Chapter 3. Intrusion Vs. Epenthesis

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Chapter 3. Intrusion Vs. Epenthesis CHAPTER 3. INTRUSION VS. EPENTHESIS In the previous chapters, I have argued for the existence of non-segmental “intrusive vowels”: copy vowels (or schwas) heard in RC or CR clusters, which are not independent segments but rather the edges of adjacent vowel gestures, heard during an interconsonantal release. This chapter concentrates on the distinction between vowel intrusion and copy vowel epenthesis. While an intrusive vowel is a vocalic interval that is not segmental or syllabic, an epenthetic vowel is segmental and syllabic, but not underlying. In the literature, both are often labeled ‘epenthesis’, but they have quite different representations, as evidenced by the differences in their conditioning environment and behavior. A VCV sequence with an intrusive vowel involves only one vowel segment and gesture, while copy vowel epenthesis involves insertion of both a segment and a gesture, as shown below. (103) Intrusion Epenthesis Input: /VCC/ /VCC/ ↓ ↓ V C (V) C V C V C Output: Because copy vowel epenthesis does not require a vowel gesture to surround a consonant, it is not restricted by *C IN V constraints. I.e., copying does not happen more often over certain consonants than others. I have found only one class of historically epenthetic vowels that show restrictions on the consonants they copy over: vowels epenthesized into loanwords. These vowels likely began as intrusive vowels, and still display one characteristic of vowel intrusion, a tendency to copy over sonorants. However, synchronically these vowels appear to have been reanalyzed as underlying. 3.1. Copy vowel epenthesis True epenthetic copy vowels are fundamentally different from intrusive vowels. While intrusive vowels are a non-segmental percept, and have a ‘copied’ quality only because they are part of the gesture of another vowel, epenthetic vowels are segments, and I will argue that their copied quality does not result from gestural overlap. As argued in chapter 1, a fixed hierarchy of *C IN V constraints restricts vowels from fully overlapping different types of consonants. Therefore, a restriction on intervening copy type can be considered a diagnostic of gestural vowel “copy”. Epenthetic copy vowels do not meet this diagnostic. This suggests that the copying 74 operates by an entirely different mechanism. I will adopt that proposed by Kitto & de Lacy 2001. Briefly, Kitto & de Lacy propose that copied quality in epenthetic segments is enforced in the same way as the copying that occurs in reduplication, which McCarthy & Prince 1995 analyze with correspondence constraints. A correspondence relation can hold between an epenthetic segment and another output segment (the “base”), and the constraint BE-IDENT-F demands that the two segments have the same feature settings. The choice of which vowel to use as the base is determined by competition among other constraints. (104) BE-IDENT-F Kitto & de Lacy 2000:3 An epenthetic segment E and its base have identical values for feature F. In a purely gestural theory, this constraint can be reformulated to demand identity between the gestures of an epenthetic vowel and its base. Under this view, the copying that happens in epenthesis has nothing to do with gestural overlap: the two segments have identical but separate gestures. Epenthetic copy vowels are not predicted to show the restrictions ascribed to effects of gestural overlap, such as copying only over sonorants. Lest it seem undesirable to have two separate mechanisms leading to copying (gestural overlap and segment-to-segment correspondence), it is important to recognize that correspondence between output segments, or some similar mechanism, is independently necessary to explain the non-local vowel copying that happens in reduplication and language games. In the English game Urupu, for example, every vowel is preceded by [r©p] and a copy of that vowel (Moira Yip, p.c.). In the examples below, spaces are inserted for ease of reading; they do not indicate pauses. (105) Urupu game sigh r`i → r`q©o`i.r`iq©o`i pen oDm → oDq©oDm sit rHs → rHq©oHs soon rtm → rTq©otv m seen rhm → rHq©ohim cute jits → jiTq©otv s In this kind of copying, the two vowels cannot possibly share a continuous gesture, since a different vowel gesture occurs in the intervening syllable [r©p]. Another indication of gestural independence is that the quality of a tense vowel is not copied perfectly; the unstressed copy is laxed. The copy vowel can undego regular contextual variation in quality because it has a separate gesture. Similarly, in a language that reduplicates multiple syllables ([paku- pakutu]), the correspondence between, for example, the [a] in the reduplicant and the [a] in the base cannot be a result of a single gesture extending from one to the other, since there is an intervening [u]. Since a mechanism not involving gestural spreading is necessary to 75 analyze long distance copying, there is no reason that this mechanism should not be available for local copying as well. 3.2. Diagnostics of non-gestural copying 3.2.1. Welsh: true epenthesis The representational difference between epenthetic and intrusive vowels predicts differences in behavior, under the theory proposed here. These differences will be demonstrated through the example of copy vowel epenthesis in Welsh (Awbery 1984). I believe that the highlighted characteristics of Welsh are common to most or all epenthetic copy vowels. Welsh epenthesizes copy vowels to break up marked coda clusters in inputs like /gwadn/, which surfaces as [gwadan]. Several characteristics of these vowels suggest that they do not involve gestural overlap, and are true segments. First, I have argued that gestural overlap is governed by a universal hierarchy of *C IN V constraints, which prohibit vowels from fully surrounding certain classes of consonants. If gestural overlap is not involved in copy vowel epenthesis, there should be no restrictions on the type of consonants that can be copied over. This holds true for Welsh, which inserts an epenthetic copy vowel into any underlying final cluster that consist of an obstruent followed by a sonorant, and also into certain sonorant-sonorant clusters. Any consonant may be copied over, including [d, v, X, l, m] as in the examples below. Alternations are given to show the epenthetic nature of the copy vowel. 76 (106) Welsh copy vowel epenthesis Awbery 1987:88-9 a. .fv `cm/ → fv `cm ‘sole’ .fv `cm + d/ → fv `cmd ‘soles’ b. .jdum/ → jdudm ‘back’ .jdum + d/ → jDumd ‘backs’ c. .otcq/ → otctq ‘rotten’ .otcq + h. → o©cqh ‘to rot’ d. .nXq. → »n˘Xnq ‘side’ .nXq+ h/ → »åXqh ‘to side’ e. .gdkl/ → »gd˘kdl ‘cornstack’ .gdkl + h/ → »gDklh ‘cornstacks’ f. .`lk. → »`l`k ‘often’ .`lk+ `X. → »`lk`X ‘more often’ A second prediction is that, since the epenthetic vowel is a true segment and its presence involves unfaithfulness to the underlying segmental string, it should only appear when a markedness constraint forces its presence. Epenthesis should always remove a structure that shows the characteristics of markedness, such as being typologically rare, acquired late by children, and actively avoided through other means besides epenthesis. As shown in chapter 1, this is not true of intrusive vowels; they actually tend to occur in rather unmarked clusters. In the case of Welsh, epenthesis clearly functions to satisfy independently motivated syllable structure constraints. It removes codas of rising sonority, which are cross-linguistically more marked than those of even or falling sonority. Less-marked codas, of falling sonority, are left alone as in the words below (note that several of these words would be prime candidates for vowel intrusion.) (107) Welsh coda clusters Awbery 1984:87 a. o`rj ‘Easter’ b. fv `¬s ‘hair’ c. c`qm ‘piece’ d. eDql ‘farm’ e. oHlo ‘five’ f. aåqc ‘table’ g. a`kX ‘proud’ 77 A restriction against CR codas is familiar from numerous languages, and they are actively avoided through other processes in Welsh. In words of two syllables, final CR clusters are repaired by deletion of one of the consonants: (108) Welsh cluster resolution by deletion Awbery 1984:89 a. /edmdrsq. → edmdrs ‘window’ /edmdrsq+ h/ → eDmDrsqh ‘windows’ b. /`m`ck/ → `m`k ‘breath’ /`m`ck+ h/ → `m`ckh ‘to breathe’ Some dialects also sporadically metathesize final CR, or change [v]R clusters to [w]R (Awbery 1984:90). In short, epenthesis is part of a ‘conspiracy’ of processes that remove the same clusters. This is in striking contrast to vowel intrusion, which happens in clusters that are not particularly marked cross-linguistically, and are not in general avoided within the language (see, for example, the discussion of Dutch truncation in chapter 2). A third prediction is that, for an epenthetic vowel to repair a marked structure, it must be phonologically “present”: it is a new segment that creates a new syllable, and should act syllabic for other diagnostics. Luckily, Welsh provides one. According to Awbery 1984:69, a long vowel does not occur before a consonant cluster, presumably because it would be in a closed syllable. If the copy vowels that appear in /CVCR/ words were intrusive, then the segmental representation would still be C-V-C-R and the word would be monosyllabic. In that case, these words should be able to contain only short vowels. If, on the other hand, the CR cluster is phonologically broken up by the epenthetic vowel, then the preceding vowel can be long (in fact, before certain consonants it must be long). As shown in (1 06), epenthesis into a CR cluster does allow the preceding vowel to be long. (109) Welsh CVCVR a. .fv `cm. → fv `cm ‘sole’ b. .jdum. → jdudm ‘back’ c. .otcq. → otctq ‘rotten’ d. .nXq. → »n˘Xnq ‘side’ This is the pattern we expect to see if the epenthetic vowel is creating a new syllable: it causes the preceding vowel to behave as if it is in an open syllable.
Recommended publications
  • Vowel Breaking Publicat a Diccionari De Lingüística on Line (
    Diccionari de lingüística online | Ciències del llenguatge i docència (Grup d'Innovació Docent UB) Page 1 of 3 Vowel breaking Publicat a Diccionari de lingüística on line (http://www.ub.edu/diccionarilinguistica) Vowel breaking Data d'edició: 17 de Abril de 2013 Autoria: Emilia Castaño Revisió: Ignasi Adiego Vowel breaking is a sound change whereby a single vowel changes to become a diphthong in specific environments. The resulting sound preserves the original vowel, which is either preceded or followed by a glide. This process is manifested in a variety of Germanic languages and is characteristic of Old English. Contents Explanation Related concepts Basic bibliography Additional bibliography Explanation In historical linguistics vowel breaking is defined as an assimilatory sound change that implies the diphthongization of single vowels due to the influence of a neighboring sound. The first segment of the resulting diphthong coincides with the original vowel whereas the second is a glide the articulatory characteristics of which are determined by the triggering sound. This process is well attested in Old English where certain front vowels, /æ/ /e/ and /i/, in their short and long variants, were diphthongized when immediately followed by a velar /x/ or a cluster containing a velarized consonant and /?/ or /r/, as its first element. /æ/ > /æu/ / ___ /x/ /æ/ > /æu/ / ___ /?/ or /r + C /e/ > /eu/ / ___ /x/ /e/ > /eu/ / ___ /?/ or /r + C /i/ > /iu/ / ___ /x/ /i/ > /iu/ / ___ /?/ or /r + C In this context, the glide appeared as a transition sound in response to the tongue movements performed when articulating the vowels at the front of the mouth and the consonants at the back of the mouth.
    [Show full text]
  • Segmental Patterns and Phonological Asymmetries in English Loans in Hugariyyah Yemeni Arabic (HYA): an OT Account
    Interdisciplinary Journal of Linguistics (IJL Vol.11) Interdisciplinary Journal of Linguistics Volume [11] 2018, Pp.106-118 Segmental Patterns and Phonological Asymmetries in English Loans in Hugariyyah Yemeni Arabic (HYA): An OT Account Yasmeen Yaseen * Hemanga Dutta * Abstract This paper intends to focus on the notion of segmental patterns registered across the English loan words in Yemeni Arabic of Hugariyyah variety. The adaptation process of loanwords in HYA conforms to the structural constraints of Arabic Phonology. During the process of adaptation onset positions are treated as more salient than coda positions. Deletion is not allowed in onset position and the ranking schemata is ONS>>*Complex >> Max >> SSP >> *Coda. In the similar fashion devoicing of the voiced obstruents is mere favourable in coda positions. In HYA English loanwords, the segments which are less salient and less sonorants are altered or deleted than their stronger counterparts in coda positions which support the accepted claim of positional faithfulness to the onset. The paper seeks to address whether idiosyncratic patterns displayed by loans in HYA supports the notion of the positional asymmetries and phonological strength in the loanwords. Furthermore, it provides reasons for the adaptation using the Optimality- Theoretic framework (Prince & Smolensky, 1993, McCarthy & Prince, 1995). Key Words: Segmental Patterns, Loanwords, Positional Faithfulness Introduction This paper is an attempt at representing the segmental patterning and phonological asymmetries within the phonological patterns of English loanwords in Yemeni Arabic of Hugariyyah. It is to examine what happens to English loanwords after lexical borrowing occurs in HYA. Borrowing of lexical items not only results in the enrichment of the lexical inventory of HYA, but also leads to a variety of changes to English borrowed words.
    [Show full text]
  • The Emergence of Obstruents After High Vowels*
    John Benjamins Publishing Company This is a contribution from Diachronica 29:4 © 2012. John Benjamins Publishing Company This electronic file may not be altered in any way. The author(s) of this article is/are permitted to use this PDF file to generate printed copies to be used by way of offprints, for their personal use only. Permission is granted by the publishers to post this file on a closed server which is accessible to members (students and staff) only of the author’s/s’ institute, it is not permitted to post this PDF on the open internet. For any other use of this material prior written permission should be obtained from the publishers or through the Copyright Clearance Center (for USA: www.copyright.com). Please contact [email protected] or consult our website: www.benjamins.com Tables of Contents, abstracts and guidelines are available at www.benjamins.com The emergence of obstruents after high vowels* David R. Mortensen University of Pittsburgh While a few cases of the emergence of obstruents after high vowels are found in the literature (Burling 1966, 1967, Blust 1994), no attempt has been made to comprehensively collect instances of this sound change or give them a unified explanation. This paper attempts to resolve this gap in the literature by introduc- ing a post-vocalic obstruent emergence (POE) as a recurring sound change with a phonetic (aerodynamic) basis. Possible cases are identified in Tibeto-Burman, Austronesian, and Grassfields Bantu. Special attention is given to a novel case in the Tibeto-Burman language Huishu. Keywords: epenthesis, sound change, aerodynamics, exemplar theory, Tibeto- Burman, Austronesian, Niger-Congo 1.
    [Show full text]
  • Sounding Southern: Identities Expressed Through Language1
    FEATURED ARTICLE Sounding Southern: Identities Expressed Through Language1 Irina Shport and Wendy Herd 2 “Where are you from?” is the question one often hears in innovations necessary to understand them, and the inter- the United States. Many English speakers from the South- play of sociolinguistic variables in identity expression ern United States can hardly avoid this question (similar through language. Elements typical to Southern speech to speakers of English as a second language) unless they are used selectively and creatively by individual Southern become bidialectal and learn to switch between a Southern speakers rather than as a stereotypical bundle that some accent and a more mainstream, Standard American accent, are accustomed to see in descriptions of Southern speech. as many Southerners nowadays do (to compare South- ern and Standard pronunciation of a bidialectal speaker, The Stabilized Representation of listen to Multimedia1 at acousticstoday.org/shportmm). Southern United States English Southern US English stands out among North American Comprehensive overviews and synthesis of acoustic English dialects as being talked about and stigmatized the research on Southern US English can be found in works most, similar to English spoken in New York City (Pres- such as the Atlas of North American English (ANAE; Labov ton, 1988). It is often portrayed in the media and public as et al., 2006) and the special issue on Southern US English having a Southern drawl, twang, nasal, or sing-song qual- of The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America (Shport ity to it. These labels serve to stereotype Southern speech and Herd, 2020; Thomas, 2020).
    [Show full text]
  • Developments of the Lateral in Occitan Dialects and Their Romance and Cross-Linguistic Context Daniela Müller
    Developments of the lateral in occitan dialects and their romance and cross-linguistic context Daniela Müller To cite this version: Daniela Müller. Developments of the lateral in occitan dialects and their romance and cross- linguistic context. Linguistics. Université Toulouse le Mirail - Toulouse II, 2011. English. NNT : 2011TOU20122. tel-00674530 HAL Id: tel-00674530 https://tel.archives-ouvertes.fr/tel-00674530 Submitted on 27 Feb 2012 HAL is a multi-disciplinary open access L’archive ouverte pluridisciplinaire HAL, est archive for the deposit and dissemination of sci- destinée au dépôt et à la diffusion de documents entific research documents, whether they are pub- scientifiques de niveau recherche, publiés ou non, lished or not. The documents may come from émanant des établissements d’enseignement et de teaching and research institutions in France or recherche français ou étrangers, des laboratoires abroad, or from public or private research centers. publics ou privés. en vue de l’obtention du DOCTORATDEL’UNIVERSITÉDETOULOUSE délivré par l’université de toulouse 2 - le mirail discipline: sciences du langage zur erlangung der doktorwürde DERNEUPHILOLOGISCHENFAKULTÄT DERRUPRECHT-KARLS-UNIVERSITÄTHEIDELBERG présentée et soutenue par vorgelegt von DANIELAMÜLLER DEVELOPMENTS OF THE LATERAL IN OCCITAN DIALECTS ANDTHEIRROMANCEANDCROSS-LINGUISTICCONTEXT JURY Jonathan Harrington (Professor, Ludwig-Maximilians-Universität München) Francesc Xavier Lamuela (Catedràtic, Universitat de Girona) Jean-Léonard Léonard (Maître de conférences HDR, Paris
    [Show full text]
  • Ling 150, Historical Linguistics Moore, Winter 2013 Types of Sound Change Lenition
    Ling 150, Historical Linguistics Moore, Winter 2013 Types of Sound Change Lenition (1) Stronger Weaker voiced voiceless (sometimes) voiceless voiced (sometimes) stops fricatives obstruents sonorants consonants semivowels oral glottal front/back central (2) Lentition: Stronger > Weaker Kara (New Ireland) *bulan > fulan ‘moon’ *tapine > tefin ‘woman’ *punti > fut ‘banana’ (3) Rhotacism: C > r Latin *ami:kosum > ami:korum ‘of friends’ *genesis > generis ‘of the type’ Sound Loss (4) Aphaeresis: initial consonant deletion Angkamuthi (Cape York) *maji > aji ‘food’ *nani > ani ‘ground’ *wapun > apun ‘head’ (5) Apocope: final vowel loss S.E. Ambrym (Vanutu) *utu > ut ‘lice’ *aŋo > aŋ ‘fly’ *asue > asu ‘rat’ *tohu > toh ‘sugarcane’ Ling 150, Historical Linguistics Moore, Winter 2013 (6) Syncope: medial vowel loss Lenakel (Vanutu) *namatana > nɨmrɨn ‘his/her eye’ (note other changes) *nalimana > nelmɨn ‘his/her hand’ *masa > mha ‘low tide’ (7) Cluster reduction: CC >C English Melanesian Pidgin dɪstɹɪkt > distrik ‘district’ poʊst > pos ‘post’ peɩnt > pen ‘paint’ tæŋk > taŋ ‘tank’ (8) Haplology: syllable loss (conditioned by adjacent similar syllable) laɩbɹəɹi > laɩbɹi ‘library’ Anglaland > England Sound Addition (9) Excrescence: consonant addition *æmtig > ɛmpti ‘empty’ *θymle > θɪmbl ‘thimble’ (10) Epenthtesis: vowel addition English Tok Pisin blæk > bilak ‘black’ blu: > bulu ‘blue’ sɪks > sikis ‘six’ (11) Prothesis: initial sound addition Moru (Papua New Guinea) *api > lahi ‘fire’ *asan > lada ‘gills’ *au > lau ‘I, me’ Ling 150, Historical Linguistics Moore, Spring 2011 Metathesis Sounds switch places. (12) *brid > *bird > bɚd æsk > æks (13) Tagalog Ilokano taŋis : sa:ŋit ‘cry’ tubus : subut ‘redeem’ tigis : si:git ‘decant’ tamis : samqit ‘sweet’ Fusion Two sounds become one, bearing features of both original sounds.
    [Show full text]
  • Sound Changes of the Borrowing Words from English Into Indonesian in Room Division Register
    SOUND CHANGES OF THE BORROWING WORDS FROM ENGLISH INTO INDONESIAN IN ROOM DIVISION REGISTER 1 2 3. Parwati, NMD , Budasi IG , Utami IAMI Jurusan Pendidikan Bahasa Inggris, Fakultas Bahasa dan Seni Universitas Pendidikan Ganesha Singaraja, Indonesia Email: [email protected] , [email protected], [email protected] Abstrak Penelitian ini bertujuan untuk mendeskripsikan jenis-jenis perubahan suara dari kata-kata pinjaman dari bahasa Inggris ke bahasa Indonesia dalam ranah divisi kamar. Penelitian ini merupakan penelitian kualitatif deskriptif. Data dikumpulkan dengan menggunakan tiga teknik, seperti: wawancara, rekaman dan observasi. Hasil analisis data menunjukkan bahwa ada 48 kata pinjaman dari bahasa Inggris ke bahasa Indonesia yang digunakan sebagai jargon di Divisi Kamar. Ada 12 kata yang menjalani proses Cluster Reduction, ada 9 kata yang mengalami proses penthesis atau anaptyxis, ada 4 kata mengalami proses vokal atau diphthongization, ada 5 kata yang mengalami proses paragoge dan ada 24 kata yang mengalami proses monpthohngization. Kata kunci: Perubahan bunyi, kata pinjaman, perubahan fonologis Abstract This study aimed at describing the types of the sound changes of the borrowing words from English to Indonesian in Room Division Register. This study was a descriptive qualitative research. The data were collected by using three techniques, such as: interview, recording and observation. The results of the data analysis show that there were 48 borrowing words from English to Indonesian used as the jargon in Room Division. There were 12 words undergone Cluster Reduction process, there were 9 words undergone penthesis or anaptyxis process, there were 4 words undergone vowel breaking or diphthongization process, there were 5 words undergone paragoge process and there were 24 words undergone monpthohngization process.
    [Show full text]
  • OSU WPL 17,206-221 (1974). (Gen(Sg}), and Genitive Ror Verbs
    OSU WPL 17,206-221 (1974). Bibliography V The English Inflectional Endings* Arnold M. Zwicky A. Introductory remarks English expresses the following nominal and verbal categories inflectionally--for nouns, nominative plural (Uom}Pl, genitive singular (Gen(Sg}), and genitive plural (GenPl}; ror verbs, third person singular present {Prs), present participle {PrsP), ~ast {Pst), and past participle (PstP). For completely regular items, the stems show no change and the suffixes have the following forms-- (Nom)Pl m Gen(Sg) = GenPl = Prs: Cs - z - izJ = S PrsP: Cil)J Pst m PstP: Ct - d - idJ • T. In addition, there are various subregular and irregular formations. For nouns, there a.re 'internalt Pls (like leaves), in which stem-final continuants f 8 s are voiced, as vell as zero Pls (like sheep) and a nwnber of entirely irregular forms (like™ and seraphim, with exceptional Pl suffixes; mice and feet, with int·ernal change; and _:e_henomena, addenda, crises, virtuosi, formulae, and foci, with distinct Sg and Pl terminations), Zero-Pl nouns have only two distinct forms (NomSg/Pl vs. GenSg/Pl, as in sheep vs. sheen's). Internal-Pl nouns have three distinct forms, with the GenPl identical to the NomPl but distinct from the GenSg (leaf: leaves/leaves' vs. leaf's}. And truly irregular nouns have four distinct forms (ma.n, man's, men, men's). For verbs, there are 1internal' Psts and PstPs, ending in alveolar stops (several types--hit, hid, bit, burnt, crjpt, built, left). In addition there are subregular formations (sank sunk) and various irregular formations (came/come, went/gone-:--rou~ etc.).
    [Show full text]
  • Sound Change
    Language variation and change Historical linguistics: Sound change Gerhard Jäger November 11, 2016 Gothic Old High German Middle High German Atta unsar þu in himinam, Fater unseer, thu pist in himile, Got vater unser, da du bist in dem himelriche gewaltic alles des dir ist, weihnai namo þein. uuihi namun dinan, geheiliget so werde din nam, qimai þiudinassus þeins. qhueme rihhi din, zuo müeze uns komen daz riche wairþai wilja þeins, uuerde uuillo diin, din. swe in himina jah ana airþai. so in himile sosa in erdu. Din wille werde dem gelich hie uf der erde als in den himeln, des hlaif unsarana þana sinteinan prooth unseer emezzihic kip gewer unsich. uns hiutu, gif uns himma daga. nu gip uns unser tegelich brot und oblaz uns sculdi unseero, swes wir dar nach dürftic sin. jah aflet uns þatei skulans sijaima, so uuir oblazem uns sculdikem, Vergib uns allen sament unser swaswe jah weis afletam schulde, enti ni unsih firleiti in khorunka, þaim skulam unsaraim. also du wilt, daz wir durch dine uzzer losi unsih fona ubile. hulde vergeben der wir ie genamen jah ni briggais uns dekeinen schaden, swie groz er si: in fraistubnjai, vor sünden kor so mache uns vri ak lausei uns af þamma ubilin. und loese uns ouch von allem übele. New High German Und vergib uns unsere Schuld, Vater unser im Himmel wie auch wir vergeben unsern Schuldigern. Geheiligt werde dein Name. Und führe uns nicht in Versuchung, Dein Reich komme. sondern erlöse uns von dem Bösen. Dein Wille geschehe, Denn dein ist das Reich wie im Himmel, so auf Erden.
    [Show full text]
  • The English Sound Changes Among Javanese Students of the English Language Education Study Program of Sanata Dharma University
    PLAGIAT MERUPAKAN TINDAKAN TIDAK TERPUJI THE ENGLISH SOUND CHANGES AMONG JAVANESE STUDENTS OF THE ENGLISH LANGUAGE EDUCATION STUDY PROGRAM OF SANATA DHARMA UNIVERSITY A SARJANA PENDIDIKAN THESIS Presented as a Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements to Obtain a Sarjana Pendidikan Degree in English Language Education By Rafael Marion Galley Margana Student Number: 111214171 ENGLISH LANGUAGE EDUCATION STUDY PROGRAM DEPARTMENT OF LANGUAGE AND ARTS EDUCATION FACULTY OF TEACHERS TRAINING AND EDUCATION SANATA DHARMA UNIVERSITY YOGYAKARTA 2016 PLAGIAT MERUPAKAN TINDAKAN TIDAK TERPUJI THE ENGLISH SOUND CHANGES AMONG JAVANESE STUDENTS OF THE ENGLISH LANGUAGE EDUCATION STUDY PROGRAM OF SANATA DHARMA UNIVERSITY A SARJANA PENDIDIKAN THESIS Presented as a Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements to Obtain a Sarjana Pendidikan Degree in English Language Education By Rafael Marion Galley Margana Student Number: 111214171 ENGLISH LANGUAGE EDUCATION STUDY PROGRAM DEPARTMENT OF LANGUAGE AND ARTS EDUCATION FACULTY OF TEACHERS TRAINING AND EDUCATION SANATA DHARMA UNIVERSITY YOGYAKARTA 2016 i PLAGIAT MERUPAKAN TINDAKAN TIDAK TERPUJI ASarjana Pmdidilan Ttesis on TIIE ENGLISH SOUND CIIANGES AMONG JAVANESE STUI}ENTS OT'TIIE ENGLISH LANGUAGE EDUCATION STUDY PROGRAM OF'SANATA DHAR]VIA UNTVERSITY By Rafael lv{arion Galley Margana Studert Ntrmber: l1l2l4l7 I Approved by Date FX. Ouda Teda Ena, S.Pd., M.pd., Ed.D. 14 Octobsr2016 1l PLAGIAT MERUPAKAN TINDAKAN TIDAK TERPUJI THE ENGLISH SOUND CHANGES AMONG JAVANESE STUDENTS OF'THE ENGLISH LANGUAGE EDUCATION STUI}Y PROGRAM OF'SANATA DHARMA UI{IVEBSITY By RAFAEL MARION GALLEY MARGANA Student Number: lll2l4l7 I Defended before the Board of Examiners on November 8e 2016 and Declared Acceptable Board of Examiners Chairperson : Yohana Venirand4 S.pd., M.Hum., ph.D.
    [Show full text]
  • Labial-Dorsal Interactions : a Phonologically Based Approach
    Title: Labial-dorsal interactions : a phonologically based approach Author: Artur Kijak : Kijak Artur. (2017). Labial-dorsal interactions : a Citation style phonologically based approach. Katowice : Wydawnictwo Uniwersytetu Śląskiego LabiaL-DorsaL interactions a PhonoLogically baseD approach LabiaL-DorsaL interactions a PhonoLogically baseD approach artur KijaK WyDaWnictWo uniWersytetu ŚLąsKiego ● KatoWice 2017 Editor of the series: Językoznawstwo Neofilologiczne Maria Wysocka Referee Eugeniusz Cyran Copy editing: Krystyna Warchał Cover design: Konrad Szcześniak Proofreading: Gabriela Marszołek Technical editing: Małgorzata Pleśniar Typesetting: Grażyna Szewczyk Copyright © 2017 by Wydawnictwo Uniwersytetu Śląskiego All rights reserved ISSN 0208-6336 ISBN 978-83-226-3252-9 (print edition) ISBN 978-83-226-3253-6 (digital edition) Publisher Wydawnictwo Uniwersytetu Śląskiego ul. Bankowa 12B, 40-007 Katowice www.wydawnictwo.us.edu.pl e-mail: [email protected] First impression. Printed sheets 12.25. Publishing sheets 14.5. Offset paper III grade, 90 g Price 28 zł (+ VAT) Printing and binding: “TOTEM.COM.PL Sp. z o.o.” Sp.K. ul. Jacewska 89, 88-100 Inowrocław tabLe of contents Preface anD acKnowleDgments . 7 Chapter One LabiaLs anD DorsaLs in cross-theoreticaL PersPective 1. Preliminaries . 11 2. Segmental phonology . 13 2.1 Major place features . 16 3. Labials and dorsals in classical distinctive feature theories . 18 3.1 Auditory-acoustic features (Jakobson, Fant and Halle 1952) . 18 3.2 Articulatory features (Chomsky and Halle 1968) . 19 3.3 SPE under fire and post-SPE advancement . 22 4. Labials and dorsals in contemporary theories . 27 4.1 Feature geometry . 27 4.2 Dependency Phonology . 33 4.3 Perceptually oriented theories . 36 5. Element Theory .
    [Show full text]
  • Sounding Southern: Phonetic Features and Dialect Perceptions
    SOUNDING SOUTHERN: PHONETIC FEATURES AND DIALECT PERCEPTIONS A Dissertation submitted to the Faculty of the Graduate School of Arts and Sciences of Georgetown University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy in Linguistics By Rachael Allbritten, M.S. Washington, DC April 19, 2011 Copyright 2011 by Rachael Allbritten All Rights Reserved ii SOUNDING SOUTHERN: PHONETIC FEATURES AND DIALECT PERCEPTIONS Rachael M. Allbritten, M.S. Thesis Advisor: Natalie Schilling, Ph.D. ABSTRACT Using an online survey of naïve listeners, this dissertation investigates which Southern speech characteristics among monophthongal (ay), velar fronting of –ing, the Southern Vowel Shift (SVS), or “drawl” contributes more to the percept of stronger Southern accent. The former two have a wealth of studies; the latter two are investigated in detail in this study. Ch.2 looks at the SVS in sociolinguistic interview data from seven young women from the Riverton, Alabama, area near Huntsville. Results show that these young women participate little in the high front vowel shift but often have shifted mid front vowels. Ch.3 explores the idea of “drawl”, finding that neither laypeople nor linguists have a clear, unified notion of what constitutes this phenomenon. The chapter presents a taxonomy of drawl types and the vowels and linguistic environments that correlate with use. Measures of Euclidean Distance and Slope were applied to drawled tokens of the Riverton women, and revealed that triphthongs have much farther movement than other types within acoustic space. I argue for more precise uses of drawl and breaking, adding the concept hiatification (inserting hiatus into a vowel) to the analytic set.
    [Show full text]