The Diachronic Coronal-Velar Nasal Relationship

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

The Diachronic Coronal-Velar Nasal Relationship The Diachronic Coronal-Velar Nasal Relationship Jenny Simone Lederer† † University of California, Berkeley E-mail: [email protected] 2. DIACHRONIC EXAMPLES ABSTRACT (2.1) Cuban Spanish This paper investigates the phonetic factors of diachronic A prototypical case of the alveolar nasal sifting to the nasal place change from the alveolar to the velar place of velar nasal is that of Cuban Spanish (Hammond 1976, articulation. Evidence is given from Cuban Spanish, Lederer 2000). In Cuban Spanish, all word-final instances Hausa, Brazilian Portuguese, and Chinese. There appear of a nasal consonant appear as the velar nasal []. By to be two distinct pathways leading to change. The first is contrast, in Peninsular Spanish, word-final nasals appear a pathway in which the word-final alveolar nasal becomes as the alveolar [n] (with the exception of dialects such as velar with no independent stage of phonemic word-final Andalusian, which also exhibits the word-final velar nasalized vowels (Cuban Spanish, Hausa, and certain nasal). dialectal groups of Chinese). In the second pathway Spain Cuba (Brazilian Portuguese), there is a documented stage of pa[n] pa[] ‘bread’ phonemically nasalized vowels. It is concluded that the esta[n] eta[] ‘they are’ change from alveolar-to-velar nasal is one of phonetic kamjo[n] kamjo[] ‘truck’ changes, in no way explained by an abstract notion of peruti[n] peruti[] ‘Peruchin’ markedness. seu[n] seu[] ‘following’ 1. INTRODUCTION Table 1: Spanish examples of the [n] Æ [] change One can hypothesize that the word-final n of pre- In many languages, velar consonants have undergone [ ] colonial Spanish evolved into the word-final spoken place of articulation shift to the alveolar region. Some of [ ] in Cuba, since the alveolar nasal is found in most parts of this diachronic place change and synchronic alternation the Iberian Peninsula, and the velar nasal is realized word- has been collected and summarized in favor of a finally only in Southern Spain, Cuba, and other former phonological argument that coronals and velars are the Spanish colonial countries in the Caribbean (e.g. Puerto two unmarked places of articulation, and further that Rico, dialects of Caribbean Mexico). coronals and velars result from a single representation, a In order to begin the process of reconstructing how a Place node with no dependent (Rice, 1996). change like this might take place it is crucial to note The goal of the current analysis is to investigate and several factors about the phonetic differences between the explain this special diachronic relationship, relying more two dialects, differences that perhaps served as catalysts upon acoustic and articulatory factors, while shying away for the resulting change in articulatory position. The first from any phonological explanation of markedness and point to be noticed is that the Cuban velar nasal is unmarkedness. The focus on nasal place shift and the pronounced in a word-final environment. In Cuban reasons for this shift will exemplify the complexity of Spanish pre-consonantal nasals assimilate to the place of these specific phonetic/phonemic changes. Nasals are articulation of the following consonant: [emfasi] susceptible to place change simply because of their ‘emphasis’, [entufar] ‘to plug in’, [mao] ‘mango’, but nasality. in the word-final environment, the velar nasal serves as the This paper is organized as follows. In Section 2, several ‘default’ nasal. In continental Spanish, the system of unrelated languages will be examined in detail as evidence phonemic nasals consists of [m], [n], []. The velar [] is for the diachronic change from n to . Shared [ ] [] simply an allophonic variant of the alveolar nasal. In phonetic characteristics of the sample languages will be Cuban Spanish, however, a phonetic neutralization seems summarized. Section 3 will consist of experimental to have occurred. Because the velar-nasal is pronounced acoustic and articulatory studies, which will serve as more word-finally, and in the unconditioned coda position evidence toward the specific acoustic and articulatory sporadically (Hammond 1976), the [] has now taken over reasons that these changes occur diachronically. And, many of the positions of [n]. finally, Section 4 will be a list of necessary phonetic Another crucial difference between the two dialects, a factors that serve as a roadmap for this specific diachronic key factor to the alveolar-velar sound change, rests on the process. quality of the vowel which precedes the word-final nasal. The vowels preceding nasals in continental Spanish are not phonologically nasalized, but the vowels preceding Newman (2000). Newman explains that the nasal nasals in Cuban Spanish are (Lederer 2000). Maria-Josep phoneme /n/ commonly occurs in syllable final-position, Solé (1995) experimentally demonstrates the difference and that, when it does, it is always homorganic with the between a phonologized pattern of pre-nasal vowel following consonant. This nasal-coda situation in Hausa nasalization in American English and phonetic co- seems at first very similar to the Cuban homorganic nasal articulatory pattern of pre-nasal vowel nasalization in coda phenomenon. However, Newman (p. 405) goes so Continental Spanish: far as to say that the nasal in this position should not be In (Continental) Spanish, vowels are oral for most of linked to the coda, but is in fact part of a “nasal their duration; onset of VP (velopharyngeal port) diphthong”, which behaves comparably to an oral opening is timed relative to the following nasal diphthong. This type of description raises the possibility consonant and peak VP size occurs at nasal consonant that Hausa vowels in pre-nasal position are phonemically onset across (speech) rates, which suggests that the nasalized as is the case in Cuba. motor commands for opening the velar port are part of (2.3) Chinese the instructions for the nasal consonant. In American In several areas of the Min dialects of Chinese, the English, on the other hand, VP opening onset historical contrast of word-final [m], [n], and [] in coincides with vowel onset and peak VP size occurs Middle Chinese (MC) have merged to be pronounced as in the middle of the vowel across rates, which the velar nasal (Chen, 1973). Chen exemplifies this indicates that opening movements are part of the ‘unidirectional merging’ by observing that in dialects that programming instructions for the vowel. It is argued merged to only two-way contrasts, either /m/ merged with that, in (Continental) Spanish, vowels followed by a /n/ or /n/ merged with //. The merges never resulted in nasal consonant are targeted as oral and are nasalized just word-final [m] and never resulted in just word-final as a result of a co-articulatory effect, whereas in [n]. Often the two-way distinction then neutralized to American English, vowels are targeted as nasalized as only one word-final nasal, the velar []: a result of a phonological rule (p. 1). I.m n Guangzhou, Xiamen, Meixian The phonetically nasalized vowels in pre-nasal position II. Peking, Jinan, Xian, Hankou, in Continental Spanish can be compared the n phonologically nasalized vowels in pre-nasal position in III. Fuzhou, Taiyuan, Wenzhou, Sanghai Cuban Spanish. Cuban Spanish pre-nasal vowels pattern Table 3: The unidirectional pattern (Chen, p.102) with the pre-nasal vowels of American English (Lederer Chen notes too, that / / and / / historically tend to drop 2000). In Cuban Spanish nasal airflow begins at the m n after nasalizing the preceding vowel more readily than / /. release of the onset and increases during the duration of A special relationship formed from the appearance of the the vowel until it peaks at the closure of the nasal. This velar nasal on nasalized vowels. A potential nasal airflow indicates that the entire pre-nasal vowel is articulatory/acoustic explanation for this occurrence will nasalized, due to the early opening of the velopharyngeal be outlined in Section 3. port, and is thus not simply a result of phonetic co- (2.4) Brazilian Portuguese articulation. Cuban Spanish differs crucially from Brazilian Portuguese represents the second pathway to Continental Spanish in this aspect of vowel nasalization. change, in which a stage of word-final velar nasals has This distinction will be shown later to be one of the most been preceded by a long stage of word-final phonemically important predictive factors in the change from alveolar to nasalized vowels. Ryan Shosted (2003 (in this volume)) velar nasal. shows how Brazilian Portuguese fits into a similar (2.2) Modern Hausa diachronic nasal place change as the other languages. Like Cuban Spanish, Hausa has undergone the same nasal Shosted has demonstrated, through the study of nasal place change. Though not as universal as the word-final airflow, that what once were simply word-final nasalized velar constraint in Cuban Spanish, modern Hausa high vowels in Brazilian Portuguese can now be demonstrates the allophonic variation in word-final documented as ending in the articulation of the velar nasal. position. In most dialects, both final / / and final / / may m n The change from bilabial and alveolar word-final nasals be realized as the velar (Bello 1992). A couple of [] (from the evidence of Latin) has come about through a examples demonstrate the sound change: very long diachronic time span, but the result of the Historical Form Modern Pronunciation change is nonetheless the same as the previous languages. (Bello 1992) ‘man’ mutum mutu The following examples demonstrate the historical form (Bello 1992) ‘there’ can ca and the current pronunciation in the Minas Gerais dialect Table 2: Hausa examples of the Æ change [n] [] of Brazilian Portuguese: The orthography of the language as well as the synchronic Historical Brazilian Alternative alternation between different dialects point to the (Shosted 2003) ‘yes’ directional change from two different word-final nasals: si si (Shosted 2003) ‘in a, in one’ and to the merged word-final velar nasal, .
Recommended publications
  • The Phonemic − Syllabic Comparisons of Standard Malay and Palembang Malay Using a Historical Linguistic Perspective
    Passage2013, 1(2), 35-44 The Phonemic − Syllabic Comparisons of Standard Malay and Palembang Malay Using a Historical Linguistic Perspective By: Novita Arsillah English Language and Literature Program (E-mail: [email protected] / mobile: +6281312203723) ABSTRACT This study is a historical linguistic investigation entitled The Phonemic − Syllabic Comparisons of Standard Malay and Palembang Malay Using a Historical Linguistic Perspective which aims to explore the types of sound changes found in Palembang Malay. The investigation uses a historical linguistic comparative method to compare the phonemic and syllabic changes between an ancestral language Standard Malay and its decent language Palembang Malay. Standard Malay refers to the Wilkinson dictionary in 1904. The participants of this study are seven native speakers of Palembang Malay whose ages range from 20 to 40 years old. The data were collected from the voices of the participants that were recorded along group conversations and interviews. This study applies the theoretical framework of sound changes which proposed by Terry Crowley in 1997 and Lily Campbell in 1999. The findings show that there are nine types of sound changes that were found as the results, namely assimilation (42.35%), lenition (20%), sound addition (3.53%), metathesis (1.18%), dissimilation (1.76%), abnormal sound changes (3.53%), split (13.53%), vowel rising (10.59%), and monophthongisation (3.53%). Keywords: Historical linguistics, standard Malay, Palembang Malay, comparative method, sound change, phoneme, syllable. 35 Novita Arsillah The Phonemic − Syllabic Comparisons of Standard Malay and Palembang Malay Using a Historical Linguistic Perspective INTRODUCTION spelling system in this study since it is considered to be the first Malay spelling This study classifies into the system that is used widely in Malaya, field of historical linguistics that Singapore, and Brunei (Omar, 1989).
    [Show full text]
  • Local Identity and Sound Change in Glasgow a Pilot Study1
    Local identity and sound change in Glasgow A pilot study1 Natalie Braber2 Zoe Butterfint3 Abstract This paper outlines a pilot study investigation into the potential link between local identity and language change in Glasgow. Results are presented from part of the pilot study, specifically the variation noted in two phonological variables - the realisation of the alveolar lateral approximant /l/, and the occurrence of so-called T-glottalling - and are discussed in the light of local identity. Glasgow is historically a heavily stigmatised, often stereotyped city and home to an equally stigmatised linguistic variety: Glaswegian. Recent investigations have highlighted processes of linguistic change occurring in this linguistic variety (most notable Stuart-Smith, 1999a, 2003; Stuart-Smith et al., 2006, 2007), and this study sets out to investigate the potential link between these changes, speaker attitudes to Glasgow and their sense of Glaswegian identity. The data elicitation method employed is an extended version of that used by Stuart- Smith & Tweedie (2000): semi-structured interviews supplemented by a read word list. Methodological issues and considerations for future investigation are discussed on the basis of the findings of this pilot study. 1. Introduction A wealth of literature exists examining the relationship between identity and linguistic change, from Labov’s pioneering investigation in Martha’s Vineyard (1963), to present-day research in Middlesborough (Llamas, 1999, 2007) and Berwick-upon- Tweed (Pichler & Watt. 2004; Pichler, Watt and Llamas, 2004). This paper discusses findings from a small-scale pilot study carried out in 2005/6, which aimed to extend that body of work, further investigating the complex ‘interdependence of language and place identity’ (Llamas, 2007:579) in one very specific locale: Glasgow.
    [Show full text]
  • Reverse Engineering: Emphatic Consonants and the Adaptation of Vowels in French Loanwords Into Moroccan Arabic *
    Brill’s Annual of Afroasiatic Languages and Linguistics 1 (2009) 41–74 brill.nl/baall Reverse Engineering: Emphatic Consonants and the Adaptation of Vowels in French Loanwords into Moroccan Arabic * Michael Kenstowicz and Nabila Louriz [email protected] [email protected] Abstract On the basis of two large corpora of French (and Spanish) loanwords into Moroccan Arabic, the paper documents and analyzes the phenomenon noted by Heath (1989) in which a pharyngeal- ized consonant is introduced in the adaptation of words with mid and low vowels such as moquette > [MokeT] = /MukiT/ ‘carpet’. It is found that French back vowels are readily adapted with pharyngealized emphatics while the front vowels tend to resist this correspondence. Th e implications of the phenomenon for general models of loanword adaptation are considered. It is concluded that auditory similarity and salience are critical alternative dimensions of faithfulness that may override correspondences based on phonologically contrastive features. Keywords pharyngealization , enhancement , auditory salience , weighted constraints , harmony 1. Introduction One of the most interesting questions in the theory of loanword adaptation concerns the role of redundant features. It is well known that phonological contrasts on consonants are often correlated with the realization of the same or a related (enhancing) feature on adjacent vowels. Such redundant proper- ties are known to play a role in speech perception and frequently share or take * A preliminary version of this paper was written while the second author was a Fulbright scholar at MIT (spring-summer 2008) and was presented at the Linguistics Colloquium, Paris 8 (November 2008). Th e paper was completed while the fi rst author was Visiting Professor at the Research Institute for Languages and Cultures of Asia and Africa, Tokyo University of Foreign Studies in the winter of 2008-9.
    [Show full text]
  • Historical Linguistics and the Comparative Study of African Languages
    Historical Linguistics and the Comparative Study of African Languages UNCORRECTED PROOFS © JOHN BENJAMINS PUBLISHING COMPANY 1st proofs UNCORRECTED PROOFS © JOHN BENJAMINS PUBLISHING COMPANY 1st proofs Historical Linguistics and the Comparative Study of African Languages Gerrit J. Dimmendaal University of Cologne John Benjamins Publishing Company Amsterdam / Philadelphia UNCORRECTED PROOFS © JOHN BENJAMINS PUBLISHING COMPANY 1st proofs TM The paper used in this publication meets the minimum requirements of American 8 National Standard for Information Sciences — Permanence of Paper for Printed Library Materials, ANSI Z39.48-1984. Library of Congress Cataloging-in-Publication Data Dimmendaal, Gerrit Jan. Historical linguistics and the comparative study of African languages / Gerrit J. Dimmendaal. p. cm. Includes bibliographical references and index. 1. African languages--Grammar, Comparative. 2. Historical linguistics. I. Title. PL8008.D56 2011 496--dc22 2011002759 isbn 978 90 272 1178 1 (Hb; alk. paper) isbn 978 90 272 1179 8 (Pb; alk. paper) isbn 978 90 272 8722 9 (Eb) © 2011 – John Benjamins B.V. No part of this book may be reproduced in any form, by print, photoprint, microfilm, or any other means, without written permission from the publisher. John Benjamins Publishing Company • P.O. Box 36224 • 1020 me Amsterdam • The Netherlands John Benjamins North America • P.O. Box 27519 • Philadelphia PA 19118-0519 • USA UNCORRECTED PROOFS © JOHN BENJAMINS PUBLISHING COMPANY 1st proofs Table of contents Preface ix Figures xiii Maps xv Tables
    [Show full text]
  • Ling 150, Historical Linguistics Moore, Winter 2013 Types of Sound Change Lenition
    Ling 150, Historical Linguistics Moore, Winter 2013 Types of Sound Change Lenition (1) Stronger Weaker voiced voiceless (sometimes) voiceless voiced (sometimes) stops fricatives obstruents sonorants consonants semivowels oral glottal front/back central (2) Lentition: Stronger > Weaker Kara (New Ireland) *bulan > fulan ‘moon’ *tapine > tefin ‘woman’ *punti > fut ‘banana’ (3) Rhotacism: C > r Latin *ami:kosum > ami:korum ‘of friends’ *genesis > generis ‘of the type’ Sound Loss (4) Aphaeresis: initial consonant deletion Angkamuthi (Cape York) *maji > aji ‘food’ *nani > ani ‘ground’ *wapun > apun ‘head’ (5) Apocope: final vowel loss S.E. Ambrym (Vanutu) *utu > ut ‘lice’ *aŋo > aŋ ‘fly’ *asue > asu ‘rat’ *tohu > toh ‘sugarcane’ Ling 150, Historical Linguistics Moore, Winter 2013 (6) Syncope: medial vowel loss Lenakel (Vanutu) *namatana > nɨmrɨn ‘his/her eye’ (note other changes) *nalimana > nelmɨn ‘his/her hand’ *masa > mha ‘low tide’ (7) Cluster reduction: CC >C English Melanesian Pidgin dɪstɹɪkt > distrik ‘district’ poʊst > pos ‘post’ peɩnt > pen ‘paint’ tæŋk > taŋ ‘tank’ (8) Haplology: syllable loss (conditioned by adjacent similar syllable) laɩbɹəɹi > laɩbɹi ‘library’ Anglaland > England Sound Addition (9) Excrescence: consonant addition *æmtig > ɛmpti ‘empty’ *θymle > θɪmbl ‘thimble’ (10) Epenthtesis: vowel addition English Tok Pisin blæk > bilak ‘black’ blu: > bulu ‘blue’ sɪks > sikis ‘six’ (11) Prothesis: initial sound addition Moru (Papua New Guinea) *api > lahi ‘fire’ *asan > lada ‘gills’ *au > lau ‘I, me’ Ling 150, Historical Linguistics Moore, Spring 2011 Metathesis Sounds switch places. (12) *brid > *bird > bɚd æsk > æks (13) Tagalog Ilokano taŋis : sa:ŋit ‘cry’ tubus : subut ‘redeem’ tigis : si:git ‘decant’ tamis : samqit ‘sweet’ Fusion Two sounds become one, bearing features of both original sounds.
    [Show full text]
  • Trask's Historical Linguistics
    Trask’s Historical Linguistics Trask’s Historical Linguistics, Third Edition, is an accessible introduction to historical linguistics – the study of language change over time. This engaging book is illustrated with language examples from all six continents, and covers the fundamental concepts of language change, methods for historical linguistics, linguistic reconstruction, sociolinguistic aspects of language change, language contact, the birth and death of languages, language and prehistory and the issue of very remote relations. This third edition of the renowned Trask’s Historical Linguistics is fully revised and updated and covers the most recent developments in historical linguistics, including: ᭹ more detail on morphological change including cutting-edge discussions of iconization ᭹ coverage of recent developments in sociolinguistic explanations of variation and change ᭹ new case studies focusing on Germanic languages and American and New Zealand English, and updated exercises covering each of the topics within the book ᭹ a brand new companion website featuring material for both professors and students, including discussion questions and exercises as well as discussions of the exercises within the book. Trask’s Historical Linguistics is essential reading for all students of language, linguistics and related disciplines. The accompanying website can be found at www.routledge.com/cw/trask Robert McColl Millar is Professor in Linguistics and Scottish Language at the University of Aberdeen. His most recent books include English Historical Sociolinguistics (2012) and (with William Barras and Lisa Marie Bonnici) Lexical Variation and Attrition in the Scottish Fishing Communities (2014). Larry Trask was Professor of Linguistics at the University of Sussex and an authority on Basque language and historical linguistics.
    [Show full text]
  • Productivity in Historical Linguistics Computational Perspectives on Word-Formation in Ancient Greek and Sanskrit
    University of California Los Angeles Productivity in Historical Linguistics Computational Perspectives on Word-Formation in Ancient Greek and Sanskrit A dissertation submitted in partial satisfaction of the requirements for the degree Doctor of Philosophy in Indo-European Studies by Ryan Paul Sandell 2015 © Copyright by Ryan Paul Sandell 2015 Abstract of the Dissertation Productivity in Historical Linguistics Computational Perspectives on Word-Formation in Ancient Greek and Sanskrit by Ryan Paul Sandell Doctor of Philosophy in Indo-European Studies University of California, Los Angeles, 2015 Professor Brent Harmon Vine, Chair “Productivity” is a simultaneously familiar and fraught topic for all linguists. Every linguist believes that he can recognize it, yet grasping what properties characterize a “productive” process in opposition to a non-productive one is difficult. The historical linguist ought to be particularly desperate for a definition that can be operationalized – distinguishing ar- chaism from innovation depends upon it. This dissertation is therefore first concerned with transforming “productivity” into an object that can be interrogated, and then seeking tools that can provide a useful characterization that object. On the explicitly diachronic side, I am concerned with how to measure, diagnose, and motivate changes in productivity. Cor- pora from two the oldest-attested Indo-European languages, Ancient Greek (here, mainly the Iliad and Odyssey, as well as the New Testament) and Vedic Sanskrit (here, mainly the R̥gveda) will guide and define these explorations. Within the realm of morphology and word- formation especially, I will argue that concerns about productivity rightly take pride of place in diachronic discussion, and that those discussions become more meaningful when made precise and psychologically motivated.
    [Show full text]
  • 2 Exploring Variation in Phonetic Reduction: Linguistic, Social, and Cognitive Factors
    Cynthia G. Clopper and Rory Turnbull 2 Exploring variation in phonetic reduction: Linguistic, social, and cognitive factors Abstract: Substantial empirical research has revealed that temporal and spec- tral phonetic vowel reduction occurs in “easy” processing contexts relative to “hard” processing contexts, including effects of lexical frequency, lexical neigh- borhood density, semantic predictability, discourse mention, and speaking style. Theoretical accounts of this phonetic reduction process include listener-oriented approaches, in which the reduction reflects the talker’s balancing the compre- hension needs of the listener with production effort constraints, talker-oriented approaches, in which reduction is argued to result entirely from constraints on speech production processes, and evolutionary approaches, in which reduction results directly from long-term interactive communication within a community. Recent research in our laboratory has revealed complex interactions among the linguistic, social, and cognitive factors involved in phonetic vowel reduction pro- cesses. These interactions reveal variation in the robustness of phonetic reduc- tion effects across linguistic factors, as well as different patterns of interactions among linguistic, social, and cognitive factors across acoustic domains. These interactions challenge aspects of each of the three existing models of phonetic reduction. We therefore propose that a more complex view of the relationship between processing demands and phonetic vowel reduction processes is neces-
    [Show full text]
  • The English Sound Changes Among Javanese Students of the English Language Education Study Program of Sanata Dharma University
    PLAGIAT MERUPAKAN TINDAKAN TIDAK TERPUJI THE ENGLISH SOUND CHANGES AMONG JAVANESE STUDENTS OF THE ENGLISH LANGUAGE EDUCATION STUDY PROGRAM OF SANATA DHARMA UNIVERSITY A SARJANA PENDIDIKAN THESIS Presented as a Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements to Obtain a Sarjana Pendidikan Degree in English Language Education By Rafael Marion Galley Margana Student Number: 111214171 ENGLISH LANGUAGE EDUCATION STUDY PROGRAM DEPARTMENT OF LANGUAGE AND ARTS EDUCATION FACULTY OF TEACHERS TRAINING AND EDUCATION SANATA DHARMA UNIVERSITY YOGYAKARTA 2016 PLAGIAT MERUPAKAN TINDAKAN TIDAK TERPUJI THE ENGLISH SOUND CHANGES AMONG JAVANESE STUDENTS OF THE ENGLISH LANGUAGE EDUCATION STUDY PROGRAM OF SANATA DHARMA UNIVERSITY A SARJANA PENDIDIKAN THESIS Presented as a Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements to Obtain a Sarjana Pendidikan Degree in English Language Education By Rafael Marion Galley Margana Student Number: 111214171 ENGLISH LANGUAGE EDUCATION STUDY PROGRAM DEPARTMENT OF LANGUAGE AND ARTS EDUCATION FACULTY OF TEACHERS TRAINING AND EDUCATION SANATA DHARMA UNIVERSITY YOGYAKARTA 2016 i PLAGIAT MERUPAKAN TINDAKAN TIDAK TERPUJI ASarjana Pmdidilan Ttesis on TIIE ENGLISH SOUND CIIANGES AMONG JAVANESE STUI}ENTS OT'TIIE ENGLISH LANGUAGE EDUCATION STUDY PROGRAM OF'SANATA DHAR]VIA UNTVERSITY By Rafael lv{arion Galley Margana Studert Ntrmber: l1l2l4l7 I Approved by Date FX. Ouda Teda Ena, S.Pd., M.pd., Ed.D. 14 Octobsr2016 1l PLAGIAT MERUPAKAN TINDAKAN TIDAK TERPUJI THE ENGLISH SOUND CHANGES AMONG JAVANESE STUDENTS OF'THE ENGLISH LANGUAGE EDUCATION STUI}Y PROGRAM OF'SANATA DHARMA UI{IVEBSITY By RAFAEL MARION GALLEY MARGANA Student Number: lll2l4l7 I Defended before the Board of Examiners on November 8e 2016 and Declared Acceptable Board of Examiners Chairperson : Yohana Venirand4 S.pd., M.Hum., ph.D.
    [Show full text]
  • Constraining Exceptionality As Prosody-Morphology Mismatch: a Study of French Nasal Vowels
    Constraining exceptionality as prosody-morphology mismatch: a study of French nasal vowels Brian Hsu University of Southern California 1 Introduction Instances of morpheme-specific phonology, where individual words or morphemes are associated with unique phonological patterns, have been a longstanding problem in generative phonology (Pater 2000; Mascaró 2007; Anttila 2002). The proposals of the paper are two-fold. First, I present a theory of prosodically-indexed markedness constraints as a means of deriving the sensitivity of phonotactic restrictions to prosodic structure. Second, I propose that exceptional allomorphy may arise through prosody-morphology mismatches which reduce prosodic boundary sizes. The workings of the theory are illustrated by an analysis of nasal vowels in French, accounting for variability in both prefix allomorphy and liaison, cases that have not previously received a uniform treatment (Côté, 2011; Tranel, 1981, and refences therein). This paper is organized as follows. The remainder of section 1 summarizes the basic distribution of nasal vowels in Standard French, and presents an account using prosodically-indexed constraints. Exceptions to the general pattern are proposed to be derived through a prosodic structure mismatch. The analysis is developed in greater depth in sections 2 and 3. Section 2 presents a description and analysis of the morpheme-internal distribution of nasal vowels. Section 3 discusses regular patterns of prefix allomorphy as compared with the exceptional prefix in-. Section 4 extends the proposals to account for liaison, analyzed as an exceptional pattern. Section 5 concludes the paper. 1.1 French nasal vowels and prosodic constraint indexation There are three basic distributions of nasal vowels and nasal consonants in French.
    [Show full text]
  • Cg> and Its Sound Correspondences in Old English and Middle English
    Old English <cg> and its sound correspondences in Old English and Middle English Gjertrud F. Stenbrenden, University of Oslo 1. Introduction1 Most elementary grammars of Old English (OE), as well as textbooks on the history of English, state that the digraph <cg> was pronounced as [dʒ], that is, a voiced post-alveolar affricate (Sweet/Davis 1983: 4; Quirk & Wrenn 1989: 16; Mitchell & Robinson 1992: 16). Even the reference grammars, however, provide little evidence in support of this claim, beyond (a) a few seeming cases of affrication across OE morpheme boundaries (so, e.g. micgern ‘fat’ from *mid+gern, cf. orceard ‘orchard’ from *ort+geard), and (b) the reflex of this segment as [dʒ] in Present-Day English (PDE): so, for example, Campbell (1959: 173-179), Wright & Wright (1982: 167), Sievers (1968: 143-155). Rather, the assumed development of [dʒ] in OE, along with that of its voiceless counterpart [tʃ], is presented as an outcome of the Anglo-Frisian palatalisations of [k] and [ɡ] in contact with front vowels: this makes it cohere with a larger chapter in the history of OE phonology, on which it then seems to depend. Otherwise, the topic looks to have attracted little further attention, with the exceptions of Weɫna (1986) and Minkova (2003, 2014, 2016, 2019). Nevertheless, in its own terms the OE digraph <cg> seems remarkably ill-suited as the correspondent of a sound-segment [dʒ]. OE <c> usually corresponds to [k] or its palatalised reflex [tʃ]/[ç]2 (as well as, though mainly in early Old Northumbrian in absolute final position in unstressed words, the fricatives [ç] and [x]), whereas OE <g> corresponds to [ɣ] or its palatalised reflex [j], or to [ɡ] or its palatalised reflex [ɟ] (at first only in combination with [ŋ]).
    [Show full text]
  • Implications for Mechanisms of Tonogenesis
    The emergence, progress, and impact of sound change in progress in Seoul Korean: implications for mechanisms of tonogenesis Hye-Young Bang∗1, Morgan Sonderegger1,2, Yoonjung Kang3,4, Meghan Clayards1,2, and Tae-Jin Yoon5 1Department of Linguistics, McGill University, Canada 2Centre for Research on Brain, Language, and Music, McGill University, Canada 3Centre for French and Linguistics, University of Toronto Scarborough, Canada 4Department of Linguistics, University of Toronto, Canada 5Department of English Language and Literature, Sungshin Women's University, Korea Abstract This study examines the origin, progression, and impact of a sound change in Seoul Korean where the primary cue to a stop contrast in phrase-initial position is shifting from VOT to f0. Because it shares similarities with the initial phase of tonogenesis, investigating this \quasi-tonogenetic" sound change provides insight into the nature of the emergence of contrastive f0 in \tonogenetic" sound changes more generally. Using a dataset from a large apparent-time corpus of Seoul Korean, we built mixed-effects regression models of VOT and f0 to examine the time-course of change, focusing on word frequency and vowel height effects. We found that both VOT contrast reduction and f0 contrast enhancement are more advanced in high-frequency words and in stops before non-high vowels, indicating that the change is spreading across words and phonetic contexts in parallel. Furthermore, speakers suppress non-contrastive variation in f0 as f0 emerges as a primary cue. Our findings suggest that one impetus for tonogenetic change is production bias coupled with an adaptive link between the cues. We further discuss the role of Korean intonational phonology on f0 which may help explain why the phonetic precondition leads to change in Seoul Korean but not in other languages.
    [Show full text]