<<

Physiological Psvchology 1980, Vol. 8 (3), 351-359 and in and other

J. M. WARREN Pennsylvania State University, University Park, Pennsylvania 16802

A survey of the developmental, genetic, paleoneurological, comparative behavior, and neuro­ psychological evidence indicates that the neural organization responsible for handedness and laterality in humans is a heritable, species-specific trait. Handedness and laterality in mon­ keys, the most intensively studied nonhuman taxon, are not homologous to handedness and laterality in humans. Monkeys learn preferences through experience and display no dif­ ference in learning by the hemispheres ipsi- and contralateral to the preferred hand. Differences in the functions of the two hemispheres are found in several other nonhuman species, but none has been correlated with paw preferences. We who studied handedness in nonhuman primates much more variable patterns of coordination are re­ once thought our work might contribute to the under­ quired than are for the stereotyped patterns of loco­ standing of functional asymmetries in the motion characteristic of terrestial mammals. . We hypothesized that handedness in monkeys A link between handedness in nonhuman primates and apes might be homologous to handedness in and in humans is suggested by the theories humans and that lesions in the hemispheres contra­ which imply that lateralized control of the hand used and ipsilateral to the preferred hand might have dif­ preferentially in complex tool making and in gestural ferential effects on cognitive behaviors in monkeys, language favored lateralization of human speech thereby providing a useful model for unilateral ce­ mechanisms in the same hemisphere (Hewes, 1976; rebral insults in man (Brookshire & Warren, 1962; Montagu, 1976; Steklis & Hamad, 1976). Hamilton, 1977a, 1977b; Warren, 1958; Warren, The assumption that handedness in humans and Abplanalp, & Warren, 1967; Warren & Nonneman, other primates is homologous was not blatantly im­ 1976). plausible, nor was the belief that handedness in non­ Neither hypothesis compels instant disbelief. Lat­ human primates might be relevant to understanding eralization of the neural control of manipulation could, laterality in man. Yet both hypotheses have been in theory, have been selected for in much the same consistently disconfirmed by the empirical facts, as way as refinements in the structure of the hand dur­ will be shown by a review of the ontogenetic, phylo­ ing primate phylogeny (Reynolds, 1975). Prosimians genetic, and comparative evidence concerning later­ have no differential control of individual digits, only alization of brain functions in primates. whole-hand control. Old World monkeys have inde­ pendent control of the thumb and of the fingers col­ ONTOGENY lectively, but lack precise and discrete control over individual fingers. have more exact The clinical and experimental evidence indicates control of separate fingers. On the assumption that that the human brain is laterally differentiated before increasing degrees of asymmetrical neural control of birth (Wada, Clark, & Hamm, 1975) and that hand­ hand use accompanied these stages in the evolution edness is very probably, like several other traits, of the hand in primates, Reynolds (1975) concluded subject to strong genetic influences (Levy, 1976). that "Laterality of function is not necessarily unique Anatomic, behavioral, and neuropsychological to man, but perhaps a human manifestation of pro­ observations all point to very early cerebral laterali­ cesses that began early in primate phylogeny" (p. 499). zation. The planum temporale and parietal operCUlum Pribram (1977) has suggested a complementary are larger on the left in fetal and neonatal , as hypothesis regarding the origin of lateralized motor they are in right-handed adults. Neonates turn their functions. Asymmetrical motor control of the fore­ heads and eyes to the right more often than they do limbs in primates arose to direct optimally the use to the left, and the direction of the tonic neck reflex of the arms, , and fingers in climbing, brachi­ at 8 months is predictive of hand preferences at 10 ating, and other critical arboreal activities, for which years. When tested as adults, persons subjected to right- and left-hemisphere decortication during infancy are selectively impaired in spatial and verbal perfor­ The author's address is: 417 Moore Building. Pennsylvania State mance, much as persons who sustain comparable University. University Park. Pennsylvania 16802. brain injuries as adults.

Copyright 1980 Psychonomic Society, Inc. 351 0090-5046/80/030351-09$01.15/0 352 WARREN

No contemporary genetic model for the inheritance cates a high level of cognitive ability in paleolithic of handedness in humans is in satisfactory agreement man (Marshack, 1976; Pere1l6, 1970). For example, with the empirical evidence. This may be because Pere1l6 (1970) scored Catalan and Spanish cave paint­ cultural pressures produce sex and class differences ings as responses to the Draw-A-Person Test, and in the phenotypic expression of handedness. It is estimated the mental age of the artists as at least more likely that current theories regarding the trans­ 13 years, with no allowance made for their inferior kit. mission genetics of handedness are incomplete or Attempts to infer the time at which humans be­ incorrect. came predominantly right-handed from such material Although the mode of inheritance of handedness remains as tools, weapons, and paintings have yielded remains to be specified, there is no reason to doubt extremely variable estimates. Dart (1949) thought the importance of genetics in the determination of that Australopithecus was right-handed, while Blau handedness and other lateralized cortical function (1946) and Wile (1934) held that humans were not in humans. Eye dominance, fingerprints, and nose predominantly right-handed until domesticated as asymmetrics are traits subject to little or no cultural farmers in the Bronze Age. influence, and each of these traits is heritable, and This literature remains contradictory and incon­ each is positively correlated with handedness (Levy, clusive to the present. Thus, Pere1l6 (1970) observed 1976). It is very difficult to imagine a cultural shaping that contemporary right-handed children draw people process which could create sex differences in the and animals facing left, and left-handed persons representation of cognitive processes in the human draw their subjects facing to the right. Since about brain (Dimond, 1977; Witelson, 1976), particularly half the mammals at Altimira and several caves in when a simple genetic correlate is obvious. Catalonia face in either direction, it might be inferred Additional support for the participation of genetic that the paleolithic painters were ambidextrous or factors in the determination of handedness derives equally often right- and left-handed. But graphic from the value of family data in establishing prog­ artists may be different from ordinary folks and pro­ noses for patients with unilateral brain damage. Right­ vide misleading information regarding the distribu­ handed individuals with lesions in the language areas tion of laterality. And, indeed, Uhrbrock's (1973) of the left hemisphere have very different prospects survey of classical, medieval, and modern art works if their families do or do not include left-handed reveals essentially no preponderance of left-facing persons. Almost all of the patients with no left-handed portraits. Recent artists, like Iberian cave painters relatives are permanently aphasic, but only about but unlike modern children, show little bias in ori­ one-quarter of the patients with left-handed relatives enting their subjects. Thus, the cave pictures could suffer permanent after damage to the left reflect the peculiarities of painters rather than the hemisphere. distribution of handedness in the general population. Four lines of evidence thus indicate that handedness Claims of paleolithic right-handedness are also and other forms of cerebral laterality are genetically equivocal inferences from the material evidence. determined in humans: (1) structural and functional Most tracings of human hands on the walls of Spanish asymmetries in neonates; (2) the of lat­ caves are of the left hand. Hicks and Kinsbourne eralized traits correlated with handedness; (3) sex (1976, 1978) infer from this that the population of differences in the lateralization of cognitive functions; cave people was right-handed, assuming that each and (4) the value of pedigree data in predicting the person held his left hand passively against the wall effects of left-hemisphere damage. Since heritable and actively applied pigment with his preferred right traits arise only as a result of natural selection, the hand. But how do we know that the model and the genetic control of human cerebral lateralization indi­ painter were the same person? Paleolithic man had cates that the human pattern of hemispheric special­ apparently a complex set of religious practices. How ization is a species-specific trait, correlated with the can we tell if tracing the left hand was not dictated peculiar life style of humans. by ritualistic rather than organismic demands? Unfortunately, no statement is possible on the Clearly, no conclusion regarding handedness in developmental and genetic evidence as to whether paleolithic populations is justified by present inter­ or not human laterality is species-unique. There are pretations of the material remains. There is; however, no adequate ontogenetic data on nonhuman primates. no serious grounds to doubt that humans have been very largely right-handed for the last 5,000 years. PHYLOGENY Analysis of 1,180 drawings, paintings, and sculptures of people using tools or weapons shows that about The artifacts left by early humans have been studied 900/0 were right-handed, from 3000 B.C. to the present to answer the behavioral questions: How intelligent (Coren & Porac, 1977). was early man? When did humans become right­ Paleoneurology provides more conclusive evidence handed? The archeological evidence consistently indi- regarding the history of cerebral laterality and hand- HANDEDNESS AND LATERALITY 353 edness in hominids than does archaeology. Endocasts chimpanzees (Collins, 1968, 1969; Finch, 1941; of fossil human crania show that structural asym­ Lehman, 1978; Martin & Webster, 1974; Tsai & metries like those associated with functional asym­ Maurer, 1930; Warren, 1953, 1958). metries in modern humans were present 40,000 years Granting that there are equal numbers of animals ago. Specifically, the parietal operculum of a Neander­ with right- and left-hand or paw preferences in non­ thaler is substantially larger on the left than on the human mammals, primates might still differ from right, strongly suggesting left-hemisphere predomi­ other mammals in the proportion of individuals with nance for speech and handedness (Holloway, 1976; strong preferences for either the right hand or left LeMay, 1976; Levy, 1976). Endocasts of older fossil hand. This possibility is tested by comparing the crania do not reveal marked cerebral asymmetries, percentage of experimentally naive animals with a but they are few in number and badly preserved. It is 90070 preference for one hand, right or left, in reaching not possible, therefore, conclusively to deny the pos­ for food: mice, 58070 (Collins, 1969); , 71070 sibility that the brains of Homo were asymmetrical (Warren, 1958); rhesus macaques, 47070 (Lehman, more than 40,000 years ago. 1978) and 55070 (Warren, 1958); and chimpanzees, The discovery of an asymmetrical Neanderthal 46070 (Finch, 1941). brain supports the view that lateralization of neural Thus, in regard to the distribution of right- and function is a heritable, species-typical characteristic left-hand preferences and to the number of cases of humans and contradicts environmental explanations with strong preferences for either the right or the of human laterality. The archaeological evidence on left hand, chimpanzees and rhesus monkeys resemble the origin of handedness in man is ambiguous, but mice and cats, and differ drastically from humans. provides no data adequate to disconfirm the theory that the human pattern of lateralized brain functions Temporal and Situational Constancy is an inherited species-typical trait. Most humans use their right hands for each of the dozen or so manipulations sampled in accepted COMPARATIVE HEHA VIOR assessments of handedness, and tend to remain right­ handed from later childhood on (Annett, 1972; Briggs Distribution of Hand Preferences & Nebes, 1975; Hecaen & de Ajuriaguerra, 1964; Most humans, about 93070, are right-handed, an Levy, 1976). The performance of rhesus monkeys estimate based upon the median of 48 investigations on the handedness tests deviates quite markedly from summarized by Hecaen and de Ajuriaguerra (1964, the human pattern of temporally and situationally Table 1). The number of right-handed persons varies consistent right-handedness. somewhat between (Dawson, 1977) and within Monkeys do not have well-defined hand preferences as a function of sex (Dawson, 1977; Hecaen & prior to extended testing on manipulation tasks in de Ajuriaguerra, 1964), educational policies (Levy, the laboratory. Their initial preferences are weak, 1976), and social class (Falek, 1959). largely task specific, and unpredictive of later pref­ Although cultural influences complicate research erences (Warren, 1977). Marmosets are ambidextrous on the genetics of human handedness, they are no­ in spontaneous activities like play, feeding, and ex­ where strong enough to normalize the asymmetrical ploiting materials (Rothe, 1973). Ettlinger, Blakemore, distribution of handedness in humans. Differences and Milner (1968) did not use the early responses in the number of right-handed persons in different of their experimentally naive monkeys to specify populations are small, ranging from 90070 in permissive their hand preferences because initial responses tend hunting and fishing societies to 98070 in strict, tra­ to be atypical. The degree to which the first responses ditional agricultural societies (Dawson, 1977). The of wild monkeys on laboratory handedness tests are proportion of left-handed students in the United States "atypical," or unrepresentative of later performance, increased from 2070 in 1930 to about 11 070 in 1970, has been quantified. The correlation between monkeys' largely because of the greater tolerance of left-handed hand-preference scores on their first 500 test trials persons by teachers. It is, however, apparent from and scores on the same test replicated 2 years later the curve Levy (1976, Figure 1) presents that increases was ± .10, and the correlation between the first test in the number of left-handers have become smaller and the average of 14 others 2 years later was - .20 and smaller with the years since sinistral liberation, (Warren, 1977). and that 11070 left-handed persons is the likely em­ With continued testing in the laboratory, this picture pirical asymptote. changes considerably in the direction of increased No comparably asymmetrical distribution of right­ constancy. Both the split-half and test-retest reliability and left-hand or paw preferences has been observed of the handedness tests increase progressively on in any other species. The number of right- and left­ successive replications (Beck & Barton, 1972; Warren, handed individuals is, within the limits of sampling 1958, 1977). Increments in the consistency of hand­ error, equal in mice, rats, cats, rhesus monkeys, and edness are also suggested by a rise in the number 354 WARREN of monkeys with statistically significant preferences, thousands of times, while other manipulations are in the degree of preference averaged over all monkeys, not practiced at all. When, after many months or in the number of significant correlations between years, the subjects are retested on the handedness tests, and by a decrease in the number of contradic­ test battery, one observes that preferences on tests tory preferences, that is, when a monkey uses its that require responses that have been highly practiced right on one task and its left on another. are stronger and more consistent than in initial test­ These changes might be construed as indicating ing, but that preferences on tests that require manip­ that trapped wild monkeys have potentially stable ulations that were not practiced are no more strong and generalized hand preferences which they are too or consistent than in the original tests. anxious and apprehensive to show on arrival in the These changes would be expected if one assumes laboratory but reveal after they have become habitu­ that (1) monkeys learn to prefer one hand for a ated to captivity. This would be wrong. Only the in­ particular sort of manipulation, and (2) monkeys crease in reliability is common to all tasks in a have a limited range of response generalization. Given handedness battery; all the other indices vary among a detectable disparity between the hands in skill or the different tests. Some tests continue to elicit weak the effort required for a given manipulation, or and sometimes contradictory preference, and remain even an environmental asymmetry that militated uncorrelated with one another and with tests that against equal practice with both hands, a differential elicit strong and consistent preferences (Warren, 1977). delay of reinforcement for responding with the left The central, critical fact about handedness in hand and right hand would develop and monkeys monkeys is that it is task and situation specific. would shape themselves under this contingency to use Results from Macaca mulatta and M. speciosa on one hand exclusively to make particular manual multiple handedness tests reveal high interest agree­ responses, such as pushing a block aside and seizing ment among tasks that acquire common manipula­ a peanut concealed beneath it. Displacing objects is tions, but low correlations among tasks that require what monkeys mostly do in the months between test-unique hand movements (Beck & Barton, 1972; series of handedness tests, so it is not surprising that Deuel, 1975; Warren, 1977). Marmosets also display monkeys tend strongly to use the hand preferred to discrepant hand preferences on different tests of manipulate objects to displace cards, to pick up rai­ manipulation (Rothe, 1973). The hand preferences of sins, and to extract peanuts from a cup; since all these monkeys are also influenced by context effects, in tasks require very similar hand movements, response that performance on a specific test, and its correla­ generalization is anticipated. Pulling in a chain and tions with other tests, varies among experiments with removing fruit impaled on its end and taking a cereal different series of monkeys, apparently as a function ring from a wire are manipulations that differ markedly of which other tests are presented in the test battery, from the well-practiced act of pushing objects about distribution of practice on the several tasks, and and similar tasks. Persistent failure to observe sig­ other details of experimental procedure. nificant correlations between the two sorts of tasks The argument from correlational analyses is sup­ is compatible with the view that response preferences ported by additional evidence of lability in monkeys' learned on the object displacement test fail to gener­ manual preferences. Some monkeys use opposite alize to chain and wire tests. hands in the light and dark, or in the Wisconsin This account of the hand preferences of experienced General Test Apparatus and in a primate chair (Deuel, monkeys as the result of task-specific learning and 1975; Ettlinger et aI., 1968). Position of the food limited response generalization is supported by the incentive relative to the monkey and the direction findings that the spontaneous hand preferences of and quality of hand movement required to secure the squirrel monkeys can be quickly and permanently food also affect the hand preferences of monkeys reversed by contingent reinforcement of the non­ (Cronholm, Grodsky, & Behar, 1%3; Lehman, 1978). preferred hand and that discriminative control of The available behavioral information on handed­ responding by the two hands under the control of vi­ ness in monkeys can be accounted for by a simple sual stimuli can be established by relatively little ad­ model which indicates that the appearance of hand­ ditional training (McGonigle & Flook, 1978). ness in monkeys is an artifact produced by the methods that have been used in chronic experiments (Warren, COMPARATIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 1977). The first experiment in which trapped wild monkeys fresh from the jungle serve is often a series The complementary division of special functions of handedness tests, since handedness testing is a between the right and left hemispheres of the human good way to shape responses that are required in brain (Hecaen & Albert, 1978; Levy, 1977) is well subsequent perception and learning experiments. In known and the evidence so extensive that an extensive such investigations, monkeys typically repeat some summary would be inappropriate here, where we are of the manipulations elicited in the handedness tests concerned with the question of whether there is, as HANDEDNESS AND LATERALITY 355 in man, an essential association between handedness lesions on the right, showed a permanent defect in and other lateralized functions in nonhuman primates. a delayed matching-to-sample task, which required If hand preferences in monkeys were valid indices that monkeys press a red key after a tone was sounded of an asymmetrical brain organization, with the and a green key after noise. It is important to hemisphere contralateral to the preferred hand pre­ emphasize that the locus of the lesion was the deter­ dominant for mediating cognitive as well as manual minant of losses on both tasks. Lesions on the left functions, the isolated "dominant" hemisphere (con­ hemisphere impaired both right- and left-handed tralateral to the preferred hand) should surpass the monkeys, but lesions on the right were ineffective in "nondominant" hemisphere (ipsilateral to the pre­ all monkeys (Dews on, Note 1). ferred paw) on learning and performance tasks. Dewson's demonstration that the processing of This proposition has been tested by two sorts of complex acoustic signals is lateralized in the left experimental designs. Groups of monkeys are tested hemisphere in monkeys is corroborated by a behav­ for handedness and matched groups are subjected ioral experiment on in M. fuscata to unilateral ablations of association cortex on the and three other species of monkeys. The discriminanda apparently dominant and nondominant hemispheres, were species-specific calls of Japanese macaques with making it possible to compare the relative capacity early and late frequency peaks. Five of five of the dominant and nondominant cortical regions macaques made significantly more correct responses that remain on the intact hemispheres. In studies of with right- than with left-ear stimulation. Only this kind, all the monkeys with unilateral lesions are, one of five representatives of other species showed in learning, frequently inferior to unoperated con­ a significant right-ear advantage. "The results suggest trols, but no significant difference has ever been ob­ that Japanese macaques engage left-hemisphere pro­ served between the effects on lesions contra- or cessors for the analysis of communicatively significant ipsilateral to the preferred hand (Ettlinger & Dawson, sounds that are analogous to the lateralized mecha­ 1969; Warren, Cornwell, & Warren, 1969; Warren, nisms used by humans in listening to speech" (Petersen, Grant, Hara, & Leary, 1963; Warren & Nonneman, Beecher, Zoloth, Moody, & Stebbins, 1978, p. 324). 1976). Petersen et al. give no information regarding the The second type of experiment provides within­ hand preference of their monkeys. Given, however, subjects comparisons from split-brained monkeys or the symmetrical distribution of hand preferences in monkeys with reversible inactivation of the separate monkeys, the probability is low (± .03) that their five hemispheres. A substantial and significant difference Japanese macaques were all right-handed as well as in the rate that the two hemispheres learn discrimina­ right-eared and left-brained. The results of the dichotic tions or in the accuracy of performance on such tests provide additional evidence for the lateraliza­ tasks as delayed response would constitute unequivocal tion of some auditory processes in monkeys, and sug­ evidence of functionallateralization in monkeys. No gest that this sort of functional asymmetry is in­ evidence of this sort is to be found in the literature. dependent of hand preference in monkeys. All of the properly controlled experiments with a The studies of handedness and auditory functions reasonable number of subjects indicate that monkeys' in monkeys reveal an important fact about lateraliza­ hemispheres are equally proficient in learning on vi­ tion in the primate nervous system: The close link sual cues (Butler, 1968; Downer, 1962; Ebner & between handedness and other lateralized functions Myers, 1962; Fuster & Bauer, 1974; Hamilton, 1977a, characteristic of human persons is not inevitable. In 1977b; Hamilton & Gazzaniga, 1964; Lehman & monkeys, the mechanisms for processing some kinds Spencer, 1972). of auditory information are lateralized, even though The complete failure of several investigators to ob­ monkeys lack the stable handedness seen in humans. tain any evidence that the con­ The neural bases of functional lateralization are tralateral to the preferred hand differs in any impor­ organized in fundamentally different ways in monkeys tant respect from the hemisphere ipsilateral to the and men. Macaques are not little men in brown fur preferred hand is clearly more compatible with the suits, whose cerebral dominance is revealed by their view that "handedness" in monkeys is an artifact hand preferences, since handedness and other later­ of learning in the laboratory rather than a trait ali zed traits appear to be correlated only in humans. that is homologous or analogous to handedness in Although it now seems unlikely that homologues humans. This interpretation derives additional strong of human handedness and laterality will be found in support from Dewson's (1978) discovery that functions other taxa, several sorts of spontaneous and induced may be lateralized in the brains of monkeys, in­ lateralized functions have recently been demonstrated dependent of any correlation with handedness. in experimental animals. The following phenomena Dewson observed that M. irus with ablations of the may reasonably be regarded as analogues of later­ left superior temporal gyrus and the dorsal bank of ali zed neural functions in humans. the superior temporal sulcus, but not equivalent (I) Section of the left hypoglossal nerve or destruc- 356 WARREN tion of the left hypoglossal nucleus disrupts singing other group if they had been denied experiences in in canaries, chaffinches, white-crowned sparrows, the enriched environment, while handled and enriched and white-throated sparrows much more severely rats with right cerebral insults had the lowest activity than do identical injuries on the right (Nottebohm, scores of any group. As measured by activity levels, 1977). handling and enrichment experiences affect the (2) Rats that can escape shock in a T-maze running right hemisphere more than the left in rats. Since to either the right or left arm manifest strong prefer­ emotional states strongly influence rats' activity in an ences for turning in one direction. Post mortem deter­ open field, the results of Dennenberg et ai. inevitably minations showed that the concentration of dopamine remind one of the findings that the right hemisphere in the striatum contralateral to the side preferred by is more consequential than the left for the expression each rat was significantly higher than on the ipsilat­ and evaluation of emotions by humans (Sackheim, eral side (Zimmerberg, Glick, & Jerussi, 1974). Gur, & Saucy, 1978). (3) Mice, like monkeys, display lateralization of More recent works demonstrate that the right and brain function independent of paw preferences. DBA left hemispheres of rats handled in infancy are func­ mice develop audiogenic seizures significantly sooner tionally asymmetrical in their control of taste aversion with monaural stimulation of the left ear than with learning, spatial turning preferences, and mouse kill­ the right. Yet, paw preferences in DBA mice are ing behavior (Sherman, Garbanati, Rosen, Yutzey, symmetrically distributed (Collins & Ward, 1970). & Dennenberg, 1980). The neurological bases of My last two examples of laterality in nonhuman hemispheric asymmetries in rats are unknown. It animals were induced, not spontaneous as those seems probable, however, that interhemispheric dif­ described above. The lateral asymmetries are not ferences in catecholamine levels (Robinson, 1979; seen in untreated populations, but are created by the Zimmerman et aI., 1974) are importantly involved. experimenter in the laboratory. These experiments obviously indicate that later­ (4) Monkeys trained on a delayed response task alization of neural functions occurs in animals other with one hand restrained in a cuff develop sig­ than man. The induction of lateral asymmetries in nificantly larger steady potential shifts in the frontal the monkeys of Stamm et ai. (1975) and in the rats of granular and precentral cortex contralateral to the Dennenberg and his colleagues is a particularly signif­ active hand than they do in the same areas ipsilateral icant observation, since it tells that there is a latent to the active hand. Later, when the initially restrained capacity for lateralization within the brains of non­ hand is freed and the previously active hand is chained, human animals, and a lateralized system can be gene­ the steady potential shift in the precentral region be­ rated by appropriate experiences. The processes that comes larger on the hemisphere contralateral to the underlie the experimental induction of lateralized currently active hand, but the steady potential shift functions in monkeys and rats may resemble, in some in the frontal granular. cortex, which is associated ways, those which permit the right hemisphere in hu­ with the mnemonic traces that mediate successful mans to mediate linguistic functions after damage to delayed response, remains larger on the hemisphere the language areas on the left hemisphere in infancy contralateral to the first-trained hand. Unilateral and childhood. training has thus induced a persistent interhemispheric Perhaps mammals and birds have generally been asymmetry that was not present before training. The preadapted to develop lateralized control systems for subjects in this experiment were tested for hand pref­ a long time. If so, it is not hard to imagine that, erences prior to the beginning of unimanual restraint. at different times and in the phylogeny of different Apparent handedness had no effect upon the genesis and unrelated species, circumstances arose in which of the differences in steady potential shifts between animals with more lateralized functions enjoyed a hemispheres (Stamm, Gadotti, & Rosen, 1975). selective advantage over less lateralized conspecifics; (5) An investigation of interaction of preweaning continued selection pressure would lead to progressively handling, postweaning exposure to an enriched more highly lateralized populations. environment, and unilateral cortical lesions in adult­ Functional lateralization may thus have arisen in­ hood on open-field activity in rats reveals a curious dependently in several lines of descent in response new form of cerebral lateralization which few of us to a variety of species-specific problems in adapta­ would have predicted (Dennenberg, Garbanati, tion; expressions of laterality in different species are Sherman, Yutzey, & Kaplan, 1978). Differential ef­ therefore probably best regarded as analogous. The fects were obtained from the animals handled from history of research on other problems in neuro­ birth to 20 days. Rats with lesions in the left hemi­ psychology strongly encourages the belief that neuro­ sphere, whether provided environmental enrichment logical analyses of analogous types of functional or not from Day 21 to Day 50, did not differ from lateralization in other animals will significantly aid the appropriate intact controls. Rats that had been us in our efforts to understand cerebral lateraliza­ handled in infancy and suffered right-hemisphere tion in humans. damage had higher scores in the open field than any The nervous system is more conservative than the HANDEDNESS AND LATERALITY 357 behavior it mediates (Warren & Kolb, 1978). The system is not unique to humans, and is perhaps positive reinforcement system is in essentially the not extremely rare in mammals and birds. Functional same place and the effects of stimulating it electrically lateralization seems now to have evolved independently are very similar in goldfish, pigeons, chickens, rats, in a number of species whenever lateralization of rabbits, guinea pigs, cats, , monkeys, porpoises, a specific function afforded a selective advantage. and humans (Valenstein, 1973), in spite of the fact The several types of cerebral laterality observed in that vertebrates differ substantially in respect to nonhuman species are most probably analogous which naturally occurring stimuli act as reinforcers. rather than homologous to the functional specifica­ Likewise, mammalian species differ from one another tion of the hemispheres in man. in almost every measurable dimension of their sex However heterogeneous the origins of species­ behavior (Dewsbury, 1972, 1975), but sex behavior specific forms of functional asymmetries in the in all species depends on the integrity of the preoptic nervous system may be, there is reason to believe that nucleus. Destruction of the ventromedial nucleus in studies of laterality in other animals may illuminate the hypothalamus makes mammals fat and mean, the mechanisms of laterality in humans. Although no matter what their species-specific patterns of species differ in the particular functions that are eating and fighting behavior may be. lateralized in their brains, the molecular and neural By analogy, it seems sensible to speculate that, bases of laterality are probably very similar in dif­ although different functionallateralizations arose in ferent species. The copulatory and feeding behaviors different species to solve different problems in adapta­ in mammals are highly variegated, but all are regulated tion, all arose from the same sort of preadapted by common neural mechanisms. neural mechanism and all operate in basically very similar ways. REFERENCE NOTE

I. Dewson, J. H. Personal communication, 1976. SUMMARY REFERENCES Developmental and genetic data indicate that handedness and associated functional asymmetries in ANNETT, M. The distribution of manual asymmetry. British Jour­ the human brain are heritable, species-specific char­ nal of Psychology, 1972,63, 343-358. BECK, C. H. M. , & BARTON, R. L. Deviation and laterality of acteristics. Paleoneurological evidence shows that the hand preference in monkeys. Cortex, 1972,7,339-363. left and right parietal opercula were structurally dif­ BLAU, A. The master hand. New York: American Orthopsychiatric ferentiated in humans 40,000 years ago. Association, 1946. Comparative behavioral and neuropsychological BRIGGS, G., & NEBES, R. Patterns of hand preference in a student population. Cortex, 1975,11,230-238. findings very strongly suggest that the human pattern BROOKSHIRE, K. H., & WARREN, J. M. The generality and con­ of handedness and cerebral laterality is species-unique sistency of handedness in monkeys. Behaviour, 1962, and that no truly homologous traits are to be found 10, 222-227. in nonhuman mammals. Handedness has been most BUTLER, C. R. A memory-record for visual discrimination habits extensively studied in macaque monkeys. An equal produced in both cerebral hemispheres of monkey when only one hemisphere has received direct visual information. Brain number of monkeys are right- and left-handed. Their Research, 1968, 10, 152-167. hand preferences are initially weak, inconsistent over COLLINS, R. L. On the inheritance of handedness: I. Laterality in tasks requiring different kinds of manipulations, and inbred mice. Journal of Heredity, 1968,59,9-12. strongly influenced by learning and experience. The COLLINS, R. L. On the inheritance of handedness: II. Selection for sinistrality in mice. Journal of Heredity, 1969,60, 117-119. cerebral hemispheres contra- and ipsilateral to the COLLINS, R. L., & WARD, R. Evidence for an asymmetry of cere­ preferred hand contribute equivalently to visual bral function in mice tested for audiogenic seizures. Nature, learning by monkeys. The left hemisphere is superior 1970,226,1062-1063. to the right in processing acoustic signals in both COREN, S., & PORAC, C. Fifty centuries of right-handedness: right- and left-handed macaques; the hemispheres The historical record. Science, 1977, 198,631-632. CRONHOLM, J. N., GRODSKY, M., & BEHAR, I. Situational factors function asymmetrically, but independently of hand in the lateral preferences of rhesus monkeys. Journal of Genetic preference. Psychology, 1963, 103, 167-174. Although the human pattern of lateralization of DART, R. A. The predatory implement technique of Australo­ function is species-unique, apparently analogous forms pithecus. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 1949, 7,1-38. of lateralization of function have been observed in DAWSON, J. L. M. B. An anthropological perspective on the evo­ the brains of several nonhuman species. For example, lution and lateralization of the brain. Annals of the New York the motor control of singing by canaries, chaffinches, Academy of Sciences, 1977, 299, 424-447. and white-crowned sparrows is mediated exclusively DENNENBERG, V. H., GARBANATI, J., SHERMAN, G., YUTZEY, D., & KAPLAN, R. Infantile stimulation induces brain lateraliza­ by the left cerebrum, and early experience preferen­ tion in rats. Science, 1978.201. 1150-1152. tially affects the right hemisphere in rats. D~:u~:L, R. K. 30 monkeys without cerebral dominance. Neurol­ Functional lateralization in the central nervous ogv, 1975,25,389. 358 WARREN

DEWSBURY, D. A. Patterns of copulatory behavior in male mam­ cerebral asymmetry. Annals of the New York Academy of mals. Quarterly Review of Biology, 1972,47, 1-33. Sciences, 1977,299,264-272. DEWSBURY, D. A. Diversity and adaptation in rodent copulatory MARSHACK, A. Some implications of the paleolithic symbolic evi­ behavior. Science, 1975, 190, 947-954. dence for the origin of language. Annals of the New York DEWSON, J . H. Some behavioral effects of removal of superior Academy of Sciences, 1976,280,289-311. temporal cortex in the monkey. In D. J. Chivers & J. Herbert MARTIN, D., & WEBSTER, W. G. Paw preference shifts follow­ (Eds.), Recent advances in primatology (Vol. I). London: ing forced practice. Physiology & Behavior, 1974, 13, 745-748. Academic Press, 1978. MCGONIGLE, B., & FLOOK, J. The learning of hand preferences DIMOND, S. J. Evolution and lateralization of the brain. Annals by squirrel monkeys. Psychological Research, 1978, 40, 93-98. of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1977,299,477-501. MONTAGU, A. Tool making, hunting and the origin of language. DOWNER, J . L. Interhemispheric integration in the visual system. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1976, 280, In V. B. Mountcastle (Ed.), Interhemispheric relations and cere­ 266-274. bral dominance. Baltimore: Johns Hopkins Press, 1962. NOTIEBOHM, F. Asymmetries in neural control of vocalization in EBNER, F. F ., & MYERS, R. E. and the inter­ the·canary. In S. Hamad, R. W. Doty, L. Goldstein, J. Jaynes, hemispheric transmission of tactual learning. Journal of Neuro­ & G. Krautheimer (Eds.), Lateratization in the nervous system. physiology, 1962,25,380-391. New York: Academic Press, 1977. ETILINGER, G., BLAKEMORE, C., & MILNER, A. Opposite hand PERELLO; J. Digressions on the biological foundations of language. preferences in two sense-modalities. Nature, 1968,218, 1276. Journal of Communication Disorders, 1970,3, 140-149. ETILINGER, G., & DAWSON, R. F. Hand preferences in the mon­ PETERSEN, M. R., BEECHER, M. D., ZOLOTH, S. R., MOODY, key: The effect of unilateral cortical removals. Neuropsychologia, D. B., & STEBBINS, W. C. Neural lateralization of species­ 1969,7,161-166. specific vocalizations by Japanese macaques (Macaca fuscata). FALEK, A. Handedness, a family study. American Journal of Science, 1978,202,324-327. Human Genetics, 1959, 11, 52-62. PRIBRAM, K. H. Hemispheric specialization: Evolution or revolu­ FINCH, G. handedness. Science, 1941,94, 117-118. tion. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1977, FUSTER, J. M., & BAUER, R. H. Visual short-term memory deficit 299, 18-22. from hypothermia of frontal cortex. Brain Research, 1974, 81, REYNOLDS, P. C. Handedness and the evolution of the primate 393-400. forelimb. Neuropsychologia, 1975,13,499-500. HAMILTON, C. R. An assessment of hemispheric specialization in ROBINSON, R. G. Differential behavioral and biochemical effects monkeys. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1977, of right and left hemispheric cerebral infarction in the rat. 299,222-232. (a) Science, 1979,205,707-710. HAMILTON, C. R. Investigations of perceptual and mnemonic ROTHE, H. Handedness in the common marmoset (Callithrix lateralization in monkeys. In S. Hamad, R. W. Doty, L. jacchus). American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 1973, Goldstein, J. Jaynes, & G. Krautheimer (Eds.), Lateratization 38,561-565. in the nervous system. New York: Academic Press, 1977. (b) SACKHEIM, H. A., GUR, R., & SAUCY, M. C. Emotions are HAMILTON, C. R., & GAZZANAGA, M. Lateralization of learning expressed more intensely on the left side of the face. Science, of colour and brightness discriminations following brain bisec­ 1978,202,434-436. tion. Nature, 1964,201,220. SHERMAN, G. F., GARBANATI, J. A., ROSEN, G. D., YUTZEY, HECAEN, H., & ALBERT, M . L . Human neuropsychology. D. A., & DENNF.NRF.RG, V. H. Brain and behavioral asymmetries New York: Wiley, 1978. for spatial preference in rats. Brain Research, 1980, 192,61-67. HECAEN, H., & DE AJURIAGUERRA, J. Left-handedness. STAMM, J. S., GADOTTI, A., & ROSEN, S. C. Interhemispheric New York: Greene & Stratton, 1964. functional differences in prefrontal cortex of monkeys. Journal HEWES, G. W. Current status of the gestural theory of language of Neurobiology, 1975,6,39-49. origin. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1976, STEKLIS, H . D., & HARNAD, S. R. From hand to mouth: Some 280, 482-504. critical stages in the evolution of language. Annals of the HICKS, R. E ., & KINSBOURNE, M. On the genesis of human New York Academy of Sciences, 1976,280,445-455. handedness: A review. Journal of Motor Behavior, 1976, 8, TSAL, L., & MAURER, S. Right handedness in white rats. Science, 257-266. 1930,72,436-438. HICKS, R. E., & KINSBOURNE, M. Human handedness. In M. UHRBROCK, R. S. Laterality in art. Journal of Aesthetics and Art Kinsbourne (Ed.), Asymmetrical function of the brain. Criticism, 1973,32,27-35. New York: Cambridge University Press, 1978. VALENSTEIN , E. S. Brain control. New York: Wiley, 1973 . HOLLOWAY, R. L. Paleoneurological evidence for language origins. WADA, J. A., CLARK, R., & HAMM, A. Cerebral hemispheric Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1976, 280, asymmetry in humans: Cortical speech zones in 100 adult and 330-348. 100 infant brains. Archives of Neurology, 1975, 32, 239-246. LEHMAN, R. A. W. Hand preference and cerebral predominance WARREN, J . M. Handedness in the rhesus monkey. Science, 1953, in 24 rhesus monkeys. Journal of the Neurological Sciences, 118, 622-623 . 1970,10,185-192. WARREN, J. M. The development of paw preferences in cats and LEHMAN, R. A. W . The handedness of rhesus monkeys-I. Dis­ monkeys. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 1958,93, 229-236. tribution. Neuropsychologia, 1978,16,33-42. WARREN, J. M. Handedness and cerebral dominance in monkeys. LEHMAN, R. A. W ., & SPENCER, D. D. Hand preference and In S. Hamad, R. W. Doty, L. Goldstein, J. Jaynes, & G. hemispheric learning in the monkey. Experimental Neurology, Krautheimer (Eds.), Lateralization in the nervous system. 1972,36,88-100. New York: Academic Press, 1977. LEMA Y, M. Morphological cerebral asymmetries of modern man, WARREN, J. M., ABPLANALP, J. M., & WARREN, H. B. Th'e fossil man, and nonhuman primate. Annals of the New York development of handedness in cats and rhesus monkeys. In Academy of Sciences, 1976,280,349-366. H. W. Stevenson, E. H. Hess, & H. Rheingold (Eds.), Early LEVY, J. A review of evidence for a genetic component in the behavior: Comparative developmental approaches. New York: determination of handedness. Behavior Genetics, 1976, 6, Wiley, 1967. 429-453. WARREN, J. M., CORNWELL, P . R., & WARREN, H. B. Unilateral LEVY , J. The mammalian brain and the adaptive advantage of frontal lesions and learning by rhesus monkeys. Journal of HANDEDNESS AND LATERALITY 359

Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 1969,69,498-505. WILE, 1. S. Handedness: Right and left. Boston: Lathrop, Lee & WARREN , J . M., GRANT, R., HARA, K., & LEARY, R. W . Im­ Shepard, 1934. paired learning by monkeys with unilateral lesions in association WITELSON, S. F. Sex and the single hemisphere: Specialization of cortex. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, the right hemisphere for spatial processing. Science, 1976, 193, 1963,56,241-253. 425-426. WARREN, J . M., & KOLB, B. Generalizations in neuropsychology. ZIMMERBERG, B ., GLICK, S. D., & JERUSSI, T. P. Neurochemical In S. Finger (Ed.), Recovery from brain damage. New York: correlate of a spatial preference in rats. Science, 1974, 185, Plenum, 1978. 623-625 . WARREN , J. M., & NONNEMAN, A. J. The search for cerebral dominance in monkeys. Annals of the New York Academy of (Received for publication March 3, 1980; Sciences, 1976, 280, 732-744. revision accepted April 28, 1980.)