Papers in Estonian Cognitive Linguistics

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Papers in Estonian Cognitive Linguistics Tartu Ülikooli üldkeeleteaduse õppetooli toimetised 2 Publications of the Department of General Linguistics 2 University of Tartu Papers in Estonian Cognitive Linguistics Tartu 2001 Tartu Ülikooli üldkeeleteaduse õppetooli toimetised 2 Publications of the Department of General Linguistics 2 University of Tartu Papers in Estonian Cognitive Linguistics Edited by Ilona Tragel Tartu 2001 Tartu Ülikooli üldkeeleteaduse õppetooli toimetised 2 Publications of the Department of General Linguistics 2 University of Tartu Toimetaja / Editor Ilona Tragel Kujundaja / Layout Roosmarii Kurvits ISSN 1406-619X ISBN 9985-4-0191-3 Tartu Ülikooli kirjastus Tiigi 78, Tartu 50410 Tellimus nr. 351 Contents Foreword................................................................................... 5 Is there a folk theory of Self. The case of Estonian ise and enese~enda 7 Haldur Õim On the conceptualization of time in Estonian 22 Ann Veismann Getting down to 'downs': some observations about the Estonian verb-particle constructions with alia and maha 48 Kaja Kährik On the genesis and loss of the adposition rinnas (‘abreast, beside’) in Literary Estonian 71 Külli Habicht Grammaticalization of või/vä questions in Estonian 90 Liina Lindström Tracing grammaticalization of oota 'wait' in Estonian conversation 119 Leelo Keevallik On Estonian core verbs 145 Ilona Tragel The polysemy of seisma 'to stand1: multiple motivations for multiple meanings 170 Renate Pajusalu Deictic projection in Estonian 192 Mari-Epp Tirkkonen Gestures in communication and their use for pointing and referring in space: Estonian examples 216 Silvi Tenjes Appendix 1 Transcription and glossing conventions 249 Appendix 2 List of abbreviations 250 Appendix 3 List of contributors 251 Foreword A cognitive linguistics seminar has regularly met at the Chair of Ge­ neral Linguistics of the University of Tartu since 1999. In addition to cognitive linguistics "proper", the seminar discussions have focussed on a range of topics within wider cognitive-functional framework. To a great extent, the present volume is a collection of papers by the participants in the cognitive linguistics seminar. This edition is published as the second volume of papers by the Chair of General Linguistics, University of Tartu, and the majority of the contributors are currently associated with the University of Tartu. Among the papers included, the metaphor theory is central to Ann Veismann's study of the conceptualization of time in Estonian and Kaja Kährik s treatment of Estonian verb-particle constructions with alia 'down' and maha down', related aspects are also present in Haldur Õirris analysis of the folk theories regarding the Estonian word ise. Several papers address the grammaticalization theory. Külli Habicht employs it to clarify the reasons for the emergence and loss of the adposition rinnas 'beside' in Old Literary Estonian. Liina Lindström is searching for the reasons why the connective või 'or' has developed into a question particle. Leelo Keevallik's paper applies the methods of conversation analysis to the study of oota 'wait' as a particle in spoken Estonian, and places it into the context of the grammaticalization theory. The papers include an analysis of the polysemy of the verb seisma stand' by Renate Pajusalu, a proposal by Ilona Tragel as to the criteria and candidates for the core verbs of the Estonian lan­ guage, a treatment of deictic projection in texts of spoken Estonian by Mari-Epp Tirkkonen and a report of a study into gestures by Silvi Tenjes. Ilona Tragel editor Is there a folk theory of Self: the case of Estonian ise and enese-enda Haldur Õim Universtity of Tartu 1. Background and theoretical considerations The English word self is used as a separate word only as a noun and it means usually personality or '’person' (typically, in its psycho­ logical aspect). In this sense it is translated into Estonian as mina (a noun which in the nominative case coincides - and this is not just a coincidence - with the first person singular personal pronoun mina T). This revealed his real self. See paljastas tema tõelise mina. But in various compounds (nouns, adjectives), for instance, self is mostly translated into Estonian by means of the pronoun ise, enese-enda (the parallel genitive forms): self-defense - enesekaitse self-esteem - eneseväärikus self-control - enesekontroll self-confident - enesekindel self-indulgent - enesekeskne, isemeelne self-righteous - ennasttäis self-sufficient - isemajandav, iseseisev In addition to these uses, however, self participates in forming re­ flexive pronouns, but only in compounds with personal pronouns: myself yourself himself herself, itself ourselves. These are also translated into Estonian by ise, enese-enda (in their corresponding case forms): He bought himself an ice cream. Ta ostis endale jäätise. The Estonian ise (from the genitive on enese-enda) has much broader use in the grammatical sense: it can also occur as a noun, an adjective, an adverb. Of these uses, I will concentrate here mainly on its use in re­ flexive constructions (including compound words and phraseological expressions). My point is that the analysis of the semantics of these 8 Is there a folk theory of Self expressions can reveal much about the folk, or naive, conception of Self, its structure, inherent in Estonian. For a grammarian, for instance, enese-enda (iseenese-iseenda) are reflexive pronouns with some morphological pecularities, but with syntactic uses typical of reflexive pronouns, and there is nothing more to talk about. In the most detailed descriptive grammar of Esto­ nian so far, Eesti keele grammatika (EKG I 1995), there is only half a page about reflexive pronouns in Estonian (p.28). By this I don't want to say that there should have been more. My intention is to point out that there can be considerable differences between the role some words (word forms) play in the grammatical system of a lan­ guage and the role these forms play in its semantic system. From the point of view of semantic analysis you could write a whole book about the forms of the Estonian reflexive pronouns enese-enda. Let us consider just some examples. (1a) Ta pesi end/ennast külma veega. He/she washed himself/herself with cold water. (1b) Ära haletse ennast kogu aeg! Don’t pity yourself all the time! (1c) Kontrolli ennast! Control yourself! (1d) Poiss ei andnud endale aru, mis ta teeb. The boy didn't realise what he was doing lit: The boy didn't give himself an account of what he was doing. (1e) Pidin endaga tükk aega võitlema, enne kui ... had to fight myself (lit: with myself) for a long time, before... (1f) Mehe enesekindlus sai tugeva hoobi. The man's self-confidence received a hard blow. What do the forms of enese-enda designate in these sentences when considered from the point of view of the person they refer to as re­ flexive pronouns? It is clear that in different sentences these forms refer to quite different “parts” of the corresponding person as a “self’ And, on the other hand, what “parts” do the corresponding subjects refer to in relation to the “part” referred to by reflexive pro­ Õim 9 nouns? In (la) it is apparent that ennast 'himself/herself'refers to the “physical part” of the person, but what about the uses in (lb) - (If)? When someone pities himself/herself (as in (lb)), ennast refers to “something” in the subject that is connected with one s achievements as related to goals, the social position, etc. But who (what “part”), then, is “doing the pitying”? When someone restrains oneself, fights “with oneself’, “pulls into oneself’ etc., what are the corresponding “parts” then? In such constructions there are always two different parts of Self under consideration, and semantically the reflexive pronoun in principle does not refer back to the same part as it refers to itself. Instead, these “parts” are interacting with one another, or otherwise stand in certain relationship with one another. One part functions as the Subject and the other part as the Object, or the Target, of the re­ lation. And in different cases the Subject and Target represent dif­ ferent parts of Self, depending on the predicate that represents the re­ lation between these parts. By a systematic analysis of such constructions we can get a more or less systematic picture of the structure of self as represented in the "folk theory of Self’ underlying the use of reflexive expres­ sions in the corresponding language - in this case, Estonian. The idea in itself is not new in cognitive semantics (and cogni­ tive anthropology). It is a commonly accepted presupposition that the lexicon (lexical system) of a language, or at least its certain coherently organized subfields are semantically based on tacit, but structured conceptualizations of the corresponding area in the reality that the words are about. Thus the analysis of these subfields can lead us to discovering the underlying conceptualizations, folk theo­ ries. In this case, though, I am not studying some group of semanti­ cally related lexical items, but specific constructions which include reflexive pronouns. Implicitly, of course, there is also a semantically delimited group of words - predicates (verbs, adjectives, but also nouns) that represent the relationship between the above-mentioned two parts of Self. These are mainly predicates denoting mental processes or states, but also various forms of communication (one can give oneself a promise, for instance). My focus in this paper is not on the group of predicates that can semantically occur in such constructions, though. I do not deny that this would be an important task, in some sense even the key to the whole issue (why do we take 2 10 Is there a folk theory of Self it as natural, for instance, that one can promise something to him­ self/herself, but we hardly do so in case of one threatening him­ self/herself?). But this would be too comprehensive a task to under­ take within the confines of the given paper.
Recommended publications
  • Your Name Here
    THE HISTORY OF THE FUTURE: MORPHOPHONOLOGY, SYNTAX, AND GRAMMATICALIZATION by LAMAR ASHLEY GRAHAM (Under the Direction of L. Chadwick Howe) ABSTRACT The appearance of new verbal paradigms in the Romance languages – namely, the synthetic future indicative and conditional paradigms – has been one of the hallmark studies within the field of grammaticalization. Already existing in Latin was a synthetic future indicative paradigm, with a full complement of endings. Many researchers (Garey 1955; Hopper and Traugott 2003; Slobbe 2004; among others) have agreed that the infinitive + HABĒRE construction was the genesis of Romance future paradigms, obviating and completely replacing the disfavored Latin synthetic future. However, even after Latin evolved into Spanish, there still existed a duality between a periphrastic, analytic future and a paradigmatic, synthetic future. Said duality implied several syntactic and morphophonological characteristics of Old and Classical Spanish, some of which are no longer extant in the modern language. Through an empirical study, employing variable rules analyses, of Old and Classical Spanish texts, it is shown that the synthetic and analytic future and conditional were not always fully complementary of another, as the synthetic began to be used in contexts customarily reserved for the analytic construction. Within this work it will be shown how morphophonological and syntactic properties necessitated the existence of the analytic construction, especially the status of the auxiliary-turned-affix-turned- inflectional ending haber. Also of interest will be the manner in which the future and conditional paradigms derived syntactically in Old and Classical Spanish, with special attention lent to the role of cliticization principles in their structure.
    [Show full text]
  • Lecture No .7 30Th March 2020 Word Formation Processes
    جامعة المنيا، كلية اﻷلسن ,Mini University, Faculty of Alsun علم الصرف ، الفرقة الثانية، Morphology , Second Year ,2020 0202 المحاضر د.إيمان العيسوى Instructor Dr Eman Elesawy Lecture no .7 30th March 2020 Word Formation Processes/ Part 3 Aims: 1.To know word formation processes and their role in enriching language vocabulary 2.To be able to judge on the word-formation process used in creating words. Main Points Word-formation processes: Are specific techniques used by language users to add new vocabulary items to their language. These processes are similar between languages, still some languages might have other processes/resources than others. In this lecture we are going to discuss four of these processes, namely, derivation, reduplication and folk etymology. 1. What is derivation? When small bits, affixes, are added to a word. There are three types. Prefixes:added before the word, e.g. Mis-, un-, pre- Suffixes:added after the word, e.g.-less, -full, -ish, -ness (foolishness hence has two suffixes) Infixes:added in the middle of the word,e.g. -bloody-, -goddam- جامعة المنيا، كلية اﻷلسن ,Mini University, Faculty of Alsun علم الصرف ، الفرقة الثانية، Morphology , Second Year ,2020 0202 المحاضر د.إيمان العيسوى Instructor Dr Eman Elesawy 2. What is Reduplication: Reduplication is sometimes called rhyming compounds (subtype of compounds) These words are compounded from two rhyming words. Examples: lovey-dovey chiller-killer There are words that are formally very similar to rhyming compounds, but are not quite compounds in English because the second element is not really a word--it is just a nonsense item added to a root word to form a rhyme.
    [Show full text]
  • Kenstowicz's "Paradigmatic Uniformity and Contrast"
    Paradigmatic Uniformity and Contrast∗ Michael Kenstowicz Massachusetts Institute of Technology This paper reviews several cases where either the grammar strives to maintain the same output shape for pairs of inflected words that the regular phonology should otherwise drive apart (paradigmatic uniformity) or where the grammar strives to keep apart pairs of inflected words that the regular phonology threatens to merge (paradigmatic contrast). 1. Introduction The general research question which this paper addresses is the proper treatment of cases of opacity in which the triggering or blocking context for a phonological process is found in a paradigmatically related word. Chomsky & Halle's (1968) discussion of the minimal pair comp[´]nsation vs. cond[E]nsation is a classic example of the problem. In general, the contrast between a full vowel vs. schwa is predictable in English as a function of stress; but comp[´]nsation vs. ∗ An initial version of this paper was read at the Harvard-MIT Phonology 2000 Conference (May 1999); a later version was delivered at the 3èmes Journées Internationales du GDR Phonologie, University of Nantes (June 2001). I thank the editors as well as François Dell and Donca Steriade for helpful comments. 2 cond[E]nsation have the same σ` σ ´σ σ stress contour and thus raise the question whether English schwa is phonemic after all. SPE's insight was that the morphological bases from which these words are derived provide a solution to the problem: comp[´]nsate has a schwa while cond[E@]nse has a full stressed vowel. Chomsky and Halle's suggestion is that such paradigmatic relations among words can be described by embedding the derivation of one inside the derivation of the other.
    [Show full text]
  • Prosodic Morphology: Constraint Interaction and Satisfaction John J
    University of Massachusetts Amherst ScholarWorks@UMass Amherst Linguistics Department Faculty Publication Series Linguistics January 1993 Prosodic Morphology: Constraint Interaction and Satisfaction John J. McCarthy University of Massachusetts, Amherst, [email protected] Alan Prince Follow this and additional works at: https://scholarworks.umass.edu/linguist_faculty_pubs Part of the Morphology Commons, Near Eastern Languages and Societies Commons, and the Phonetics and Phonology Commons Recommended Citation McCarthy, John J. and Prince, Alan, "Prosodic Morphology: Constraint Interaction and Satisfaction" (1993). Linguistics Department Faculty Publication Series. 14. Retrieved from https://scholarworks.umass.edu/linguist_faculty_pubs/14 This Book is brought to you for free and open access by the Linguistics at ScholarWorks@UMass Amherst. It has been accepted for inclusion in Linguistics Department Faculty Publication Series by an authorized administrator of ScholarWorks@UMass Amherst. For more information, please contact [email protected]. Prosodic Morphology Constraint Interaction and Satisfaction John J. McCarthy Alan Prince University of Massachusetts, Amherst Rutgers University Copyright © 1993, 2001 by John J. McCarthy and Alan Prince. Permission is hereby granted by the authors to reproduce this document, in whole or in part, for personal use, for instruction, or for any other non-commercial purpose. Table of Contents Acknowledgments ......................................................... v Introduction to
    [Show full text]
  • Lightest to the Right: an Apparently Anomalous Displacement in Irish Ryan Bennett Emily Elfner James Mccloskey
    Lightest to the Right: An Apparently Anomalous Displacement in Irish Ryan Bennett Emily Elfner James McCloskey This article analyzes mismatches between syntactic and prosodic con- stituency in Irish and attempts to understand those mismatches in terms of recent proposals about the nature of the syntax-prosody interface. It argues in particular that such mismatches are best understood in terms of Selkirk’s (2011) Match Theory, working in concert with con- straints concerned with rhythm and phonological balance. An appar- ently anomalous rightward movement that seems to target certain pronouns and shift them rightward is shown to be fundamentally a phonological process: a prosodic response to a prosodic dilemma. The article thereby adds to a growing body of evidence for the role of phonological factors in shaping constituent order. Keywords: syntax, prosody, VSO order, rightward movement, weak pronouns, prosodic displacement 1 Introduction What are the mechanisms that shape word order in natural language? A traditional and still widely favored answer to that question is that syntax has exclusive responsibility in this domain; in some traditions of investigation, in fact, syntax just is the study of word order. More recently the possibility has emerged, though, that word order is determined postsyntactically, in the process of what Berwick and Chomsky (2011) call ‘‘externalization’’—the translation of the hierarchical and recursive representations characteristic of syntax and semantics into the kinds of serial repre- sentations that the sensorimotor systems can manipulate. Given that overall conception, it is natural that some aspects of constituent order should be shaped by demands particular to phonology, and in recent years there have been many studies arguing for the role of phonology in shaping word The research reported on here has its origins in the meetings of the Prosody Interest Group at the University of California, Santa Cruz.
    [Show full text]
  • 1 Chapter 8 Historical Linguistics
    Chapter 8 Historical Linguistics <Start chapter overview> Overview In this chapter your will learn about historical linguistics, the subfield of linguistics that studies language change and past language stages. You will • acquire an understanding of the development of languages across time • learn about the changes that occur in their phonology, morphology, syntax, and lexicon • compare present language stages to past language stages • learn about the reasons for language change • gain knowledge of the reconstruction of unattested languages • study and apply the methods of comparative reconstruction <End chapter overview> <H1> 8.1 What is historical linguistics? Historical linguistics is the field that studies past language stages (“dead languages”) and language change over time. Languages are constantly changing, and historical linguists study how and why this happens. • Historical linguists study languages that are no longer spoken, like Old English or Old French • Historical linguists also study changes between two language stages, for example, the changes from Old English to Middle English and from Middle English to Modern English • Historical linguists moreover reconstruct even earlier language stages based on attested languages You have probably noticed that the variety of English you speak (your personal idiolect) is subtly different from the English of your grandparents. Over time, such miniscule changes accumulate and lead to quite profound differences between past and present stages of a language – just compare your idiolect to the English of William Shakespeare around 1600 CE, or to that of the Old English poem Beowulf (ca. 800 CE). Languages change constantly, and there is nothing we can do about it. Yet there is a common notion that language change is somehow inherently “bad”, a sign of deterioration or just general sloppiness on the 1 part of “lazy” or “uneducated” speakers or “bad parents”.
    [Show full text]
  • THE BRETON of the CANTON of BRIEG 11 December
    THE BRETON OF THE CANTON OF BRIEC1 PIERRE NOYER A thesis submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy Celtic Studies Program Faculty of Arts and Social Sciences The University of Sydney 2019 1 Which will be referred to as BCB throughout this thesis. CONCISE TABLE OF CONTENTS DETAILED TABLE OF CONTENTS ............................................................................................ 3 DEDICATION ............................................................................................................................... 21 ABBREVIATIONS USED IN THIS WORK ................................................................................. 24 1. INTRODUCTION ...................................................................................................................... 29 2. PHONOLOGY ........................................................................................................................... 65 3. MORPHOPHONOLOGY ......................................................................................................... 115 4. MORPHOLOGY ....................................................................................................................... 146 5. SYNTAX .................................................................................................................................... 241 6. LEXICON.................................................................................................................................. 254 7. CONCLUSION.........................................................................................................................
    [Show full text]
  • State-Of-The-Article: Distributed Morphology Heidi Harley and Rolf Noyer, University of Pennsylvania Appeared in GLOT 4.4, April 1999, Pp
    1 State-of-the-Article: Distributed Morphology Heidi Harley and Rolf Noyer, University of Pennsylvania appeared in GLOT 4.4, April 1999, pp. 3-9 Whenever a major revision to the architecture of UG is proposed, it takes some time for sufficient work to accumulate to allow evaluation of the viability of the proposal, as well as for its broad outlines to become familiar to those not immediately involved in the investigation. The introduction of Distributed Morphology in the early 1990s, by Morris Halle and Alec Marantz, is a case in point. In the four-year period since the first paper outlining the framework appeared, a reasonably substantial body of work has appeared, addressing some of the key issues raised by the revision. The goal of this article is to introduce the motivation and core assumptions for the framework, and at the same time provide some pointers to the recent work which revises and refines the basic DM proposal and increases DM’s empirical coverage. Since the particular issues we discuss cover such a broad range of territory, we do not attempt to provide complete summaries of individual papers, nor, for the most part, do we attempt to relate the discussion of particular issues to the much broader range of work that has been done in the general arena. What we hope to do is allow some insight into (and foster some discussion of) the attitude that DM takes on specific issues, with some illustrative empirical examples. This article is organized as follows. Section 1 sketches the layout of the grammar and discusses the division of labor between its components.
    [Show full text]
  • Quo Vadis Morphology? MMM10 On-Line Proceedings
    Quo vadis morphology? MMM10 On-line Proceedings Edited by: Jenny Audring Francesca Masini Wendy Sandler UNIVERSITY OF LEIDEN | UNIVERSITY OF BOLOGNA | UNIVERSITY OF HAIFA Quo vadis morphology? MMM10 On-line Proceedings Edited by: Jenny Audring Francesca Masini Wendy Sandler On-line Proceedings of the Tenth Mediterranean Morphology Meeting (MMM10) Haifa, Israel, 7-10 September 2015 UNIVERSITY OF LEIDEN | UNIVERSITY OF BOLOGNA | UNIVERSITY OF HAIFA The MMM Permanent Committee Jenny Audring (Leiden University) Geert Booij (Leiden University) Nikos Koutsoukos (Université catholique de Louvain) Francesca Masini (University of Bologna) Angela Ralli (University of Patras) Sergio Scalise (University of Bologna) on-line proceedings of the mediterranean morphology meetings ISSN: 1826-7491 MMM10 Online Proceedings iii Table of contents Foreword ......................................................................................................................................... v Abstracts ......................................................................................................................................... vi Suspended affixation with derivational suffixes and lexical integrity Faruk Akkuş .................................................................................................................................... 1 The Semitic templates from the perspective of reciprocal predicates Elitzur A. Bar-Asher Siegal .......................................................................................................... 16
    [Show full text]
  • On Some Effects of Utterance Finality, with Special Considera- Tion of South Asian Languages
    View metadata, citation and similar papers at core.ac.uk brought to you by CORE provided by Journal of South Asian Linguistics On some effects of utterance finality, with special considera- tion of South Asian languages HANS HENRICH HOCK, University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign Received JANUARY 2019; Revised OCTOBER 2019 ABSTRACT Pitch lowering, avoidance of prosodic prominence, and segmental reductions in utterance-final position are well known crosslinguistic tendencies. In verb-final languages the prosodic effects of Utterance Finality intersect with an independent, crosslinguistic tendency of verbs to receive relatively weak prominence within larger prosodic domains. As a consequence, verbs in SOV languages are special targets for the effects of Utterance Finality. After providing crosslinguis- tic illustrations of these effects I focus on a number of phenomena in South Asian languages which can be explained in terms of the intersection between Utterance Finality and Verb Final- ity. These include the relative order of negation and verb and the apparent optionality of ‘be’- deletion in Hindi, the difference in verb accentuation between main and dependent clauses in Vedic, and (possibly) the fact that Kashmiri ki/zi-clauses, unlike relative clauses, have V2, ra- ther than verb-final order. 1 Introduction As a historical process, the crosslinguistically common phenomenon of final devoicing (Hock 1986/1991: 88, 92–93, 95–96) has been known at least since the 19th century, and as a synchronic phenomenon it has figured prominently in both Praguian and generative phonology. Although its domain is commonly considered the word, it has also been suggested that it originates in a larger prosodic domain, i.e.
    [Show full text]
  • Word-Formation and Reduplication in Standard Arabic: a New 1 Distributed Morphology Approach 2
    Word-Formation and Reduplication in Standard Arabic: A New 1 Distributed Morphology Approach 2 Muteb Alqarni 3 King Khalid University 4 Reviewed and Accepted, to appear in PSiCL, mid-2022 5 Abstract 6 The current paper aims to capture the properties of reduplication within the distributed 7 morphology model (Halle and Marantz, 1993, 1994; Embick and Noyer, 2007; 8 Siddiqi, 2009; Embick, 2010). Taking Standard Arabic (SA) as a representative, the 9 article shows that SA, like many other languages, has both full and partial 10 reduplication. Full reduplication repeats entire stems while partial reduplication 11 doubles part of it. Rather than the available two analyses, i.e. the readjustment 12 approach (Raimy, 2000; Frampton, 2009) and the affixation approach (Haugen, 2008, 13 2010, 2011; Haugen and Harley, 2010), the current paper provides a novel approach 14 to the phenomenon of reduplication in the world languages. It argues that root 15 consonants and vowels should be decomposed into non-phonetic distinctive features 16 that undergo late insertion at PF. These non-phonetic distinctive features are supplied 17 with sound items at PF in the same fashion that the terminal nodes with 18 morphosyntactic features are fed with vocabulary items. This approach serves three 19 purposes. It accounts for speech errors, captures the non-concatenative morphology in 20 Semitic languages, and allows the reduplicant form to copy all the distinctive features 21 of the roots, yielding instances of full reduplication. Instances of partial reduplication 22 can be generated by root-sensitive impoverishment rules which target and delete some 23 of the features of the stem or the reduplicant form.
    [Show full text]
  • Grammaticalization, Constructions and the Incremental Development of Language: Suggestions from the Development of Degree Modifiers in English
    Grammaticalization, constructions and the incremental development of language: Suggestions from the development of Degree Modifiers in English Elizabeth Closs Traugott 1. Introduction When we consider the topic of “language evolution: cognitive and cultural factors”, one of the first questions that come to mind is what is meant by “evolution”: language change over the course of recorded history (the time- span of which is very short, some seven thousand years only), or the bio- logical and cognitive development of the language capacity in the species (the time-span of which is geological, possibly of over two hundred thou- sand years). As a historical linguist I will be discussing “language evolu- tion” in the first sense, assuming that the historical record (all written until about a century and a half ago, when live recording began) reflects modern cognitive ability and a stable stage in the evolved human language capacity.1 By hypothesis, millennia ago, there were earlier stages that had not evolved as far; possibly, millennia from now, it will evolve further, but neither the very distant past nor the future are accessible, and can only be the subject of speculation. Another question is which model of the human language capacity seems likely to be plausible in the pursuit of hypotheses about the origins and development of language in the biological sense. In this paper I explore some aspects of the relationship between linguistic constructions and grammaticalization. I assume that language is fundamen- tally a symbolic system that pairs form and meaning. Since constructions as theoretical objects are designed to capture systematic associations between form and meaning, I assume that constructions, conceived in recent tradi- tions of Construction Grammar (e.g., Goldberg 1995; Kay & Fillmore 1999), 1 When this modern capacity developed is controversial; so too is the extent to which its development was rapid or slow (see Wong 2005 for discussion).
    [Show full text]