UC Santa Cruz UC Santa Cruz Electronic Theses and Dissertations

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

UC Santa Cruz UC Santa Cruz Electronic Theses and Dissertations UC Santa Cruz UC Santa Cruz Electronic Theses and Dissertations Title Foot-conditioned phonotactics and prosodic constituency Permalink https://escholarship.org/uc/item/0hj9f173 Author Bennett, Ryan Thomas Publication Date 2012 Peer reviewed|Thesis/dissertation eScholarship.org Powered by the California Digital Library University of California UNIVERSITY OF CALIFORNIA SANTA CRUZ FOOT-CONDITIONED PHONOTACTICS AND PROSODIC CONSTITUENCY A dissertation submitted in partial satisfaction of the requirements for the degree of DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY in LINGUISTICS by Ryan T. Bennett September 2012 The Dissertation of Ryan T. Bennett is approved: Professor Junko Itô, Chair Professor Jaye Padgett, Chair Professor Armin Mester Assistant Professor Grant McGuire Dean Tyrus Miller Vice Provost and Dean of Graduate Studies Copyright © by Ryan T. Bennett 2012 Table of Contents List of Figures vi List of Tables vii Abstract ix Dedication xi Acknowledgments xii 1 Introduction 1 1.1 Background .................................. 8 1.1.1 Thecrucibleoffoot-conditionedphonotactics . .. 9 1.2 Empiricalcontributions. 13 1.3 Theoreticaloverview:thebasics . 14 1.3.1 Themetricalfoot ........................... 14 1.3.2 Non-accentualevidenceforfooting . 20 1.4 Twometricalframeworks........... ........... ..... 26 1.4.1 Prosodichierarchytheory . 26 1.4.2 Simplifiedbracketedgridtheory . 35 2 Rhythmicity, prominence, and the uniqueness of footing 44 2.1 Huariapano................................... 44 2.2 PhonologyofHuariapano. 45 2.2.1 Segmentalinventory ......... ........... ..... 45 2.2.2 Syllablestructure ........................... 46 2.2.3 Stressplacement ........................... 47 2.2.4 Coda [h] epenthesis.......................... 50 2.3 DisjointfootinginHuariapano? . .. 54 2.3.1 Stressplacement ........................... 55 2.3.2 Coda [h] epenthesis.......................... 56 2.4 TowardsaunifiedaccountofHuariapano . 59 2.4.1 Stressplacement ........................... 61 iii 2.4.2 Coda [h] epenthesis.......................... 67 2.4.3 Are epenthetic [h]smoraic? ..................... 76 2.4.4 OTimplementation.......................... 81 2.4.5 Exceptionalstress........................... 96 2.4.6 Exceptionalepenthesisblocking. 103 2.4.7 ComparisonwithGonzález(2003) . 107 2.4.8 Huariapano as an argument for prosodic constituency . 114 2.4.9 A derivational alternative to foot-initial prominence? . ... 119 2.4.10 Interimconclusion . 123 2.5 Evidenceforfoot-initialprominence . ... 124 2.5.1 Yupik.................................. 125 2.5.2 Russian................................. 127 2.5.3 TataltepecChatino .......... ........... ..... 129 2.5.4 Canela ................................. 131 2.5.5 Karo .................................. 132 2.5.6 Kaapor ................................. 136 2.5.7 TokyoJapanese ............................ 137 2.5.8 Initialprominenceinotherprosodicdomains . 138 2.5.9 Foot-initial augmentation and the weight-to-stress principle . 141 2.5.10 Foot-initial strengthening vs. weak branch weakening . ..... 143 2.6 Onrecursivefooting.............................. 144 2.6.1 Recursion atotherlevelsof theprosodichierarchy . ... 144 2.7 Projection, tiers, and metrical organization: mismatches in tonal lan- guages...................................... 149 2.7.1 MetricalorganizationinBamana . 150 2.8 Conclusion................................... 159 3 ‘Unbounded’ stress, bounded phonotactics: binary feet in non-rhythmic lan- guages 161 3.1 Introduction .................................. 161 3.2 Uspanteko ................................... 162 3.2.1 StressandtoneinUspanteko . 162 3.2.2 FootinginUspanteko. 168 3.2.3 Conclusion............................... 175 3.3 Digression: Uspanteko as an argument for parallel evaluation. 176 3.3.1 UspantekoinHarmonicSerialism. 177 3.3.2 Conclusion............................... 193 3.4 Irish....................................... 194 3.5 Irishplurals .................................. 194 3.5.1 A subregularity: -(e)anna and -(e)acha ............... 196 3.5.2 -(e)anna and -(e)acha ascontextualallomorphs . 199 3.5.3 TheIrishstresssystem . 202 3.5.4 The exceptional status of /ax/ .................... 207 iv 3.5.5 AnOTanalysisofIrishpluralallomorphy . 222 3.6 Moreon wspFt:sonority-drivenepenthesis . 242 3.7 Conclusion................................... 249 3.8 Someremainders ............................... 250 3.8.1 Syncope ................................ 250 3.8.2 Munsterplurals ............................ 251 3.9 Generaldiscussion .............................. 254 4 Foot structure and cognitive bias: an artificial grammar investigation 257 4.1 Introduction .................................. 257 4.2 Background .................................. 258 4.3 Thepervasivenessoffootstructure . 259 4.3.1 EOS systems and foot structure: case studies . 262 4.3.2 Thepuzzle............................... 263 4.4 Explanation and experimentation . 266 4.4.1 Artificialgrammarexperiments . 269 4.5 Experiment1.................................. 271 4.5.1 Stimuli ................................. 271 4.5.2 Design ................................. 276 4.5.3 Predictions............................... 280 4.5.4 Results ................................. 281 4.5.5 Experiment1discussion . 288 4.6 Computational modeling: the Hayes & Wilson Maximum Entropy Phono- tacticLearner ................................. 293 4.6.1 MaxEntresults ............................ 296 4.7 Experiment2.................................. 302 4.7.1 Design ................................. 304 4.7.2 Results ................................. 306 4.7.3 MaxEntmodelingofExperiment2 . 313 4.8 Generaldiscussion .............................. 315 4.8.1 Alternative explanations for footing in EOS languages . 316 4.8.2 Arethereanylanguageswithoutfeet? . 320 4.8.3 Consequencesforprosodicconstituency . 325 4.8.4 Conclusions .............................. 328 5 Conclusion 332 A MaxEnt specifications 336 v List of Figures 1.1 Theprosodichierarchy ............................ 28 2.1 Hasse diagram of core ranking arguments for stress-related constraints 95 3.1 Partial selectional structure of Irish plural morphology . ........ 201 4.1 Spectrogram, pitch track, and intensity contour for [sí.ki.tu] ....... 275 vi List of Tables 2.1 Huariapanovowelinventory . 46 2.2 Huariapanoconsonantinventory . 46 3.1 SomeIrishnominalparadigms. 195 3.2 Distributions of -(e)anna [-@n@] and -(e)acha [-@x@] ............. 198 3.3 CompoundingprefixesinIrish. 203 3.4 Quantity-sensitivity in Munster Irish . 208 3.5 Epenthesizingforms ............................. 242 3.6 Non-epenthesizingforms. 243 4.1 Schematic 3σ training phase stimuli (with assumed foot boundaries) . 272 4.2 Schematic 5σ test stimuli (initial stress only) . 278 4.3 d′ results, 3σ condition,Exp.1........................ 283 4.4 d′ results, 5σ condition, initial stress, Exp. 1 . 283 4.5 d′ results, 5σ condition, peninitial stress, Exp. 1 . 283 4.6 MeanRTs,3σ condition,Exp.1(inms). 284 4.7 MeanRTs,5σ condition,Exp.1(inms). 285 4.8 Significant fixed-effects in RT model for 3σ test stimuli, Exp. 1 . 286 4.9 Significant fixed-effects in RT model for 5σ test stimuli, Exp. 1 . 287 4.10 Versions of target phonotactic for Exp. 1 appearing in MaxEnt grammars 297 4.11 Correlation levels for MaxEnt scores (40 constraints) and empirical well- formedness scores from Exp. 1 (Spearman’s ρ)............... 298 4.12 Correlation levels for MaxEnt scores (100 constraints) and empirical well-formedness scores from Exp. 1 (Spearman’s ρ)............ 299 4.13 d′ results, 3σ condition,Exp.2........................ 307 4.14 d′ results, 5σ condition, initial stress, Exp. 2 . 307 4.15 d′ results, 5σ condition, peninitial stress, Exp. 2 . 307 4.16 Mean RTs, 3σ condition,Exp.2(inms). 308 4.17 Mean RTs, 5σ condition,Exp.2(inms). 308 4.18 Significant fixed-effects in RT model for 3σ test stimuli, Exp. 2 . 309 4.19 Significant fixed-effects in RT model for 5σ test stimuli, Exp. 2 . 310 4.20 Correlation levels for MaxEnt scores (40 constraints) and empirical well- formedness scores from Exp. 2 (Spearman’s ρ)............... 314 vii 4.21 Correlation levels for MaxEnt scores (100 constraints) and empirical well-formedness scores from Exp. 2 (Spearman’s ρ)............ 314 4.22DestressinginSBG .............................. 328 4.23 Prohibited phonotactic configuration from Experiments 1 and 2 in three prosodicframeworks .. ........... ........... ..... 329 A.1 Feature specifications for MaxEnt models (Chapter 4) . 337 viii Abstract Foot-conditioned phonotactics and prosodic constituency by Ryan T. Bennett There has been a recurrent debate in generative phonology concerning the inclusion of hierarchical prosodic structure in phonological representations. On one side, there are those who argue that prosodic structure plays an indispensable role in the conditioning of phonological phenomena, especially stress, intonation, and segmental phonotactics. On the other side of the divide are researchers who suggest that all such phenomena yield to empirically adequate non-structural analyses, which are independently fa- vored by criteria of theoretical parsimony. This dissertation focuses on one aspect of the larger debate over prosodic orga- nization: the existence of the metrical foot. In standard conceptions of phonological structure the foot is a prosodic constituent, falling between the syllable and the word, which mediates the assignment of word-level stress. The foot obviously has no role to play in non-structural theories of prosody. Such frameworks assume that stress
Recommended publications
  • Siollabas Ardleibhéal 1 (C1)
    Siollabas Ardleibhéal 1 (C1) Ionad na dTeangacha, Ollscoil na hÉireann, Má Nuad Clár lch 1. Réamhrá…………………………………………………….…………………… 3 2. Cad é atá i gceist le hArdleibhéal 1 (C1)? ………………………….…………... 4 2.1 Cur síos ginearálta ar Ardleibhéal 1 (C1)………………………….……….…… 4 2.2 Téamaí agus stór focal…………………………………..........…………………. 5 2.3 Feidhmeanna ……………………………….........…………................................ 8 3. Comhairle maidir le clár teagaisc a chur le chéile ................................................ 8 4. An fheasacht teanga agus an próiseas foghlama teanga ....................................... 9 4.1 Ginearálta ……………………………….........…………..................................... 9 4.2 Ról an mhúinteora ……………………………….........…………...................... 10 5. Na scileanna ……………………………….........…………............................... 12 6. Cluastuiscint ……………………………….........…………............................... 12 6.1 Ginearálta ……………………………….........…………................................... 12 6.2 Cé chomh maith? ……………………………….........…………....................... 12 6.3 Cineálacha téacsanna éisteachta ……………………….........…………............ 13 6.4 Scenarios, téacsanna agus tascanna ……………………….........…………....... 13 6.5 Strateisí le cuidiú le foghlaimeoirí agus iad ag éisteacht le téacscanna ............. 17 6.6 Treoracha don mhúinteoir le cuidiú le foghlaimeoirí agus iad ag éisteacht ........19 7. Labhairt ……………………………….........…………...................................... 20 7.1 Ginearálta ……………………………….........…………................................... 20 7.2 Cé chomh maith?
    [Show full text]
  • Downstep and Recursive Phonological Phrases in Bàsàá (Bantu A43) Fatima Hamlaoui ZAS, Berlin; University of Toronto Emmanuel-Moselly Makasso ZAS, Berlin
    Chapter 9 Downstep and recursive phonological phrases in Bàsàá (Bantu A43) Fatima Hamlaoui ZAS, Berlin; University of Toronto Emmanuel-Moselly Makasso ZAS, Berlin This paper identifies contexts in which a downstep is realized between consecu- tive H tones in absence of an intervening L tone in Bàsàá (Bantu A43, Cameroon). Based on evidence from simple sentences, we propose that this type of downstep is indicative of recursive prosodic phrasing. In particular, we propose that a down- step occurs between the phonological phrases that are immediately dominated by a maximal phonological phrase (휙max). 1 Introduction In their book on the relation between tone and intonation in African languages, Downing & Rialland (2016) describe the study of downtrends as almost being a field in itself in the field of prosody. In line with the considerable literature on the topic, they offer the following decomposition of downtrends: 1. Declination 2. Downdrift (or ‘automatic downstep’) 3. Downstep (or ‘non-automatic downstep’) 4. Final lowering 5. Register compression/expansion or register lowering/raising Fatima Hamlaoui & Emmanuel-Moselly Makasso. 2019. Downstep and recursive phonological phrases in Bàsàá (Bantu A43). In Emily Clem, Peter Jenks & Hannah Sande (eds.), Theory and description in African Linguistics: Selected papers from the47th Annual Conference on African Linguistics, 155–175. Berlin: Language Science Press. DOI:10.5281/zenodo.3367136 Fatima Hamlaoui & Emmanuel-Moselly Makasso In the present paper, which concentrates on Bàsàá, a Narrow Bantu language (A43 in Guthrie’s classification) spoken in the Centre and Littoral regions of Cameroon by approx. 300,000 speakers (Lewis et al. 2015), we will first briefly define and discuss declination and downdrift, as the language displays bothphe- nomena.
    [Show full text]
  • 5 Phonology Florian Lionnet and Larry M
    5 Phonology Florian Lionnet and Larry M. Hyman 5.1. Introduction The historical relation between African and general phonology has been a mutu- ally beneficial one: the languages of the African continent provide some of the most interesting and, at times, unusual phonological phenomena, which have con- tributed to the development of phonology in quite central ways. This has been made possible by the careful descriptive work that has been done on African lan- guages, by linguists and non-linguists, and by Africanists and non-Africanists who have peeked in from time to time. Except for the click consonants of the Khoisan languages (which spill over onto some neighboring Bantu languages that have “borrowed” them), the phonological phenomena found in African languages are usually duplicated elsewhere on the globe, though not always in as concen- trated a fashion. The vast majority of African languages are tonal, and many also have vowel harmony (especially vowel height harmony and advanced tongue root [ATR] harmony). Not surprisingly, then, African languages have figured dispro- portionately in theoretical treatments of these two phenomena. On the other hand, if there is a phonological property where African languages are underrepresented, it would have to be stress systems – which rarely, if ever, achieve the complexity found in other (mostly non-tonal) languages. However, it should be noted that the languages of Africa have contributed significantly to virtually every other aspect of general phonology, and that the various developments of phonological theory have in turn often greatly contributed to a better understanding of the phonologies of African languages. Given the considerable diversity of the properties found in different parts of the continent, as well as in different genetic groups or areas, it will not be possible to provide a complete account of the phonological phenomena typically found in African languages, overviews of which are available in such works as Creissels (1994) and Clements (2000).
    [Show full text]
  • Understanding Irish Spelling
    Understanding Irish Spelling A Handbook for Teachers and Learners Nancy Stenson and Tina Hickey Understanding Irish Spelling A Handbook for Teachers and Learners Nancy Stenson and Tina Hickey i © Stenson and Hickey 2018 ii Acknowledgements The preparation of this publication was supported by a grant from An Chomhairle um Oideachas Gaeltachta agus Gaelscolaíochta, and we wish to express our sincere thanks to COGG, and to Muireann Ní Mhóráin and Pól Ó Cainín in particular. We acknowledge most gratefully the support of the Marie Skłodowska-Curie Fellowship scheme for enabling this collaboration through its funding of an Incoming International Fellowship to the first author, and to UCD School of Psychology for hosting her as an incoming fellow and later an as Adjunct Professor. We also thank the Fulbright Foundation for the Fellowship they awarded to Prof. Stenson prior to the Marie Curie fellowship. Most of all, we thank the educators at first, second and third level who shared their experience and expertise with us in the research from which we draw in this publication. We benefitted significantly from input from many sources, not all of whom can be named here. Firstly, we wish to thank most sincerely all of the participants in our qualitative study interviews, who generously shared their time and expertise with us, and those in the schools that welcomed us to their classrooms and facilitated observation and interviews. We also wish to thank the participants at many conferences, seminars and presentations, particularly those in Bangor, Berlin, Brighton, Hamilton and Ottawa, as well as those in several educational institutions in Ireland who offered comments and suggestions.
    [Show full text]
  • The Colon As a Separate Prosodic Category: Tonal Evidence from Paicî (Oceanic, New Caledonia)
    The Colon as a Separate Prosodic Category: Tonal Evidence from Paicî (Oceanic, New Caledonia) Florian Lionnet 1. Introduction This paper presents new evidence supporting the inclusion of the colon (κ) as a separate category in the Prosodic Hierarchy, on the basis of tonal data from Paicî, an Oceanic language of New Caledonia. The colon is a constituent intermediate between the foot and the word, made of two feet, as schematized in (1) (Stowell 1979; Halle & Clements 1983: 18-19; Hammond 1987; Hayes 1995: 119; Green 1997; a.o.). (1) Prosodic Word (!) [{(σσ)F t(σσ)F t}κ]! | Colon (κ) {(σσ)F t(σσ)F t}κ | Foot (F t) (σσ)F t(σσ)F t | Syllable (σ) σσσσ | Mora (µ) Justification for the colon (κ) rests mostly on the existence of tertiary stress. As clearly summarized by Green (1997: 102), “it is clear that in order to derive four levels of stress (primary, secondary, tertiary, unstressed), four levels of structure (prosodic word, colon, foot, syllable) are called for.” Colon-based analyses have so far been proposed for a dozen languages, listed in (2) below. (2) a. Passamaquoddy (Stowell 1979; Hayes 1995: 215-216; Green 1997: 104-109), b. Tiberian Hebrew (Dresher 1981), c. Garawa (Halle & Clements 1983: 20-21; Halle & Vergnaud 1987: 43; Hayes 1995: 202), d. Hungarian (Hammond 1987; Hayes 1995: 330; Green 1997: 102-104), e. Maithili (Hayes 1995: 149-162), f. Eastern Ojibwa (Hayes 1995: 216-218; Green 1997: 109-112), g. Asheninca (Hayes 1995: 288-296; Green 1997 112-114), h. the Neo-Štokavian dialect of Serbo-Croatian (Green 1997: 115-116), i.
    [Show full text]
  • Part 1: Introduction to The
    PREVIEW OF THE IPA HANDBOOK Handbook of the International Phonetic Association: A guide to the use of the International Phonetic Alphabet PARTI Introduction to the IPA 1. What is the International Phonetic Alphabet? The aim of the International Phonetic Association is to promote the scientific study of phonetics and the various practical applications of that science. For both these it is necessary to have a consistent way of representing the sounds of language in written form. From its foundation in 1886 the Association has been concerned to develop a system of notation which would be convenient to use, but comprehensive enough to cope with the wide variety of sounds found in the languages of the world; and to encourage the use of thjs notation as widely as possible among those concerned with language. The system is generally known as the International Phonetic Alphabet. Both the Association and its Alphabet are widely referred to by the abbreviation IPA, but here 'IPA' will be used only for the Alphabet. The IPA is based on the Roman alphabet, which has the advantage of being widely familiar, but also includes letters and additional symbols from a variety of other sources. These additions are necessary because the variety of sounds in languages is much greater than the number of letters in the Roman alphabet. The use of sequences of phonetic symbols to represent speech is known as transcription. The IPA can be used for many different purposes. For instance, it can be used as a way to show pronunciation in a dictionary, to record a language in linguistic fieldwork, to form the basis of a writing system for a language, or to annotate acoustic and other displays in the analysis of speech.
    [Show full text]
  • Gender in Irish Between Continuity and Change1
    Gender in Irish between continuity and change1 Alessio S. Frenda Trinity College Dublin The gender system of Irish appears to have undergone a process of simplification: traditionally depending on both formal and semantic assignment rules, agreement in contemporary spoken Irish is still rather conservative within the noun phrase, but almost exclusively semantic anaphorically. Language contact and the resulting obsolescence seem to have had some influence on these developments: for instance, structures that have a functional counterpart in English seem more resilient than others. But language-internal developments, particularly the phonetic erosion and loss of word-final syllables, may have played an important role, too: similar developments have been observed in non-obsolescent languages like Dutch and French. In this article, I illustrate some specific aspects of the Irish situation with examples drawn from a corpus of spoken Irish and frame the simplification process in terms of structural convergence in the context of language contact. Keywords: Irish, grammatical gender, language change, language obsolescence, contact, convergence 1. Introduction In this article I present the findings of a comparison between two corpora of spoken Irish, representative, respectively, of a traditional Gaeltacht vari- 1 This research was funded by a Government of Ireland Exchange Scholarship and by an IRCHSS Postgraduate Scholarship. I am grateful to the following people for their com- ments on earlier drafts of this article: John Saeed, Brian Nolan, Gearóid Ó Donnchadha, Pauline Welby and two anonymous reviewers; thanks are also due to Eamonn Mullins for his assistance with the statistical analysis of the data. I take full responsibility for any errors or oversights.
    [Show full text]
  • Morphological Sources of Phonological Length
    Morphological Sources of Phonological Length by Anne Pycha B.A. (Brown University) 1993 M.A. (University of California, Berkeley) 2004 A dissertation submitted in partial satisfaction of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy in Linguistics in the Graduate Division of the University of California, Berkeley Committee in charge: Professor Sharon Inkelas (chair) Professor Larry Hyman Professor Keith Johnson Professor Johanna Nichols Spring 2008 Abstract Morphological Sources of Phonological Length by Anne Pycha Doctor of Philosophy in Linguistics University of California, Berkeley Professor Sharon Inkelas, Chair This study presents and defends Resizing Theory, whose claim is that the overall size of a morpheme can serve as a basic unit of analysis for phonological alternations. Morphemes can increase their size by any number of strategies -- epenthesizing new segments, for example, or devoicing an existing segment (and thereby increasing its phonetic duration) -- but it is the fact of an increase, and not the particular strategy used to implement it, which is linguistically significant. Resizing Theory has some overlap with theories of fortition and lenition, but differs in that it uses the independently- verifiable parameter of size in place of an ad-hoc concept of “strength” and thereby encompasses a much greater range of phonological alternations. The theory makes three major predictions, each of which is supported with cross-linguistic evidence. First, seemingly disparate phonological alternations can achieve identical morphological effects, but only if they trigger the same direction of change in a morpheme’s size. Second, morpheme interactions can take complete control over phonological outputs, determining surface outputs when traditional features and segments fail to do so.
    [Show full text]
  • Irish Landscape Names
    Irish Landscape Names Preface to 2010 edition Stradbally on its own denotes a parish and village); there is usually no equivalent word in the Irish form, such as sliabh or cnoc; and the Ordnance The following document is extracted from the database used to prepare the list Survey forms have not gained currency locally or amongst hill-walkers. The of peaks included on the „Summits‟ section and other sections at second group of exceptions concerns hills for which there was substantial www.mountainviews.ie The document comprises the name data and key evidence from alternative authoritative sources for a name other than the one geographical data for each peak listed on the website as of May 2010, with shown on OS maps, e.g. Croaghonagh / Cruach Eoghanach in Co. Donegal, some minor changes and omissions. The geographical data on the website is marked on the Discovery map as Barnesmore, or Slievetrue in Co. Antrim, more comprehensive. marked on the Discoverer map as Carn Hill. In some of these cases, the evidence for overriding the map forms comes from other Ordnance Survey The data was collated over a number of years by a team of volunteer sources, such as the Ordnance Survey Memoirs. It should be emphasised that contributors to the website. The list in use started with the 2000ft list of Rev. these exceptions represent only a very small percentage of the names listed Vandeleur (1950s), the 600m list based on this by Joss Lynam (1970s) and the and that the forms used by the Placenames Branch and/or OSI/OSNI are 400 and 500m lists of Michael Dewey and Myrddyn Phillips.
    [Show full text]
  • Galway and Roscommon Education and Training Board
    Galway and Roscommon Education and Training Board Annual Report 2016 2 Galway and Roscommon ETB Annual Report 2016 Contents: Chapter 1 – Organisation and Corporate Governance Chairperson’s Statement 7 CE Introduction 8 Legislative Background 9 Corporate Governance 10 Capital Projects 2014 13 Financial Information 16 Chapter 2 – Second Level Schools and Provision 18 Coláiste an Ardeaspaig Mhic Éil /Archbishop McHale College 19 Coláiste an Chreagáin 20 Coláiste an Eachréidh, An Coiléar Bán 21 Coláiste an Chláirín / Clarin College 22 Coláiste Bhaile Chláir 23 Coláiste Bhríde / St. Brigid’s College 24 Coláiste Chilleáin Naofa / St. Killian’s College 25 Coláiste Cholmcille 26 Coláiste Ghobnait 27 Coláiste Mhuirlinne / Merlin College 28 Coláiste na bPiarsach 29 Coláiste na Coiribe 30 Coláiste Naomh Éinne 31 Coláiste Naomh Eoin 32 Coláiste Naomh Feichín 33 Coláiste Pobail Ail Finn / Elphin Community College 34 Coláiste Pobail na Gaillimhe / Galway Community College 35 Coláiste Pobail na Mainistreach /Abbey Community College 36 Coláiste Pobail Ros Comáin / Roscommon Community College 37 Institiúid Theicniúil na Gaillimhe – GTI 38 Galway and Roscommon ETB Principals, Deputy Principals and Schools 39 Chapter 3 – Further Education and Training Services 40 Map highlighting the locations of GRETB schools and centres of Further Education and Training 42 Management Overview of Further Education and Training Services 2013/14 44 GRETB Training Centre 48 Community Education 49 Adult Guidance and Information Service 50 Provision of Teaching Services to
    [Show full text]
  • Your Name Here
    THE HISTORY OF THE FUTURE: MORPHOPHONOLOGY, SYNTAX, AND GRAMMATICALIZATION by LAMAR ASHLEY GRAHAM (Under the Direction of L. Chadwick Howe) ABSTRACT The appearance of new verbal paradigms in the Romance languages – namely, the synthetic future indicative and conditional paradigms – has been one of the hallmark studies within the field of grammaticalization. Already existing in Latin was a synthetic future indicative paradigm, with a full complement of endings. Many researchers (Garey 1955; Hopper and Traugott 2003; Slobbe 2004; among others) have agreed that the infinitive + HABĒRE construction was the genesis of Romance future paradigms, obviating and completely replacing the disfavored Latin synthetic future. However, even after Latin evolved into Spanish, there still existed a duality between a periphrastic, analytic future and a paradigmatic, synthetic future. Said duality implied several syntactic and morphophonological characteristics of Old and Classical Spanish, some of which are no longer extant in the modern language. Through an empirical study, employing variable rules analyses, of Old and Classical Spanish texts, it is shown that the synthetic and analytic future and conditional were not always fully complementary of another, as the synthetic began to be used in contexts customarily reserved for the analytic construction. Within this work it will be shown how morphophonological and syntactic properties necessitated the existence of the analytic construction, especially the status of the auxiliary-turned-affix-turned- inflectional ending haber. Also of interest will be the manner in which the future and conditional paradigms derived syntactically in Old and Classical Spanish, with special attention lent to the role of cliticization principles in their structure.
    [Show full text]
  • Lecture No .7 30Th March 2020 Word Formation Processes
    جامعة المنيا، كلية اﻷلسن ,Mini University, Faculty of Alsun علم الصرف ، الفرقة الثانية، Morphology , Second Year ,2020 0202 المحاضر د.إيمان العيسوى Instructor Dr Eman Elesawy Lecture no .7 30th March 2020 Word Formation Processes/ Part 3 Aims: 1.To know word formation processes and their role in enriching language vocabulary 2.To be able to judge on the word-formation process used in creating words. Main Points Word-formation processes: Are specific techniques used by language users to add new vocabulary items to their language. These processes are similar between languages, still some languages might have other processes/resources than others. In this lecture we are going to discuss four of these processes, namely, derivation, reduplication and folk etymology. 1. What is derivation? When small bits, affixes, are added to a word. There are three types. Prefixes:added before the word, e.g. Mis-, un-, pre- Suffixes:added after the word, e.g.-less, -full, -ish, -ness (foolishness hence has two suffixes) Infixes:added in the middle of the word,e.g. -bloody-, -goddam- جامعة المنيا، كلية اﻷلسن ,Mini University, Faculty of Alsun علم الصرف ، الفرقة الثانية، Morphology , Second Year ,2020 0202 المحاضر د.إيمان العيسوى Instructor Dr Eman Elesawy 2. What is Reduplication: Reduplication is sometimes called rhyming compounds (subtype of compounds) These words are compounded from two rhyming words. Examples: lovey-dovey chiller-killer There are words that are formally very similar to rhyming compounds, but are not quite compounds in English because the second element is not really a word--it is just a nonsense item added to a root word to form a rhyme.
    [Show full text]