6 • Great Ape Cognitive Systems ANNE E

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

6 • Great Ape Cognitive Systems ANNE E 6 • Great ape cognitive systems ANNE E. RUSSON Psychology Department, Glendon College of York University, Toronto INTRODUCTION CONCEPTS AND MODELS OF COGNITION This chapter considers cognition in great apes as inte- grated systems that orchestrate the many abilities that Situating great ape cognition comparatively hinges on great apes express, systems for which satisfactory char- mental processes that support symbolism, notably rep- acterizations remain elusive. In part, difficulties owe to resentation, metarepresentation, and hierarchization. research trends. Empirical studies have been guided by Weaker and stronger conceptualizations exist for each diverse and sometimes contradictory models, questions, and which is used affects assessments of great apes’ capa- measures, tasks, and living conditions. Performance bilities. levels have proven inconsistent across individuals, rear- Weak meanings of symbolism include reference by ing conditions, and testing conditions, and evidence is arbitrary convention (Peirce 1932/1960), using inter- patchy across species for virtually any facet of cogni- nal signs like mental images to stand for referents tion. Evidence on wild great apes, the most important rather than using direct sensations or motor actions, from an evolutionary perspective, is especially patchy and solving problems mentally versus experientially. In because research has favored captives; much of what the strong sense, symbolism refers to self-referring sys- is available was collected for other purposes, so it was tems wherein phenomena owe their significance and neither described nor analyzed with cognition in mind. even existence to other symbols in the system rather The issues at stake are also hard-felt ones that touch on than to sensorimotor entities (e.g., Deacon 1997; Donald the human–nonhuman boundary, so entrenched beliefs 2000; Langer 2000). Representation can refer to any infect how the literature is interpreted and even what of form of mental coding that stands for entities, perceptual it is read. included (Perner 1991; Whiten 2000) or, more strongly, Attempts have none the less been madetodevelop to recalling to mind or “re-presenting” mental codes an integrated model of great ape cognition using avail- forentities and simple object relations in the absence able evidence. They include both edited survey vol- of normal sensorimotor cues (P&M; Whiten 2000). umes (Matsuzawa 2001a; Parker, Mitchell & Miles 1999; Meanings of metarepresentation range from represent- Russon, Bard&Parker1996)andintegrative reviews, ing other representations (e.g., Leslie 1987; Matsuzawa three of the latter as major books (Byrne 1995 (RWB), 1991; Whiten & Byrne 1991) to representing a represen- Parker & McKinney 1999 (P&M); Tomasello & Call tation as a representation, i.e., an interpretation of a sit- 1997 (T&C)) and others as articles (e.g., Byrne 1997; uation (Perner 1991). Meanings of hierarchization span Suddendorf & Whiten 2001; Thompson & Oden 2000; creating new, higher-order cognitive structures from Whiten & Byrne 1991). My aim is not to analyze this lower-level ones (i.e., structures with superordinate– terrain, yet again, in detail, but to offer a compact mise subordinate features: Byrne & Byrne 1991; Case 1985; adate` to ground evolutionary reconstruction. Guiding Langer 1998) to generating cognitive structures that questions are “what, if anything, about great ape cog- show embedding (e.g., classification showing nesting of nition requires evolutionary explanations beyond those classes: Langer 1998). developed for other nonhuman primates?”, and “how is Developmentalists commonly consider weak and great ape cognition best characterized with respect to strong forms to be related in humans, as basic and evolutionary questions?” advanced ontogenetic achievements of early and later Copyright Anne Russon and David Begun 2004. 76 Great ape cognitive systems 77 childhood respectively (Table 6.1). Comparative pri- problem-specific structures – effectively eliminating mate cognition often shares this view (P&M; Whiten chances for detecting use of multiple or general purpose 2000). Insofar as symbols must be grounded in real processes. Given the lack of relevant empirical evidence, world referents at some point (Donald 1991) and weak these models remain speculative concerning great apes. symbolism is the more likely in great apes, I consider Cognitive science models portray the mind as a great ape cognition relative to weak symbolism and its device for processing, storing, integrating, and trans- associated processes (strong representation, weak hier- forming information. Some of their concepts have been archization, weak metarepresentation). The terms sym- incorporated into models of cognitive development (e.g., bolic, representation, hierarchization, and metarepre- Case 1985; Leslie 1987; Pascual-Leone 1987), others sentation henceforth refer to these meanings. have aided in detecting hierarchization in great ape cog- The models guiding empirical studies of great ape nition (Byrne & Byrne 1991; Byrne, Corp & Byrne 2001; cognition also contribute to disparities because of the Byrne & Russon 1998; P&M; Russon 1998). Limita- ways in which they shape the generation of evidence tions concern portraying cognition in static, mechanistic and the interpretive frameworks they impose. Several terms that may not apply to living beings. important models are sketched below to suggest their Models of human cognitive development have strengths and limitations for understanding great ape proven valuable for assessing primate cognition com- cognition. paratively because they provide conceptual and method- Animal models designed for nonhuman mentality ological tools for assessing non-symbolic and symbolic have been frequent frameworks for studies of great ape cognition within one unified framework and the gen- cognition. They concentrate on the non-symbolic, asso- eration and structure of cognition. Piaget’s model sup- ciative processes presumed to govern nonhuman cog- ported the first developmental studies of great ape cog- nition, for example trial-and-error experiential learn- nition; among its greatest contributions is its portrayal of ing or behavior chains. This leaves them conceptually cognition as constructed progressively during ontogeny and methodologically impoverished concerning sym- and directly affected by interaction with the environ- bolic cognition (Anderson 1996; RWB;Rumbaugh 1970; ment. Early piagetian studies focused on sensorimotor T&C), quantification and logic being important excep- (human infant) cognition, which relies on pre-symbolic tions (e.g., Boysen & Hallberg 2000; Thompson & Oden processes similar to those portrayed in animal models, so 2000), so relatively little of the evidence they have gener- similar limits apply.Recently,neo-piagetian models have ated helps determine whether great apes, or any species, supported studies of the rudimentary symbolic range are capable of symbolic cognition. (for an overview, see P&M). Models inspired by Vygot- Generality–modularity models are potentially sky, which portray socio-cultural forces like apprentice- important because they concern cognitive architecture. ship or enculturation as fundamental to cognitive devel- In this view, favored by evolutionary psychologists and opment (e.g., Donald 2000; T&C), have spawned many neo-nativists, there exist cognitive “modules,” problem- studies on social cognition and cognitive development specific cognitive structures that represent innately in great apes. Given how richly primate lives are socially specified neurological systems and operate with rela- embedded, their merits are obvious. Among these mod- tive autonomy, as well as general purpose or central els may be included models of understanding others’ processes that apply across problem types and affect mental states, or theory of mind, which some propose to system-wide properties (e.g., representation, executive underpin much cognitive progress in early human child- control structures, working memory). These models hood (e.g., Carruthers & Smith 1996). Two such models have influenced understandings of great ape cognition have been applied to great apes, both proposing that with their assumption that modular architecture char- general-purpose cognitive processes in the rudimen- acterizes nonhuman cognition and generality evolved tary symbolic range underwrite this progress (second- uniquely in humans (e.g., Mithen 1996; T&C; Tooby order representation – Leslie 1987; secondary represen- &Cosmides 1992). Little if any empirical study has tation – Perner 1991). While both offer useful tools for examined cognitive architecture in great apes, however. assessing rudimentary symbolic processes, their focus Studies of great ape cognition have typically assumed on one ability series in the social domain risks under- modularity and have aimed for clean tests of individual representing the breadth of great apes’ achievements. Table6.1. Processes and structures posited in rudimentary sybolic level cognition, in human developmental perspective Age Piaget Case1 Langer2 Leslie3 Perner4 (yr) (general) (causality) (logic-math) (Theory of Mind) (Theory of Mind) 0 Sensorimotor stage Sensorimotor stage 1st-order cognition 1st-order representation Primary representation Schemata Operational consolidation 1.5 (Inter-) relational cognition 2nd-order cognition 2nd-order representation Secondary representation 2 unifocal relational operations metarepresentation re-represent primary stage 6, symbols, representations
Recommended publications
  • Spatial Representation of Magnitude in Gorillas and Orangutans ⇑ Regina Paxton Gazes A,B, , Rachel F.L
    Cognition 168 (2017) 312–319 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Cognition journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/COGNIT Original Articles Spatial representation of magnitude in gorillas and orangutans ⇑ Regina Paxton Gazes a,b, , Rachel F.L. Diamond c,g, Jasmine M. Hope d, Damien Caillaud e,f, Tara S. Stoinski a,e, Robert R. Hampton c,g a Zoo Atlanta, Atlanta, GA, United States b Bucknell University, Lewisburg, PA, United States c Department of Psychology, Emory University, Atlanta, GA, United States d Neuroscience Graduate Program, Emory University, Atlanta, GA, United States e Dian Fossey Gorilla Fund International, Atlanta, GA, United States f Department of Anthropology, University of California Davis, Davis, CA, United States g Yerkes National Primate Research Center, Atlanta, GA, United States article info abstract Article history: Humans mentally represent magnitudes spatially; we respond faster to one side of space when process- Received 1 March 2017 ing small quantities and to the other side of space when processing large quantities. We determined Revised 24 July 2017 whether spatial representation of magnitude is a fundamental feature of primate cognition by testing Accepted 25 July 2017 for such space-magnitude correspondence in gorillas and orangutans. Subjects picked the larger quantity in a pair of dot arrays in one condition, and the smaller in another. Response latencies to the left and right sides of the screen were compared across the magnitude range. Apes showed evidence of spatial repre- Keywords: sentation of magnitude. While all subjects did not adopt the same orientation, apes showed consistent Space tendencies for spatial representations within individuals and systematically reversed these orientations SNARC Ape in response to reversal of the task instruction.
    [Show full text]
  • (Pan Troglodytes Verus) in Guinea-Bissau, West Africa
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
    [Show full text]
  • Animal Communication Animals Are Smarter Than You Think
    Animal Communication Animals are smarter than you think. Joseph Poulshock, PhD It’s not going to be Planet of the Apes any time soon. But animal communicators are still amazing. Key Words/Outline Design Features Animals? • Duality--二重性 Main ! Point • Birds and Duality Vervet Monkeys • Arbitrariness--恣意性 Animals can communicate. They can use symbols or calls to • communicate with each other. When working with humans, • Gray Parrots • Displacement --転位; Stimulus Freedom they can learn to communicate with humans and use human • Koko the Gorilla --刺激反応自由 language in surprising ways. • Lucy the Chimp • Structure Dependence --構造依存性 Kanzi • • Creativity--創造性 • Rico and Chaser • Recursion --帰納 (反復) Once upon a time, N’kisi: Gray Parrot Alex a lady went for a walk, and she met a parrot. The parrot said: N'Kisi is a gray parrot. • New York times, reporting on the death • of Alex. • Aimee Morgana is N’kisi’s human. • He learned more than 100 English • Morgana claims N'Kisi knows 950 words. words. Dr. Irene Pepperberg & Griffin Parrots The Vervet Monkey • Vervet have a special alarm call for each enemy. • Talking birds show the design feature of duality. • A “rraup” for eagles/hawks. • They can learn many words and phrases. • ワシ • Do they produce words and phrases based on structural rules, or • A “chutter” for snakes. are they memorizing chunks? • ヘビ • A “chirp” for lions/leopards. Can Monkeys Talk? • ライオン/ヒョウ The Vervet Monkey Vervet Monkeys • Specific calls for each predator. • They use alarm calls to escape predators. Chimps and Hawk! Run to the center of the tree. Each call shows a different kind of • danger.
    [Show full text]
  • Understanding and Sharing Intentions: the Origins of Cultural Cognition
    BEHAVIORAL AND BRAIN SCIENCES (2005) 28, 000–000 Printed in the United States of America Understanding and sharing intentions: The origins of cultural cognition Michael Tomasello, Malinda Carpenter, Josep Call, Tanya Behne, and Henrike Moll Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology, D-04103 Leipzig, Germany [email protected] [email protected] [email protected] [email protected] [email protected] Abstract: We propose that the crucial difference between human cognition and that of other species is the ability to participate with others in collaborative activities with shared goals and intentions: shared intentionality. Participation in such activities requires not only especially powerful forms of intention reading and cultural learning, but also a unique motivation to share psychological states with oth- ers and unique forms of cognitive representation for doing so. The result of participating in these activities is species-unique forms of cultural cognition and evolution, enabling everything from the creation and use of linguistic symbols to the construction of social norms and individual beliefs to the establishment of social institutions. In support of this proposal we argue and present evidence that great apes (and some children with autism) understand the basics of intentional action, but they still do not participate in activities involving joint intentions and attention (shared intentionality). Human children’s skills of shared intentionality develop gradually during the first 14 months of life as two ontogenetic pathways intertwine: (1) the general ape line of understanding others as animate, goal-directed, and intentional agents; and (2) a species-unique motivation to share emotions, experience, and activities with other persons.
    [Show full text]
  • AN EXPLORATION of OBJECT-WORD ACQUISITION in CANIS FAMILIARIS By
    AN EXPLORATION OF OBJECT-WORD ACQUISITION IN CANIS FAMILIARIS by Brianna L. Artz A Thesis Submitted to the Graduate Faculty of George Mason University in Partial Fulfillment of The Requirements for the Degree of Master of Arts Psychology Committee: ___________________________________________ Director ___________________________________________ ___________________________________________ ___________________________________________ Department Chairperson ___________________________________________ Dean, College of Humanities and Social Sciences Date: _____________________________________ Summer Semester 2019 George Mason University Fairfax, VA An Exploration of Object-Word Acquisition in Canis familiaris A Thesis submitted in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Master of Arts at George Mason University by Brianna L. Artz Bachelor of Science George Mason University, 2017 Director: Doris Bitler Davis, Professor Department of Psychology Summer Semester 2019 George Mason University Fairfax, VA Copyright 2019 Brianna L. Artz All Rights Reserved ii DEDICATION This thesis is dedicated to my amazing grandfather, John H. Cameron. iii ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS I would like to thank Kirk and Doris Davis for their endless support of my research dreams. I would also like to thank Erin Murdoch and Linda Chrosniak for their roles as committee members and wonderful mentors. Thank you to Megan Tiller for help entering and organizing my data. Finally, thank you to all of my family and friends who supported me throughout this process. iv TABLE
    [Show full text]
  • Animal Communication 2020
    BY JOSEPH POULSHOCK, PHD Planet of the Apes ELEMENT 2 - ANIMAL COMMUNICATION ▸ Key Questions for self-quizzing before and after. Main ! Point ▸ What can VERVET MONKEYS do? • Caesar and the apes evolved. Now they Animals can communicate. They can use symbols or calls to ▸ What can KOKO the GORILLA do? fight with humans for control of the world. communicate with each other. When working with humans, ▸ What two acts of communication did LUCY do with they can learn to communicate with humans and use human sign language? It's never going to happen. Right? • language in surprising ways. ▸ What kind of animal is KANZI? What can KANZI do? • Animals are smarter than we think! ▸ How many words did Chaser learn? ▸ So what? Low resolution "fair use" image providing critical commentary on the film. Design Features Animals? N'kisi Parrots • Duality--二重性 N’kisi is an African Gray. Once upon a time, • Arbitrariness--恣意性 When seeing a picture of a person on • a lady went for a walk, • Talking birds seem to show the design feature of duality. and she met a parrot.... the phone N'kisi said "What cha doin' • • Displacement --転位 on the phone?" The parrot said, "@#$@ YOU!" • They can learn many words and phrases. • Stimulus Freedom --刺激反応自由 • When seeing a picture of flowers, N'kisi said, "That's a pic of flowers." • How do they do it? Grammar? Just chunks? • Creativity--創造性 • Aimee Morgana claims N'kisi knows 950 words. • Recursion --帰納 (反復) Image Wikipedia The Vervet Monkey The Vervet Monkey • Vervets have alarm calls for each enemy.
    [Show full text]
  • And Chimpanzees (P
    eScholarship International Journal of Comparative Psychology Title Monitoring Spatial Transpositions by Bonobos (Pan paniscus) and Chimpanzees (P. troglodytes) Permalink https://escholarship.org/uc/item/5099j6v4 Journal International Journal of Comparative Psychology, 13(1) ISSN 0889-3675 Authors Beran, Michael J. Minahan, Mary F. Publication Date 2000 License https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ 4.0 Peer reviewed eScholarship.org Powered by the California Digital Library University of California - 1 - International Journal of Comparative Psychology, 2000, 13, 1-15. Copyright 2000 by the International Society for Comparative Psychology Monitoring Spatial Transpositions by Bonobos (Pan paniscus) and Chimpanzees (P. troglodytes) Michael J. Beran and Mary F. Minahan Georgia State University, U.S.A. Two bonobos (Pan paniscus) and three chimpanzees (P. troglodytes) monitored spatial transpositions, or the simultaneous movement of multiple items in an array, so as to select a specific item from the array. In the initial condition of Experiment 1, food reward was hidden beneath one of four cups, and the apes were required to select the cup containing the reward in order to receive it. In the second condition, the test board on which the cups were located was rotated 180 degrees after placement of the food reward. In the third condition, two of the three cups switched locations with one another after placement of the food reward. All five apes performed at very high levels for these conditions. Ex- periment 2 was a computerized simulation of the tasks with the cups in which the apes had to track one of four simultaneously moving stimuli on a computer monitor.
    [Show full text]
  • 1. Would Humans Without Language Be Apes?
    In J. Valsiner (Series Ed.) & A. Toomela (Vol. Ed.) (2003). Cultural guidance in the development of the human mind: Vol. 7. Advances in Child Development within Culturally Structured Environments (pp. 9-26). Greenwich, CT: Ablex Publishing Corporation. 1. Would Humans Without Language Be Apes? Jacques Vauclair THE POSTULATE OF MENTAL CONTINUITY The bedrock of comparative psychology of cognition, especially where non- human primates are concerned, rests on Darwin's famous account according to which continuity would be the main trait leading from the animal to the human mind. This idea was popularized through the statement in which Darwin postu- lated only quantitative differences between humans and the other species, namely "the difference in mind between man and the higher animals, great as it is, certainly is one of degree and not of kind" (Darwin, 1871, p. 128). We can only agree with Darwin's continuity position as concerns the existence of some kind of mental organizations in animals, in particular in nonhuman primates, as a necessary part of the perception of objects and their localization and interrelationships in space and time (Walker, 1983) and in many adaptive functions, including problem solving and memory (e.g., Vauclair, 1996). In effect, human and animal brain functions show sufficient similarity to allow comparisons if one assumes that animal brains are devices for selecting and organizing per- ceived information, and that the neural systems that accomplish perception and memory exhibit evolutionary continuity. It thus appears that these global func- tions are performed by the animal in ways that are basically similar to human performance, that is, through the construction and use of representations of various degrees of schematization and abstraction (Roitblat, 1982).
    [Show full text]
  • The Assessment of Cognitive Bias in Capuchin Monkeys Using a Computerized Task
    Georgia State University ScholarWorks @ Georgia State University Psychology Theses Department of Psychology 5-8-2020 The Assessment of Cognitive Bias in Capuchin Monkeys Using a Computerized Task Kristin French Follow this and additional works at: https://scholarworks.gsu.edu/psych_theses Recommended Citation French, Kristin, "The Assessment of Cognitive Bias in Capuchin Monkeys Using a Computerized Task." Thesis, Georgia State University, 2020. https://scholarworks.gsu.edu/psych_theses/216 This Thesis is brought to you for free and open access by the Department of Psychology at ScholarWorks @ Georgia State University. It has been accepted for inclusion in Psychology Theses by an authorized administrator of ScholarWorks @ Georgia State University. For more information, please contact [email protected]. THE ASSESSMENT OF COGNITIVE BIAS IN CAPUCHIN MONKEYS USING A COMPUTERIZED TASK by KRISTIN A. FRENCH Under the Direction of Michael J. Beran, PhD ABSTRACT Cognitive bias refers to the influence of affective state on the interpretation of ambiguous stimuli and has been used to assess emotional state in nonhuman animals. The current study assessed cognitive bias in 12 brown-tufted capuchin monkeys using three distinct computerized psychophysical tasks and a novel manipulation to affect that involved giving moneys gelatin foods that tasted either pleasant or unpleasant. In addition, monkeys were trained on several positive and negative training cues. Results showed that food type was not a factor in monkeys’ responses to ambiguous stimuli. Behavioral observation during test sessions revealed the unpleasant food may have acted as a form of enrichment, thereby providing the monkeys with two pleasant activities prior to assessments of their emotional states.
    [Show full text]
  • What Is an Animal Emotion?
    Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci. ISSN 0077-8923 ANNALS OF THE NEW YORK ACADEMY OF SCIENCES Issue: The Year in Cognitive Neuroscience What is an animal emotion? Frans B.M. de Waal Living Links, Yerkes National Primate Research Center, and Psychology Department, Emory University, Atlanta, Georgia Address for correspondence: Frans B.M. de Waal, Ph.D., Living Links, Yerkes Primate Center, Emory University, 954 N. Gatewood Road Atlanta, GA 30322. [email protected] Emotions suffuse much of the language employed by students of animal behavior—from “social bonding” to “alarm calls”— yet are carefully avoided as an explicit topic in scientific discourse. Given the increasing interest in human emotional intelligence and the explicit attention in neuroscience to the emotions, both human and nonhuman, the taboo that has reigned for so long in animal behavior research seems outdated. The present review seeks to recall the history of our field in which emotions and instincts were mentioned in the same breath and in which neither psychologists nor biologists felt that animal emotions were off limits. One of the tenets supporting a renewed interest in this topic is to avoid unanswerable questions and to view emotions as mental and bodily states that potentiate behavior appropriate to environmental challenges. Understanding the emotionally deep structure of behavior will be the next frontier in the study of animal behavior. Keywords: evolution; animal cognition; empathy; facial expressions; instinct Emotions used to be an uncontroversial part of any ior to attribute it to an emotion if our only ev- description of animal behavior, as uncontroversial idence of the emotion is the very behaviour the as the instincts with which they were compared emotion is supposed to explain.” Since the 1970s, and equated.
    [Show full text]
  • A Comparative Perspective on Three Primate Species' Responses to A
    animals Article A Comparative Perspective on Three Primate Species’ Responses to a Pictorial Emotional Stroop Task Lydia M. Hopper 1,* , Matthias Allritz 2, Crystal L. Egelkamp 1, Sarah M. Huskisson 1 , Sarah L. Jacobson 1,3 , Jesse G. Leinwand 1 and Stephen R. Ross 1 1 Lester E. Fisher Center for the Study and Conservation of Apes, Lincoln Park Zoo, Chicago, IL 60614, USA; [email protected] (C.L.E.); [email protected] (S.M.H.); [email protected] (S.L.J.); [email protected] (J.G.L.); [email protected] (S.R.R.) 2 School of Psychology and Neuroscience, University of St Andrews, St Andrews KY16 9AJ, UK; [email protected] 3 Psychology, Graduate School and University Center, City University of New York, New York, NY 10016, USA * Correspondence: [email protected] Simple Summary: As animals cannot tell us how they feel, we must develop tests to make inferences about how they are feeling to assess their welfare. Here, we adapted a task that has been previously used with humans and chimpanzees to assess how chimpanzees, gorillas, and Japanese macaques respond to pictures of different emotional valences. Specifically, if the primates perceive emotionally arousing photographs differently as compared to neutral photographs, we would expect them to “trip up” more when responding to emotional stimuli. We presented the primates with a touchscreen task in which they had to select one of two differently colored squares. However, the squares always contained either positive photographs (a preferred food), negative photographs (a snake), or neutral Citation: Hopper, L.M.; Allritz, M.; photographs (human-made objects).
    [Show full text]
  • Kiko Record on Appeal (Vol. 2 of 2)
    New York County Clerk’s Index No. 150149/16 New York Supreme Court APPELLATE DIVISION — FIRST DEPARTMENT >> >> In the Matter of a Proceeding under Article 70 of the CPLR for a Writ of Habeas Corpus, THE NONHUMAN RIGHTS PROJECT, INC., on behalf of KIKO, Petitioner-Appellant, against CARMEN PRESTI, individually and as an officer and director of The Primate Sanctuary, Inc., CHRISTIE E. PRESTI, individually and as an officer and director of The Primate Sanctuary, Inc., and THE PRIMATE SANCTUARY INC., Respondents. RECORD ON APPEAL VOLUME II OF II Pages 394 to 780 CARMEN PRESTI, ELIZABETH STEIN, ESQ. individually and as an officer 5 Dunhill Road and director of The Primate New Hyde Park, New York 11040 Sanctuary Inc. 516-747-4726 2764 Livingston Avenue [email protected] Niagara Falls, New York 14303 and 716-284-6118 [email protected] STEVEN M. WISE, ESQ. (of the bar of the State of Respondent Pro Se Massachusetts) by permission of the Court CHRISTIE E. PRESTI individually and as an officer 5195 NW 112th Terrace and director of The Primate Coral Springs, Florida 33076 Sanctuary Inc. 954-648-9864 2764 Livingston Avenue [email protected] Niagara Falls, New York 14303 Attorneys for Petitioner-Appellant 716-284-6118 [email protected] Respondent Pro Se (Additional Counsel Continued Inside) Printed on Recycled Paper THE PRIMATE SANTUARY INC. 2764 Livingston Avenue Niagara Falls, New York 14303 716-284-6118 [email protected] Respondent Pro Se Table of Contents Page Volume I Pre-Argument Statement .......................................................................... 1 Notice of Appeal, dated February 9, 2016 ................................................ 5 Declined Order to Show Cause and Memorandum of the Honorable Barbara Jaffe, Appealed From ........................................
    [Show full text]