Congenital Prosopagnosia Without Object Agnosia? a Literature Review

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Congenital Prosopagnosia Without Object Agnosia? a Literature Review Cognitive Neuropsychology ISSN: 0264-3294 (Print) 1464-0627 (Online) Journal homepage: http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/pcgn20 Congenital prosopagnosia without object agnosia? A literature review Jacob Geskin & Marlene Behrmann To cite this article: Jacob Geskin & Marlene Behrmann (2017): Congenital prosopagnosia without object agnosia? A literature review, Cognitive Neuropsychology, DOI: 10.1080/02643294.2017.1392295 To link to this article: https://doi.org/10.1080/02643294.2017.1392295 View supplementary material Published online: 22 Nov 2017. Submit your article to this journal Article views: 7 View related articles View Crossmark data Full Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at http://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journalCode=pcgn20 Download by: [Carnegie Mellon University] Date: 28 November 2017, At: 13:35 COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY, 2017 https://doi.org/10.1080/02643294.2017.1392295 Congenital prosopagnosia without object agnosia? A literature review Jacob Geskin and Marlene Behrmann Department of Psychology and Center for the Neural Basis of Cognition, Carnegie Mellon University, Pittsburgh, PA, USA ABSTRACT ARTICLE HISTORY A longstanding controversy concerns the functional organization of high-level vision, and the Received 16 January 2017 extent to which the recognition of different classes of visual stimuli engages a single system or Revised 3 October 2017 multiple independent systems. We examine this in the context of congenital prosopagnosia (CP), Accepted 6 October 2017 a neurodevelopmental disorder in which individuals, without a history of brain damage, are KEYWORDS impaired at face recognition. This paper reviews all CP cases from 1976 to 2016, and explores domain specificity; face the evidence for the association or dissociation of face and object recognition. Of the 238 CP recognition; modularity; cases with data permitting a satisfactory evaluation, 80.3% evinced an association between object recognition; impaired face and object recognition whereas 19.7% evinced a dissociation. We evaluate the prosopagnosia strength of the evidence and correlate the face and object recognition behaviour. We consider the implications for theories of functional organization of the visual system, and offer suggestions for further adjudication of the relationship between face and object recognition. One of the longstanding controversies in the neurop- perhaps interleaved with other subclusters mediating sychology literature concerns the functional organiz- other classes of input, as evident from high-resolution ation of high-level vision, and the extent to which functional imaging studies (e.g., McGugin, Gatenby, the recognition of different classes of visual stimuli Gore, & Gauthier, 2012a). (for example, faces, words, and common objects) This domain-specific versus domain-general con- engages a single domain-general mechanism or mul- troversy has played out in almost all domains of cog- tiple, independent underlying psychological and nitive neuroscience, oftentimes with vigorous debate neural mechanisms. The distributed view of cortical (Gauthier, 2017a; Kanwisher, 2017), and many investi- function suggests that object discrimination depends gations have addressed this issue using neuroimaging on dispersed regions spread across visual cortex, (e.g., Gauthier, Skudlarski, Gore, & Anderson, 2000; Tarr some of which may support the recognition of more & Gauthier, 2000; Yovel & Kanwisher, 2004), electro- than one stimulus class (e.g., Behrmann & Plaut, physiological measures (e.g., Allison, McCarthy, 2015; Haxby et al., 2001;O’Toole, Jiang, Abdi, & Nobre, Puce, & Belger, 1994; Carmel & Bentin, 2002), Haxby, 2005; Robinson, Plaut, & Behrmann, 2017). In imaging studies in non-human primates (e.g., Arcaro contrast, the more modular account proposes that & Livingstone, 2017; Chang & Tsao, 2017; Landi & Frei- Downloaded by [Carnegie Mellon University] at 13:35 28 November 2017 different categories of objects are represented in wald, 2017), and investigations of neuropsychological and processed by functionally distinct cortical cases. Full coverage of this debate is beyond the scope regions (e.g., Kanwisher, 2017; McKone, Crookes, of this review but many comprehensive papers are Jeffery, & Dilks, 2012; McKone & Kanwisher, 2005). Of available (Grill-Spector & Weiner, 2014; Kravitz, course, there are also various alternatives that fall in Saleem, Baker, Ungerleider, & Mishkin, 2013). between these extreme positions—for example, it is With respect to neuropsychological investigations, possible that the organization is one in which subclus- this topic has received especially pointed attention in ters of visual classes are subserved by the same under- discussions of acquired prosopagnosia (AP), an impair- lying mechanism (for example, configural processing) ment of face recognition in premorbidly normal indi- or is one in which inputs from a single visual class are viduals following a brain lesion, with the critical processed by a small cluster of adjacent regions question being whether this disorder is limited to the CONTACT Marlene Behrmann [email protected] © 2017 Informa UK Limited, trading as Taylor & Francis Group 2 J. GESKIN AND M. BEHRMANN recognition of faces or is, instead, more general, affect- damage or any other obvious neurological disorder ing the recognition of other non-face visual classes as (for recent reviews of this disorder, see Barton & well. On the one hand, many investigations of AP Corrow, 2016a; Cook & Biotti, 2016; Grill-Spector, have argued for domain specificity in which the impair- Weiner, Kay, & Gomez, 2017). ment is restricted to the perception of faces (Barton, Some authors use the term “developmental proso- 2008; Busigny & Rossion, 2010; Riddoch, Johnston, Bra- pagnosia” (DP) to refer to these cases but because cewell, Boutsen, & Humphreys, 2008), and 27 cases of some of the reported DP cases do have brain prosopagnosia without an accompanying deficit in damage, we have opted for the term CP instead. object recognition (object agnosia) are listed in the lit- However, we do use DP as an abbreviation in the erature review by Farah (1991). On the other hand, the text or appendices if other authors have used that des- results of many studies favour a more domain-general ignation to refer to their participants. Also, because, in explanation for the prosopagnosia deficit and have the majority of cases, we cannot confirm the heredi- reported an impairment not only for face recognition tary nature of the deficit (which is usually labelled as but for the recognition of other stimuli, as well (e.g., “hereditary prosopagnosia”; HP), we opt for CP rather Bukach, Bub, Gauthier, & Tarr, 2006; Gauthier, Behr- than HP, as CP is agnostic with respect to an explicit mann, & Tarr, 1999; for recent review, see Barton & hereditary component (for papers on heritability of Corrow, 2016b) and 37 cases of prosopagnosia with face perception, see Shakeshaft & Plomin, 2015; an additional recognition deficit are identified in the Wilmer et al., 2010; Zhu et al., 2010). review by Farah (1991). Whereas the former finding of The aim of the current paper, then, is to provide a domain-specificity is more compatible with the brain survey of the existing literature on CP to elucidate being organized into modules, each with a circum- the nature of the relationship between impairments scribed function as proposed by, for example, Fodor in face and non-face object recognition and, in so (1983) or Lenneberg (1967), the latter is more sugges- doing, to characterize the functional architecture of tive of an organization that may be contingent on a visual recognition. In addition to addressing this scien- more general mechanism, such as the sensitivity to cur- tific question, we also evaluate the methodological vature (Nasr, Echavarria, & Tootell, 2014; Ponce, Hart- approach to data collection in such individuals, carry mann, & Livingstone, 2017) or spatial frequency out a meta-analysis of the findings, and offer some (Woodhead, Wise, Sereno, & Leech, 2011) or the suggestions that might allow for a more directed reliance on holistic processing (Richler, Palmeri, & Gau- approach to testing the hypotheses. We start by pro- thier, 2012). Understanding the functional organization viding some context from neuropsychology and iden- of the visual system is critical from a theoretical point of tifying methodological challenges to the survey view but also from a translational perspective in that a approach, before presenting the method and findings precise characterization of the deficits and their under- of our investigation. lying mechanism may be of value in customizing reha- bilitation approaches for those with visuoperceptual The Farah approach Downloaded by [Carnegie Mellon University] at 13:35 28 November 2017 disorders (e.g., Woodhead et al., 2013). In an effort to shed further light on the question of As noted above, neuropsychological investigations structure–function relations in high-level vision, the have explored the association or dissociation current paper examines the visual recognition behav- between the recognition of different classes of visual iour of individuals with congenital prosopagnosia, a stimuli. For example, the relationship between the rec- disorder that, although recognized several decades ognition of faces versus other visual stimuli such as ago (e.g., Temple, 1992), is currently receiving con- objects and words has been assessed in several siderable scientific attention. Congenital prosopagno-
Recommended publications
  • Cognitive Emotional Sequelae Post Stroke
    11/26/2019 The Neuropsychology of Objectives 1. Identify various cognitive sequelae that may result from stroke Stroke: Cognitive & 2. Explain how stroke may impact emotional functioning, both acutely and long-term Emotional Sequelae COX HEALTH STROKE CONFERENCE BRITTANY ALLEN, PHD, ABPP, MBA 12/13/2019 Epidemiology of Stroke Stroke Statistics • > 795,000 people in the United States have a stroke • 5th leading cause of death for Americans • ~610,000 are first or new strokes • Risk of having a first stroke is nearly twice as high for blacks as whites • ~1/4 occur in people with a history of prior stroke • Blacks have the highest rate of death due to stroke • ~140,000 Americans die every year due to stroke • Death rates have declined for all races/ethnicities for decades except that Hispanics have seen • Approximately 87% of all strokes are ischemic an increase in death rates since 2013 • Costs the United States an estimated $34 billion annually • Risk for stroke increases with age, but 34% of people hospitalized for stroke were < 65 years of • Health care services age • Medicines to treat stroke • Women have a lower stroke risk until late in life when the association reverses • Missed days of work • Approximately 15% of strokes are heralded by a TIA • Leading cause of long-term disability • Reduces mobility in > 50% of stroke survivors > 65 years of age Source: Centers for Disease Control Stroke Death Rates Neuropsychological Assessment • Task Engagement • Memory • Language • Visuospatial Functioning • Attention/Concentration • Executive
    [Show full text]
  • Prosopagnosia by B
    J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiat., 1959, 22, 124. PROSOPAGNOSIA BY B. BORNSTEIN and D. P. KIDRON From the Department of Neurology, Beilinson Hospital, Petah Tiqva, Israel "And what is the nature of this knowledge or recollection? I mean to ask, Whether a person, who having seen or heard or in any way perceived anything, knows not only that, but has a conception of something else which is the subject, not of the same but of some other kind of knowledge, may not be fairly said to recollect that of which he has the conception?" "And when the recollection is derived from like things, then another consideration is sure to arise, which is, Whether the likeness in any degree falls short or not of that which is recollected?" "The Philosophy of Plato " Phaedo (the Jowett translation). Does visual agnosia exist in a partial or isolated bances in sensation time, in adaptation time, in form, in which certain qualities only are affected, visual acuity, and in brightness discrimination. as opposed to generalized visual agnosia? Many Ettlinger (1956) rejected Bay's contentions. After workers cast doubt on this concept, maintaining analysing 30 cases of head injury, he showed that that partial visual agnosia is no more than a com- some patients had neither field nor perceptual bination of defects in vision, memory, and orienta- defects, others had field but not perceptual defects, tion, appearing together. and only in eight of the 30 patients were field and The clinical elucidation of partial visual agnosia perceptual defects found together. It is true that is likely to be affected by the patient's intellectual visual agnosia is frequently associated with homony- capacity, his mental state at the time of examination, mous hemianopsia, but despite this there are cases and his ability to cooperate without being influenced of hemianopsia without gnostic defects.
    [Show full text]
  • Can We Lose Memories of Faces? Content Specificity and Awareness in a Prosopagnosic
    Can We Lose Memories of Faces? Content Specificity and Awareness in a Prosopagnosic Nancy L. Etcoff Department of Brain and Cognitive Sciences Massachusetts Institute of Technology Neuropsychology Laboratory Massachusetts General Hospital Downloaded from http://mitprc.silverchair.com/jocn/article-pdf/3/1/25/1755723/jocn.1991.3.1.25.pdf by guest on 18 May 2021 Roy Freeman Division of Neurology New England Deaconess Hospital Beth Israel Hospital Harvard Medical School Kyle R. Cave Department of Psychology University of California, San Diego Abstract H Prosopagnosia is a neurological syndrome in which patients nonfacial channels. The only other categories of shapes that he cannot recognize faces. Kecently it has been shown that some has marked trouble recognizing are animals and emotional prosopagnosics give evidence of “covert” recognition: they expressions, though even these impairments were not as severe show greater autonomic responses to familiar faces than to as the one for faces. Three measures (sympathetic skin re- unfamiliar ones, and respond differently to familiar faces in sponse, pupil dilation, and learning correct and incorrect learning and interference tasks. Although some patients do not names of faces) failed to show any signs of covert face recog- show covert recognition, this has usually been attributed to an nition in LH, though the measures were sensitive enough to “apperceptive” deficit that impairs perceptual analysis of the reflect autonomic reactions in LH to stimuli other than faces, input. The implication is that prosopagnosia is a deficit in access and face familiarity in normal controls. Thus prosopagnosia to, or awareness of, memories of faces: the inducing brain cannot always be attributed to a mere absence of awareness injury does not destroy the memories themselves.
    [Show full text]
  • Abadie's Sign Abadie's Sign Is the Absence Or Diminution of Pain Sensation When Exerting Deep Pressure on the Achilles Tendo
    A.qxd 9/29/05 04:02 PM Page 1 A Abadie’s Sign Abadie’s sign is the absence or diminution of pain sensation when exerting deep pressure on the Achilles tendon by squeezing. This is a frequent finding in the tabes dorsalis variant of neurosyphilis (i.e., with dorsal column disease). Cross References Argyll Robertson pupil Abdominal Paradox - see PARADOXICAL BREATHING Abdominal Reflexes Both superficial and deep abdominal reflexes are described, of which the superficial (cutaneous) reflexes are the more commonly tested in clinical practice. A wooden stick or pin is used to scratch the abdomi- nal wall, from the flank to the midline, parallel to the line of the der- matomal strips, in upper (supraumbilical), middle (umbilical), and lower (infraumbilical) areas. The maneuver is best performed at the end of expiration when the abdominal muscles are relaxed, since the reflexes may be lost with muscle tensing; to avoid this, patients should lie supine with their arms by their sides. Superficial abdominal reflexes are lost in a number of circum- stances: normal old age obesity after abdominal surgery after multiple pregnancies in acute abdominal disorders (Rosenbach’s sign). However, absence of all superficial abdominal reflexes may be of localizing value for corticospinal pathway damage (upper motor neu- rone lesions) above T6. Lesions at or below T10 lead to selective loss of the lower reflexes with the upper and middle reflexes intact, in which case Beevor’s sign may also be present. All abdominal reflexes are preserved with lesions below T12. Abdominal reflexes are said to be lost early in multiple sclerosis, but late in motor neurone disease, an observation of possible clinical use, particularly when differentiating the primary lateral sclerosis vari- ant of motor neurone disease from multiple sclerosis.
    [Show full text]
  • THE CLINICAL ASSESSMENT of the PATIENT with EARLY DEMENTIA S Cooper, J D W Greene V15
    J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry: first published as 10.1136/jnnp.2005.081133 on 16 November 2005. Downloaded from THE CLINICAL ASSESSMENT OF THE PATIENT WITH EARLY DEMENTIA S Cooper, J D W Greene v15 J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 2005;76(Suppl V):v15–v24. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.2005.081133 ementia is a clinical state characterised by a loss of function in at least two cognitive domains. When making a diagnosis of dementia, features to look for include memory Dimpairment and at least one of the following: aphasia, apraxia, agnosia and/or disturbances in executive functioning. To be significant the impairments should be severe enough to cause problems with social and occupational functioning and the decline must have occurred from a previously higher level. It is important to exclude delirium when considering such a diagnosis. When approaching the patient with a possible dementia, taking a careful history is paramount. Clues to the nature and aetiology of the disorder are often found following careful consultation with the patient and carer. A focused cognitive and physical examination is useful and the presence of specific features may aid in diagnosis. Certain investigations are mandatory and additional tests are recommended if the history and examination indicate particular aetiologies. It is useful when assessing a patient with cognitive impairment in the clinic to consider the following straightforward questions: c Is the patient demented? c If so, does the loss of function conform to a characteristic pattern? c Does the pattern of dementia conform to a particular pattern? c What is the likely disease process responsible for the dementia? An understanding of cognitive function and its anatomical correlates is necessary in order to ascertain which brain areas are affected.
    [Show full text]
  • Prosopagnosia: a Clinical, Psychological, and Anatomical Study of Three Patients
    J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry: first published as 10.1136/jnnp.40.4.395 on 1 April 1977. Downloaded from Journal ofNeurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 1977, 40, 395-403 Prosopagnosia: a clinical, psychological, and anatomical study of three patients A. M. WHITELEY' AND ELIZABETH K. WARRINGTON From the Department ofNeurology, The London Hospital, and the Department ofPsychology, National Hospital, Queen Square, London SUMMARY Three patients with prosopagnosia are described of whom two had right occipital lesions. An analysis of visual and perceptual functions demonstrated a defect in perceptual classi- fication which appeared to be stimulus-specific. A special mechanism for facial recognition is postu- lated, and the importance of the right sided posterior lesion is stressed. Prosopagnosia is a rare but interesting condition unreliably, as pointers to cerebral lesions, and most in which recognition of faces is impaired. The cases have a left homonymous defect indicating right sufferer is quite unable to identify people purely by hemisphere disease, but not excluding a left sided their facial appearance but can do so without lesion (Meadows, 1974a). There are many cases, Protected by copyright. difficulty by their voice and by visual clues such as however, with bilateral field defects indicating clothing, hair colour, and gait. Recognition of other bilateral lesions, but there are cases with right visual material can be intact, but in some cases highly homonymous defects and cases with no field defects discriminative visual skills, such as species of birds at all. There are several case reports where surgery and types of fruit, are impaired (Bornstein, 1963; to right temporal and occipital lobes is responsible, De Renzi et al., 1968).
    [Show full text]
  • Disorders of the Visual System in Alzheimer's Disease
    © 1990 Raven Press, Ltd.. New York Disorders of the Visual System in Alzheimer's Disease Mario F. Mendez, M.D., Robert L. Tomsak, M.D., Ph.D., and Bernd Remler, M.D. Alzheimer's disease (AD) is associated with distur­ Alzheimer's disease (AD) is the most prevalent bances in basic visual, complex visual, and oculomotor form of dementia affecting greater than 2.5 million functions. The broad range of visual system disorders in AD may result from the concentration of neuropathol­ people in the U.S., with the numbers expected to ogy in visual association cortex and optic nerves in this double by the year 2040 (1). Despite the absence of disease. AD patients and their caregivers frequently re­ a clinical test for AD, the recent establishment of port visuospatial difficulties in these patients. Examina­ highly accurate clinical criteria permit a more pre­ tion of the visual system in AD may reveal visual field cise evaluation of the deficits associated with this deficits, prolonged visual evoked potentials, depressed contrast sensitivities, and abnormal eye movement re­ disorder (2-4) (see Table 1). In addition to the cordings. Complex visual disturbances include construc­ usual memory and other cognitive deficits, AD pa­ tional and visuoperceptual abnormalities, spatial agno­ tients have disturbances in basic visual, complex sia and Balint's syndrome, environmental disorienta­ visual, and oculomotor functions, and AD patients tion, visual agnosia, facial identification problems, and in greater numbers are undergoing more thorough visual hallucinations. The purpose of this article is to review the spectrum of visual system disturbances evaluations of their visual systems (4-8).
    [Show full text]
  • DISORDERS of AUDITORY PROCESSING: EVIDENCE for MODULARITY in AUDITION Michael R
    DISORDERS OF AUDITORY PROCESSING: EVIDENCE FOR MODULARITY IN AUDITION Michael R. Polster and Sally B. Rose (Psychology Department, Victoria University of Wellington, Wellington, New Zealand) ABSTRACT This article examines four disorders of auditory processing that can result from selective brain damage (cortical deafness, pure word deafness, auditory agnosia and phonagnosia) in an effort to derive a plausible functional and neuroanatomical model of audition. The article begins by identifying three possible reasons why models of auditory processing have been slower to emerge than models of visual processing: neuroanatomical differences between the visual and auditory systems, terminological confusions relating to auditory processing disorders, and technical factors that have made auditory stimuli more difficult to study than visual stimuli. The four auditory disorders are then reviewed and current theories of auditory processing considered. Taken together, these disorders suggest a modular architecture analogous to models of visual processing that have been derived from studying neurological patients. Ideas for future research to test modular theory more fully are presented. Key words: auditory processing, modularity, review INTRODUCTION Neuropsychological investigations of patients suffering from brain damage have flourished in recent years and helped to produce more detailed and neuroanatomically plausible models of several aspects of cognitive function. For example, models of language processing are often closely aligned with studies of aphasia (e.g., Caplan, 1987; Goodglass, 1993) and models of memory draw heavily upon studies of amnesia (e.g., Schacter and Tulving, 1994; Squire, 1987). Most of this research has relied on visually presented materials, and as a result visual processing disorders tend to be more well-documented and better understood than their auditory counterparts.
    [Show full text]
  • 26 Aphasia, Memory Loss, Hemispatial Neglect, Frontal Syndromes and Other Cerebral Disorders - - 8/4/17 12:21 PM )
    1 Aphasia, Memory Loss, 26 Hemispatial Neglect, Frontal Syndromes and Other Cerebral Disorders M.-Marsel Mesulam CHAPTER The cerebral cortex of the human brain contains ~20 billion neurons spread over an area of 2.5 m2. The primary sensory and motor areas constitute 10% of the cerebral cortex. The rest is subsumed by modality- 26 selective, heteromodal, paralimbic, and limbic areas collectively known as the association cortex (Fig. 26-1). The association cortex mediates the Aphasia, Memory Hemispatial Neglect, Frontal Syndromes and Other Cerebral Disorders Loss, integrative processes that subserve cognition, emotion, and comport- ment. A systematic testing of these mental functions is necessary for the effective clinical assessment of the association cortex and its dis- eases. According to current thinking, there are no centers for “hearing words,” “perceiving space,” or “storing memories.” Cognitive and behavioral functions (domains) are coordinated by intersecting large-s- cale neural networks that contain interconnected cortical and subcortical components. Five anatomically defined large-scale networks are most relevant to clinical practice: (1) a perisylvian network for language, (2) a parietofrontal network for spatial orientation, (3) an occipitotemporal network for face and object recognition, (4) a limbic network for explicit episodic memory, and (5) a prefrontal network for the executive con- trol of cognition and comportment. Investigations based on functional imaging have also identified a default mode network, which becomes activated when the person is not engaged in a specific task requiring attention to external events. The clinical consequences of damage to this network are not yet fully defined. THE LEFT PERISYLVIAN NETWORK FOR LANGUAGE AND APHASIAS The production and comprehension of words and sentences is depen- FIGURE 26-1 Lateral (top) and medial (bottom) views of the cerebral dent on the integrity of a distributed network located along the peri- hemispheres.
    [Show full text]
  • Face-Specific Impairment in Holistic Perception Following Focal Lesion of the Right Anterior Temporal Lobe
    Neuropsychologia 56 (2014) 312–333 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Neuropsychologia journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/neuropsychologia Face-specific impairment in holistic perception following focal lesion of the right anterior temporal lobe Thomas Busigny a,d,n, Goedele Van Belle a, Boutheina Jemel b, Anthony Hosein b, Sven Joubert c, Bruno Rossion a a Institute of Research in Psychology and Institute of Neuroscience, Université Catholique de Louvain, Louvain-la-Neuve, Belgium b Neuroscience and Cognitive Electrophysiology Research Lab, Hôpital Rivière-des-Prairies, Montréal, Canada c Département de Psychologie, Université de Montreal & Centre de Recherche Institut Universitaire de Gériatrie de Montréal, Canada d Université de Toulouse, UPS, Centre de Recherche Cerveau et Cognition (CNRS, Cerco), Toulouse, France article info abstract Article history: Recent studies have provided solid evidence for pure cases of prosopagnosia following brain damage. Received 29 May 2013 The patients reported so far have posterior lesions encompassing either or both the right inferior occipital Received in revised form cortex and fusiform gyrus, and exhibit a critical impairment in generating a sufficiently detailed holistic 21 January 2014 percept to individualize faces. Here, we extended these observations to include the prosopagnosic patient LR Accepted 24 January 2014 (Bukach, Bub, Gauthier, & Tarr, 2006), whose damage is restricted to the anterior region of the right Available online 4 February 2014 temporal lobe. First, we report that LR is able to discriminate parametrically defined individual exemplars of Keywords: nonface object categories as accurately and quickly as typical observers, which suggests that the visual Acquired prosopagnosia similarity account of prosopagnosia does not explain his impairments.
    [Show full text]
  • Transient Gerstmann Syndrome As Manifestation of Stroke Case Report and Brief Literature Review
    Dement Neuropsychol 2017 June;11(2):202-205 Case Report doi: 10.1590/1980-57642016dn11-020013 Transient Gerstmann syndrome as manifestation of stroke Case report and brief literature review Rafael Batista João1, Raquel Mattos Filgueiras2, Marcelo Lucci Mussi3, João Eliezer Ferri de Barros4 ABSTRACT. Gerstmann Syndrome (GS) is a rare neurological condition described as a group of cognitive changes corresponding to a tetrad of symptoms comprising agraphia, acalculia, right-left disorientation and finger agnosia. It is known that some specific brain lesions may lead to such findings, particularly when there is impairment of the angular gyrus and adjacent structures. In addition, the possibility of disconnection syndrome should be considered in some cases. The purpose of this article is to report a case of a young, cardiac patient, non-adherent to treatment, who presented with a stroke in which transient clinical symptoms were compatible with the tetrad of GS. The case report is followed by a discussion and brief review of the relevant literature. Key words: Gerstmann syndrome, disconnection syndrome, insular cortex, parietal lobe, frontal lobe. SÍNDROME DE GERSTMANN TRANSITÓRIA COMO MANIFESTAÇÃO DE ACIDENTE VASCULAR ENCEFÁLICO: RELATO DE CASO E BREVE REVISÃO DE LITERATURA RESUMO. A síndrome de Gerstmann (SG) é uma condição neurológica rara, caracterizada por um grupo de alterações cognitivas que correspondem a uma tétrade composta por agrafia, acalculia, desorientação direita-esquerda e agnosia para dedos. Sabe-se que certas lesões encefálicas específicas podem levar a tais achados, particularmente quando ocorre acometimento do giro angular e estruturas adjacentes. Além disso, a possibilidade de síndrome de desconexão deve ser considerada em alguns casos.
    [Show full text]
  • Fusiform Gyrus Face Selectivity Relates to Individual Differences in Facial Recognition Ability
    Fusiform Gyrus Face Selectivity Relates to Individual Differences in Facial Recognition Ability Nicholas Furl, Lúcia Garrido, Raymond J. Dolan, Jon Driver, and Bradley Duchaine Abstract ■ Regions of the occipital and temporal lobes, including a re- robust relationships between face selectivity and face identifi- gion in the fusiform gyrus (FG), have been proposed to con- cation ability in FG across our sample for several convergent stitute a “core” visual representation system for faces, in part measures, including voxel-wise statistical parametric mapping, because they show face selectivity and face repetition suppres- peak face selectivity in individually defined “fusiform face areas” sion. But recent fMRI studies of developmental prosopagnosics (FFAs), and anatomical extents (cluster sizes) of those FFAs. (DPs) raise questions about whether these measures relate to None of these measures showed associations with behavioral face processing skills. Although DPs manifest deficient face expression or object recognition ability. As a group, DPs had processing, most studies to date have not shown unequivocal reduced face-selective responses in bilateral FFA when com- reductions of functional responses in the proposed core re- pared with non-DPs. Individual DPs were also more likely than gions. We scanned 15 DPs and 15 non-DP control participants non-DPs to lack expected face-selective activity in core regions. with fMRI while employing factor analysis to derive behavioral These findings associate individual differences in face process- components related to face identification or other processes. ing ability with selectivity in core face processing regions. Repetition suppression specific to facial identities in FG or to This confirms that face selectivity can provide a valid marker expression in FG and STS did not show compelling relation- for neural mechanisms that contribute to face identification ships with face identification ability.
    [Show full text]