1080/10874200802398824 161 162 JOURNAL OF NEUROTHERAPY

These represent phenotypic expressions of the underlying pathology that leads to autistic symp- toms. Examples demonstrate how these connectivity metrics can be used to understand autistic disturbances and formulate neurofeedback strategies for remedying these difficulties.

KEYWORDS. Assessment, autism, coherence, EEG, hypo=hyperconnectivity, neurofeed- back, theory, treatment

Autistic spectrum disorders (ASDs) are a 1998; Hamilton, 2000; McCandless, 2005; group of pervasive developmental disabilities Sicile-Kira, 2004; Siegel, 1996). characterized by deficits in communication, social interaction, and restricted repetitive behavior. The spectrum includes Autistic NEUROPATHOLOGY IN AUTISM Disorder, Rett’s Disorder, Childhood Dis- integrative Disorder, Asperger Disorder, Early evidence of abnormal brain morphol- and Pervasive Developmental Disorder Not ogy came from Kanner (1943), who noted an Otherwise Specified (PDD-NOS; Tidmarsh enlargement of the heads of autistic children. & Volkmar, 2003). Research indicates the Kanner’s anecdotal findings were then corro- prevalence of these disorders is on the rise, borated by a plethora of more controlled stu- with studies indicating that about 1 out of dies. Macrocephaly has been found in 150 eight-year-old children will be diagnosed approximately 20% of autistic individuals with an ASD (Centers for Disease Control (Bailey, Luthert, & Bolton, 1993; Courchesne, and Prevention, 2006). Although persons Carper, & Akshoomoff, 2003; Davidovich, with ASD share some similarity, each disor- Patterson, & Gartside, 1996) and is supported der has its own unique characteristics, which by results from neuroimaging (Courchesne et permit different diagnoses. Autistic Disorder al., 2001; Filipek et al., 1992; Piven, Arndt, is characterized by impaired social interac- Bailey, & Andreasen, 1996) and increased tion, delay, or total lack of spoken brain weight (Bailey et al., 1998; Courchesne, and communication, as well as repetitive ste- Muller, & Saitoh, 1999). Specifically, there reotyped behaviors, interests or activities may be an increase in temporal, parietal, and (American Psychiatric Association, 2000). volume but not the Asperger’s Disorder is often associated with (Piven et al., 1996). This pattern may be inter- high cognitive function, literal pedantic preted as either abnormal posterior brain speech, difficulty comprehending implied enlargement or a frontal lobe abnormality in meaning, problems with fluid movement, which the frontal lobes lag behind the rest of and inappropriate social interaction. PDD- the brain in their development. NOS refers to the category of deficits in Kemper and Bauman (1993) investigated language and social skills that do not meet alterations in limbic areas by reviewing the the criteria for other disorders. In contrast, findings of several case reports. Their ana- Childhood Disintegrative Disorder and Rett’s lyses from six autistic subjects of varying Disorder are characterized by intervals of ages revealed increased cell packing density normal early development followed by loss and reduced cell size in the , of previously acquired skills. Although com- subiculum, , and to a lesser extent munication and social skill deficits are com- in the , mammillary bodies, mon among these conditions, there remains and septal nuclei. Other studies showed a a substantial degree of variability in terms of decrease in Purkinje cell density in the onset and severity of symptomatology within of autistic cases (Bailey et al., the autistic spectrum of disorders (Attwood, 1998; Kember & Bauman, 1993). Kemper Scientific Articles 163 and Bauman (1998) interpreted these find- and increased cerebral spinal fluid volume. In ings as a hindrance in the normal develop- addition, white matter was reduced in the mental process, probably largely prenatal in cerebellum, left , and fornices origin. Other brain abnormalities include (McAlonan et al., 2004). Herbert et al. (2003) cortical dysgenesis, as evidenced by increased performed an MRI-based morphometric ana- neuronal density, thickened cortex, presence lysis of the of 17 autistic children to of in the molecular layer, irregular investigate both total brain volume and speci- laminar patterns, and poor gray-white fic brain regions and found whole brain matter boundaries (Bailey et al., 1998), sug- volume was moderately increased. Factor ana- gesting aberration in cortical lamination lysis was then used to subgroup brain regions and apoptosis (Rorke, 1994). basedonintercorrelations. The first grouping In an attempt to localize cerebral dys- showed enlargement of diencephalon, caudate, function in Autism, various functional neuroi- globus pallidus-putamen, cerebellum and maging studies were conducted. Schmitz et al. brainstem areas. Their second grouping sug- (2006) utilized functional magnetic resonance gested possible smaller adjusted volumes in imaging (fMRI) to examine the neural corre- the autistic group in and lates of executive function in ASD. Their find- hippocampus-amygdala. The third grouping, ings indicated that ASD individuals had which included cerebral white matter, showed significantly increased brain activity, as indi- increased volume compared to controls. cated by the blood oxygen level dependent The study by Herbert et al. (2003) demon- (BOLD) response, associated with the left strated the heterogeneity of brain differences inferior and orbital frontal (associated in autism and the difficulty in looking for with motor inhibition), left insula (regulating a specific brain area to be implicated in the interference inhibition), and parietal lobes disorder. It was suggested that there may (required for set shifting). Increased frontal be abnormalities in the pathways and that gray matter density in areas of increased func- pervasive core processing deficits, impaired tional activation was also observed in ASD complex information processing, or weak (Schmitz et al., 2006). In a proton magnetic central coherence in Autism may be asso- resonance spectroscopy study, increased fron- ciated with abnormal white matter. Deficits tal metabolite levels were associated with in cross-modal information processing and obsessional behavior in Asperger Syndrome intrahemispheric as well as corticocortical (Murphy et al., 2002). Anomalies in brain connections may be linked to behavior and metabolites were reported in amygdala- communication impairment in Autism hippocampal regions in ASD (Page et al., (Herbert et al., 2004). Cell minicolumn 2006). Using positron emission tomography anomalies of the cerebral cortex representing (PET), Boddaert et al. (2002) found significant connectivity linking afferent, efferent, and bilateral temporal hypoperfusion in the super- interneuronal connections have been repor- ior temporal gyrus and superior temporal ted in Autism (Casanova, Buxhoeveden, & in 77% of the children with Autism. Brown, 2002). Abnormal neural connectivity Other research utilizing functional neuroima- can affect different levels of processing ging linked social cognition dysfunction and within local networks (Bertone, Mottron, language deficits in Autism to neural substrates Jelenic, & Faubert, 2005). Research utilizing (Just, Cherkassky, Keller, & Minshew, 2004; two-dimensional voxel-based morphometry McAlonan et al., 2005; Pelphrey, Adolphs, & to identify white matter density as an Morris, 2005; Welchew et al., 2005). index of neural connectivity indicated that McAlonan et al. (2005) investigated brain Autism is associated with reduced white structural changes linked to Autism using matter concentration (Chung, Dalton, voxel-based MRI. Their results indicated that Alexander, & Davidson, 2004). Piven, children with Autism had a significant reduc- Bailey, Ranson, & Arndt (1997) examined tion in total gray matter volume, particularly regions of the in autistic within fronto-striatal and parietal networks individuals, finding decreased size in the 164 JOURNAL OF NEUROTHERAPY body and posterior regions but not anterior functional connectivity between various brain subregions of the corpus callosum. regions. Specifically, they found significantly decreased connectivity between the anterior cingulate region with the posterior cingulate NEURAL CONNECTIVITY and , between the left parahippo- DEFICITS IN AUTISM campal gyrus with various other regions, and lower overall left hemisphere connectiv- The aforementioned research and multiple ity. In addition, they found a positive correla- brain regions implicated in Autism provide tion between the size of the rostral body of the support for neural connectivity deficits corpus callosum and functional connectivity in Autism. Frith (2003) suggested that autistic between the left frontal and posterior regions behaviors may be explained by their lack of in Autism. The lack of coordination among ability to integrate information and instead neural centers in Autism is evident in the rest- to focus obsessively on details. She believed ing state as well as during complex tasks and that this was because of a lack of communica- provides support for Autism as a disorder of tion between frontal brain areas that would neural systems rather than a focal disorder typically integrate the information with more (Cherkassky et al., 2006). posterior areas (Wickelgren, 2005). Since that Autism has also been classified as a disorder time, much research has been conducted in of arousal-modulating systems associated with support of this connectivity deficit hypothesis. atypically increased functional connectivity in Research utilizing fMRI has reported a pat- addition to areas of underconnectivity. tern of underconnectivity in Autism (Just, Research utilizing fMRI BOLD signal during Cherkassky, Keller, Kana, & Minshew, simple visuomotor coordination has indicated 2007). A decreased degree of synchronization greater thalamocortical functional connectiv- (the correlation of time series activation) ity in Autism. Excessive connectivity was between frontal and parietal areas of activa- noted in the left insula, right postcentral, and tion was noted during a task of executive func- middle frontal regions. Increased thalamocor- tion. Furthermore, the size of the genu of the tical functional connectivity may be associated corpus callosum was correlated with frontal- with excessive synaptic generation and reduced parietal connectivity. These findings suggest pruning, which may be linked to brain enlarge- that cortical underconnectivity is associated ment in Autism (Mizuno, Villalobos, Davies, with a deficit in the neural and cognitive inte- Dahl,&Muller,2006). gration of information (Just, Cherkassky, Kel- Courchesne and Pierce (2005) described a ler, Kana, & Minshew, 2007). McAlonan et al. pattern of overconnectivity (hyperconnec- (2005) found anomalies in the anatomy (reduc- tivity) within the frontal lobe, with long- tion in total gray matter volume) and connec- distance disconnection (hypoconnectivity) tivity (associated with interregional gray between the frontal lobe and other brain matter correlations) of limbic-striatal social regions associated with ASD. This pattern of brain systems in Autism. Just, Cherkassky, abnormal connectivity disrupts the ability of Keller, & Minshew (2004) found evidence of the frontal lobes to integrate information from underconnectivity associated with reduced emotional, language, sensory, and autonomic cortical activation and synchronization as indi- systems (Courchesne & Pierce, 2005). Because cated by number of voxels activated during an EEG measures electrical activity across the fMRI sentence comprehension task. brain with high temporal resolution, it lends In addition to disturbed connectivity during itself well to the investigation of connectivity cognitive tasks, functional underconnectivity through EEG coherence measurement. was reported in the resting brain state in Autism (Cherkassky, Kana, Keller, & Just, 2006). By comparing the resting-state in an EEG FINDINGS IN AUTISM autistic group to a control group using fMRI, the two groups showed similar levels of activa- Computerized EEG analyses indicated tion. However, the autistic group showed lower that children with Autism have significantly Scientific Articles 165 greater coherence between hemispheres in most of the two cerebral hemispheres and the beta band than mentally handicapped, plays a major role in cerebral connectivity. normal children, or toddlers (Cantor, Several studies have found this pathway to Thatcher, Hrybyk, & Kaye, 1986). They also be aberrant in Autism. Courchesne, Press, had higher coherence in the alpha band than and Yeung-Courchesne (1993) found thin- normal controls and less inter- and intrahe- ning, particularly in the posterior body of mispheric asymmetry than either normal the corpus callosum. Since their original controls or mentally handicapped children. finding, several others have found abnor- These findings would appear to suggest that malities of the corpus callosum including the EEG is a useful and valid means of decreased density in the genu, and splenium measuring connectivity anomalies in this (Chung et al., 2004; Vidal et al., 2006), dis- population. proportionately smaller corpus callosum Murias, Webb, Greenson, and Dawson relative to cerebral volume (Boger-Megiddo (2007) assessed functional connectivity with et al., 2006), and small corpus callosum EEG coherence during an eyes closed resting volumes, high mean diffusivity, low aniso- state. Relative to controls, individuals with tropy, and increased radial diffusivity ASD showed long range alpha band (Alexander et al., 2007). coherence reductions in frontal-occipital Although numerous studies suggest there and fronal-parietal areas. The alpha band is an increase in total brain volume in represents more globally dominant func- Autism, this anomaly does not appear to tions, which are more dependent on cortico- be present at birth. Rather, during the cortical and callosal fibers (Nunez, 1995; first 2 years of life there is overgrowth, Nunez & Srinivasan, 2006). Individuals with followed by a decrease in the normal ASD also showed increased coherence at growth process (Courchesne, 2004; Courch- temporal recording sites between 3 and esne et al., 2001). It was suggested that the 6 Hz, reflecting intact locally dominant corti- reason for this abnormal growth process is cal activity. These findings support the that there may be dysfunction in the normal hypothesis of a weak connection between pruning process (Frith, 2003). By dividing frontal and other areas. cerebral white matter with a white matter Coben, Clarke, Hudspeth, and Barry parcellation technique, Herbert et al. (2008) using quantitative EEG (QEEG) (2004) found that the increase in white mat- found that autistic children showed decrea- ter was in the radiate (outer) zones of all sed intrahemispheric coherences across cerebral lobes and longer myelinating short-medium as well as long interelec- regions. In contrast, inner zone white mat- trode distances within delta and theta ter volumes showed no difference compared bands. In addition, there were reduced to a control group. Because deeper myelina- interhemispheric coherences in the alpha tion occurs earlier on, the authors inter- band in temporal regions, and reduced preted this finding as supporting a interhemispheric coherences in beta in cen- postnatal disturbance which disrupts pri- tral, parietal, and occipital regions. marily intrahemispheric and coritco-cortical Greater relative theta was especially preva- connections. lent in the right posterior region, while In a review of neuropathological find- lower beta was noted across the right ings in Autism, Herbert (2005) indicated hemisphere, especially over the right fron- that neuroinflammation is present in Aut- tal region. ism and contributes to the increased cranial volume. The overall increase in volume may result in dysfunction of the ability to integrate information between different CONNECTIVITY THEORY parts of the brain. Herbert further specu- lated that disconnectivity may result in spe- The most robust white matter fiber tract cific dysfunction, not just pervasive, in the brain, the corpus callosum, connects nonspecific deficits. Therefore, domains 166 JOURNAL OF NEUROTHERAPY most likely to be affected by the inflamma- CONNECTIVITY PATTERNS tory response are those which require more IN AUTISM coordination and communication between brain areas, such as language and executive Evidence from the research studies just functioning. discussed and data from recent EEG coher- Courchesne, Redcay, Morgan, and ence studies indicate that there may be as Kennedy (2005) provide a detailed expla- many as seven abnormal connectivity pat- nation of the disruption of connectivity in terns in Autism. These connectivity patterns the autistic brain. There are an excess num- were determined based on research studies ber of neurons in the frontal cortex but not we have conducted as well as clinical obser- in lower=basic level systems. Excess frontal vation and experience. QEEG analysis cortical neurons may produce excessive con- measures functional connectivity between nectivity, as neurons that are excited near different areas of the brain based on phase one another will tend to connect to each synchrony, or the degree to which two sig- other. Nonfrontal cortical connectivity, on nals maintain a phase locked relationship the other , would be diminished over time (Walker, Norman, & Weber, because of a lack of coherent bursts of 2002). Another, more pure view of coherence activity. The result is a hyperconnected is defined by Otnes and Enochson (1972) as frontal cortex ‘‘disconnected’’ from the rest the squared cross-correlation between two of the brain (Courchesne & Pierce, 2005). waveforms within a given frequency band This local hyperconnectivity within fron- that has been normalized for amplitude. tal cortex is disorganized, however, pre- Consistent with existing research showing venting normal frontal lobe functioning. disordered connectivity, coherence anoma- Normally, frontal cortex is responsible for lies appear to be the primary dysfunction in integrating and directing different brain Autism. Treatment goals may be based on areas. Clearly, this ability is critically depen- coherence anomalies identified by QEEG dent on intact connections with other brain analysis. Based on specific patterns of regions, and is shown to be severely dis- increased or decreased coherence identified rupted in autism. neurofeedback can be used to return these The connectivity theory of autism has patterns to normal or near normal levels. become an empirically supported theory Neurofeedback is typically based on an describing the neurobiological basis of operant conditioning model, in which a Autism, with evidence suggesting that it is computer analyzed signal is fed back to the an overgrowth of white matter during the trainee in real time along with a more first 2 years of life, followed by a retardation easily understood signal on a computer of growth thereafter, which leads to dis- monitor, which can be reinforced (Walker ordered connectivity (Hughes, 2007). At & Kozlowski, 2005). Increased coherence least two critical issues result from the between brain regions may be downtrained, aforementioned findings. First, through whereas decreased coherence between brain scientific investigation, we must learn how regions may be uptrained. Studies have to prevent these problems from taking shown that in addition to changing coher- place. Second, we must improve the evalua- ence as assessed by EEG, coherence training tion and treatment of connectivity distur- produces behavioral and functional bances after they occur. The EEG appears improvements (Walker & Kozlowski, 2005; to be good candidate for the evaluation of Walker et al., 2002). We propose and pro- neural connectivity in Autism, based on vide evidence that by identifying the specific coherence analyses. Specifically, we propose coherence anomaly in Autism, neurofeed- that EEG neurofeedback can be utilized back can be used to remedy the aberrant to remedy aberrant coherence patterns. connectivity pattern, resulting in cognitive Next we discuss connectivity evaluations and behavioral improvements as well. In and how neurofeedback has been used to sum, coherence anomalies may be the treat them. primary dysfunction in autism, EEG can Scientific Articles 167 identify this, and neurofeedback can remedy anterior-temporal regions. Their use of a it, at least in part. bipolar montage permitted the neurofeed- The following images are shown as back to reinforce interhemispheric communi- exemplars of the connectivity abnormal- cation while reducing hyperconnectivity ities that we have identified. These within and across brain regions. Reward figures utilize the NRep analysis system bands were established based on the regions (Hudspeth, 1999). The value, use, and inter- and frequencies of maximal hyperconnectiv- pretation of these images are discussed in ity. The hypothesis underlying this approach detail in Hudspeth’s (2008) article in the was that as the two waveforms at these loca- upcoming companion volume on connectiv- tions become more disparate (i.e., reward ity. The interested reader can refer to this band increases its amplitude), the coherence article as an aide to interpreting the images. (or similarity) between the sites should decrease. QEEG analysis indicated that 77% of the experimental group showed a Frontotemporal Hyperconnectivity decrease in hyperconnectivity (see Figure 1c as an example of reduced connectivity fol- Figures 1a and 1b show an autistic indivi- lowing neurofeedback). The reduction of dual who displayed a pattern of bilateral hyperconnectivity patterns in the experimen- frontotemporal interhemispheric hypercon- tal group was statistically significant; in nectivity. Evidence in support of this pattern addition, the reduction in cerebral hypercon- comes from white matter functional aniso- nectivity was associated with positive clinical tropy investigated through Diffusion Tensor outcomes as indicated by reduced scores on Imaging (Barnea-Goraly et al., 2004), the Autism Treatment Evaluation Checklist increases in cerebral white matter volume in (Rimland & Edelson, 2000), Gilliam frontal cortex (Herbert et al., 2004), abnor- Asperger’s Disorder Scale (Gilliam, 2001), mally small minicolumns in frontal areas Behavior Rating in Executive Functioning (Buxhoeveden, Semendeferi, Schenker, & (Gioia, Isquith, Guy, & Kenworthy, 2000), Courchesne, 2004), abnormally long and and Personality Inventory for Children–2 thin dendritic spines present in high density (Lachar & Gruber, 2001). Improved ratings (Belmonte et al., 2004), and numerous stu- of ASD symptoms reflected an 89% success dies showing behavioral and neuropsycholo- rate, and there was an average 40% reduc- gical (executive) dysfunction (Courchesne & tion in core ASD symptomatology. The cru- Pierce, 2005), all of which are suggestive of cial factor in explaining improved clinical frontal dysfunction through overconnectiv- outcomes in the experimental group may be ity. In an fMRI study, Mizuno et al. (2006) the use of assessment-guided neurofeedback found more extensive connectivity in the to reduce cerebral hyperconnectivity. left insula and right postcentral and middle Reducing interhemispheric bilateral frontal regions in comparison to control hyperconnectivity in autistic children can participants. lead to improvements in many realms. This Coben and Padolsky (2007) performed the often includes, but it is not limited to, largest neurofeedback study demonstrating enhancements of attention, self-regulatory efficacy for Autism. Their individualized functions, social behavior, and communica- treatment protocol was based on the tion skills. combined use of multiple assessment instru- ments (neuropsychological testing, rating scales, etc.) with a heavy emphasis on initial Frontal (Orbitofrontal) Hypoconnectivity QEEG, which included analysis of absolute, relative power, and connectivity mea- Figure 2a shows an example of an autistic sures. Assessment-guided neurofeedback individual with frontal (orbitofrontal) hypo- was conducted in 20 sessions with a focus connectivity, an area implicated in many on reducing hyperconnectivity, which was aspects of social behavior. Reasoning, the frequently observed in posterior frontal to ability to represent the mental states of 168 JOURNAL OF NEUROTHERAPY

FIGURE 1. (a) Case example of an Autistic individual showing horizontal Eigenimages representing multivari- ate connectivity in Delta, Theta, Alpha, and Beta ranges, with frontal hyperconnectivity in the Alpha band. (b) Case example of an Autistic individual showing norm referenced multivariate connectivity z -score images with frontal hyperconnectivity in the Alpha band. Top row represents intrahemispheric coherence; bottom row represents interhemispheric connectivity. Greatest connectivity is shown interhemispherically. (c) Case exam- ple of an Autistic individual showing change scores in the measurement of multivariate connectivity, indicating a decrease in connectivity after treatment. Top row represents intrahemispheric connectivity; bottom row represents interhemispheric connectivity.

others, plays a crucial role in social abilities mind abilities, may be impaired following and has been established as a core deficit in amygdala and other orbitofrontal damage Autism (Baron-Cohen, 2001). Neuropsy- (Hornak, Rolls, & Wade, 1996). In a PET chological evidence suggests that emotion study, temporal hypoperfusion in the super- recognition, thought to tap into theory of ior temporal gyrus and superior temporal Scientific Articles 169

FIGURE 2. (a) Case example of an Autistic individual showing horizontal Eigenimages representing multivariate connectivity in Delta, Theta, Alpha, and Beta, with (orbitofrontal) hypoconnectivity in the Alpha band. Longer lines indicate less connectivity. (b) Averaged group data of Autistic individuals showing change scores in the measure- ment of multivariate connectivity, indicating an increase in connectivity after treatment with Passive Infrared Hemoencephalogrpahy (PIR HEG). Top row represents intrahemispheric connectivity; bottom row represents interhemispheric coherence. Greatest connectivity is shown intra- and interhemispherically.

sulcus was found in 77% of autistic children EEG, and infrared [IR] imaging) near IR (Boddaert et al., 2002). Abnormalities have (NIR) or passive IR (PIR) HEG for at least also been found in the functional integration 20 sessions, conducted twice weekly, for a of the amygdala and duration of 15 to 25 min. A threshold-based (Welchew et al., 2005). Sabbagh and Taylor control over a DVD player provided operant (2000) performed an event-related potential reinforcement of targeted brain activity. The study and identified and objective was to increase the NIR=PIR HEG medial temporal cortex as strongly related signal to exceed the threshold setting for the to theory of mind decoding, based on the movie to continue or resume playing. Maxi- low-resolution electromagnetic tomography mum output of IR radiation was associated technique. Because this ability is known to with mental states of low anxiety and be impaired in Autism, the authors conclude frustration while maintaining a high degree that this brain area is likely implicated in the of sustained concentration. It is believed that disorder and may provide clues for effective prefrontal PIR HEG training tends to sup- therapeutic strategies. press dysregulated activity throughout the Coben (2006) investigated the effects of brain, not just frontally (Carmen, 2004). hemoencephalography (HEG) neurofeed- Results indicated significant and dramatic back on Autism symptoms. In this study, improvements in multiple areas, including 28 autistic patients were provided assessment social interaction, social communication, and guided (based on neuropsychological testing, cognitive impairments as rated by the Autism 170 JOURNAL OF NEUROTHERAPY

Treatment Evaluation Checklist. These func- FIGURE 3. (a) Case example of a horizontal image tional improvements accompanied significant displaying mu rhythm complex in an individual with changes between pre- and post-EEG (see Autism. (b) Mu Rhythm Coherence Training showing Figure 2b) and IR imaging sessions, which an increase in connectivity and a corresponding indicatedanincreaseinfrontal (orbitofrontal) decrease in Mu. connectivity. Coben’s results indicate that remediating hypoconnectivity in orbitofrontal areas may result in social and behavioral imp- rovements. This result is consistents with dys- function in these areas in autism and neuropsychological studies implicating these brain areas with social behavior.

Mu Rhythm Complex

Mirror neurons were first discovered by Rizzolatti, Fadiga, Gallese, and Fogassi (1996) in the (area F5, or Broca’s area in humans) of macaque mon- keys with single unit electrode recordings. These are neurons that fire in response to the observation of an action by a conspecific (an organism belonging to the same species as another) that an individual would nor- mally perform. This mirror system (MNS) consists of an observation=execution cution system, which seems to be critical in the ability to understand and imitate other’s behavior and has been implicated in several of the cognitive functions known to be impaired in Autism, including , mu is a sign of dysfunction in the MNS, as it theory of mind, language, and empathy should be suppressed. In this figure, a large (Oberman et al., 2005). Oberman and collea- area of hypocoherence can be seen between gues recently found evidence of a dysfunc- central and frontal sites. The displacement tional MNS in high functioning individuals of electrodes from where they normally with ASD. EEG oscillations in the mu would be causes some areas to be bunched frequency (8–13Hz) over the sensorimotor up (hypercoherent) and others to be spread cortex were associated with deficient mirror out (hypocoherent). Impairment in the neuron activity. Mu power is reduced MNS may contribute to social deficits in or suppressed in individuals with typical understanding and appropriately respond- development when they perform movements ing to other’s behavior evident in high- or observe the actions of others. It was found functioning individuals with ASD (Oberman that high-functioning individuals with ASD et al., 2005). failed to suppress mu wave activity of the Because mu rhythm is generated by MNS while observing hand movement. In sensorimotor cortex and is suppressed when contrast, controls were able to suppress mu performing or observing an action, the dis- wave activity while observing as well as turbance of mu suppression in Autism is imitating hand movement. likely the result of a deficiency in coherence Findings in Figure 3a show the mu between central and frontal areas. This the- rhythm complex (coherence patterns) as seen ory is also consistent with all the evidence in an individual with autism. The presence of suggesting deficient connectivity between Scientific Articles 171 frontal and other areas, as described Ouston, & Lee, 1988) and that they tend to previously. If autistic individuals do not focus on facial features more than the gestalt decrease mu while engaging in social activ- of a face (Weeks & Hobson, 1987). Autistic ities, they likely will not learn how to interact patients do not appear to process faces in effectively and in a way that facilitates the same way as nonautistic individuals, healthy relationships. Coherence training, suggesting there may be abnormal brain in which brain waves of specific frequency pathways involved. In fact, Pierce, Mu¨ller, are trained to peak and trough at the same Ambrose, Allen, and Courchesne (2001) time, may allow for the suppression of demonstrated with fMRI that they do not mu. Coben, Hudspeth, Sherlin, Myers, and use the same brain regions as a group of Kurowski (2008) examined connectivity and normal controls when viewing faces. source localization of amplitude=power Furthermore, whereas the control group changes related to mu oscillations in autistic showed maximum activation in the expected children. The findings showed mu to involve fusiform area, those in the autistic group a system localized to temporo-limbic and showed a pattern of ‘‘individual-specific, frontal structures with hypoconnectivity scattered activation,’’ suggesting that faulty between frontal and temporal=central sites. connections in this area preclude these As a result, they theorized that increasing individuals from using traditional areas coherence between these regions might red- used in face processing. Instead, of using uce mu oscillations. To test this hypothesis, traditional areas (fusiform), autistic indivi- Coben and Hudspeth (2006) provided EEG duals use idiosyncratic patterns of activa- coherence training to autistic individuals by tion which allow them to process the rewarding the coherence between central information. electrode sites with those of the lateral In addition, McPartland, Dawson, Webb, frontal region, where mirror neurons are Panagiotides, and Carver (2004) conduc- located around the . ted an event-related potential study to Their results indicated significant reduc- investigate the temporal dynamics of face tions in mu activity (see Figure 3b for a processing in Autism. In their study the sample of reduced mu activity) and autistic group showed longer latencies improvements in social skills and social to faces but not to nonface objects and pragmatics. These reductions are shown as failed to show the expected longer N170 percentage increases in coherence. Areas latency to inverted faces relative to upright with the largest change scores in area 3b faces. The fact that the autistic group pro- correspond to area 3a which were shown cesses inverted faces more similarly to to be hypocoherent. upright faces than the control group can be taken to indicate that autistic indivi- duals do not process faces the same as Right Posterior (Occipital-Parietal- nonautistics. In this specific type of mea- Temporal) Hypoconnectivity surement (N170 latency), there was no difference between viewing faces upright or Coben et al. (2008) investigated quantita- inverted for autistics. tive EEG findings in Autism during an One patient’s EEG data showing right eyes-closed resting condition. Findings posterior hypoconnectivity is displayed in showed excessive slow wave (theta) activity Figure 4a. Change scores in multivariate over right posterior regions in addition to connectivity are depicted in Figure 4b, which underconnectivity across delta, theta, and indicate that following coherence training, beta bands. Further evidence for posterior connectivity increased in posterior regions. abnormal connectivity comes from studies With increased connectivity in areas which of face processing. Behavioral studies have were previously under connected, there will shown that autistic patients do not show be increased communication and transfer the typically seen advantage of viewing faces of information. Coherence training between upright as opposed to inverted (Hobson, occipital-temporal and medial temporal 172 JOURNAL OF NEUROTHERAPY

FIGURE 4. (a) Case example of an Autistic individual showing horizontal Neuroelectric Eigenimages represent- ing multivariate connectivity in Delta, Theta, Alpha, Beta, and Total with right posterior hypoconnectivity. Longer lines indicate less connectivity. (b) Case example of an Autistic individual showing change scores in the mea- surement of multivariate connectivity, indicating an increase in right posterior connectivity after treatment. Top row represents intrahemispheric connectivity; bottom row represents interhemispheric connectivity.

areas on the right side increased coherence found reduced intra- and interhemispheric between these areas, which correspond to connectivity between frontal and parietal the areas of facial processing. Improvements areas. A recent functional connectivity mag- following such coherence training often netic resonance imaging study (fcMRI) include enhanced social pragmatics, interac- investigated the integrity of the dorsal stream tion skills, visual-perceptual=facial=prosodic connection of visual information between processing skills. There are often associated occipital and posterior=superior parietal cor- improvements in attention and academic tex (Villalobos, Mizuno, Dahl, Kemmotsu, functioning as well. and Mu¨ller, 2005). However, they also used this pathway to examine the involvement of frontal regions possibly containing mirror Frontal–Posterior Hypoconnectivity neurons, as evidence suggests that the dorsal stream also connects to frontal areas As previously noted, cortical undercon- (Goodale, 2000). In an fMRI study of autis- nectivity between frontal and parietal areas tic men, Villalobos et al. (2005) found that, was observed with fMRI during tasks of whereas functional connectivity between executive functioning (Just et al., 2007). primary and superior parietal Horowitz, Rumsey, Grady, and Rapoport regions was intact, connectivity between (1988) used PET during a resting state primary visual cortex and inferior frontal to examine connectivity through glucose cortex (BA 44 and 45 areas presumed to metabolism correlations in Autism and contain mirror neurons) was significantly Scientific Articles 173 reduced. The authors interpreted this finding between frontal and posterior regions, parti- as indicating a deficient connection between cularly in the Delta and Theta bands, follow- frontal areas containing mirror neurons ing coherence training. and visual cortical areas, consistent with pre- vious findings of dysfunction Left Hemisphere Hypoconnectivity in Autism. Belmonte et al. (2004) reported preliminary EEG data on decreased and=or In a baseline-resting state fMRI study, delayed gamma activity over the frontal cor- Cherkassky et al. (2006) found that tex and visual processing areas, which they connectivity within the left hemisphere was took to suggest disrupted neural signaling significantly lower in an autistic group rela- and abnormal regional activation patterns. tive to a group of control subjects. As Recent EEG data collected on an autistic described previously, Just et al. (2004) mea- patient (see Figure 5a) serve as an example sured functional connectivity with fMRI of Belmonte’s findings and further support during a sentence comprehension task. They the frontal-posterior hypoconnectivity pat- found that connectivity was lower through- tern seen in Autism. This pattern of hypo- out left hemisphere language areas relative connectivity can be reduced and coherence to controls, indicating reduced synchroniza- enhanced through coherence training. tion and poor integration of information at Figure 5b shows this increase in connectivity more complex levels of processing, as

FIGURE 5. (a) Case example of an Autistic individual showing horizontal Neuroelectric Eigenimages repre- senting multivariate connectivity in Delta, Theta, Alpha, Beta, and Total with frontal-posterior hypoconnectivity. Longer lines indicate less connectivity. (b) Case example of an Autistic individual showing change scores in the measurement of multivariate connectivity, indicating an increase in frontal–posterior connectivity after treatment. Top row represents intrahemispheric connectivity; bottom row represents interhemispheric connectivity. 174 JOURNAL OF NEUROTHERAPY

FIGURE 6. (a) Case example of an Autistic individual showing horizontal Neuroelectric Eigenimages represent- ing multivariate connectivity in Delta, Theta, Alpha, Beta, and Total with left hemisphere hypoconnectivity. Longer lines indicate less connectivity. (b) Case example of an Autistic individual showing change scores in the measurement of multivariate connectivity, indicating an increase in left hemisphere coherence after treat- ment. Top row represents intrahemispheric connectivity; bottom row represents interhemispheric connectivity.

opposed to single word decoding which also been associated with a pattern of left showed intact functioning. Figure 6a hemisphere hyperconnectivity. Murias et al. shows evidence of left hemisphere hypocon- (2007) examined EEG coherence during a nectivity in an autistic individual. Decreased resting state in male adults with Autism. connectivity in the left hemisphere is indi- They found increased coherence in the delta cated by the long lines between electrode range of frontal and temporal regions of the sites of the left hemisphere. However, follow- left hemisphere in comparison to nonautistic ing coherence training, change scores in individuals, suggesting local overconnectiv- Figure 6b show a clear increase in connectiv- ity. Although there may be limited evidence ity in the Delta band, and particularly in the from neuroimaging studies regarding hyper- Theta band. connectivity in the left hemisphere in Autism, the language deficits apparent in almost all autistics suggests that the left Left Hemisphere Intrahemispheric hemisphere may be dysfunctionally con- Hyperconnectivity nected. There is EEG evidence to suggest that autistics show atypical patterns of In contrast to the previous pattern of left cerebral lateralization, in which the right hemisphere hypoconnectivity, Autism has hemisphere may be dominant for both Scientific Articles 175

FIGURE 7. (a) Case example of an Autistic individual showing horizontal Neuroelectric Eigenimages repre- senting multivariate connectivity in Delta, Theta, Alpha, Beta, and Total left hemisphere intrahemispheric hyperconnectivity. Longer lines indicate less connectivity. (b) Case example of an Autistic individual showing change scores in the measurement of multivariate connectivity, indicating a decrease in left hemisphere intra- hemispheric connectivity after treatment. Top row represents intrahemispheric connectivity; bottom row repre- sents interhemispheric connectivity.

language and spatial functions (Dawson, it difficult to pinpoint a specific cause of Warrenburg, & Fuller, 1982), further impli- the behavioral manifestation of the disorder. cating abnormal left hemisphere function- The connectivity theory of Autism presents a ing. As has been discussed throughout this new and empirically supported view of the article, Autism is associated with both cause of dysfunction seen. There are abnor- under- and overconnectivity. Either pattern malities in major white matter tracts that is abnormal so we would expect these indi- form the connections between neural regions viduals to express this dysfunction behavio- (Chung et al., 2004, Herbert et al., 2004), rally. Figure 7a shows this pattern of left particularly in the corpus callosum intrahemispheric hyperconnectivity in an (Cherkassky et al., 2006; Courchesne et al., autistic individual. Figure 7b shows a 1993; Just et al., 2007). Functional imag- clear decrease in connectivity in the left ing demonstrated abnormal connectivity hemisphere following coherence training. patterns (Cherkassky et al., 2006; Mizuno Treatment of coherence anomalies over et al., 2006) and recent studies have used the left often translates EEG coherence to show abnormal connec- into improvements in communication and tivity patterns (Coben et al., 2008; Coben & academic skills. As a result, improvements Hudspeth, 2006; Murias et al., 2007). We in other realms can be evident as well. provide evidence for seven different aberrant connectivity patterns from existing research and clinical case examples and the value of DISCUSSION EEG coherence in both the assessment and treatment of these patterns. These anoma- Autism has been associated with numer- lous connectivity patterns form a beginning ous neuroanatomical abnormalities, making typology of the phenotypic expression of 176 JOURNAL OF NEUROTHERAPY underlying autistic brain dysfunction. Boddaert, N., Chabane, N., Barthelemy, C., Perhaps the most prominent pattern is that Bourgeois, M., Poline, J. B., Brunelle, F., et al. of frontal hyperconnectivity, in which fron- (2002). Bitemporal lobe dysfunction in infantile tal areas are overconnected with themselves, autism: Positron emission tomography study. as proposed by Courchesne and Pierce Journal of Radiology, 83, 1829–1833. Boger-Megiddo, I., Shaw, D. W., Friedman, S. D., (2005). This pattern leads to a lack of com- Sparks, B. F., Artru, A. A., Giedd, J. N., et al. munication between frontal areas because (2006). Corpus callosum morphometrics in young existing connections are faulty. In addition, children with disorder. Journal frontal areas are underconnected with other of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36(6), brain areas, rendering them unable to per- 733–739. form their normal integrative functions in Buxhoeveden, D., Semendeferi, K., Schenker, N., & directing other brain regions. Treatment Courchesne, E. (2004). Decreased cell column goals may be based on coherence anomalies spacing in autism. Society for Neuroscience identified by QEEG analysis. By establishing Abstracts, 30. reward bands between electrode sites, Cantor, D. S., Thatcher, R. W., Hrybyk, M., & Kaye, increased coherence between brain regions H. (1986). Computerized EEG analyses of autistic children. Journal of Autism and Developmental may be downtrained while decreased Disorders, 16(2), 169–187. coherence between brain regions may be Carmen, J. A. (2004). Passive infrared hemoencephalo- uptrained. graphy: Four years and 100 migraines. Journal of Neurotherapy, 8(3), 23–51. Casanova, M. F., Buxhoeveden, D. P., & Brown, C. REFERENCES (2002). Clinical and macroscopic correlates of mini- columnar pathology in autism. Journal of Child Alexander, A. L., Lee, J. E., Lazar, M., Boudos, R., Neurology, 17(9), 692–695. DuBray, M. B., Oakes, T. R., et al. (2007). Diffu- Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. (2006). sion tensor imaging of the corpus callosum in How common are autistic spectrum disorders autism. NeuroImage, 34, 61–73. (ASD)? Atlanta, GA: Author. Retrieved December American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic 1, 2006, from http://www.cdc.gov/ncbddd/ and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed., autism/asd_common.htm text rev.). Washington, DC: Author. Cherkassky, V. L., Kana, R. K., Keller, T. A., & Just, Attwood, T. (1998) Asperger’s syndrome: A guide for M. A. (2006). Functional connectivity in a baseline parents and professionals. London: Jessica Kingsley. resting-state network in autism. NeuroReport, Bailey, A., Luthert, P., & Bolton, P. (1993). Autism 17(16), 1687–1690. and megalencephaly. Lancet, 341, 1225–1226. Chung, M. K., Dalton, K. M., Alexander, A. L., & Bailey, A., Luthert, P., Dean, A., Harding, B., Janota, Davidson, R. J. (2004). Less white matter concen- I., Montgomery, M., et al. (1998). A clinicopatholo- tration in Autism: 2D voxel-based morphometry. gical study of autism. Brain, 121, 889–905. NeuroImage, 23(1), 242–251. Barnea–Goraly, N., Kwon, H., Menon, V., Eliez, S., Coben, R. (2006, September). Hemoencephalography Lotspeich, L., & Reiss, A. L. (2004). White matter for Autistic Spectrum Disorder. Paper presented at structure in autism: Preliminary evidence from the 14th Annual Conference of the International diffusion tensor imaging. Biological Psychiatry, Society for Neuronal Regulation, Atlanta, GA. 55, 323–326. Coben, R., Clarke, A. R., Hudspeth, W. & Barry, R. J. Baron-Cohen, S. (2001). Theory of mind and autism: (2008). EEG power and coherence in Autistic A review. International Review of Research in Spectrum Disorder. Clinical Neurophysiology, 119, Mental Retardation, 23, 169–184. 1002–1009. Belmonte, M. K., Allen, G., Beckel-Mitchener, A., Coben, R., & Hudspeth, W. (2006, September). Boulanger, L. M., Carper, R. A., & Webb, S. Mu-like rhythms in autistic spectrum disorder: EEG (2004). Autism and abnormal development of analyses and neurofeedback outcome. Paper presented brain connectivity. Journal of Neuroscience, 2442, at the 14th Annual Conference for the International 9228–9231. Society for Neuronal Regulation, Atlanta, GA. Bertone, A., Mottron, L., Jelenic, P., & Faubert, J. Coben, R., Hudspeth, W. J., Sherlin, L., Myers, T. & (2005). Enhanced and diminished visuo-spatial Kurowski, D. (2008). Mu oscillations in autistic chil- information processing in autism depends on stimu- dren: Source localization and connectivity analyses. lus complexity. Brain, 128(10), 2430–2441. Manuscript in preparation. Scientific Articles 177

Coben, R. & Padolsky, I. (2007). Assessment-guided Goodale, M. A. (2000). Occipitotemporal and occipi- neurofeedback for autistic spectrum disorder. toparietal visual pathways in the primate brain. Journal of Neurotherapy, 11(1), 5–23. Advances in Neurology, 84, 75–86. Courchesne, E. (2004). Brain development in autism: Hamilton, L. (2000) Facing autism: Giving parents rea- Early overgrowth followed by premature arrest of sons for hope and guidance for help. Colorado growth. Mental Retardation and Developmental Dis- Springs, CO: WaterBrook Press. abilities Research Reviews, 10, 106–111. Herbert, M. R. (2005). Large brains in Autism: The Courchesne, E., Carper, R., & Akshoomoff, N. (2003). challenge of pervasive abnormality. The Neuro- Evidence of brain overgrowth in the first year of scientist, 11(5), 417–440. life in autism. Journal of the American Medical Herbert, M. R., Ziegler, D. A., Deutsch, C. K., Association, 290, 337–344. O’Brien, L. M., Lange, N., Bakardjiev, A., et al. Courchesne, E., Karns, C. M., Davis, H. R., Ziccardi, (2003). Dissociations of cerebral cortex, subcortical R., Carper, R. A., Tigue, Z. D., et al. (2001). Unu- and cerebral white matter volumes in autistic boys. sual brain growth patterns in early life in patients Brain, 126, 1182–1192. with autistic disorder: An MRI study. Neurology, Herbert, M. R., Ziegler, D. A., Makris, N., Filipek, 57, 245–254. P. A., Kemper, T. L., Normandin, J. J., et al. Courchesne, E., Muller, R. A., & Saitoh, O. (1999). (2004). Localization of white matter volume Brain weight in autism: Normal in the majority of increase in autism and developmental language cases, megalencephalic in rare cases. Neurology, disorder. Annals of Neurology, 55(4), 530–540. 52, 1057–1059. Hobson, R. P., Ouston, J., & Lee, A. (1988). What’s Courchesne, E. & Pierce, K. (2005). Why the frontal in a face? The case of autism. British Journal of cortex in autism might be talking only to itself: Psychology, 79, 441–453. Local over-connectivity but long-distance dis- Hornak, J., Rolls, E. T., & Wade, D. (1996). Face and connection. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 15, voice expression identification in patients with 225–230. emotional and behavioural changes following Courchesne, E., Press, G. A., & Yeung-Courchesne, R. ventral frontal lobe damage. Neuropsychologia, 34, (1993). abnormalities detected with 247–261. MR in patients with infantile autism. American Horowitz, B., Rumsey, J., Grady, C., & Rapoport, S. Journal of Roentgenology, 160, 387–393. (1988). The cerebral metabolic landscape in autism. Courchesne, E., Redcay, E., Morgan, J. T., & Archives of Neurology, 45, 749–755. Kennedy, D. P. (2005). Autism at the beginning: Hudspeth, W. J. (1999). NeuroRep: The QEEG Microstructural and growth abnormalities under- analysis and report system. Los Osos, CA: lying the cognitive and behavioral phenotype of Neuropsychometrix. autism. Development and Psychopathology, 17, Hudspeth, W. J. (2008). Multivariate connectivities 577–597. within a spherical brain space. Journal of Neuro- Davidovitch, M., Patterson, B., & Gartside, P. (1996). therapy (forthcoming). Head circumference measurements in children with Hughes, J. R. (2007). Autism: The first firm finding ¼ autism. Journal of Child Neurology, 11, 389–393. underconnectivity? and Behavior, 11(1), Dawson, G., Warrenburg, S., & Fuller, P. (1982). 20–24. Cerebral lateralization in individuals diagnosed as Just, M. A., Cherkassky, V. L., Keller, T. A., Kana, Autistic in early childhood. Brain and Language, R. K., & Minshew, N. J. (2007). Functional and 15, 353–368. anatomical cortical underconnectivity in autism: Filipek, P. A., Richelme, C., Kennedy, D. N., Evidence from an fMRI study of an executive Rademacher, J., Pitcher, D. A., Zidel, S., et al. function task and corpus callosum morphometry. (1992). Morphometric analysis of the brain in Cerebral Cortex, 17(4), 951–961. developmental language disorders and autism. Just, M. A., Cherkassky, V. L., Keller, T. A., & Annals of Neurology, 32, 475. Minshew, N. J. (2004). Cortical activation and Frith, C. (2003). What do imaging studies tell us about synchronization during sentence comprehension in the neural basis of autism?. Novartis Foundation high-functioning autism: Evidence of under- Symposium, 251, 149–166. connectivity. Brain, 127(8), 1811–1821. Gilliam, J. E. (2001). Gilliam asperger’s disorder scale Kanner, L. (1943). Autistic disturbances of affective examiner’s manual. Austin, Texas: Pro-Ed. contact. Nervous Child, 2, 217–307. Gioia, G. A., Isquith, P. K., Guy, S. C., & Kenworthy, Kemper, T. L. & Bauman, M. L. (1993). The L. (2000). Behavior rating inventory of executive contribution of neuropathologic studies to the function. Lutz, FL: Psychological Assessment understanding of autism. Behavioral Neurology, Resources. 11, 175–187. 178 JOURNAL OF NEUROTHERAPY

Kemper, T. L. & Bauman, M. (1998). Neuropathology Pierce, K., Mu¨ller, R.-A., Ambrose, J., Allen, G., & of infantile autism. Journal of Neuropathology and Courchesne, E. (2001). Face processing occurs out- Experimental Neurology, 57, 645–652. side the fusiform ‘‘face area’’ in autism: Evidence Lachar, D. & Gruber, C. P. (2001) Personality inven- from fMRI. Brain, 124, 2059–2073. tory for children, second edition. Los Angeles, CA: Piven, J., Arndt, S., Bailey, J., & Andreasen, N. (1996). Western Psychological Services. Regional brain enlargement in autism: A magnetic McAlonan, G. M., Cheung, V., Cheung, C., Suckling, resonance imaging study. Journal of the American J., Lam, G. Y., Tai, K. S., et al. (2005). Mapping Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 35, the brain in autism: A voxel-based MRI study of 530–536. volumetric differences and intercorrelations in aut- Piven, J., Bailey, J., Ranson, B. J., & Arndt, S. (1997). ism. Brain, 128(2), 268–276. An MRI study of the corpus callosum in McCandless, J. (2005) Children with starving brains: A autism. American Journal of Psychiatry, 154, medical treatment guide for autism spectrum disor- 1051–1056. der. Putney, VT: Bramble Books. Rimland, B. & Edelson, S. M. (2000). Autism treatment McPartland, J., Dawson, G., Webb, S. J., evaluation checklist (ATEC). Retrieved October 23, Panagiotides, H., & Carver, L. J. (2004). Event- 2007, from http://www.autismeval.com/ari-atec/ related brain potentials reveal anomalies in report1.html temporal processing of faces in autism spectrum Rizzolatti, G., Fadiga, L., Gallese, V., & Fogassi, L. disorder. Journal of Child Psychology and Psy- (1996). Premotor cortex and the recognition of chiatry, 45(7), 1235–1245. motor actions. Cognitive Brain Research, 3, Mizuno, A., Villalobos, M. E., Davies, M. M., Dahl, 131–141. B. C., & Muller, R. A. (2006). Partially enhanced Rorke, L. B. (1994). A perspective: the role of dis- thalamocortical functional connectivity in autism. ordered genetic control of neurogenesis in the Brain Research, 1104(1), 160–174. pathogenesis of migration disorders. Journal of Murias, M., Webb, S. J., Greenson, J., & Dawson, G. Neuropathology and Experimental Neurology, 53, (2007). Resting state cortical connectivity reflected 105–117. in EEG Coherence in individuals with Autism. Sabbagh, M. A. & Taylor, M. (2000). Neural corre- Biological Psychiatry, 62(3), 270–273. lates of theory of mind reasoning in adults: An Murphy, D. G. M., Critchley, H. D., Schmitz, N., event-related potential study. Psychological Science, McAlonan, G., Van Amelsvoort, T., Robertson, 11, 46–50. D., et al. (2002). A Proton magnetic resonance spec- Schmitz, N., Rubia, K., Daly, E., Smith, A., Williams, troscopy study of the brain. Archives of General S., & Murphy, D. G. (2006). Neural correlates of Psychiatry, 59(10), 885–891. executive function in autistic spectrum disorders. Nunez, P. L. (1995) Neocortical dynamics and human Biological Psychiatry, 59(1), 7–16. EEG rhythms. New York: Oxford University Press. Sicile-Kira, C. (2004) Autism spectrum disorders: The Nunez, P. L. & Srinivasan, R. (2006). Electric fields of complete guide to understanding autism, asperger’s the brain: The neurophysics of EEG (2nd ed.). New syndrome, pervasive developmental disorder, and York: Oxford University Press. ASDs. New York: Berkley. Oberman, L. M., Hubbard, E. M., McCleery, J. P., Siegel, B. (1996). The world of the autistic child: Under- Altschuler, E. L., Ramachandran, V. S., & Pineda, standing and treating autistic spectrum disorders. J. A. (2005). EEG evidence for mirror neuron dis- New York: Oxford University Press. function in autism specrum disorders. Cognitive Tidmarsh, L. & Volkmar, F. R. (2003). Diagnosis Brain Research, 24, 190–198. and epidemiology of autism spectrum dis- Otnes, R. K. & Enochson, L. (1972) Digital time series orders. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 48(8), analysis. New York: Wiley & Sons. 517–525. Page, L. A., Daly, E., Schmitz, N., Simmons, A., Vidal, C. N., Nicolson, R., DeVito, T. J., Hayashi, K. Toal, F., Deeley, Q., et al. (2006). In vivo M., Geaga, J. A., Drost, D. J., et al. (2006). H-magnetic resonance spectroscopy study of Mapping corpus callosum deficits in autism: An amygdala-hippocampal and parietal regions in index of aberrant cortical connectivity. Biological autism. American Journal of Psychiatry, 163, Psychiatry, 60(3), 218–225. 2189–2192. Villalobos, M. E., Mizuno, A., Dahl, B. C., Pelphrey, K. A., Adolphs, R., & Morris, J. P. (2005). Kemmotsu, N., & Mu¨ller, R.-A. (2005). Neuroanatomical substrates of social cognition Reduced functional connectivity between V1 dysfunction in autism. Mental Retardation and and inferior frontal cortex associated with visuo- Developmental Disabilities Research Reviews, 10, motor performance in autism. NeuroImage, 25, 259–271. 916–925. Scientific Articles 179

Walker, J. E. & Kozlowski, G. P. (2005). Neurofeedback of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied treatment of epilepsy. Child and Adolescent Psychia- Disciplines, 28, 137–151. tric Clinics of North America, 14, 163–176. Welchew, D. E., Ashwin, C., Berkouk, K., Salvador, Walker, J. E., Norman, C. A., & Weber, R. K. (2002). R., Suckling, J., Baron-Cohen, S., et al. (2005). Impact of qEEG-guided coherence training for Functional disconnectivity of the medial temporal patients with a mild closed head injury. Journal of lobe in asperger’s syndrome. Biological Psychiatry, Neurotherapy, 6(2), 31–43. 57, 991–998. Weeks, S. J. & Hobson, R. P. (1987). The salience of Wickelgren, I. (2005). Autistic brains out of synch?. for autistic children. Journal Science, 308, 1856–1858.