An Interpretation of Farmed Mink's Motivation for a Water Bath

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

An Interpretation of Farmed Mink's Motivation for a Water Bath CORE Metadata, citation and similar papers at core.ac.uk Provided by University of Lincoln Institutional Repository AABS revision 28-5-2010 1 Title: 2 To swim or not to swim: An interpretation of farmed mink’s 3 motivation for a water bath. 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 Running headline: 11 Swimming water for farmed mink 12 13 14 Authors: 15 VINKE, C.M., HANSEN, S.W., MONONEN, J., MASON, G.J., KORHONEN, H., COOPER, J.J., 16 MOHAIBES, M., BAKKEN, M. SPRUIJT, B.M. 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 25 26 27 28 29 30 Correspondence to: 31 C.M. Vinke. Ethology & Welfare Group, Department of Animals, Science & Society, Faculty of 32 Veterinary Medicine, University of Utrecht, P.O. Box 80166, 3508 TD Utrecht, The Netherlands. 33 T: ++ 31.30.2534373 34 F: ++ 31.30.2539227 35 Email: [email protected] 1 AABS revision 28-5-2010 36 Summary 37 38 How an animal’s behavioural (ethological) needs can be met is a pivotal issue in the assessment 39 of welfare for captive animals. The value of swimming water for farmed mink is an example how 40 scientific and societal questions relating to animal welfare can be answered. A number of studies 41 have addressed the issue of the indispensability of swimming water for mink; however, so far with 42 inconclusive evidence. In this paper, the results of these studies and related literature are 43 reviewed. 44 First, the biological definition of need is discussed. Subsequently, attention is paid to the effects of 45 the presence, absence and the removal of swimming water on behavioural and physiological 46 correlates of well-being including stereotypic and anticipatory behaviour and urinary cortisol. 47 Thereafter we discuss individual differences in the use of swimming water, the price animals pay 48 for access to a water bath, and the effect of access to swimming water on juvenile play. 49 The main conclusions of the literature review are that 1) the use of a water bath for mink is 50 most likely related to foraging behaviour (foraging areas: land and water); 2) absence of 51 swimming water, without prior experience, does not lead to consistent changes in level of 52 stereotypic behaviour, or anticipatory responses; 3) removal of a previously experienced water 53 bath may induce short-term stress as indicated by behavioural parameters and elevated cortisol 54 responses; 4) mink work hard for access to a swimming bath and running wheel in consumer 55 demand studies. Other cage modifications such as tunnels and biting objects, may also provide 56 environmental enrichment, if they are added to otherwise impoverished conditions; 5) There are 57 individual differences in the use of swimming water: these are related in part to variation in prior 58 experience of aquatic resources.; 6) As prior experience is important both with respect to 59 individual use of swimming water and the response to deprivation, swimming water can not be 60 described as biological need in the sense of a fixed requirement for survival. As swimming water 61 appears to act as an incentive that induces its own motivation a more accurate term may be an 62 “incentive induced or environmentally facilitated need”. Given the available evidence, it is not 63 possible to conclude whether mink that have never experienced swimming water, suffer as a 64 consequence of its absence. However, it is possible to predict that mink with access to water have 65 improved quality of life, due to increased behavioural opportunities, in comparison to farmed mink 66 without access to swimming water. In practical terms, it is still open to debate whether mink 67 should be provided with swimming water, or if alternative, less valued, but easier to install and 68 maintain forms of environmental enrichment, should be provided in mink housing. 69 To clarify these issues a number of future studies would be valuable. These include; 1) whether 70 specific environmental cues affect motivation to swim, such as the form of drinking water delivery 71 systems ; 2) whether prior experience of swimming water affects its incentive value; in other 72 words “can you miss what you never experienced?”; 3) do behavioural parameters such as 73 stereotypic behaviour; rebound effects and vacuum activity have any general utility in assessing 2 AABS revision 28-5-2010 74 the value of absent resources; 4) what are preferences for and the value of alternative resources 75 which may act as substitutes for swimming water. In addition we would recommend further work 76 investigating: relationship between access to swimming water and positive indicators of welfare 77 such as play and/or anticipatory behaviour; the effects of preventing the performance of rewarding 78 behaviours and deprivation of a previous experienced resource; and health and hygeine issues 79 related to provision of a water bath. In future work, it would be desirable to present be the actual 80 percentages of animals using a water bath during the experiment and the use of power analyses, 81 to aid their interpretation. 82 83 84 Keywords: farmed mink, animal welfare, biological need, motivation, swimming water, animal 85 housing 3 AABS revision 28-5-2010 86 1. Introduction 87 88 The deprivation of behavioural or ethological needs is a key feature of poor welfare of animals 89 kept in captivity (see Dawkins 1988; Friend, 1989; Rushen et al., 1993). Animals have a certain 90 degree of plasticity to adapt their behavioural repertoire to environmental demands, in which case, 91 hey can acquire new behaviours (learning), omit non-adaptive behaviours or adapt behavioural 92 strategies to available resources. However, where behavioural plasticity is limited and the animal 93 is equipped with pre-programmed specific behavioural strategies ("standard answers") to cope 94 with “standard” demands of the environment to which the species has been adapted during 95 evolution, the capacity to adapt to captive animals may be restricted (see for coping strategies: 96 e.g. Fokkema et al., 1995; Koolhaas et al., 1999). 97 The role of swimming water for farmed mink has elicited much debate with respect to 98 scientific and the societal concerns regarding animal welfare and related ethical issues associated 99 with this agriculture activity in some European countries.Whether access to swimming water 100 represents a behavioural need for farmed mink, thus far has not been resolved conclusively. 101 Therefore, a concerted action of a combined set of studies was launched to attempt to resolve this 102 issue: Is swimming water an indispensable stimulus, a conditional need, or is swimming water no 103 need at all for farmed mink? 104 This paper aims to discuss the main results and conclusions of these studies (the separate 105 full studies are published elsewhere) as well as other available literature on the meaning of 106 swimming water for farmed mink. Attention is paid to the effects of the presence, the absence and 107 the removal of swimming water (deprivation) on stereotypic behaviour, anticipatory activity, levels 108 of urinary cortisol, individual differences in use of water resources, preference and consumer 109 demand studies, and the effect of swimming water on juvenile play as a potential positive indicator 110 of animal well being. The present paper does not address animal management issues such as 111 the economic consequences of the introduction of swimming water on farms, the potential 112 legislation to cover swimming water delivery in different countries, or the effect of swimming water 113 on reproduction, pelt quality and health. Nor does this paper discuss the ethical aspects on the 114 purpose wherefore mink is commercially bred. 115 116 The paper starts with a short summary on the concept of behavioural needs, followed by 117 minks’ natural habitats on land and water, and the use swimming behaviour by captive mink.. 118 Subsequently, we discuss: the development of stereotypies in farmed mink in the presence and 119 absence of swimming water; consumer-demand tests and substitutability of alternative resources; 120 the importance of prior experience with swimming water; anticipatory and juvenile play behaviour 121 in the presence and absence of a water bath; and the effects of deprivation and challenges 122 involved in the study of behavioural deprivation. Finally, we question why mink might be motivated 4 AABS revision 28-5-2010 123 to have swimming water, draw conclusions on the necessity of providing swimming water to 124 farmed mink, and provide advice for future research and for farming practice. 125 126 2. Behavioural needs and reward 127 128 Behavioural needs: definitions, concepts and some characteristics 129 Behavioural needs or “what is indispensable to an animal”, have been described in various ways. 130 One approach is to reflect on the wild or natural environment and suggest all elements animals 131 that are denied in captivity can be described as lacking or deprived (Thorpe, 1965; Martin, 1979). 132 This approach has been largely rejected by animal welfare scientist, citing the organism’s 133 behavioural plasticity and the effects of domestication and humans’ selective breeding programs 134 (e.g. Dawkins, 1980, 1983; Poole, 1992; Veasey et al., 1996 Price, 1999) who concluded that it 135 would be inappropriate to assume captive animals would require the same elements in their 136 environment as their wild conspecifics. 137 138 A more classical definition of behavioural needs can be found in “Who needs behavioural 139 needs?” by Jensen and Toates (1993). They defined a behavioural need as a specific behavioural 140 pattern that should be performed, irrespective of the environment, even when the physiological 141 needs related to this essential behaviour have been met. The biological relevance or adaptive 142 value of performance of these types of behaviours may reside in long-term benefits for the 143 individual or its offspring (e.g.
Recommended publications
  • The Behavioral Effects of Feeding Enrichment on a Zoo-Housed Herd of African Elephants (Loxodonta Africana)
    Winthrop University Digital Commons @ Winthrop University Graduate Theses The Graduate School 8-2017 The Behavioral Effects of Feeding Enrichment on a Zoo-Housed Herd of African Elephants (Loxodonta africana) Caroline Marie Driscoll Winthrop University, [email protected] Follow this and additional works at: https://digitalcommons.winthrop.edu/graduatetheses Part of the Biology Commons, and the Zoology Commons Recommended Citation Driscoll, Caroline Marie, "The Behavioral Effects of Feeding Enrichment on a Zoo-Housed Herd of African Elephants (Loxodonta africana)" (2017). Graduate Theses. 71. https://digitalcommons.winthrop.edu/graduatetheses/71 This Thesis is brought to you for free and open access by the The Graduate School at Digital Commons @ Winthrop University. It has been accepted for inclusion in Graduate Theses by an authorized administrator of Digital Commons @ Winthrop University. For more information, please contact [email protected]. THE BEHAVIORAL EFFECTS OF FEEDING ENRICHMENT ON A ZOO- HOUSED HERD OF AFRICAN ELEPHANTS (LOXODONTA AFRICANA) A Thesis Presented to the Faculty of the College of Arts and Sciences in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree of Master of Science in the Department of Biology Winthrop University July, 2017 By Caroline Marie Driscoll ii ABSTRACT A comprehensive study on the behavioral effects of feeding enrichment was conducted on six African elephants housed at the North Carolina Zoological Park in Asheboro, NC. The herd is comprised of are two adult males, three adult females, and one subadult female. The study was conducted over a 10-month period and consisted of focal sample observations across three conditions. Observations were recorded during the baseline condition (June to September) and continued through the introduction of feeding enrichment.
    [Show full text]
  • Adaptability of Innate Motor Patterns and Motor Control Mechanisms
    BEHAVIORAL AND BRAIN SCIENCES (1986) 9, 585-638 Printed in the United States of America Adaptability of innate motor patterns and motor control mechanisms M. B. Berkinblit," A. G. Feldman," and O. I. Fukson" "Institute of Information Transmission Problems, Academy of Sciences, Moscow 101447, U.S.S.R. and "Moscow State University, Moscow 117234, U.S.S.R. Abstract: The following factors underlying behavioral plasticity are discussed: (1) reflex adaptability and its role in the voluntary control of movement, (2) degrees of freedom and motor equivalence, and (3) the problem of the discrete organization of motor behavior. Our discussion concerns a variety of innate motor patterns, with emphasis on the wiping reflex in the frog. It is proposed that central regulation of stretch reflex thresholds governs voluntary control over muscle force and length. This suggestion is an integral part of the equilibrium-point hypothesis, two versions of which are compared. Kinematic analysis of the wiping reflex in the spinal frog has shown that each stimulated skin site is associated with a group of different but equally effective trajectories directed to the target site. Such phenomena reflect the principle of motor equivalence - the capacity of t h e neuronal structures responsible for movement to select one or another of a set of p o s s i b l e trajectories leading to the goal. Redundancy of degrees of freedom at the neuronal level as well as at the mechanical level of the body's joints makes motor equivalence possible. This sort of equivalence accommodates the overall flexibility of motor behavior.
    [Show full text]
  • Hutnan Ethology Bulletin
    Hutnan Ethology Bulletin VOLUME 12, ISSUE 1 ISSN 0739-2036 MARCH 1997 © 1997 The International Society for Human Ethology obviously not in the interests of the slaves. Why don't they go on strike? Because the slaves are not genetically related to anything that comes out of the nest where they are now working. Any gene that tended to make them go on strike would have no possibility of being benefited by the striking action. The copies of their genes, the ·copies of these striking workers genes, would be back in the home nest, and they would be being turned out by the queen, which the striking workers left behind. So there would be no opportunity for a phenotypic effect, namely striking, to benefit germ line copies of themselves. You also write about an ant species called Monomorium santschii in which there are no workers. The queen invades a nest of another species, and then uses chemicals to induce the An Interview of workers to adopt her, and to kill their own queen. How is it possible that natural sdection Richard Dawkins did not act against such incredible deception and manipulation, which must have been going By Frans Roes, Lauriergracht 127-II, 1016 on for millions of years? RK Amsterdam, The Netherlands In any kind of arms race, it is possible for one Richard Dawkins is a zoologist and Professor of . side in the arms race to lose consistently. Public. Understanding of Science at Oxford Monomorium santschii is a very rare species. If University. Of his best-selling books, The you look back in the ancestry of the victim- Selfish Gene (1976) probably did most in species over many millions of years, many of bringing the evolutionary message home to both their ancestors may never have encountered a professional and a general readership.
    [Show full text]
  • Chihirosukikarathesis.Pdf (391.3Kb)
    Szent István University Faculty of Veterinary Science Budapest Institute of Animal Breeding, Nutrition and Laboratory Animal Science HOW THE DIFFERENT NOISE TYPES MAY INFLUENCE THE OPEN-FIELD BEHAVIOUR OF RATS ? by Chihiro Sukikara Supervisor: Prof. Dr. Sándor György Fekete , DVM, DSc SZIU Faculty of Veterinary Science - 2013 – 1 Table of Content Page Introduction 3 Review of the Literature 5 Own Investigations 9 Material and Methods 9 Results 11 Discussion 23 Conclusion 27 References 33 Acknowledgement 32 Summary 30 Összefoglalás 31 Author’s Declaration 37 Supervisor’s Allowance 38 2 Introduction It is said that the different tools of environmental enrichment influence the animal behaviour and/or physiological status. In the frame of the 3rd „R” (Refinement), the laboratory animal science focuses on the feeding and on the physical environment of the animals, which are related to sensory stimuli, like visual, olphactory, auditory and tactile (BAUMAN et al. 2011). Noise is also one of the elements of the environment. It has been thought as a powerful stressor. Generally, the acoustic environment does has an influence on the behaviour and physiological state of the humans and animals. Amongst those the different noises have an important role. According to the general definition the noise is the ratio of the meaningful signals and those, of carrying no information. In the bioacoustics, noise is equals to the unpleasant sounds. The noise music is an avangard music and sound art, which is akin to the futurism and dadaism. It is employing the elements of cacophony, dissonance, atonality, noise, irregularity and repetition. The manifesto of Luigi Russolo: The art of noise (1913) is considered as the first step of this movement.
    [Show full text]
  • Subject Index
    Subject Index Action-specific energy, 29, 135, 152 table 6.1, Aggressive behavior (aggression). See also 154, 247. See also Ethology; Surplus Energy Playfighting; Serious fighting Theory development in, 220, 347–348, 365–367 Adaptive function (value), 11, 121 fish, 347 ethological aim three, 11–12, 15 table 1.1, 18, frog, 310 70 human, 383 methods, basic means of studying, 114–117 and play scales, 68 Addictions. See also Brain areas; Dopamine recreational, 388, 401 drugs, 383, 390–392, 398, 404 redirected, 332, 338 gambling, 7, 10, 13, 24–25, 39, 391–393 sex differences, 53 rewards systems, 391–393 and social isolation, 388 Affect. See Emotion Appetitive behavior, 40, 134, 402. See also Amphetamines Instinctive behavior depression, treatment of, 39 and behavioral and drug addictions and stereotypies, 62–64 Aristotle’s four causes, 13 use, 59, 60 Autotelic Theory, 40–41, 102, 108, 136 Animal behavior, 6, 13, 133, 186 Animal Mind (Washburn, Margaret Floy), 186– Batesian mimicry, 363 189 Behavioral catalog. See Ethogram Animal welfare, 386 Behavioral ecology (ists), 12, 133–134, 286, Anthropomorphism(ic), 73, 92, 184–189 385 approach to play, 13, 109 Behavioral repertoire and bird play, 246–247, 272 instinctive, 170–171 critical, 67, 178, 186, 189 fiddler crabs and, 370 and fish play, 316, 319, 326, 331, 357 genes of play, 380 and invertebrates, 359, 370 keas and, 257 and locomotor play, 84, 276 octopuses and, 372 by omission, 187 snakes and, 291 and parental care, 51 species characteristic, 30, 83, 135, 149, and play signals, 92 385 uncritical, 6, 26, 246, 328, 374 and surplus resource theory, 165, 129 492 Subject Index Behavioral study, 48–49 Cognitive development, 41–42 operational definitions, 48 Piagetian stages of, 36, 96–99, 131 Behavioral systems Comparative biology(ists), 3, 10 and motivation, 133–138 Comparative psychology, 10, 12, 24, 48, 145, play types, 150 185, 315, 362.
    [Show full text]
  • The Scope of Neuroethology
    THE BEHAVIORAL AND BRAIN SCIENCES (1984) 7, 367-412 Printed in the United States of America The scope of neuroethology Graham Hoyle Institute of Neuroscience, University of Oregon, Eugene, Oreg. 97403 Abstract: Neuroethology, an interdisciplinary subdivision of neuroscience, has emerged in recent years. Since 1976 there has been a regular session under this heading at the annual meeting of the Society for Neuroscience. In 1980 two introductory texts in English were published on the subject (Ewert 1980; Guthrie 1980), and a third (Camhi 1984) was published recently. There is widespread interest in neural mechanisms underlying behavior, but they encompass such a vast array of often unrelated topics that proponents do not share common goals. This article describes the emergence of ethology as a discipline, pointing out that its practitioners were successful because they confined their research to stereotyped, complex, nonlearned, innate behavioral acts. A limited number of profoundly significant principles emerged. Each of these is redefined. The major concepts of earlier ethology were embodied in a simple hydraulic model used by Konrad Lorenz in 1949 (Lorenz 1950). It is pointed out that this model implies the existence of common neurophysiological mechanisms and neuronal circuitry. This model has now been made obsolete by neurophysiological progress, but with appropriate ~nodificationsan updated version may still be useful in focusing attention on possible principles. The initial aim of neuroethology should be to examine the neurophysiological events in a variety of behaviors, exhibited by diverse animals from different phyla, which meet the criteria of innate behavioral acts. The behaviors should be sufficiently complex to interest ethologists, yet they should be addressable with neurophysiological methods down to the cellular level.
    [Show full text]
  • Evolution and Animal Welfare Author(S): Marian Stamp Dawkins Source: the Quarterly Review of Biology, Vol
    Evolution and Animal Welfare Author(s): Marian Stamp Dawkins Source: The Quarterly Review of Biology, Vol. 73, No. 3 (Sep., 1998), pp. 305-328 Published by: The University of Chicago Press Stable URL: http://www.jstor.org/stable/3036918 . Accessed: 19/07/2013 06:03 Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at . http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp . JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide range of content in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new forms of scholarship. For more information about JSTOR, please contact [email protected]. The University of Chicago Press is collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and extend access to The Quarterly Review of Biology. http://www.jstor.org This content downloaded from 129.67.117.205 on Fri, 19 Jul 2013 06:03:00 AM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions VOLUME 73, No. 3 THE QUARTERLY REVIEW OF BIOLOGY SEPTEMBER 1998 EVOLUTION AND ANIMAL WELFARE MARIAN STAMP DAWKINS Departmentof Zoology, Universityof Oxford Oxford,OX] 3PS United Kingdom E-MAIL: [email protected] ABSTRACT Animalwelfare is a tbpicoften thought to reside outside mainstream biology. The complexity of themethods used to assess welfare (such as health,physiology, immunological state, and behavior) requirean understandingof a widerange of biological phenomena. Furthermore, the "welfare" of an animal providesa frameworkin whicha diversityof its responsescan be understoodas fitness-enhancingmechanisms.Different methodsfor assessing animal welfare are discussed, with particularemphasis on therole of an animal's own choicesand reinforcementmechanisms.
    [Show full text]
  • Bio 314 Animal Behv Bio314new
    NATIONAL OPEN UNIVERSITY OF NIGERIA SCHOOL OF SCIENCE AND TECHNOLOGY COURSE CODE: BIO 314: COURSE TITLE: ANIMAL BEHAVIOUR xii i BIO 314: ANIMAL BEHAVIOUR Course Writers/Developers Miss Olakolu Fisayo Christie Nigerian Institute for Oceanography and Marine Research, No 3 Wilmot Point Road, Bar-beach Bus-stop, Victoria Island, Lagos, Nigeria. Course Editor: Dr. Adesina Adefunke Ministry of Health, Alausa. Lagos NATIONAL OPEN UNIVERSITY OF NIGERIA xii i BIO 314 COURSE GUIDE Introduction Animal Behaviour (314) is a second semester course. It is a two credit units compustory course which all students offering Bachelor of Science (BSc) in Biology must take. This course deals with the theories and principles of adaptive behaviour and evolution of animals. The course contents are history of ethology. Reflex and complex behaviour. Orientation and taxes. Fixed action patterns, releasers, motivation and driver. Displays, displacement activities and conflict behaviour. Learning communication and social behaviour. The social behaviour of primates. Hierarchical organization. The physiology of behaviour. Habitat selection, homing and navigation. Courtship and parenthood. Biological clocks. What you will learn in this course In this course, you have the course units and a course guide. The course guide will tell you briefly what the course is all about. It is a general overview of the course materials you will be using and how to use those materials. It also helps you to allocate the appropriate time to each unit so that you can successfully complete the course within the stipulated time limit. The course guide also helps you to know how to go about your Tutor-Marked-Assignment which will form part of your overall assessment at the end of the course.
    [Show full text]
  • Animal Behavior Desk Reference
    ANIMAL BEHAVIOR DESK REFERENCE A Dictionary of Animal Behavior, Ecology, and Evolution Third Edition ANIMAL BEHAVIOR DESK REFERENCE A Dictionary of Animal Behavior, Ecology, and Evolution Third Edition Edward M. Barrows CRC Press Taylor & Francis Group 6000 Broken Sound Parkway NW, Suite 300 Boca Raton, FL 33487-2742 © 2011 by Taylor and Francis Group, LLC CRC Press is an imprint of Taylor & Francis Group, an Informa business No claim to original U.S. Government works Printed in the United States of America on acid-free paper 10 9 8 7 6 5 4 3 2 1 International Standard Book Number: 978-1-4398-3651-4 (Hardback) This book contains information obtained from authentic and highly regarded sources. Reasonable efforts have been made to publish reliable data and information, but the author and publisher cannot assume responsibility for the validity of all materials or the consequences of their use. The authors and publishers have attempted to trace the copyright holders of all material reproduced in this publication and apologize to copyright holders if permission to publish in this form has not been obtained. If any copyright material has not been acknowledged please write and let us know so we may rectify in any future reprint. Except as permitted under U.S. Copyright Law, no part of this book may be reprinted, reproduced, transmitted, or utilized in any form by any electronic, mechanical, or other means, now known or hereafter invented, including photocopying, microfilming, and recording, or in any information storage or retrieval system, without written permission from the publishers. For permission to photocopy or use material electronically from this work, please access www.copyright.com (http://www.copyright.com/) or contact the Copyright Clearance Center, Inc.
    [Show full text]
  • Behavioural Deprivation: a Central Problem in Animal Welfare
    Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 20 (1988) 209-225 209 Elsevier Science Publishers B.V., Amsterdam -- Printed in The Netherlands Behavioural Deprivation: A Central Problem in Animal Welfare MARIAN STAMP DAWKINS Animal Behaviour Research Group, Department of Zoology, University of Oxford (Gt. Britain) (Accepted for publication 27 January 1988) ABSTRACT Dawkins, M.S., 1988. Behavioural deprivation: a central problem in animal welfare. Appl. Anita. Behav. Sci., 20: 209-225. A key issue in animal welfare is whether keeping animals in conditions where they cannot or do not perform behaviour typical of more naturally-kept members of their species causes them to suffer. Various measures have been used to resolve this issue. The cost an animal is prepared to pay for the opportunity to perform different behaviour can be used as a measure of the importance of that behaviour to the animal. Manipulation of time-budgets is the most reliable method of measuring such costs and of relating "deprivation" to "suffering". 1. INTRODUCTION To be concerned about animal welfare is to be concerned with the subjective feelings of animals, particularly the unpleasant subjective feelings of suffering and pain (Dawkins, 1980; Baxter, 1983b; van Rooijin, 1984; Zayan and Dun- can, 1987). One of the most important tasks in the study of animal welfare is, therefore, to identify observable external indicators of these internal subjective feelings. Of these, health criteria are the least controversial, since disease and injury are widely accepted as sources of suffering (e.g. Loliger, 1985 ). However, where the animal is apparently healthy and the suffering, if it exists, does not result in clinical symptoms, there is still a need to clarify which indicators, both physiological and behavioural, are the most useful (van Putten, 1981; Brown, 1981; Tschantz, 1982; Banks, 1982; Dantzer et al., 1983; Smidt, 1983; Craig and Adams, 1984; Duncan 1974; Ewbank, 1985; McBride and Craig, 1985 ).
    [Show full text]
  • Cycles of Contingency
    Cycles of Contingency Cycles Cover image: Of related interest Cycles of Contingency Developmental Systems and Evolution A new theoretical approach to the evolution of develop- The Evolution of Cognition Cycles of Contingency edited by Susan Oyama, Paul E. Griffiths, and mental systems and the relationship between genes edited by Cecilia Heyes and Ludwig Huber Russell D. Gray and biological form, such as advocated in this volume, Developmental Systems and Evolution also entails new tools of empirical analysis and hypoth- In the last decade, "evolutionary psychology" has come to refer exclusively to research on The nature/nurture debate is not dead. esis testing. Much of the present methodology of genet- human mentality and behavior, motivated by a nativist interpretation of how evolution Dichotomous views of development still underlie ic, developmental research is geared toward disclosing operates. This book encompasses the behavior and mentality of nonhuman as well as many fundamental debates in the biological and the very proximate roles of individual genes in single, human animals and a full range of evolutionary approaches. Rather than a collection by social sciences. Developmental systems theory mostly two-dimensional, snapshots of developmental and for the like-minded, it is a debate about how evolutionary processes have shaped (DST) offers a new conceptual framework with events. But development is a genuinely three-dimen- cognition. The debate is divided into five sections: Orientations, Categorization, Causality, which to resolve such debates. DST views ontogeny sional process of coordinating cell behavior over time, Consciousness, and Culture. as contingent cycles of interaction among a varied which requires an understanding of the dynamics of set of developmental resources, no one of which interactions among genes, cells, and tissues, which Where Biology Meets Psychology controls the process.
    [Show full text]
  • Wed. Aug 31 Outline FIXED ACTION PATTERNS
    8/26/2011 BIONB 4240: NEUROETHOLOGY L02: ROOTS in ETHOLOGY Writing Assignment Aug. 26, 2011; C. D. Hopkins W3: Due Monday Aug. 29 Apis melifera, Frisch’s 1) How does a reflex differ from a fixed action favorite study species. pattern? 2) Define “Instinct” Tinbergen uses models to evoke 3) Define IRM (Innate Releasing Mechanism) egg rolling behavior. He discovers Herring gull chicks peck at the red spot on the parent’s bill, or a model, but prefer that convex objects work better a red stick with candy-stripe patterns at than cubic objects. the sharp tip: a super-normal stimulus. Make stickleback fish (above) respond to models of female that have enlarged Lorenz’s psychohydraulic model of motivation explains the changing abdomens. threshold of a stimulus. Control theory model to the right of the same. First Discussion: Wed. Aug 31 Outline Kaas and Catania (2002) How do features of sensory 1. The ethological tradition representations develop? BioEssays 24 (4) 334-343. 2. Fixed action patterns 3. The ethogram Writing (W4) 4. Innate release mechanisms and sign stimuli 5. Courtship behavior of the common fruit fly Carl Hopkins will lead the discussion 6. Influential (neuro)ethologists Ethology and the Ethological Tradition FIXED ACTION PATTERNS The study of natural behavior of animals, usually conducted in the natural setting. - observe & describe behavior: “ethograms” - Tinbergen’s: goals of Ethology are to understand causation, ontogeny, function, and evolution of behavior. EGG ROLLING in Grey-lag Goose (Lorenz & Tinbergen, 1938) Neuroethologists: Waving display Taxis und Instinkthandlung in der in Fiddler crab. Observe behavior and make a descriptions.
    [Show full text]