Do Stroke Patients Require Vision Assessment?

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Do Stroke Patients Require Vision Assessment? Age and Ageing 2009; 38: 188–193 C The Author 2008. Published by Oxford University Press on behalf of the British Geriatrics Society. doi: 10.1093/ageing/afn230 All rights reserved. For Permissions, please email: [email protected] Published electronically 21 November 2008 Visual impairment following stroke: do stroke patients require vision assessment? FIONA ROWE1,DARREN BRAND2,CAROLE A. JACKSON2,ALISON PRICE3,LINDA WALKER4, SHIRLEY HARRISON5,CARLA ECCLESTON6,CLAIRE SCOTT7,NICOLA AKERMAN8, CAROLINE DODRIDGE9,CLAIRE HOWARD10,TRACEY SHIPMAN11,UNA SPERRING12, SONIA MACDIARMID13,CICELY FREEMAN14 1 Directorate of Orthoptics and Vision Science, University of Liverpool, Thompson Yates Building, Brownlow Hill, Liverpool L69 Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/ageing/article/38/2/188/16402 by guest on 29 September 2021 3GB, UK 2Department of Orthoptics, Royal United Hospitals NHS Trust, Combe Park, Bath BA1 3NG, UK 3Department of Orthoptics, Sandwell and West Birmingham NHS Trust, City Hospital, Birmingham B18 7QH, UK 4Department of Orthoptics, East Lancashire Hospitals NHS Trust, Royal Blackburn Hospital, Lancashire, BB2 3HH, UK 5Department of Orthoptics, Bury PCT, Bury, Lancashire BL9 0EN, UK 6Department of Orthoptics, Derby Hospitals NHS Foundation Trust, Derby City General Hospital, Derby, Derbyshire, DE22 3NE, UK 7Department of Orthoptics, Ipswich Hospital NHS Trust, Ipswich, Suffolk IP4 5PD, UK 8Department of Orthoptics, University Hospitals NHS Trust, Southampton, Hampshire SO16 6YD, UK 9Department of Orthoptics, Oxford Radcliffe Hospitals NHS Trust, Headley Way Headington, Oxford OX3 9DU, UK 10Department of Orthoptics, Salford Primary Care Trust, Pendleton Way, Salford M6 5FW, UK 11Department of Orthoptics, Sheffield Teaching Hospitals Foundation Trust, Glossop Road, Sheffield S10 2JF, UK 12Department of Orthoptics, Swindon and Marlborough NHS Trust, Marlborough Road, Swindon SN3 6BB, UK 13Department of Orthoptics, Wrightington, Wigan and Leigh NHS Trust, Wigan Lane, Wigan WN1 2NN, UK 14Department of Orthoptics, Worcestershire Acute Hospitals NHS Trust, Charles Hasting Way, Worcester WR5 1DD, UK Address correspondence to: F. Rowe. Tel: (+44) 0151 7945732; Fax: (+44) 0151 7945781. Email: [email protected] Abstract Background: the types of visual impairment followings stroke are wide ranging and encompass low vision, eye movement and visual field abnormalities, and visual perceptual difficuilties. Objective: the purpose of this paper is to present a 1-year data set and identify the types of visual impairment occurring following stroke and their prevalence. Methods: a multi-centre prospective observation study was undertaken in 14 acute trust hospitals. Stroke survivors with a suspected visual difficulty were recruited. Standardised screening/referral and investigation forms were employed to document data on visual impairment specifically assessment of visual acuity, ocular pathology, eye alignment and movement, visual perception (including inattention) and visual field defects. Results: three hundred and twenty-three patients were recruited with a mean age of 69 years [standard deviation (SD) 15]. Sixty-eight per cent had eye alignment/movement impairment, 49% had visual field impairment, 26.5% had low vision and 20.5% had perceptual difficulties. Conclusions: of patients referred with a suspected visual difficulty, only 8% had normal vision status confirmed on exam- ination. Ninety-two per cent had visual impairment of some form confirmed which is considerably higher than previous publications and probably relates to the prospective, standardised investigation offered by specialist orthoptists. However, under-ascertainment of visual problems cannot be ruled out. Keywords: visual impairment, stroke, ocular motility, visual field, visual perception, elderly Introduction Low vision can relate to associated vascular pathology or to other co-existent ocular abnormalities [10]. Visual field Every year, an estimated 110,000 people in the UK have loss commonly will involve a homonymous hemianopia or a stroke [1]. The visual sequelae of stroke are many [2–9]. quadrantanopia [3, 4, 7, 12]. Ocular motility disorders can be 188 Visual impairment following stroke divided into cortical deficits of eye movements and brain stem signs, investigation of visual field, ocular motility and percep- defects. Visual perception difficulties are wide ranging [13]. tual aspects. Visual fields are assessed by confrontation if the The most commonly recognised is visual inattention/neglect patient is seen on the ward or rehabilitation unit. When seen [8, 9]. in clinic, quantitative measures of visual field are undertaken Visual problems such as complete hemianopia or large by Humphrey automated perimeter and Goldmann manual strabismus can be spotted easily. Subtle disorders such as perimeter. Complete homonymous hemianopia was defined impaired fast eye movements and gaze defects can be diffi- as loss of visual field to one side from central fixation and the cult to identify without correct evaluation of the visual sys- vertical meridian of the field outwards. Partial homonymous tem. A significant proportion of patients in stroke units have hemianopia was defined as loss of visual field to one side that unrecognised visual problems resulting in little or no advice was incomplete with some residual vision on the affected side or management [2, 14]. The eye-care team can address many near the vertical meridian of the field. of these issues directly along with timely feedback of such Assessment of ocular alignment and motility consists information to other health professionals involved in the care of cover test, evaluation of saccadic, smooth pursuit and Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/ageing/article/38/2/188/16402 by guest on 29 September 2021 of these patients [11]. vergence eye movements, retinal correspondence (Bagolini The impact of visual impairment can be wide ranging. glasses), fusional vergence (20D or fusional range), stereopsis The impact on functional performance can include general (Frisby near test), prism cover test and lid and pupil function. mobility, ability to judge distances due to diplopia or impaired Perceptual deficits are recorded after questioning of the stereo vision, reading impairment due to cortical or ocular patient and/or carers and relatives. Inattention is assessed dysfunction and visual hallucinations. Impact to quality of by means of a combination of assessments including line life includes changes to independent living, ability to drive, bisection, Albert’s test, cancellation tests, memory tests using loss of confidence and links to depression [16–20]. verbal description and drawing. Although it is recognised that many patients will suffer Visual acuity was assessed at near and distance fixation visual disability as a result of stroke, data are lacking as to the with Snellen or logMAR acuity tests. Low visual acuity was prevalence of disability, extent and recovery. There are little considered in two categories. The first defined low visual data with respect to strabismus and ocular motility disorders acuity as less than best corrected 6/12 Snellens acuity or following stroke [3–5, 21]. 0.3 logMAR in accordance with UK driving standards [22]. In May 2006, a prospective multi-centre trial commenced The second defined low visual acuity as <6/18 Snellens acuity in the UK. The primary aim is to evaluate the effect of or 0.5 logMAR and equal to or better than 3/60 Snellens stroke on vision: specifically ocular perception, alignment acuity as per World Health Organisation (WHO) guidelines and movement, how these impact on quality of life and the [23]. Additionally, WHO define low vision as a corresponding information that is required from visual assessment for the visual field loss to <20 degrees in the best corrected eye. multidisciplinary team. The purpose of this paper is to high- WHO define blindness as a visual acuity of <3/60 Snellens light the extent of visual impairment occurring within this acuity or a corresponding visual field loss to <10 degrees in stroke population. the best corrected eye. Stroke details are recorded from patient notes accounting for stroke laterality, type and area involved. Ocular treatment Methods and materials details are recorded along with outcome. Results were inputted to the statistical package SPSS The design is a prospective multi-centre observational case version 15. cohort trial with ethical approval. The vision in stroke (VIS) group consists of local investigators responsible for assess- ing stroke patients and collecting patient data. The data are Results collated centrally at the University of Liverpool. The study is being undertaken in accordance with the Tenets of Helsinki. Data are presented from a 1-year data set: 1 May 2006 to The target population was stroke patients suspected of 30 April 2007. Five hundred and one patients were referred having a visual difficulty. Referrals could be made from for visual assessment. One hundred and seventy-eight pa- inpatient wards, rehabilitation units, community services or tients were excluded (49% male, 51% female: Table 1). Pa- outpatient clinics. Patients were given an information sheet tients were not excluded because of the type or severity of and recruited after informed, written consent. The patients were excluded if they were unable to consent, unwilling to Table 1. Reasons for exclusion consent, if their diagnosis was that of transient ischaemic attack or if they were discharged without vision assessment. Inability to consent (cognitive
Recommended publications
  • Prosopagnosia by B
    J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiat., 1959, 22, 124. PROSOPAGNOSIA BY B. BORNSTEIN and D. P. KIDRON From the Department of Neurology, Beilinson Hospital, Petah Tiqva, Israel "And what is the nature of this knowledge or recollection? I mean to ask, Whether a person, who having seen or heard or in any way perceived anything, knows not only that, but has a conception of something else which is the subject, not of the same but of some other kind of knowledge, may not be fairly said to recollect that of which he has the conception?" "And when the recollection is derived from like things, then another consideration is sure to arise, which is, Whether the likeness in any degree falls short or not of that which is recollected?" "The Philosophy of Plato " Phaedo (the Jowett translation). Does visual agnosia exist in a partial or isolated bances in sensation time, in adaptation time, in form, in which certain qualities only are affected, visual acuity, and in brightness discrimination. as opposed to generalized visual agnosia? Many Ettlinger (1956) rejected Bay's contentions. After workers cast doubt on this concept, maintaining analysing 30 cases of head injury, he showed that that partial visual agnosia is no more than a com- some patients had neither field nor perceptual bination of defects in vision, memory, and orienta- defects, others had field but not perceptual defects, tion, appearing together. and only in eight of the 30 patients were field and The clinical elucidation of partial visual agnosia perceptual defects found together. It is true that is likely to be affected by the patient's intellectual visual agnosia is frequently associated with homony- capacity, his mental state at the time of examination, mous hemianopsia, but despite this there are cases and his ability to cooperate without being influenced of hemianopsia without gnostic defects.
    [Show full text]
  • THE CLINICAL ASSESSMENT of the PATIENT with EARLY DEMENTIA S Cooper, J D W Greene V15
    J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry: first published as 10.1136/jnnp.2005.081133 on 16 November 2005. Downloaded from THE CLINICAL ASSESSMENT OF THE PATIENT WITH EARLY DEMENTIA S Cooper, J D W Greene v15 J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 2005;76(Suppl V):v15–v24. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.2005.081133 ementia is a clinical state characterised by a loss of function in at least two cognitive domains. When making a diagnosis of dementia, features to look for include memory Dimpairment and at least one of the following: aphasia, apraxia, agnosia and/or disturbances in executive functioning. To be significant the impairments should be severe enough to cause problems with social and occupational functioning and the decline must have occurred from a previously higher level. It is important to exclude delirium when considering such a diagnosis. When approaching the patient with a possible dementia, taking a careful history is paramount. Clues to the nature and aetiology of the disorder are often found following careful consultation with the patient and carer. A focused cognitive and physical examination is useful and the presence of specific features may aid in diagnosis. Certain investigations are mandatory and additional tests are recommended if the history and examination indicate particular aetiologies. It is useful when assessing a patient with cognitive impairment in the clinic to consider the following straightforward questions: c Is the patient demented? c If so, does the loss of function conform to a characteristic pattern? c Does the pattern of dementia conform to a particular pattern? c What is the likely disease process responsible for the dementia? An understanding of cognitive function and its anatomical correlates is necessary in order to ascertain which brain areas are affected.
    [Show full text]
  • Disorders of the Visual System in Alzheimer's Disease
    © 1990 Raven Press, Ltd.. New York Disorders of the Visual System in Alzheimer's Disease Mario F. Mendez, M.D., Robert L. Tomsak, M.D., Ph.D., and Bernd Remler, M.D. Alzheimer's disease (AD) is associated with distur­ Alzheimer's disease (AD) is the most prevalent bances in basic visual, complex visual, and oculomotor form of dementia affecting greater than 2.5 million functions. The broad range of visual system disorders in AD may result from the concentration of neuropathol­ people in the U.S., with the numbers expected to ogy in visual association cortex and optic nerves in this double by the year 2040 (1). Despite the absence of disease. AD patients and their caregivers frequently re­ a clinical test for AD, the recent establishment of port visuospatial difficulties in these patients. Examina­ highly accurate clinical criteria permit a more pre­ tion of the visual system in AD may reveal visual field cise evaluation of the deficits associated with this deficits, prolonged visual evoked potentials, depressed contrast sensitivities, and abnormal eye movement re­ disorder (2-4) (see Table 1). In addition to the cordings. Complex visual disturbances include construc­ usual memory and other cognitive deficits, AD pa­ tional and visuoperceptual abnormalities, spatial agno­ tients have disturbances in basic visual, complex sia and Balint's syndrome, environmental disorienta­ visual, and oculomotor functions, and AD patients tion, visual agnosia, facial identification problems, and in greater numbers are undergoing more thorough visual hallucinations. The purpose of this article is to review the spectrum of visual system disturbances evaluations of their visual systems (4-8).
    [Show full text]
  • DISORDERS of AUDITORY PROCESSING: EVIDENCE for MODULARITY in AUDITION Michael R
    DISORDERS OF AUDITORY PROCESSING: EVIDENCE FOR MODULARITY IN AUDITION Michael R. Polster and Sally B. Rose (Psychology Department, Victoria University of Wellington, Wellington, New Zealand) ABSTRACT This article examines four disorders of auditory processing that can result from selective brain damage (cortical deafness, pure word deafness, auditory agnosia and phonagnosia) in an effort to derive a plausible functional and neuroanatomical model of audition. The article begins by identifying three possible reasons why models of auditory processing have been slower to emerge than models of visual processing: neuroanatomical differences between the visual and auditory systems, terminological confusions relating to auditory processing disorders, and technical factors that have made auditory stimuli more difficult to study than visual stimuli. The four auditory disorders are then reviewed and current theories of auditory processing considered. Taken together, these disorders suggest a modular architecture analogous to models of visual processing that have been derived from studying neurological patients. Ideas for future research to test modular theory more fully are presented. Key words: auditory processing, modularity, review INTRODUCTION Neuropsychological investigations of patients suffering from brain damage have flourished in recent years and helped to produce more detailed and neuroanatomically plausible models of several aspects of cognitive function. For example, models of language processing are often closely aligned with studies of aphasia (e.g., Caplan, 1987; Goodglass, 1993) and models of memory draw heavily upon studies of amnesia (e.g., Schacter and Tulving, 1994; Squire, 1987). Most of this research has relied on visually presented materials, and as a result visual processing disorders tend to be more well-documented and better understood than their auditory counterparts.
    [Show full text]
  • Transient Gerstmann Syndrome As Manifestation of Stroke Case Report and Brief Literature Review
    Dement Neuropsychol 2017 June;11(2):202-205 Case Report doi: 10.1590/1980-57642016dn11-020013 Transient Gerstmann syndrome as manifestation of stroke Case report and brief literature review Rafael Batista João1, Raquel Mattos Filgueiras2, Marcelo Lucci Mussi3, João Eliezer Ferri de Barros4 ABSTRACT. Gerstmann Syndrome (GS) is a rare neurological condition described as a group of cognitive changes corresponding to a tetrad of symptoms comprising agraphia, acalculia, right-left disorientation and finger agnosia. It is known that some specific brain lesions may lead to such findings, particularly when there is impairment of the angular gyrus and adjacent structures. In addition, the possibility of disconnection syndrome should be considered in some cases. The purpose of this article is to report a case of a young, cardiac patient, non-adherent to treatment, who presented with a stroke in which transient clinical symptoms were compatible with the tetrad of GS. The case report is followed by a discussion and brief review of the relevant literature. Key words: Gerstmann syndrome, disconnection syndrome, insular cortex, parietal lobe, frontal lobe. SÍNDROME DE GERSTMANN TRANSITÓRIA COMO MANIFESTAÇÃO DE ACIDENTE VASCULAR ENCEFÁLICO: RELATO DE CASO E BREVE REVISÃO DE LITERATURA RESUMO. A síndrome de Gerstmann (SG) é uma condição neurológica rara, caracterizada por um grupo de alterações cognitivas que correspondem a uma tétrade composta por agrafia, acalculia, desorientação direita-esquerda e agnosia para dedos. Sabe-se que certas lesões encefálicas específicas podem levar a tais achados, particularmente quando ocorre acometimento do giro angular e estruturas adjacentes. Além disso, a possibilidade de síndrome de desconexão deve ser considerada em alguns casos.
    [Show full text]
  • 2-2-Patterns Neuropsychological Data Agnosia Patient GS
    Neuropsychological Data • Agnosia • Term coined by Sigmund Freud 2-2-patterns • From the Greek word for “lack of knowledge” • The inability to recognize objects when using a given sense (e.g. vision), even though that sense is basically intact (Nolte, 1999) Agnosia Patient GS • Sensory abilities intact • Language normal • Usually involves damage to the occipito-parietal • Unable to name objects pathway Agnosia Prosopagnosia • Apperceptive • Specific inability to recognize faces – Object recognition failure due to perceptual processing – Difficulty recognizing pictures w/deleted segments • Are faces and other objects in the world – Unable to utilize top-down information for pattern recognition represented in fundamentally different • Associative – Perceptual processing intact but subject cannot use information ways in memory? to recognize objects – Can draw objects but not say what they are • Does face-memory depend on – Language otherwise intact fundamentally different brain systems? – Often don’t know other things about object (how it’s used, etc.) 1 Are Faces Special? Are Faces Special? • Subjects presented with a face and asked to represent a face-part • Houses: similar performance for parts & wholes • Subjects presented with a house and asked to • Faces: whole-object advantage represent a house-part Are Faces Special? Models of Pattern Recognition • Template Models • Feature Models • Prototype Models • Neural Network Models • Objects represented in parts and holistically • Faces represented holistically Word Superiority Effect IAC
    [Show full text]
  • Apraxia, Neglect, and Agnosia
    REVIEW ARTICLE 07/09/2018 on SruuCyaLiGD/095xRqJ2PzgDYuM98ZB494KP9rwScvIkQrYai2aioRZDTyulujJ/fqPksscQKqke3QAnIva1ZqwEKekuwNqyUWcnSLnClNQLfnPrUdnEcDXOJLeG3sr/HuiNevTSNcdMFp1i4FoTX9EXYGXm/fCfl4vTgtAk5QA/xTymSTD9kwHmmkNHlYfO by https://journals.lww.com/continuum from Downloaded Apraxia, Neglect, Downloaded CONTINUUM AUDIO INTERVIEW AVAILABLE and Agnosia ONLINE from By H. Branch Coslett, MD, FAAN https://journals.lww.com/continuum ABSTRACT PURPOSEOFREVIEW:In part because of their striking clinical presentations, by SruuCyaLiGD/095xRqJ2PzgDYuM98ZB494KP9rwScvIkQrYai2aioRZDTyulujJ/fqPksscQKqke3QAnIva1ZqwEKekuwNqyUWcnSLnClNQLfnPrUdnEcDXOJLeG3sr/HuiNevTSNcdMFp1i4FoTX9EXYGXm/fCfl4vTgtAk5QA/xTymSTD9kwHmmkNHlYfO disorders of higher nervous system function figured prominently in the early history of neurology. These disorders are not merely historical curiosities, however. As apraxia, neglect, and agnosia have important clinical implications, it is important to possess a working knowledge of the conditions and how to identify them. RECENT FINDINGS: Apraxia is a disorder of skilled action that is frequently observed in the setting of dominant hemisphere pathology, whether from stroke or neurodegenerative disorders. In contrast to some previous teaching, apraxia has clear clinical relevance as it is associated with poor recovery from stroke. Neglect is a complex disorder with CITE AS: many different manifestations that may have different underlying CONTINUUM (MINNEAP MINN) mechanisms. Neglect is, in the author’s view, a multicomponent disorder 2018;24(3,
    [Show full text]
  • Visual Agnosia I
    Visual Agnosia I. Biran, MD, and H. B. Coslett, MD Address Clinical Variants Department of Neurology, University of Pennsylvania School of Medi- Visual agnosia can be specific to certain kinds of objects. cine, 3400 Spruce Street, Philadelphia, PA 19104, USA. Accordingly, there are agnosias for objects, agnosias for E-mail: [email protected] faces or “prosopagnosia,” agnosias for words or “pure Current Neurology and Neuroscience Reports 2003, 3:508–512 word blindness,” agnosias for colors, and agnosias for Current Science Inc. ISSN 1528–4042 Copyright © 2003 by Current Science Inc. the environment, including landmarks. Finally, the dis- order of simultanagnosia—an inability to “see” more than one object at a time—is often regarded as a type of The visual agnosias are an intriguing class of clinical phe- visual agnosia. nomena that have important implications for current theo- ries of high-level vision. Visual agnosia is defined as impaired Agnosia for objects object recognition that cannot be attributed to visual loss, Inability to recognize familiar objects is the most com- language impairment, or a general mental decline. At least mon of the agnostic syndromes. Patients are impaired in in some instances, agnostic patients generate an adequate naming regular objects and are often unable to describe internal representation of the stimulus but fail to recognize them or mimic their use. Object agnosias are further it. In this review, we begin by describing the classic works classified as either apperceptive (the percept is not fully related to the visual agnosias, followed by a description of constructed and, therefore, patients are unable to copy the major clinical variants and their occurrence in degener- drawings) or associative (the percept is relatively intact ative disorders.
    [Show full text]
  • Toe Agnosia in Gerstmann Syndrome
    Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry 1997;63:399–403 399 J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry: first published as 10.1136/jnnp.63.3.399 on 1 September 1997. Downloaded from SHORT REPORT Toe agnosia in Gerstmann syndrome Oliver Tucha, Anne Steup, Christian Smely, Klaus W Lange Abstract debate concerning the Gerstmann syndrome The following case report presents a has been described elsewhere.10 14–16 We de- patient exhibiting Gerstmann syndrome scribe a patient who had a focal lesion in the accompanied by toe agnosia. A 72 year old angular gyrus of the left hemisphere which was right handed woman had a focal lesion in caused by a glioblastoma multiforme. The the angular gyrus of the left hemisphere patient exhibited Gerstmann syndrome ac- which was caused by a glioblastoma companied by toe agnosia. multiforme. The first symptom she had complained of was severe headache. Case report Standardised neuropsychological tests of A 72 year old right handed woman who had intelligence, memory, attention, fluency, had no history of neurological or psychiatric apraxia, and language functions as well as diseases was admitted to the Department of tests for the assessment of agraphia, acal- Neurosurgery for treatment of a glioblastoma culia, right-left disorientation, and digit multiforme in the left parietal lobe. The first agnosia were performed. The patient symptom she had complained of was severe displayed all four symptoms of the Gerst- headache. The patient had worked as an accountant until the age of 64. mann syndrome—namely, agraphia, acal- copyright. culia, right-left disorientation, and finger A cranial CT scan disclosed a tumour agnosia.
    [Show full text]
  • The Anatomical Basis of Prosopagnosia
    J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry: first published as 10.1136/jnnp.37.5.489 on 1 May 1974. Downloaded from Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 1974, 37, 489-501 The anatomical basis of prosopagnosia J. C. MEADOWS' From the Aphasia Unit, Boston V.A. Hospital, Boston, U.S.A., and National Hospital, Queen Square, London SYNOPSIS Evidence is presented that patients with prosopagnosia have right anterior inferior occipital lesions in the region of the occipital temporal junction. Many if not all cases have an additional lesion in the left hemisphere; this is often but apparently not always symmetrical with the right hemisphere lesion. This evidence is discussed in relation to the anatomical connections of these regions and the results of experiments in animals. Prosopagnosia is the specific inability to recog- recently drawn attention to the occurrence of nize familiar faces. Its clinical features have been bilateral lesions in published cases that have well reviewed by previous authors-for example, come to necropsy, although they did not con- Protected by copyright. Hecaen and Angelergues (1962). It will be sider in detail the possible significance of the pointed out here only that the severely prosopa- location of these lesions. gnosic patient typically has no difficulty recogniz- In the present author's view, there is already ing everyday objects, although he may be quite good evidence concerning the localization of unable to recognize even members of his family lesions in prosopagnosia. This conclusion is unless they speak, when he immediately recog- based upon an analysis of clinical case reports nizes their voices. Some severely prosopagnosic and a re-evaluation of pathological findings in patients may recognize certain people by utilizing reported cases.
    [Show full text]
  • Congenital Prosopagnosia Without Object Agnosia? a Literature Review
    Cognitive Neuropsychology ISSN: 0264-3294 (Print) 1464-0627 (Online) Journal homepage: http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/pcgn20 Congenital prosopagnosia without object agnosia? A literature review Jacob Geskin & Marlene Behrmann To cite this article: Jacob Geskin & Marlene Behrmann (2017): Congenital prosopagnosia without object agnosia? A literature review, Cognitive Neuropsychology, DOI: 10.1080/02643294.2017.1392295 To link to this article: https://doi.org/10.1080/02643294.2017.1392295 View supplementary material Published online: 22 Nov 2017. Submit your article to this journal Article views: 7 View related articles View Crossmark data Full Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at http://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journalCode=pcgn20 Download by: [Carnegie Mellon University] Date: 28 November 2017, At: 13:35 COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY, 2017 https://doi.org/10.1080/02643294.2017.1392295 Congenital prosopagnosia without object agnosia? A literature review Jacob Geskin and Marlene Behrmann Department of Psychology and Center for the Neural Basis of Cognition, Carnegie Mellon University, Pittsburgh, PA, USA ABSTRACT ARTICLE HISTORY A longstanding controversy concerns the functional organization of high-level vision, and the Received 16 January 2017 extent to which the recognition of different classes of visual stimuli engages a single system or Revised 3 October 2017 multiple independent systems. We examine this in the context of congenital prosopagnosia (CP), Accepted 6 October 2017 a neurodevelopmental disorder in which individuals, without a history of brain damage, are KEYWORDS impaired at face recognition. This paper reviews all CP cases from 1976 to 2016, and explores domain specificity; face the evidence for the association or dissociation of face and object recognition.
    [Show full text]
  • Visuomotor System Uses Target Features Unavailable to Conscious Awareness
    Visuomotor system uses target features unavailable to conscious awareness Gordon Binsted*†, Kyle Brownell*, Zofia Vorontsova‡, Matthew Heath§, and Deborah Saucier¶ *College of Kinesiology, Neural Systems and Plasticity Research Group, University of Saskatchewan, Saskatoon, SK, Canada S7N 5B2; ‡Department of Psychology, University of Waterloo, Waterloo, ON, Canada N2L 3G1; §School of Kinesiology, University of Western Ontario, London, ON, Canada N6A 5B8; and ¶Canadian Centre for Behavioural Neuroscience, Department of Neuroscience, University of Lethbridge, Lethbridge, AB, Canada T1K 3M4 Edited by Dale Purves, Duke University Medical Center, Durham, NC, and approved June 25, 2007 (received for review March 12, 2007) After lesions to primary visual cortex, patients lack conscious determine their size or orientation. However, when asked to pick awareness of visual stimuli. Interestingly, however, some retain up an object, she does so quickly (although slower than age- the ability to make accurate judgments about the visual world (i.e., matched controls) and accurately, without hesitation; that is, her so-called blindsight). Similarly, damage to inferior occipitotempo- movements are metrically tuned to the size and orientation of the ral regions of cortex (e.g., lateral occipital cortex) can result in an object. inability to perceive object properties while retaining the ability to Based on the clinical observations of GY and DF, we inves- act on them (i.e., visual form agnosia). In the present work, we tigated the conscious access to visual object size information to demonstrate that the ability to interact with objects in the absence support the control of goal-directed reaching movements in of conscious awareness is not isolated to those with restricted neurologically intact individuals.
    [Show full text]