The Relexification of Locative Constructions in Sranan

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

The Relexification of Locative Constructions in Sranan Yakpo, Kofi & Adrienne Bruyn. 2015. Transatlantic patterns: The relexification of locative constructions in Sranan. In Pieter Muysken & Norval Smith (eds.), Surviving the Middle Passage: The West Africa-Surinam Sprachbund, 135–75. (Trends in Linguistics, Studies and Monographs (TiLSM) 275). Berlin: De Gruyter Mouton. AUTHOR MANUSCRIPT VERSION Trans-Atlantic patterns: the relexification of locative constructions in Sranan Kofi Yakpo and Adrienne Bruyn 1. Introduction Sranan and the other creoles of Suriname have long been noted for their use of postpositions in the expression of spatial relations (cf. e.g. Muysken 1987). This characteristic sets these languages apart from the vast majority of Afro-Caribbean English-lexifier Creoles, both in the Americas as well as in West Africa. The use of postpositions, some of which are derived from English words for body-parts, is one of the more conspicuous features pointing towards substrate influence in Sranan. Beyond this particularly visible African presence, the grammar of spatial relations in Sranan contains many more features that suggest a diffusion from Africa, and to be more precise, from the Gbe languages, as well as Western Bantu via Kikongo (for the dominant role of the Gbe substrate of Sranan, as well as the secondary role of the Kikongo substrate cf. e.g. Arends 1995; Arends, Kouwenberg, and Smith 1995; Huttar 1981, 1986; Migge 1998a, 1998b, 2000, 2003; Smith 1987; Winford 2000). The affinities of Sranan with these African language(s)/families can be traced in the semantics of individual locative elements. For example, the word baka, derived from Engl. ‘back’ is the regular form employed for the expression of the body part as well as the spatial concept ‘behind’ in Sranan. The semantics of baka overlaps with that of the Gbe (Ewe) item mègbé ‘back’ which is also employed with both senses. There is good reason to assume that such systematic correspondences in meaning and function represent cases of local relexification, that is, of individual forms. The main purpose of this chapter is, however, to show that the participation of relexified Sranan forms like baka in multi-constituent locative constructions constitute cases of pattern relexification. We will show that the concept of pattern relexification can explain the behaviour of Sranan locative elements in instances where an account based on local relexification alone would be stretched to its limits. Pattern relexification makes allowance for differences between Sranan and the substrate languages in the behaviour of individual items. The reason is that the 136 Kofi Yakpo and Adrienne Bruyn Relexification of patterns involves the transfer of lexical properties of individual forms plus their relational properties. A central part of the argument for pattern relexification is that Niger-Congo substrate patterns manifest a large degree of homogeneity, and that this probably facilitated the relexification in Sranan of morphosyntactic blue-prints or skeletons. At the same time, we will see that Sranan locative constructions also reveal the intricate interplay of substrate patterns, patterns inherited from the lexifier English, influence from Dutch, which has served as a superstrate for more than three hundred years, as well as internal development. In this context, we should clarify our use of the terms “lexifier” and “superstrate”. Suriname consitutes a case, in which the lexifier (the language that provides the bulk of the lexicon, and that of the basic lexicon in particular) and the superstrate (the language that has serves as the language of the socially dominant group) of the creoles are not identical (cf. Selbach 2008). The ancestor language of Sranan and the other Surinamese creole languages was formed during a relatively brief period of English colonial rule (cf. Smith, this volume on the early history of Suriname), with English serving as the lexifier, and by default, also as the superstrate language. When the Dutch took control of Suriname in 1667, Dutch replaced English as the colonial language and has thenceforth also served as the superstrate (i.e. socially dominant) language of Suriname. One conclusion drawn from the data presented in this chapter is that the presence of substrate patterns in locative constructions is significant, both in a diachronic and a synchronic perspective. The strong parallels in the grammar of spatial relations between Sranan and Gbe in particular provide further support for the existence of a Transatlantic Sprachbund that unites the Surinamese creoles and the Gbe languages with respect to a substantial number of isoglosses. The Sranan examples in this chapter for which references are not provided stem from a corpus of primary data collected in Suriname and the Netherlands in 2011 by Kofi Yakpo as part of the “Traces of Contact” project of Radboud University Nijmegen. Unless indicated otherwise, examples from the Gbe languages are also from field data, collected by Kofi Yakpo in Ghana and Togo between 2003 and 2011 and speaker intuitions of Kofi Yakpo. Tone-marking is provided for the authors’ primary data and wherever contained in the sources. After providing an overview of locative elements in Sranan in section 2, we describe the expression of three important spatial relations in Sranan and Gbe in section 3. In section 4, we attempt to explain the variation encountered in Sranan locative constructions by additionally drawing on Kikongo data. Section 5 summarizes and systematizes the findings, and section 6 concludes the chapter. The relexification of locative constructions in Sranan 137 2. Locative elements in Sranan This section provides a brief overview of the forms and functions of locative elements in Sranan. The inventory of locative elements (i.e. prepositions and locative nouns) in modern Sranan largely consists of items of English origin, with a minority of Dutch origin. However, these locative elements may appear in constructions that represent substantial departures from the corresponding ones in the English and Dutch. We conclude that the differences between Sranan on the one hand, and the English and the Dutch on the other, are largely due to substrate transfer. We base our analyses on examples from the Gbe languages Ewe (Ghana, Togo), Gen (Togo), Aja (Togo, Benin), Gun (Benin) and Fon (Benin). We should mention here that the historical evidence adduced by the authors mentioned further above suggests that Fon varieties (hence the eastern reaches of the Gbe continuum) constituted the single most important substrates of Sranan, rather than more western varieties like Gen and Ewe. However, we have found it useful to consider corresponding structures from varieties other than Fon because it shows that the templates for expressing spatial relations in all the Gbe languages are virtually identical. This strengthens the argument for a general Gbe origin of the patterns employed to express spatial relations in Sranan, since there is no need to show an exclusive, or even predominant influence of Fon. The Sranan locative elements employed for expressing the basic spatial relations relevant for the discussion are given in 6.1: Table 1. Sranan locative elements Locative element Meaning Source language(s) ini inner part, in ‘in’ (Du./Eng.) na doro outside ‘LOC door’ (Eng.) tapu top, on ‘(on) top (of)’ ondro bottom, under ‘onder/under’ (Du./Eng.) fesi face; in front ‘face’ baka back, behind ‘(at the) back (of)’ fu general location; SOURCE- ‘for’ (Eng.) oriented na general location ‘na’ (Igbo/Port, cf e.g. Parkvall 2001: 108) Some of the forms in the Table have corresponding near-homophones in English and Dutch (i.e. Sranan ondro, English under, Dutch onder). Although simultaneous influences from Dutch and English, hence convergence, are in principle possible, the entire system of specific prepositions is derived from the lexifier English. 138 Kofi Yakpo and Adrienne Bruyn We therefore assume English forms to be the source forms unless the contrary can be proven. The forms ini ‘inside’ and ondro ‘under’) are are derived from the corresponding prepositions in English. The elements baka ‘back, behind’ and tapu ‘top, on’ are only found in complex locative structures in English (e.g. at the back of the car) and have nominal uses (e.g. my back) as well. The element fesi is only found with a locative sense in specialized contexts in English but not with a general meaning as in Sranan (e.g. ‘the face of the building’). The element na doro, literally ‘at the door’ and with the meaning ‘outside’ is a Sranan neologism that has no exact correspondence in English or Dutch. Among its spatial senses, the element fu functions as a general locative preposition to denote a PLACE, however less prominently so than na, which follows below. The preposition fu may also be employed to denote a SOURCE and if this is the case, appear without support from other PATH-denoting locative elements, as in (1). One possible explanation for the PLACE and SOURCE senses of this preposition is that the English prepositions for and from may have converged into fu during the formative period of the language: (1) ala den wroko disi wi leri fu mi papa. all DEF.PL work this 1PL learn ABL 1SG father ‘All these (types of) works we learnt from my father.’ (Sranan; Hart 1996: 17) At the bottom of the table we find the only element without a Dutch or English etymology, namely the general locative preposition na ‘LOC’ (with its modern variant a). Reflexes of this form are present throughout the family of Afro-Caribbean English-lexifier Creoles, even if na is not found in all languages. In Sranan, the preposition na ‘LOC’ functions as a general GROUND marker and may introduce participants with PLACE (2), GOAL (3), SOURCE (4) and PATH (5) roles. In the following sections we will see how corresponding forms fulfil very similar functions in the substrate languages of Sranan: (2) mi e tan na boiti.
Recommended publications
  • Quichua-Spanish Language Contact in Salcedo, Ecuador: Revisiting Media Lengua Syncretic Language Practices
    QUICHUA-SPANISH LANGUAGE CONTACT IN SALCEDO, ECUADOR: REVISITING MEDIA LENGUA SYNCRETIC LANGUAGE PRACTICES BY MARCO SHAPPECK DISSERTATION Submitted in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy in Linguistics in the Graduate College of the University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign, 2011 Urbana, Illinois Doctoral Committee: Professor Hans Henrich Hock, Director of Research Professor Rajeshwari Vijay Pandharipande, Chair Associate Professor Anna María Escobar Professor José Ignacio Hualde Abstract The purpose of the current thesis is to develop a better understanding of the interaction between Spanish and Quichua in the Salcedo region and provide more information for the processes that might have given rise to Media Lengua, a ‘mixed’ language comprised of a Quichua grammar and Spanish lexicon. Muysken attributes the formation of Media Lengua to relexification, ruling out any influence from other bilingual phenomena. I argue that the only characteristic that distinguishes Media Lengua from other language contact varieties in central Ecuador is the quantity of the overall Spanish borrowings and not the type of processes that might have been employed by Quichua speakers during the genesis of Media Lengua. The results from the Salcedo data that I have collected show how processes such as adlexification, code-mixing, and structural convergence produce Media Lengua-type sentences, evidence that supports an alternative analysis to Muysken’s relexification hypothesis. Overall, this dissertation is developed around four main objectives: (1) to describe the variation of Spanish loanwords within a bilingual community in Salcedo; (2) to analyze some of the prominent and recent structural changes in Quichua and Spanish; (3) to determine whether Spanish loanword use can be explained by the relationship consultants have with particular social categories; and (4) to analyze the consultants’ language ideologies toward syncretic uses of Spanish and Quichua.
    [Show full text]
  • Jargons and Pidgins and Creoles, Oh My!
    University of Nebraska - Lincoln DigitalCommons@University of Nebraska - Lincoln UReCA: The NCHC Journal of Undergraduate Research & Creative Activity National Collegiate Honors Council 2016 Jargons and Pidgins and Creoles, Oh My! Emily Gray University of Tennessee at Chattanooga Follow this and additional works at: https://digitalcommons.unl.edu/ureca Part of the Educational Methods Commons, Gifted Education Commons, Higher Education Commons, Language Description and Documentation Commons, and the Other Linguistics Commons Gray, Emily, "Jargons and Pidgins and Creoles, Oh My!" (2016). UReCA: The NCHC Journal of Undergraduate Research & Creative Activity. 72. https://digitalcommons.unl.edu/ureca/72 This Article is brought to you for free and open access by the National Collegiate Honors Council at DigitalCommons@University of Nebraska - Lincoln. It has been accepted for inclusion in UReCA: The NCHC Journal of Undergraduate Research & Creative Activity by an authorized administrator of DigitalCommons@University of Nebraska - Lincoln. Jargons and Pidgins and Creoles, Oh My! by Emily Gray University of Tennessee at Chattanooga Linguistics, as defined by the Oxford English Dictionary, is “the science of studying language, including phonetics, phonology, morphology, syntax, semantics, pragmatics, and historical linguistics” (OED.com). Within this field, the study of pidgin and creole languages is the source of much controversy and disagreement. Due to their divergence from typical linguistic features and development patterns, pidgins and creoles have long been ignored by the linguistics community. Considered by many to be “inferior, haphazard, broken” versions of “older, more established languages,” these so-called “bastard tongues” were written off as unworthy of study (Todd 1). Only recently have these forms of language garnered interest from linguistic scholars known as Creolists.
    [Show full text]
  • Why Languages and Contacts?
    WHY LANGUAGES AND1 CONTACTS? Petr Zima, Faculty of Humanities Charles University, Praha 1. Preliminaries The concept and terminology of “languages in contact” developed by various structuralist schools since Weinreich's time is undoubtedly a great advance over the neogrammarian viewpoint of the traditional comparative linguistics. As for the neogrammarians, they considered the phenomena of mutual influence between languages to be a sort of “dustbin”, where exceptions and complications, impossible to be explained by internal laws of historical development of genetic language trees were to be laid off. That is why the generation of Weinreich has proposed to analyse this field in the light of two (or more) contacting language structures. Yet, this very term and concept of “contact linguistics” – though formulated in opposition to the traditional language trees of the comparativist past – was itself coined under the symbolic influence of the comparativist terminology and concepts. Whereas the neogrammarians were influenced by analogies with trees and branches of Darwin’s biology and Linné’s botany, the concept of languages in contact was also under the influence of figurative thinking, that of the science of man, antropology. In fact, while the neogrammarians presented languages in the light of their analogies with plants and animals, the early structuralist and even post-structuralist approach since Weinreich’s and Haugen’s time presented languages as human individuals in contact. Hence, though critically opposing the neogrammarians’ analogies, this approach built its own theoretical position under the unnoticed, but heavy influence of the same type of methodical parallels rooted in the figurative thinking of another discipline. 2.
    [Show full text]
  • The Routledge Linguistics Encyclopedia
    THE ROUTLEDGE LINGUISTICS ENCYCLOPEDIA The Routledge Linguistics Encyclopedia is a single- Optimality Theory volume encyclopedia covering all major and Research Methods in Linguistics subsidiary areas of linguistics and applied lin- Slang guistics. The seventy nine entries provide in-depth coverage of the topics and sub-topics of the field. The following entries have been recommissioned Entries are alphabetically arranged and exten- or substantially revised: sively cross-referenced so the reader can see how Animals and Language, Artificial Languages, areas interrelate. Including a substantial intro- Computational Linguistics to Language Engi- duction which provides a potted history of lin- neering, Contrastive Analysis/Contrastive Linguis- guistics and suggestions for further reading, this tics, Corpus Linguistics, Critical Discourse is an indispensable reference tool for specialists Analysis, Dialectology, Discourse Analysis, Dys- and non-specialists alike. lexia, Genre Analysis, Historical Linguistics, Into- This third edition has been thoroughly revised nation, Language and Education, Language, and updated, with new entries on: Gender and Sexuality, Language Origins, Lan- guage Surveys, Language Universals, Linguistic Attitudes to Language Typology, Metaphor, Pragmatics, Rhetoric, Conversation Analysis Semantics, Semiotics, Sociolinguistics, Stylistics, English Language Teaching Systemic-Functional Grammar, Writing Systems. Gesture and Language Idioms Language and Advertising Kirsten Malmkjær is Professor of Translation Language
    [Show full text]
  • Decal Detailed Outline
    OVERVIEW: 1. Introduction 2. History 3. Typology 4. Sound systems 5. Writing systems 6. Signing systems 7. Morphology 8. Syntax 9. Universals / Naturalness 10. Vocabulary Generation 11. Semantics, metaphor, glossing, translation 12. Conculture 13. Language evolution / aging / families 14. ……? 1. Introduction Time: 1-2 days References: Conlang FAQ; LJ comm.’s; CONLANG-L Pre-HW: 1. Course sign-up form, polls Post-HW: 1. Look through conlangs_decal comm. 2. Read Conlang FAQ 3. Look at conlangs comm., conlang-l 4. Start thinking about own goals 5. Read manifestos, intros from reader / online. 6. Browse other online resources Intro speech (Klingon, Quenya, Lojban, Esperanto, English) Who does this? G B LH Ls Famous people (Dante) Powerful people (Korean, Turkish) Hobbyists Linguists Non-linguists Fiction writers Random people What are conlangs? “Constructed language” i.e., L created w/ intent Spectrum of natural artificial Goals (vs. natlangs which have none); cont’d later Criteria, prototypes, evaluation of success Spectrum of scope Code (Morse, Hobo Sign) Relexification (Thieves’ Cant, Pig Latin) • By algorithm • Slang • Pronunciation Minimal-grammar conlang (naming language) Language addition (natlang+) • Modularity Low-grammar conlang (short phrases, sayings, citations, etc) Low-vocab conlang Full conlang Living language (Esperanto etc.) Spectrum of naturalness Universals Natlang, a priori, a posteriori Stealth conlangs (Korean, Hebrew, Turkish, etc) What goes into developing a conlang? GOAL Aesthetics Crypto Sapir-Whorf World-building Logic / philosophical Auxiliary L • Local / international Break/test “Universals” Machine translation • Pivot / meta language • AI language internal descriptions “Fixing” natlangs (not this class) Proto-language extrapolation Communication w/ God (glossolalia) “Ideal” language Learn linguistics Special purpose Fun! DISCUSS: Other goals? Top-down vs.
    [Show full text]
  • Re-Evaluating Relexification: the Case of Jamaican Creole
    Re-evaluating Relexification: The Case of Jamaican Creole Kyle Davin Parsard Advisor: Claire Bowern Submitted to the faculty of the Department of Linguistics in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Bachelor of Arts 1 Contents Abstract 3 1 Out of Many, One Language 4 1.1 Substrate Influence 4 1.2 The Creole Continuum 6 1.3 Current Study 7 2 Thirty-Five Years of Creole Genesis Models 8 2.1 Universalist Accounts 8 2.2 Substratist Accounts 8 2.3 Superstratist Accounts 8 3 The Relexification Hypothesis 8 3.1 Predictions of Relexification 11 3.2 DeGraff’s (2002) Objections to Relexification 12 4 Relexification in Jamaican Creole 13 4.1 The Noun Phrase 14 4.1.1 Articles 14 4.1.2 Demonstratives 15 4.1.3 Plural Marking 17 4.1.4 Possessive Marking 20 4.2 Tense and Aspect 22 4.2.1 Past Tense Marking 22 4.2.2 Frequentative Reduplication 23 4.2.3 Bare Verbs 24 4.2.4 Summary 25 4.3 The Verb Phrase 25 4.3.1 Copular Constructions 25 4.3.2 Serial Verb Se 28 4.3.3 Summary 30 4.4 Focalization 30 5 Discussion 33 5.1 Issues 33 5.2 Evaluation of the Hypothesis 34 5.3 Contributions of Different Substrates 34 References 35 2 Abstract The traditional view of creole genesis holds that a creole begins as a pidgin, a makeshift language that forms when two or more groups without a native language come into contact. When the next generation of speakers then acquires the pidgin, it is transformed into a natural language known as a creole through the use of an innate faculty (see Bickerton 1981).
    [Show full text]
  • In 2018 Linguapax Review
    linguapax review6 62018 Languages, Worlds and Action Llengües, mons i acció Linguapax Review 2018 Languages, Worlds and Actions Llengües, mons i acció Editat per: Amb el suport de: Generalitat de Catalunya Departament de Cultura Generalitat de Catalunya Departament d’Acció Exterior Relacions Institucionals i Transparència Secretaria d’Acció Exterior i de la Unió Europea Coordinació editorial: Alícia Fuentes Calle Disseny i maquetació: Maria Cabrera Callís Traduccions: Marc Alba / Violeta Roca Font Aquesta obra està subjecta a una llicència de Reconeixement-NoComercial-CompartirIgual 4.0 Internacional de Creative Commons CONTENTS - CONTINGUTS Introduction. Languages, Worlds and action. Alícia Fuentes-Calle 5 Introducció. Llengües, mons i acció. Alícia Fuentes-Calle Túumben Maaya K’aay: De-stigmatising Maya Language in the 14 Yucatan Region Genner Llanes-Ortiz Túumben Maaya K’aay: desestigmatitzant la llengua maia a la regió del Yucatán. Genner Llanes-Ortiz Into the Heimat. Transcultural theatre. Sonia Antinori 37 En el Heimat. Teatre transcultural. Sonia Antinori Sustaining multimodal diversity: Narrative practices from the 64 Central Australian deserts. Jennifer Green La preservació de la diversitat multimodal: els costums narratius dels deserts d’Austràlia central. Jennifer Green A new era in the history of language invention. Jan van Steenbergen 101 Una nova era en la història de la invenció de llengües. Jan van Steenbergen Tribalingual, a startup for endangered languages. Inky Gibbens 183 Tribalingual, una start-up per a llengües amenaçades. Inky Gibbens The Web Alternative, Dimensions of Literacy, and Newer Prospects 200 for African Languages in Today’s World. Kọ́lá Túbọ̀sún L’alternativa web, els aspectes de l’alfabetització i les perspectives més recents de les llengües africanes en el món actual.
    [Show full text]
  • Cross-Linguistic Influences of Malay Through Cartoons on Indonesian Children’S Language Use in the Home Domain
    Indonesian Journal of Applied Linguistics, Vol. 7 No. 2, September 2017, pp. 339-348 CROSS-LINGUISTIC INFLUENCES OF MALAY THROUGH CARTOONS ON INDONESIAN CHILDREN’S LANGUAGE USE IN THE HOME DOMAIN Zulfadli A. Aziz Yunisrina Qismullah Yusuf Chairina Nasir Claudia Masyithah Syiah Kuala University, Banda Aceh, Indonesia [email protected] First received: 7 July 2017 Final proof received: 26 September 2017 Abstract The Malay cartoons that air on Indonesian television channels are now popular among children in Indonesia. The shows Upin & Ipin, Boboboi, and Pada Zaman Dahulu are especially popular with Acehnese children. Initial observations found that these cartoons have played a great role in the increased use of contemporary Malay in their Indonesian. Thus, this study discusses the cross- linguistic influences of Malay from cartoons in Indonesian children’s language use in the home domain. Data were collected through recordings and field notes. Some steps based on the framework by Lacey and Luff (2007) were taken to analyze the data. The results were transcribed and sifted to distinguish the significant data. The recording and its transcription were then re-checked. The last step was categorizing the data into cross-linguistic influences based on Ringbom (1987). These influences include borrowing, hybrids, phonetic influence, and relexification. The results showed the presence of only two aspects of cross-linguistic influences: borrowing and phonetic influences. Borrowing was the most frequent aspect, followed by phonetic influence. Relexification and hybrid did not appear in the data. Nevertheless, there were other cross-linguistic aspects that were found in the data, including intonation, parenthetic remark, and interjection.
    [Show full text]
  • Translation of Slang in Croatian and Russian Translations of Anthony Burgess's a Clockwork Orange
    Translation of slang in Croatian and Russian translations of Anthony Burgess's A Clockwork Orange Mikac, Tamara Master's thesis / Diplomski rad 2019 Degree Grantor / Ustanova koja je dodijelila akademski / stručni stupanj: University of Zagreb, University of Zagreb, Faculty of Humanities and Social Sciences / Sveučilište u Zagrebu, Filozofski fakultet Permanent link / Trajna poveznica: https://urn.nsk.hr/urn:nbn:hr:131:282823 Rights / Prava: In copyright Download date / Datum preuzimanja: 2021-09-29 Repository / Repozitorij: ODRAZ - open repository of the University of Zagreb Faculty of Humanities and Social Sciences University of Zagreb Faculty of Humanities and Social Sciences Department of English Department of East Slavic Languages and Literatures Translation of slang in Croatian and Russian translations of Anthony Burgess’ A Clockwork Orange Tamara Mikac Co-supervisor: Nataša Pavlović, PhD Co-supervisor: Branka Barčot, PhD Zagreb, September 2019 Table of Contents Biography.................................................................................................................................. 4 Abstract ..................................................................................................................................... 4 Аннотация ............................................................................................................................... 4 Key words ................................................................................................................................. 5 Ключевые
    [Show full text]
  • Black English in New York. PUB DATE Apr 71 NOTE 7P
    DOCUMENT RESUME ED 053 136 TE 002 527 AUTHOR Dillard, J.L. TITLE Black English in New York. PUB DATE Apr 71 NOTE 7p. JOURNAL CIT English Record; v21 n4 p114-120 Apr 1971 EDRS PRICE EDRS Price MF -$O.65 HC-$3.29 DESCRIPTORS African Languages, *English, *Language Development, *Negro Culture, Negro History, *Nonstandard Dialects, *Tenl ABSTRACT Black English has existed for a considerable length of time in the North as well as in the South. West African slaves who came to New York in 1625 found a contact language useful and mandatory in order to function in the slave community. The earliest slaves in the New York area may have used Pidgin English, Pidgin Portuguese, or Pidgin French along with their own dialects for a language of wider communication. There are many reasons why Pidgin English would have been the most useful of these closely related languages. Dutch Creoles were very prominent in this early period, and their patois bears a strong resemblance to Pidgin English. Except for a few writers, no one had looked at the language of Black people in Northern cities until the 1960's. It is hoped that a sensible attitude toward Black English, a language with a long and honorable historical background, will be incorporated into the resultant efforts.(CK) U.S. DEPARTMENT OF HEALTH. EDUCATION & WELFARE DFFICE OF EDUCATION English Record THIS DOCUMENT HAS SEEN REPRODUCED EXACTLY AS RECEIVED FROM THE PERSON OR Vol.21, No. 4 , ORGANIZATION ORIGINATING IT POINTS OF VIEW OR OPINIONS STATED DO NOT NECES April 1971 SARILY REPRESENT OFFICIAL OFFICE OF EDU CATION POSITION OR POLICY Section 5: The Historical Perspective O BLACK ENGLISH IN NEW YORK °LV .r, c-a J.
    [Show full text]
  • Roots of Language
    Roots of language Derek Bickerton language Classics in Linguistics 3 science press Classics in Linguistics Chief Editors: Martin Haspelmath, Stefan Müller In this series: 1. Lehmann, Christian. Thoughts on grammaticalization 2. Schütze, Carson T. The empirical base of linguistics: Grammaticality judgments and linguistic methodology 3. Bickerton, Derek. Roots of language ISSN: 2366-374X Roots of language Derek Bickerton language science press Derek Bickerton. 2016. Roots of language (Classics in Linguistics 3). Berlin: Language Science Press. This title can be downloaded at: http://langsci-press.org/catalog/book/91 © 2016, Derek Bickerton Published under the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 Licence (CC BY 4.0): http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ ISBN: 978-3-946234-08-1 (Digital) 978-3-946234-09-8 (Hardcover) 978-3-946234-10-4 (Softcover) 978-1-523647-15-6 (Softcover US) ISSN: 2366-374X DOI:10.17169/langsci.b91.109 Cover and concept of design: Ulrike Harbort Typesetting: Felix Kopecky, Sebastian Nordhoff Proofreading: Jonathan Brindle, Andreea Calude, Joseph P. DeVeaugh-Geiss, Joseph T. Farquharson, Stefan Hartmann, Marijana Janjic, Georgy Krasovitskiy, Pedro Tiago Martins, Stephanie Natolo, Conor Pyle, Alec Shaw Fonts: Linux Libertine, Arimo, DejaVu Sans Mono Typesetting software:Ǝ X LATEX Language Science Press Habelschwerdter Allee 45 14195 Berlin, Germany langsci-press.org Storage and cataloguing done by FU Berlin Language Science Press has no responsibility for the persistence or accuracy of URLs for external or third-party
    [Show full text]
  • 1 the Restructuring of Tense/Aspect Systems In
    THE RESTRUCTURING OF TENSE/ASPECT SYSTEMS IN CREOLE FORMATION. DONALD WINFORD, DEPARMENT OF LINGUISTICS, THE OHIO STATE UNIVERSITY, 1712 NEIL AVENUE, COLUMBUS OH 43210. Tel. (614) 292 0362. E-mail: [email protected] 1 ABSTRACT. Current controversy over the nature of creole formation revolves around the relative roles of L1 and target language (TL) input, the nature of the target involved, and the processes of language acquisition by which creole grammar emerged. This paper attempts to reconcile conflicting views on these issues by examining the developmental processes that characterize creole formation, with special attention to the emergence of tense/aspect systems in Haitian French Creole and Sranan Tongo. I describe the creation of creole grammar as involving a process of restructuring, that is, the process by which interlanguage (IL) grammars are created and elaborated in the course of acquisition. This process of restructuring involves three major components: input (intake) from the TL, L1 influence, and internally motivated innovations. These factors operate in varying degrees in different cases of creole formation, yielding differences in the outcomes. I argue that such differences result primarily from the nature of the superstrate input, and the degree of access that learners had to it, which depended in turn on such aspects of the contact situation as demographics, degree of social interaction among superstrate and substrate speakers. Greater degrees of access to superstrate models led to more input from the latter, while reduced access led to greater degrees of substrate influence. This is reflected in the differences that Haitian Creole and Sranan Tongo display, not only in their inventory of tense/aspect categories, but also in the forms that they use to express them.
    [Show full text]