Pre Test Excerpt
Total Page:16
File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb
Cognitive and Cultural Diversity in Human Evolution Larissa Mendoza Straffon ([email protected]) Centre for Early Sapiens Behaviour, SapienCE University of Bergen, Norway Abstract sociality. Accordingly, their emergence has been assigned a late evolutionary date, with or after the appearance of H. Most well-accepted models of cognitive evolution define the modern human mind in terms of an amalgamation of species- sapiens, between 200 and 100,000 years ago (McBrearty & specific cognitive mechanisms, many of which are described as Brooks, 2000). adaptive. Likewise, these models often use the rich archaeological The timing of the origin of such adaptations is sometimes record of Homo sapiens to illustrate how ‘uniquely human’ mental estimated on the basis of indirect archaeological evidence, abilities gave our species an evolutionary advantage over extinct for example by using art or personal ornaments as proxies hominins. Recent evidence from various fields, however, indicates for language and symbolic thought (d’Errico et al., 2005), that closely related species, particularly Neanderthals and and other times just by inferring that due to their current Denisovans, likely had cognitive capacities very similar to ours, universality these traits must have been in place by the time and that several key aspects of ‘modern’ cognition are not H. sapiens migrated out of Africa, minimally 100,000 years exclusive to our lineage. The sum of these data therefore requires a timely revision of human cognitive evolution models. On the one ago (Mellars, 2006). This in turn has fuelled ongoing hand, claims of species-specific cognitive mechanisms have been discussions of whether the modern human mind and body weakened. On the other hand, there are tangible differences among evolved gradually together (McBrearty & Brooks, 2000) or extinct and extant humans that call for an explanation. One way to if there was a lag between anatomical and accommodate these differences is to understand cognition as cognitive/behavioural modernity, with the latter evolving shaped by sociocultural and environmental factors, and to argue for much later than the former (Tattersall, 2017). culture-specific rather than species-specific cognition over the However, a growing number of archaeological finds course of human evolution. suggest that many of the allegedly species-specific Keywords: hominin cognition, human evolution, cognitive adaptations evolved neither with nor after the emergence of archaeology, culture, cognitive diversity our species,1 but prior to it. In fact, they were likely shared with other ‘archaic’ humans, in particular Neanderthals and Introduction perhaps Denisovans, whose cognitive capacities appear to For decades, scholars have warned against the implicit have been not too dissimilar to ours (Breyl, 20202; Shea biases that span interpretations in palaeoanthropology and 2011). Therefore, such traits can no longer be held as unique particularly in cognitive archaeology, such as those to H. sapiens and cannot by themselves explain our implying that modern humans embody the pinnacle of ‘success’ as a species. evolution (Cartmill, 1990; Corbey & Roebroeks, 2001; Gould, 1990; Landau, 1993). Despite increasing awareness 1Following the phylogenetic species concept, a species is here of these issues, recent reviews have found that the field of understood as a lineage of organisms, distinguished from other human cognitive evolution is still largely dominated by lineages by its evolutionary trajectory, bound in time by its narratives which ultimately propose that our species has origin in a speciation event and its eventual disappearance by been endowed with certain abilities that gave it an further speciation or extinction (Sterelny & Griffiths, 1999). evolutionary edge and allowed it to survive and outcompete Accordingly, Neanderthals, Denisovans and Homo sapiens are considered separate species (Stringer, 2012). This means that extinct hominins (Barham & Everett, 2020; Breyl, 2020; whether these other human groups were absorbed by modern Langbroek, 2012). African populations or died out, they constitute separate lineages Those supposedly advantageous abilities, often referred to by virtue of their own particular evolutionary paths, which as ‘species-specific’ behavioural or cognitive adaptations diverged for at least 400,000 years, since the split from a last (Tooby & Cosmides, 1992), include among others, common ancestor (Hublin, 2013). Therefore, in this paper ‘our imitation, pedagogy, language, theory of mind, and species’ refers to all individuals classified as H. sapiens (aka symbolic thinking. Namely, traits which are thought to modern humans), from the Jebel Irhoud fossils c.300,000 BP underlie much of modern human complex culture and (Hublin et al., 2017) to contemporary populations. 541 Hominin Brains and Complex Behaviour cubic cm in H. sapiens), their brain structure was more similar to ours. That is, naledi had a relatively well- Although several researchers have long argued for the developed frontal cortex, related to sociality, and presented cognitive and behavioural complexity of Neanderthals and an occipital petalial asymmetry, related to language ability other extinct hominins (Villa & Roebroeks, 2014), a series (Holloway et al., 2018). Because naledi is a distant relative, of discoveries over the past few years have lent strong these data suggest that current brain structure may have support to those appeals. In addition to long-known facts featured since the beginning of the hominin clade and that such as the Neanderthal’s big brains, complex hunting behaviours characteristic of Homo like tool-making, strategies and technologies, and high social skills sociality, and hunting may not be the result of the evolution (Roebroeks & Soressi, 2016), it has now been confirmed of cognition but might themselves have acted as selective that they were able to control fire (Henry, 2017), made pressures for brain organization (Holloway et al., 2018). adhesives and produced composite tools of wood and stone H. naledi thus demonstrates that, a) encephalization was (Niekus et al., 2019), made rope and perhaps textiles out of not a generalized trend in Homo (Montgomery, 2018) as it vegetal fibres (Hardy et al., 2020), created and frequented has been assumed (Bruner et al., 2003); b) that the ritual spaces (Jaubert et al., 2016), made and used pigments Pleistocene African environment did not select strictly for and created some visual symbols (Hoffmann et al., 2018; big brains (Dusseldorp & Lombard, 2021); and c) that a Rodriguez et al., 2014). It is argued that many of these large brain may not be a prerequisite for (rudimentary) behaviours entail higher cognitive abilities (but see Tennie language ability (Holloway et al., 2018). It also suggests et al., 2016) as well as cultural strategies for information that key neurological aspects of cognition, such as general transfer such as pedagogical instruction and potential ‘funds brain size and structure, and likely behavioural ones too, of knowledge’ (Moll et al., 1992). may be traced back to the very start of the Homo genus As far as Denisovans are concerned, not yet much is (Stade, 2020). known but the fact that they interbred with both Neanderthals and modern humans may be used as an indicator of the probable abilities of those mysterious Modern Human Cognition and Culture hominins (Reich et al. 2010). Furthermore, they seem to So, what, if anything, could be unique about modern human have produced visual symbols similar to those made by cognition? Given the previous discussion, it has become Neanderthals and early modern humans (Li et al., 2019). clear that any hypotheses of species-specific cognitive or The issue of the chronology of language in our lineage is behavioural adaptations must either be supported by still open to debate (Fitch, 2017). Nevertheless, a large evidence or predicted by it. Clearly, we can no longer corpus of (indirect) evidence supports the presence of provide lists of traits presumed to be exclusive to modern complex linguistic capacities among extinct hominin species humans and then simply reverse-engineer multiple likely (Barham & Everett 2020; Dediu and Levinson, 2013). In functions (which often leads to unfalsifiable ‘just-so any case, all the adaptations required for complex stories’). If we propose that a trait evolved only in H. vocalization were already in place by the time of the sapiens, we need to back the argument with data preferably assumed last common ancestor of Neanderthals and H. from different fields, and put forward possible selective sapiens, H. erectus, some half a million years ago, if not contexts that would have led to the emergence of such trait earlier (de Boer, 2017). In addition, many of the hominin in our species and not in other related hominins (a testable behaviours mentioned above, like making composite tools, prediction). That is, we need to argue why the trait appeared may require verbal instruction (Ambrose, 2010). Finally, the when it did (within the evolutionary range of our species, genetic contribution of extinct hominins to current over the past 300,000 years) and how it may have yielded an populations is another clue that they were probably capable advantage to H. sapiens populations in particular. of communicating and sharing information with modern Preferably, such explanations should also incorporate humans through language (Mafessoni 2019; Villanea & archaeological data, as this is the only empirical evidence Schraiber,