An Introduction to Sociolinguistics Accent and Dialect Future Learn Course

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

An Introduction to Sociolinguistics Accent and Dialect Future Learn Course An Introduction to Sociolinguistics: Accents, Attitudes and Identity University of York https://www.futurelearn.com/courses/accents-attitudes-and-identity-an- introduction-to-sociolinguistics/2/todo/63077 Introductions 357 comments Welcome to our course on sociolinguistics. Over the next four weeks we will be exploring a range of sociolinguistic research methods and learn what these methods tell us about our individual sense of identity. You will also study their impact on social factors like gender, social class, and national affiliation. Plus, you will explore the real-world implications of language attitudes on our society as a whole. By the end of the course, we will have explored: • The study of language attitudes • How do you measure attitudes? • What is the relationship between language and identity? • Why language attitudes matter My name is Dr Sam Hellmuth and I will be one of your guides through this course along with my colleagues Dr Claire Childs, Dr Dom Watt, Sarah Lapacz and Justin Lo, so please follow us by clicking the links above… and then coming back! Please note that active tutor facilitation is limited to the number of teaching weeks from when the course run started. Please do not therefore expect any further tutor engagement from 10 August 2020, though you are very welcome to continue to work through the course materials in a self- directed fashion from that point. As you progress through the following steps you will hear from us and a range of other contributors, as we explore the concepts and methods that sociolinguists use to understand how accents, attitudes and identity interact. We look forward to learning with you over the next couple of weeks, and hope that you enjoy the course. Getting the most out of the course 129 comments How this all works You will work independently through the course at your own pace. All of the course content is now available to you, so if you need to, you can work ahead – but as social learning is an important part of the course it would be best if we tried to work through it together! We strongly recommend that you participate in the discussions and share your learning experiences. For any of you who are considering a path in higher education, this will be useful practice for participating in seminars or other group-based learning. Each week will have a theme, and we’ll learn about it through videos, articles and discussion. There will also be other optional tasks and exercises you can try to enhance your learning. As every week builds on the previous one, we strongly recommend you work through the course in order. Each of the four weeks should take you 3 to 4 hours to complete, if you do all the tasks, and there is a test at the end of each week to give an indication of what you’ve learnt. Using FutureLearn If this is your first FutureLearn course, you may find it useful to read the ‘how it works’ guide. We also recommend reading five tips and tools for social learning on FutureLearn to get the most out of the interactive and social learning features of this course. Let’s get started This week we’ll be focusing on how we define accents and language attitudes. So, let’s get started! (And when you have finished reading a section, please “Mark as complete”, and move onto the next step). What is sociolinguistics? Here, Dr Claire Childs explains how Sociolinguistics is a subject which covers a wide range of topics, such as what drives language variation. Is there anything from this short video which surprises you? 0:03Skip to 0 minutes and 3 secondsSociolinguistics, at its core, focuses on the relationship between language and society. Language is naturally diverse - it varies from place to place, over time, between different social groups, and on an individual level. 0:22Skip to 0 minutes and 22 secondsWe also know that people speak differently based on their social context, or when they are trying to communicate something about themselves to an audience. This kind of language variation is completely normal and all around us. As sociolinguistic researchers, we can investigate this diversity in how people sound or the words or grammar they use, to learn more about different communities and how members of these communities are seen by others. We can explore why people speak differently in different situations, and how language is used to communicate social meaning. 0:58Skip to 0 minutes and 58 secondsFor example, as I’m talking to you now, I’m making conscious decisions about my language use that are influenced by my current social context. I’m aware that I’m being recorded, and I want what I say to be understood by an audience of people that I haven’t met before. In these sorts of situations, people might try to speak more ‘carefully,’ perhaps by slowing down their speech, or by using words which are likely to be more widely known. But there are also aspects of my identity that are being communicated to you as I speak which are perhaps beyond my control. 1:31Skip to 1 minute and 31 secondsWhen we listen to people speak - or read what someone has written - we can infer information about the sort of person who produced that language. For example, you might be able to detect where someone is from based on their accent. That said, as we’ll see in this course, the link between how people express their identity through language – and how they are actually perceived by others – is not always straightforward. To begin with on this course, we’ll listen to a recording of a conversation. As you listen to the speakers, think about what sort of things you can tell from their speech, and how you were able to infer that information. We’ll discuss our thoughts shortly. How do linguists define accents? 527 comments Before we have a listen to some different accents, let’s clarify what we mean in this course by ‘accent.’ Across different areas of linguistic research, there is variation in what the term ‘accent’ means. For some researchers, the word ‘accent’ is rather broad in that it describes both the words used by speakers from a given community, as well as the sounds that these speakers make. For other researchers, the term ‘accent’ solely refers to the sounds which make one community sound distinct from another. For example, a Scottish English speaker will make sounds which other listeners might associate as being ‘Scottish’ (rather than English, Welsh or some other nationality). This creates a distinction between an accent (i.e. variation in pronunciation) and dialect (i.e. words and grammar). In this course, we’ll focus on how users of spoken English pronounce words differently to one another, and what this means for how we perceive others. As we’ll see in the next few weeks, there are many different approaches to exploring accents - each with their upsides and downsides for us to consider. That said, we can make a few generalisations about the study of accents in modern-day linguistic research as a whole: • Every speaker has an accent. Sometimes we think of our speech as being ‘normal’, ‘standard’ or perhaps even as not particularly unusual. In truth, we all display different aspects of our identity through our speech. What’s more, different listeners may perceive the same voice differently, each based on their own life experiences. We’ll have the chance to test this point out shortly! • All accents vary. This is something you can hear, for example, in films or TV programmes. While we can still understand the content (or gist) of speech in older recordings - we can also hear that their speech sounds different from modern-day speakers today. Accents are therefore able to change - along with the people who speak them. • All accents (and languages!) are of equal value. In linguistic research, no accent is more ‘correct’ or treated as ‘better’ or ‘worse’ than another. In turn, linguists view all accents as being very much worthy of being studied. What is interesting about this perspective is how it can sometimes differ from the views of non-linguists. It is commonly found in sociolinguistic research that non-linguists might have highly positive or negative opinions about an accent. These opinions can even reach the point where non-linguists might see some accents as being ‘better’ than others. Researchers in linguistics will often try to avoid allowing these more personal opinions to affect their research. To summarise everything so far, in this course, we’ll be thinking about ‘accents’ in terms of how speakers pronounce words differently to one another. We’ll also view accents as something which everyone has, which are however variable (and unique to individual speakers and groups). Finally, we’ll treat all accents as being equally important to linguistic research as a whole. All of these points mean that describing your own experiences with accents in the discussion sections of this course is going to be both insightful and useful for other learners. Listening task: accent variation In this recording, you’ll hear best friends Abbie and Catherine talking about their own accents. Take a note of any impressions you have about these speakers - these can be anything which comes to mind. For example, you might consider the following questions: • What accents do these speakers have? • Were you able to identify where they were from? • What else could you infer from these speakers’ voices? Transcript listening task: accent variation Abbie: But I think just probably because like my parents don’t have particularly distinct accents Catherine: No neither do mine Abbie: Mum is very – Mum’s very RP Catherine: Yeah Abbie: So um I think a lot of where my my stuff has come from is is probably via her Catherine: Mmm Abbie: But um yeah I think it’s just if I’ve if I’ve ever had any comments about my accent it’s usually like oh it’s posh Catherine: Yeah Abbie: Or um people are like oh you’re from the South.
Recommended publications
  • Variations in Language Use Across Gender: Biological Versus Sociological Theories
    UC Merced Proceedings of the Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society Title Variations in Language Use across Gender: Biological versus Sociological Theories Permalink https://escholarship.org/uc/item/1q30w4z0 Journal Proceedings of the Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society, 28(28) ISSN 1069-7977 Authors Bell, Courtney M. McCarthy, Philip M. McNamara, Danielle S. Publication Date 2006 Peer reviewed eScholarship.org Powered by the California Digital Library University of California Variations in Language Use across Gender: Biological versus Sociological Theories Courtney M. Bell (cbell@ mail.psyc.memphis.edu Philip M. McCarthy ([email protected]) Danielle S. McNamara ([email protected]) Institute for Intelligent Systems University of Memphis Memphis, TN38152 Abstract West, 1975; West & Zimmerman, 1983) and overlap We examine gender differences in language use in light of women’s speech (Rosenblum, 1986) during conversations the biological and social construction theories of gender. than the reverse. On the other hand, other research The biological theory defines gender in terms of biological indicates no gender differences in interruptions (Aries, sex resulting in polarized and static language differences 1996; James & Clarke, 1993) or insignificant differences based on sex. The social constructionist theory of gender (Anderson & Leaper, 1998). However, potentially more assumes gender differences in language use depend on the context in which the interaction occurs. Gender is important than citing the differences, is positing possible contextually defined and fluid, predicting that males and explanations for why they might exist. We approach that females use a variety of linguistic strategies. We use a problem here by testing the biological and social qualitative linguistic approach to investigate gender constructionist theories (Bergvall, 1999; Coates & differences in language within a context of marital conflict.
    [Show full text]
  • The Standardisation of African Languages Michel Lafon, Vic Webb
    The Standardisation of African Languages Michel Lafon, Vic Webb To cite this version: Michel Lafon, Vic Webb. The Standardisation of African Languages. Michel Lafon; Vic Webb. IFAS, pp.141, 2008, Nouveaux Cahiers de l’Ifas, Aurelia Wa Kabwe Segatti. halshs-00449090 HAL Id: halshs-00449090 https://halshs.archives-ouvertes.fr/halshs-00449090 Submitted on 20 Jan 2010 HAL is a multi-disciplinary open access L’archive ouverte pluridisciplinaire HAL, est archive for the deposit and dissemination of sci- destinée au dépôt et à la diffusion de documents entific research documents, whether they are pub- scientifiques de niveau recherche, publiés ou non, lished or not. The documents may come from émanant des établissements d’enseignement et de teaching and research institutions in France or recherche français ou étrangers, des laboratoires abroad, or from public or private research centers. publics ou privés. The Standardisation of African Languages Language political realities CentRePoL and IFAS Proceedings of a CentRePoL workshop held at University of Pretoria on March 29, 2007, supported by the French Institute for Southern Africa Michel Lafon (LLACAN-CNRS) & Vic Webb (CentRePoL) Compilers/ Editors CentRePoL wishes to express its appreciation to the following: Dr. Aurelia Wa Kabwe-Segatti, Research Director, IFAS, Johannesburg, for her professional and material support; PanSALB, for their support over the past two years for CentRePoL’s standardisation project; The University of Pretoria, for the use of their facilities. Les Nouveaux Cahiers de l’IFAS/ IFAS Working Paper Series is a series of occasional working papers, dedicated to disseminating research in the social and human sciences on Southern Africa.
    [Show full text]
  • CHAPTER 2 Registers of Language
    Duranti / Companion to Linguistic Anthropology Final 12.11.2003 1:28pm page 23 Registers of CHAPTER 2 Language Asif Agha 1INTRODUCTION Language users often employ labels like ‘‘polite language,’’ ‘‘informal speech,’’ ‘‘upper-class speech,’’ ‘‘women’s speech,’’ ‘‘literary usage,’’ ‘‘scientific term,’’ ‘‘reli- gious language,’’ ‘‘slang,’’ and others, to describe differences among speech forms. Metalinguistic labels of this kind link speech repertoires to enactable pragmatic effects, including images of the person speaking (woman, upper-class person), the relationship of speaker to interlocutor (formality, politeness), the conduct of social practices (religious, literary, or scientific activity); they hint at the existence of cultural models of speech – a metapragmatic classification of discourse types – linking speech repertoires to typifications of actor, relationship, and conduct. This is the space of register variation conceived in intuitive terms. Writers on language – linguists, anthropologists, literary critics – have long been interested in cultural models of this kind simply because they are of common concern to language users. Speakers of any language can intuitively assign speech differences to a space of classifications of the above kind and, correspondingly, can respond to others’ speech in ways sensitive to such distinctions. Competence in such models is an indispensable resource in social interaction. Yet many features of such models – their socially distributed existence, their ideological character, the way in which they motivate tropes of personhood and identity – have tended to puzzle writers on the subject of registers. I will be arguing below that a clarification of these issues – indeed the very study of registers – requires attention to reflexive social processes whereby such models are formulated and disseminated in social life and become available for use in interaction by individuals.
    [Show full text]
  • A Literature Review on Code-Switching
    1 Code-switching as a Result of Language Acquisition: A Case Study of a 1.5 Generation Child from China1 Yalun Zhou, Ph.D.2 Michael Wei, Ph.D.3 Abstract Despite individual differences, all bilinguals share the ability to act in their native language, in their second language, and to switch back and forth between the two languages they know (Van Hell, 1998). Chinese is the largest Asian American ethnic group in the United States. Their use of code-switching is an increasingly important issue in understanding their language choice and language development. This study on code-switching between a 1.5 generation Chinese child and her parents will add perspectives on the growing literature of Chinese American families, their language interaction and language development. Introduction There are several definitions for code-switching. Gumperz (1982 b) defined code-switching as “the juxtaposition within the same speech exchange of passages of speech belonging to two different grammatical systems or subsystems” (p. 59). The emphasis is on the two grammatical systems of one language, although most people refer to code-switching as the mixed use of 1 This paper was presented at the 2007 Annual Conference of Teaching English to Speakers of Other Languages (TESOL), Seattle, Washington. 2 Yalun Zhou, Ph.D., Assistant Professor, Director of Chinese Minor Program, Dept. of Communication and Media, Rensselaer Polytechnic Institute, [email protected] 3 Michael Wei, Ph.D., Associate Professor, TESOL Program Director, School of Education, University of Missouri-Kansas City, [email protected] 2 languages. Milroy and Muysken (1995) stated that code-switching is “the alternative use by bilinguals of two or more languages in the same conversation” (p.7).
    [Show full text]
  • Chapter 1. Introduction
    1 Chapter 1. Introduction Once an English-speaking population was established in South Africa in the 19 th century, new unique dialects of English began to emerge in the colony, particularly in the Eastern Cape, as a result of dialect levelling and contact with indigenous groups and the L1 Dutch speaking population already present in the country (Lanham 1996). Recognition of South African English as a variety in its own right came only later in the next century. South African English, however, is not a homogenous dialect; there are many different strata present under this designation, which have been recognised and identified in terms of geographic location and social factors such as first language, ethnicity, social class and gender (Hooper 1944a; Lanham 1964, 1966, 1967b, 1978b, 1982, 1990, 1996; Bughwan 1970; Lanham & MacDonald 1979; Barnes 1986; Lass 1987b, 1995; Wood 1987; McCormick 1989; Chick 1991; Mesthrie 1992, 1993a; Branford 1994; Douglas 1994; Buthelezi 1995; Dagut 1995; Van Rooy 1995; Wade 1995, 1997; Gough 1996; Malan 1996; Smit 1996a, 1996b; Görlach 1998c; Van der Walt 2000; Van Rooy & Van Huyssteen 2000; de Klerk & Gough 2002; Van der Walt & Van Rooy 2002; Wissing 2002). English has taken different social roles throughout South Africa’s turbulent history and has presented many faces – as a language of oppression, a language of opportunity, a language of separation or exclusivity, and also as a language of unification. From any chosen theoretical perspective, the presence of English has always been a point of contention in South Africa, a combination of both threat and promise (Mawasha 1984; Alexander 1990, 2000; de Kadt 1993, 1993b; de Klerk & Bosch 1993, 1994; Mesthrie & McCormick 1993; Schmied 1995; Wade 1995, 1997; de Klerk 1996b, 2000; Granville et al.
    [Show full text]
  • Making Sense of "Bad English"
    MAKING SENSE OF “BAD ENGLISH” Why is it that some ways of using English are considered “good” and others are considered “bad”? Why are certain forms of language termed elegant, eloquent, or refined, whereas others are deemed uneducated, coarse, or inappropriate? Making Sense of “Bad English” is an accessible introduction to attitudes and ideologies towards the use of English in different settings around the world. Outlining how perceptions about what constitutes “good” and “bad” English have been shaped, this book shows how these principles are based on social factors rather than linguistic issues and highlights some of the real-life consequences of these perceptions. Features include: • an overview of attitudes towards English and how they came about, as well as real-life consequences and benefits of using “bad” English; • explicit links between different English language systems, including child’s English, English as a lingua franca, African American English, Singlish, and New Delhi English; • examples taken from classic names in the field of sociolinguistics, including Labov, Trudgill, Baugh, and Lambert, as well as rising stars and more recent cutting-edge research; • links to relevant social parallels, including cultural outputs such as holiday myths, to help readers engage in a new way with the notion of Standard English; • supporting online material for students which features worksheets, links to audio and news files, further examples and discussion questions, and background on key issues from the book. Making Sense of “Bad English” provides an engaging and thought-provoking overview of this topic and is essential reading for any student studying sociolinguistics within a global setting.
    [Show full text]
  • THE ANALYSIS of LANGUAGE VARIATION USED in FAST and FURIOUS 8 MOVIE a Sociolinguistics Study By: Arkin Haris, S.Pd., M.Hum
    THE ANALYSIS OF LANGUAGE VARIATION USED IN FAST AND FURIOUS 8 MOVIE A Sociolinguistics Study By: Arkin Haris, S.Pd., M.Hum. Email: [email protected] Website: arkinharis.com A. Background of Study As human beings, people can not be separated from the process of communication. In their lives, people need to interact with others since they can’t live by themselves. Through communication process, people can change their minds, ideas, thoughts, and intentions. They can also deliver messages to others. In conducting communication, people need a medium to express their intentions and messages. The most appropriate medium is language since language can carry a message by symbols. This is in line with what has been suggested by Wardaugh (1992: 8) who states that ―Language allows people to say things to each other and expresses communicate needs‖. In short, language is constantly used by humans in their daily life as a means of communication. Language is very important in social interaction. In interlace good relation, people will use appropriate language that can be understood by others in particular event. Some communities have their own language that is used in daily activity which different with other communities. Every community have different characteristic from their culture which determined the variety of language that they use. Some of them make uncommon languages that only can be understood by the member of communities in order to keeping their attribute or keeping a secret. Family relation, work place, friendship, and social class also can be causes of language varieties. Beside language varieties, changed or mix a language to another can be the way to establish a communication depend on who is the partner and the context.
    [Show full text]
  • JOURNAL of LANGUAGE and LINGUISTIC STUDIES ISSN: 1305-578X Journal of Language and Linguistic Studies, 13(1), 379-398; 2017
    Available online at www.jlls.org JOURNAL OF LANGUAGE AND LINGUISTIC STUDIES ISSN: 1305-578X Journal of Language and Linguistic Studies, 13(1), 379-398; 2017 The impact of non-native English teachers’ linguistic insecurity on learners’ productive skills Giti Ehtesham Daftaria*, Zekiye Müge Tavilb a Gazi University, Ankara, Turkey b Gazi University, Ankara, Turkey APA Citation: Daftari, G.E &Tavil, Z. M. (2017). The Impact of Non-native English Teachers’ Linguistic Insecurity on Learners’ Productive Skills. Journal of Language and Linguistic Studies, 13(1), 379-398. Submission Date: 28/11/2016 Acceptance Date:04/13/2017 Abstract The discrimination between native and non-native English speaking teachers is reported in favor of native speakers in literature. The present study examines the linguistic insecurity of non-native English speaking teachers (NNESTs) and investigates its influence on learners' productive skills by using SPSS software. The eighteen teachers participating in this research study are from different countries, mostly Asian, and they all work in a language institute in Ankara, Turkey. The learners who participated in this work are 300 intermediate, upper-intermediate and advanced English learners. The data related to teachers' linguistic insecurity were collected by questionnaires, semi-structured interviews and proficiency tests. Pearson Correlation and ANOVA Tests were used and the results revealed that NNESTs' linguistic insecurity, neither female nor male teachers, is not significantly correlated with the learners' writing and speaking scores. © 2017 JLLS and the Authors - Published by JLLS. Keywords: linguistic insecurity, non-native English teachers, productive skills, questionnaire, interview, proficiency test 1. Introduction There is no doubt today that English is the unrivaled lingua franca of the world with the largest number of non-native speakers.
    [Show full text]
  • 4 Variation in Language and Gender
    98 Suzanne Romaine 4 Variation in Language and Gender SUZANNE ROMAINE 1 Introduction This chapter addresses some of the main research methods, trends, and findings concerning variation in language and gender. Most of the studies examined here have employed what can be referred to as quantitative variationist meth- odology (sometimes also called the quantitative paradigm or variation theory) to reveal and analyze sociolinguistic patterns, that is, correlations between variable features of the kind usually examined in sociolinguistic studies of urban speech communities (e.g. postvocalic /r/ in New York City, glottalization in Glasgow, initial /h/ in Norwich, etc.), and external social factors such as social class, age, sex, network, and style (see Labov 1972a). When such large-scale systematic research into sociolinguistic variation began in the 1960s, its main focus was to illuminate the relationship between language and social structure more generally, rather than the relationship between language and gender specifically. However, the category of sex (un- derstood simply as a binary division between males and females) was often included as a major social variable and instances of gender variation (or sex differentiation, as it was generally called) were noted in relation to other socio- linguistic patterns, particularly, social class and stylistic differentiation. Because the way in which research questions are formed has a bearing on the findings, some of the basic methodological assumptions and the historical context in which the variationist approach emerged are discussed briefly in section 2. The general findings are the focus of section 3, with special reference to connections between sex differentiation, social class stratification, and style shifting.
    [Show full text]
  • Sociolinguistics: Language Change Wardhaugh Chapter 8
    Sociolinguistics: Language Change Wardhaugh chapter 8 (Labov’s homepage: http://www.ling.upenn.edu/~wlabov/home.html) Non-prestige dialects of English and Language Dispersion A common preconception about non-prestige dialects or colloquial forms of English is that they are unsystematic and 'lazy' forms of language, and that they either reflect or even encourage illogical thought. Over the past 4 decades many linguists have studied non-prestige and colloquial forms of English (and other languages) and arrived at the conclusion that these varieties are just as systematic as prestige varieties of English, that their 'non-standard' features are typically features found in prestige varieties of other languages, and that there is no basis for claiming that their phonology, morphology or syntax reflects 'illogical' or lazy thinking. We focus here on the dialect of English that has received the most attention: it is known variously as "Black English Vernacular" (BEV), "African American Vernacular English" (AAVE) or "Ebonics". The first two terms are the most commonly used terms used in sociolinguistic research; the third term has achieved wide recognition in the wake of a highly controversial resolution of the Oakland (CA) Board of Education involving the role of Ebonics in K-12 education. In what follows I will use the term AAVE, which is the most widespread term in current linguistic research. Some excellent readily accessible articles on AAVE are available on-line. Note1: the colloquial English spoken by African American communities spans a wide varieties of styles, often identical to or barely distinguishable from the English spoken by other ethnic groups, including the prestige white variety.
    [Show full text]
  • 'Standard' Vs. 'Nonstandard' Attitudes Towards Language Effects of Social Class on Dialect
    Today ‘Standard’ vs. ‘nonstandard’ Attitudes towards language Effects of social class on dialect Standard vs. Non-standard Standard dialect typically used by political leaders, upper classes, in the media taught in schools considered the dominant or ‘prestige’ dialect Non-standard dialect any dialect not perceived as ‘standard’ Standard vs. Non-standard Some standard dialects of English Standard American English (SAE) characterized by grammatical (morphological & syntactic) features Received Pronunciation (RP) (in UK) characterized by phonological features Standard vs. Non-standard Some non-standard dialects African-American English (AAE) Multiple negatives: He don’ know nothin’. Appalachian English Double modals: I might could do that. He useta couldn’t swim. a-prefix: go a-fishin’, come a-runnin’ Overt vs. Covert prestige Overt prestige: Attached to a particular dialect by the community at large that defines how people should speak to gain status in that community Covert prestige: Exists among nonstandard speakers and defines how people should speak to be considered members of that particular group Social class and dialect William Labov New York City ‘r’-lessness Studied variation in [r] pronunciation as it relates to socioeconomic class Dept Store study - Method Interviewed salespeople at Saks 5th Ave., (upper), Macy’s (middle), S. Klein (lower class) “Excuse me, where are the ____?” “Fourth floor.” (casual speech) “Excuse me?” “Fourth floor.” (careful speech with emphasis) Dept Store study - Results Klein Macy’s Saks Casual 8% 44%
    [Show full text]
  • A Study on Diglossia: English Language Variety Choice by English Language Course Students in Public Universities and Its Factors
    A STUDY ON DIGLOSSIA: ENGLISH LANGUAGE VARIETY CHOICE BY ENGLISH LANGUAGE COURSE STUDENTS IN PUBLIC UNIVERSITIES AND ITS FACTORS BY CHE WAN NURUL IFFAH BINTI CHE WAN ROSLAN INTERNATIONAL ISLAMIC UNIVERSITY MALAYSIA 2020 A STUDY ON DIGLOSSIA: ENGLISH LANGUAGE VARIETY CHOICE BY ENGLISH LANGUAGE COURSE STUDENTS IN PUBLIC UNIVERSITIES AND ITS FACTORS BY CHE WAN NURUL IFFAH BINTI CHE WAN ROSLAN A Final Year Project submitted in fulfillment of the requirement for the degree of English for International Communication JANUARY, 2020 ABSTRACT This paper reports a study on students' language variety choice by analyzing the answers from a set of questionnaire by the English language undergraduate students from different public universities in Malaysia. Specifically, this study is attempted to identify the university students' language choice and its factors. A set of survey questions was used and spreaded in different Whatsapps’ group in order to get data from university students in five different public universities in Malaysia, including International Islamic University Malaysia, Universiti Putra Malaysia, University of Malaya, Universiti Kebangsaan Malaysia, and also Universiti Sultan Zainal Abidin. The survey asked the students who are pursuing their degree in different English courses such as communication, language studies, literature, and also teaching English as second language on their language choice. Other than that, they were also asked about factors regarding environmental and status of the speakers that influenced their choice of language varieties. KEY WORD: diglossia, the usage of English language in Malaysia, the language variety choice and factors that influenced i DECLARATION I hereby declare that this final year project is the result of my own investigations, except where otherwise stated.
    [Show full text]