Dissonant Views on Consonance

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Dissonant Views on Consonance Dissonant Views on Consonance The Cultural Dependency of Consonance and its Reinterpretation as Euphony June 2015 MA Thesis in Arts & Culture: Musicology First supervisor: Dr. Barbara Titus Second reader: Dr. Rutger Helmers Author: Tim Ruijgrok Student number: 10633014 Date: 18-06-2015 Contents Preface ∙ ∙ ∙ ∙ ∙ ∙ ∙ ∙ ∙ ∙ 2 Chapter 1 - Consonant views on consonance? ∙ ∙ ∙ ∙ ∙ 4 The cultural approach ∙ ∙ ∙ ∙ ∙ ∙ ∙ ∙ 5 Chapter 2 - Consonance in the Western discourse ∙ ∙ ∙ ∙ 11 Introduction to the case studies ∙ ∙ ∙ ∙ ∙ ∙ 17 Chapter 3 - European common-practice and tonal consonance ∙ ∙ ∙ 20 The contrapuntal concept ∙ ∙ ∙ ∙ ∙ ∙ ∙ 21 The triadic/tonal concept ∙ ∙ ∙ ∙ ∙ ∙ ∙ 22 The sensory concept ∙ ∙ ∙ ∙ ∙ ∙ ∙ ∙ 24 Chapter 4 - North-Indian classical music and the (absence of) harmony ∙ ∙ 29 Hindustani music ∙ ∙ ∙ ∙ ∙ ∙ ∙ ∙ 31 Vādī & samvādī ∙ ∙ ∙ ∙ ∙ ∙ ∙ ∙ 33 The tritone ∙ ∙ ∙ ∙ ∙ ∙ ∙ ∙ 37 Chapter 5 - Javanese gamelan and inharmonic sounds ∙ ∙ ∙ ∙ 40 Spectrum and timbre ∙ ∙ ∙ ∙ ∙ ∙ ∙ ∙ 42 Conclusion ∙ ∙ ∙ ∙ ∙ ∙ ∙ ∙ ∙ ∙ 45 Bibliography ∙ ∙ ∙ ∙ ∙ ∙ ∙ ∙ ∙ ∙ 47 1 Preface During the two years of enrolment at the University of Amsterdam, my view towards diverse expressions of music has broadened significantly. Attending the UvA with the intention to potentially increase my knowledge or interest in Asian music, I followed the "cultural trajectory" of Musicology. I was fortunate to study Russian popular music and North-Indian (classical) music, both of which – I can conclude – have enriched my knowledge. I could not come closer to my objective in terms of education, hence I attempted to steer my thesis in that direction… however, to no avail. Being unable to put my knowledge into practice in my main field of interest, i.e. Asian music, I had to make the ultimate sacrifice and leave this out of my scope. This thesis is the result of a long struggle between my own expectations of it and the actual possibilities I felt I had. However, in its present form, this thesis is heavily influenced by experiences during the classes on North-Indian music and the gamelan class I was invited to by my supervisor. The attempts to emulate the singing of the North-Indian vocal tradition, led by dr. Wim van der Meer, and the efforts to play the gamelan instruments under guidance of dr. Citra Aryandari made me realize – mentally and physically – how different music-making can be. A single impetus in the form of a blog post about a "study on consonance", then, was enough to make me wonder about a variety of implications for the particular music I had experienced up to now. Thanks Even though I managed to put all my amazement, enthusiasm and criticism towards the subject together in this research, I did not achieve this solely on my own. I want to express my sincere gratitude to all who helped me with the realization of this master's thesis. My expression of thanks goes to my supervisor Barbara Titus, for all the help and encouragements she has given me during our meetings – and for inviting me to the gamelan class! Also, I am especially grateful to my distant but close friend Adrienne Prudenciado for proofreading my thesis and offering countless corrections on my writings. Last but not least, I want to thank my parents and brother for all the emotional support they have given me over the past year. 2 Thesis outline This thesis is a literary study on the concept of consonance, and will focus particularly the alleged universal application of the concept. As I will show, consonance is a term with apparent semantic problems, and not many satisfying solutions or explanations have been formulated up to now. In spite of this, consonance is often treated as a monolithic concept, and its usage outside the context of Western tonal music is, consequently, often faulty or neglected. By approaching consonance as a form of "euphony", I hope to (re)address the issue of the cultural dependency of consonance, and show that consonance employs many different dimensions that cannot be viewed similarly between musical cultures. This will be exemplified with three case studies in which I will zoom in on the operation of euphony within specific musical cultures. The aforementioned ideas and problems will be dealt with in five chapters. Chapter 1 will introduce the reader with the term consonance, and the cultural approach that has been advocated decades ago. This cultural approach is taken up by introducing the more neutral term euphony, of which its benefits will be outlined, as well as applied in later chapters. Chapter 2 will provide the reader with an introduction on consonance within the Western discourse, including a brief literary review of important studies on the subject. It will concluded that most views regarding consonance are primarily functional and based on the European tonal music theory. This limitation will be the starting point of the three case studies, in which a particular musical culture will be examined to make clear how diverging notions of euphony actually are. Chapter 3 addresses euphony in the European common-practice, with the goal to trace this generic and limited view on consonance. This formulation will be identified as a conflation of 18th-century ideas of euphony. Chapter 4 will challenge the mutual relationship of consonance and harmony. Additionally, I will turn the discussion to North-Indian classical music to investigate how euphony is seen in a musical practice with a static harmonic background. In this case, the melodic handling of consonance, and the flexible nature of a rāga move away from the fixed and functional approach to euphony. Chapter 5 will offer a totally different view on euphony in Javanese gamelan music by emphasizing the relation between spectrum and scale. It will be highlighted how the scales employed in Java are logical and have a high euphonious value as a result of the spectra of the instruments used. This will all point to the conclusion that consonance is not universal and is dependent on the musical culture in which it operates, and that euphony proves to be a broader and more appropriate concept to address the issue outside the Western discourse. 3 Chapter 1 Consonant views on consonance? In daily conversations, consonance is often treated as a monolithic concept of which people tacitly assume others know what they are talking about.1 A simplification of the term is evident especially in musical education, where it is taken for granted that particular chords or intervals are consonant and others are not. While it seems that consonance is generally understood in the same way, the contrary is true. Consonance is like culture; a term that immediately confronts one with inevitable semantic problems. Shifting meanings and uses of consonance in the course of history, the result of different cultural and discursive environments, have troubled many scholars in defining the concept in a satisfactory way. So many thoughts, meanings and theories about consonance have been formulated, that a clear-cut definition has not yet been coined – which is, astonishingly, practically impossible. Therefore, before delving into deeper discussions of the subject, we first have to address some fundamental aspects regarding consonance. The most crucial aspect of consonance is its varied usage, which is determined by its meaning and the context in which it operates. Is it a quality of a tone? Is it purely a music-theoretical term? Is it part of a practice? A social construct? An experience or value judgment? In fact, all of these attributes can apply, which is the reason why consonance cannot be viewed as a monolithic concept. Some encyclopedic sources like Grove Music Online acknowledge this multiplicity of usages and distinguish an acoustical, psychological, musical and perceptual meaning. In Western tonal music, however, consonance is mostly treated as a music-theoretical term. Within this contracted view, different meanings can be derived by closer examination. Take the interval of a fourth, for example. Regarded as a consonant outside musical context, it may be seen as a dissonant in certain chord inversions within the contrapuntal practice. The same is true for the tritone, but indicating the exact opposite (Gauldin 2004: 17). In acoustical science, however, the intervals would be either consonant or dissonant depending on the frequency relations of their sound waves. Thus, in this case, one may observe a difference between consonance used as a term, as a musical practice, or as tonal quality, related to two discursive environments. One can extend this idea further than most theoreticians do. The ambiguous state of the term consonance makes it very likely that its change in meaning is not limited historically, but likewise culturally. This step is seldom addressed in academic writings about the subject of 1 When discussing consonance as a concept, I also mean its antonym dissonance, unless explicitly stated. 4 consonance. Surprisingly Robert Gauldin, in his Harmonic Practice in Tonal Music, hits the nail on the head regarding this issue. He says: There must be more to the question of consonance and dissonance than mere physics, however, or people of all cultures and historical periods would define the terms in the same way. (Gauldin 2004: 17) Even though it was just mentioned in passing, I would contend that Gauldin is right to say that neither historically nor culturally, consonance has ever had the same meaning. The first claim, that meanings of consonance changed historically, is acknowledged by many academic writers such as Norman Cazden (1945; 1980), William Sethares (2005), Richard Parncutt & Graham Hair (2011), to name a few. Additionally, it is the core assumption of James Tenney's book A History of "Consonance" and "Dissonance", which is one of the key works on the subject. The latter claim, that meanings of consonance differ culturally, is mostly not addressed. Fortunately, there are exceptions to the rule. The cultural approach In the Western discourse of consonance, there are at least two calls for a "cultural approach" towards consonance, both of which seemed to be voices calling in the wilderness as no one extended on these topics (see Lundin 1947; Cazden 1945).
Recommended publications
  • Chords and Scales 30/09/18 3:21 PM
    Chords and Scales 30/09/18 3:21 PM Chords Charts written by Mal Webb 2014-18 http://malwebb.com Name Symbol Alt. Symbol (best first) Notes Note numbers Scales (in order of fit). C major (triad) C Cmaj, CM (not good) C E G 1 3 5 Ion, Mix, Lyd, MajPent, MajBlu, DoHar, HarmMaj, RagPD, DomPent C 6 C6 C E G A 1 3 5 6 Ion, MajPent, MajBlu, Lyd, Mix C major 7 C∆ Cmaj7, CM7 (not good) C E G B 1 3 5 7 Ion, Lyd, DoHar, RagPD, MajPent C major 9 C∆9 Cmaj9 C E G B D 1 3 5 7 9 Ion, Lyd, MajPent C 7 (or dominant 7th) C7 CM7 (not good) C E G Bb 1 3 5 b7 Mix, LyDom, PhrDom, DomPent, RagCha, ComDim, MajPent, MajBlu, Blues C 9 C9 C E G Bb D 1 3 5 b7 9 Mix, LyDom, RagCha, DomPent, MajPent, MajBlu, Blues C 7 sharp 9 C7#9 C7+9, C7alt. C E G Bb D# 1 3 5 b7 #9 ComDim, Blues C 7 flat 9 C7b9 C7alt. C E G Bb Db 1 3 5 b7 b9 ComDim, PhrDom C 7 flat 5 C7b5 C E Gb Bb 1 3 b5 b7 Whole, LyDom, SupLoc, Blues C 7 sharp 11 C7#11 Bb+/C C E G Bb D F# 1 3 5 b7 9 #11 LyDom C 13 C 13 C9 add 13 C E G Bb D A 1 3 5 b7 9 13 Mix, LyDom, DomPent, MajBlu, Blues C minor (triad) Cm C-, Cmin C Eb G 1 b3 5 Dor, Aeo, Phr, HarmMin, MelMin, DoHarMin, MinPent, Ukdom, Blues, Pelog C minor 7 Cm7 Cmin7, C-7 C Eb G Bb 1 b3 5 b7 Dor, Aeo, Phr, MinPent, UkDom, Blues C minor major 7 Cm∆ Cm maj7, C- maj7 C Eb G B 1 b3 5 7 HarmMin, MelMin, DoHarMin C minor 6 Cm6 C-6 C Eb G A 1 b3 5 6 Dor, MelMin C minor 9 Cm9 C-9 C Eb G Bb D 1 b3 5 b7 9 Dor, Aeo, MinPent C diminished (triad) Cº Cdim C Eb Gb 1 b3 b5 Loc, Dim, ComDim, SupLoc C diminished 7 Cº7 Cdim7 C Eb Gb A(Bbb) 1 b3 b5 6(bb7) Dim C half diminished Cø
    [Show full text]
  • 3 Manual Microtonal Organ Ruben Sverre Gjertsen 2013
    3 Manual Microtonal Organ http://www.bek.no/~ruben/Research/Downloads/software.html Ruben Sverre Gjertsen 2013 An interface to existing software A motivation for creating this instrument has been an interest for gaining experience with a large range of intonation systems. This software instrument is built with Max 61, as an interface to the Fluidsynth object2. Fluidsynth offers possibilities for retuning soundfont banks (Sf2 format) to 12-tone or full-register tunings. Max 6 introduced the dictionary format, which has been useful for creating a tuning database in text format, as well as storing presets. This tuning database can naturally be expanded by users, if tunings are written in the syntax read by this instrument. The freely available Jeux organ soundfont3 has been used as a default soundfont, while any instrument in the sf2 format can be loaded. The organ interface The organ window 3 MIDI Keyboards This instrument contains 3 separate fluidsynth modules, named Manual 1-3. 3 keysliders can be played staccato by the mouse for testing, while the most musically sufficient option is performing from connected MIDI keyboards. Available inputs will be automatically recognized and can be selected from the menus. To keep some of the manuals silent, select the bottom alternative "to 2ManualMicroORGANircamSpat 1", which will not receive MIDI signal, unless another program (for instance Sibelius) is sending them. A separate menu can be used to select a foot trigger. The red toggle must be pressed for this to be active. This has been tested with Behringer FCB1010 triggers. Other devices could possibly require adjustments to the patch.
    [Show full text]
  • Helmholtz's Dissonance Curve
    Tuning and Timbre: A Perceptual Synthesis Bill Sethares IDEA: Exploit psychoacoustic studies on the perception of consonance and dissonance. The talk begins by showing how to build a device that can measure the “sensory” consonance and/or dissonance of a sound in its musical context. Such a “dissonance meter” has implications in music theory, in synthesizer design, in the con- struction of musical scales and tunings, and in the design of musical instruments. ...the legacy of Helmholtz continues... 1 Some Observations. Why do we tune our instruments the way we do? Some tunings are easier to play in than others. Some timbres work well in certain scales, but not in others. What makes a sound easy in 19-tet but hard in 10-tet? “The timbre of an instrument strongly affects what tuning and scale sound best on that instrument.” – W. Carlos 2 What are Tuning and Timbre? 196 384 589 amplitude 787 magnitude sample: 0 10000 20000 30000 0 1000 2000 3000 4000 time: 0 0.23 0.45 0.68 frequency in Hz Tuning = pitch of the fundamental (in this case 196 Hz) Timbre involves (a) pattern of overtones (Helmholtz) (b) temporal features 3 Some intervals “harmonious” and others “discordant.” Why? X X X X 1.06:1 2:1 X X X X 1.89:1 3:2 X X X X 1.414:1 4:3 4 Theory #1:(Pythagoras ) Humans naturally like the sound of intervals de- fined by small integer ratios. small ratios imply short period of repetition short = simple = sweet Theory #2:(Helmholtz ) Partials of a sound that are close in frequency cause beats that are perceived as “roughness” or dissonance.
    [Show full text]
  • Playing Music in Just Intonation: a Dynamically Adaptive Tuning Scheme
    Karolin Stange,∗ Christoph Wick,† Playing Music in Just and Haye Hinrichsen∗∗ ∗Hochschule fur¨ Musik, Intonation: A Dynamically Hofstallstraße 6-8, 97070 Wurzburg,¨ Germany Adaptive Tuning Scheme †Fakultat¨ fur¨ Mathematik und Informatik ∗∗Fakultat¨ fur¨ Physik und Astronomie †∗∗Universitat¨ Wurzburg¨ Am Hubland, Campus Sud,¨ 97074 Wurzburg,¨ Germany Web: www.just-intonation.org [email protected] [email protected] [email protected] Abstract: We investigate a dynamically adaptive tuning scheme for microtonal tuning of musical instruments, allowing the performer to play music in just intonation in any key. Unlike other methods, which are based on a procedural analysis of the chordal structure, our tuning scheme continually solves a system of linear equations, rather than relying on sequences of conditional if-then clauses. In complex situations, where not all intervals of a chord can be tuned according to the frequency ratios of just intonation, the method automatically yields a tempered compromise. We outline the implementation of the algorithm in an open-source software project that we have provided to demonstrate the feasibility of the tuning method. The first attempts to mathematically characterize is particularly pronounced if m and n are small. musical intervals date back to Pythagoras, who Examples include the perfect octave (m:n = 2:1), noted that the consonance of two tones played on the perfect fifth (3:2), and the perfect fourth (4:3). a monochord can be related to simple fractions Larger values of mand n tend to correspond to more of the corresponding string lengths (for a general dissonant intervals. If a normally consonant interval introduction see, e.g., Geller 1997; White and White is sufficiently detuned from just intonation (i.e., the 2014).
    [Show full text]
  • Course Name : Indonesian Cultural Arts – Karawitan (Seni Budaya
    Course Name : Indonesian Cultural Arts – Karawitan (Seni Budaya Indonesia – Karawitan) Course Code / Credits : BDU 2303/ 3 SKS Teaching Period : January-June Semester Language Instruction : Indonesian Department : Sastra Nusantara Faculty : Faculty of Arts and Humanities (FIB) Course Description The course of Indonesian Cultural Arts (Karawitan) is a compulsory course for (regular) students of Faculty of Cultural Sciences Universitas Gadjah Mada, especially for the first and second semesters. The course is held every semester and is offered and can be taken by every student from semester 1 to 2. There are no prerequisites for Karawitan courses. The position of Indonesian Culture Arts (Karawitan) as the compulsory course serves to introduce the students to one aspect of Indonesian (or Javanese) art and culture and the practical knowledge related to the performance of traditional Javanese musical instruments, namely gamelan. This course also aims to provide both introduction and theoretical and practical understanding for the students of the Faculty of Cultural Science on gamelan instrument techniques, namely gendhing technique, that is found in Karawitan. Topics in this course include identification of Javanese gamelan instruments, exploration of tones in Javanese gamelan, gendhing instrument method and practice, as well as observation of traditional art performances. Proportionally, 30% of these courses contains briefing theoretical insights, 40% contains gamelan practice, and 30% contains provision of experience in a form of group collaboration and interaction Course Objectives The course of Indonesian Culture Arts (Karawitan) in general aims to provide theoretical and practical supplies through skill, application, and carefulness to recognize various instruments of Gamelan. Through this course, students are observant in identifying the various instruments of the gamelan and its application as instrumental and vocal art in karawitan.
    [Show full text]
  • Tuning: at the Grcssroads
    WendyGarloo ?O. Box1024 Tuning:At the New YorkCit, New York 10276USA Grcssroads lntrodrciion planned the construction of instruments that per- formed within the new "tunitrg of choice," and all The arena o{ musical scales and tuning has cer_ publishedpapers or books demonstretingthe supe- tainly not been a quiet place to be for the past thlee dority of their new scales in at least some way over hundred yeals. But it might iust as well have beenif €qual temperament.The tradition has continued we iudge by the results: the same 12V2 equally with Yunik and Swi{t {1980),Blackwood (1982),and temperedscale established then as the best avail- the presentauthor (Milano 1986),and shows no able tuning compromise, by J. S. Bach and many sign of slowing down despite the apparent apathy otheis lHelrnholtz 1954j Apel 1972),remains to with which the musical mainstream has regularly this day essentially the only scale heard in Westem grceted eech new proposal. music. That monopoly crossesall musical styles, of course therc's a perf€ctly reasonable explana- {rom the most contemporary of jazz and av^rf,t' tion lor the mainstream's evident preferetrce to rc- "rut-bound" gardeclassical, and musical masteeieces from the main when by now there are at least past, to the latest technopop rock with fancy s)'n- a dozen clearly better-sounding ways to tune our thesizers,and everwvherein between.Instruments scales,i{ only for at least part of the time: it re' ol the symphonyorchestra a((empr with varyirrg quires a lot of effort ol several kinds. I'm typing this deSreesof successto live up ro lhe 100-centsemi manuscript using a Dvorak keyboard (lor the ffrst tone, even though many would find it inherently far time!), and I assureyou it's not easyto unlearn the easierto do otherwise: the stdngs to "lapse" into QWERTY habits of a lifetime, even though I can Pythagoieen tuning, the brass into several keys of akeady feel the actual superiodty of this unloved lust irtonation lBarbour 1953).And th€se easily but demonstrablv better kevboard.
    [Show full text]
  • Tuning Continua and Keyboard Layouts
    October 4, 2007 8:34 Journal of Mathematics and Music TuningContinua-RevisedVersion Journal of Mathematics and Music Vol. 00, No. 00, March 2007, 1–15 Tuning Continua and Keyboard Layouts ANDREW MILNE∗ , WILLIAM SETHARES , and JAMES PLAMONDON † ‡ § Department of Music, P.O. Box 35, FIN-40014, University of Jyvaskyl¨ a,¨ Finland. Email: [email protected] † 1415 Engineering Drive, Dept Electrical and Computer Engineering, University of Wisconsin, Madison, WI 53706 USA. ‡ Phone: 608-262-5669. Email: [email protected] CEO, Thumtronics Inc., 6911 Thistle Hill Way, Austin, TX 78754 USA. Phone: 512-363-7094. Email: § [email protected] (received April 2007) Previous work has demonstrated the existence of keyboard layouts capable of maintaining consistent fingerings across a parameterised family of tunings. This paper describes the general principles underlying layouts that are invariant in both transposition and tuning. Straightforward computational methods for determining appropriate bases for a regular temperament are given in terms of a row-reduced matrix for the temperament-mapping. A concrete description of the range over which consistent fingering can be maintained is described by the valid tuning range. Measures of the resulting keyboard layouts allowdirect comparison of the ease with which various chordal and scalic patterns can be fingered as a function of the keyboard geometry. A number of concrete examples illustrate the generality of the methods and their applicability to a wide variety of commas and temperaments, tuning continua, and keyboard layouts. Keywords: Temperament; Comma; Layout; Button-Lattice; Swathe; Isotone 2000 Mathematics Subject Classification: 15A03; 15A04 1 Introduction Some alternative keyboard designs have the property that any given interval is fingered the same in all keys.
    [Show full text]
  • Invariance of Controller Fingerings Across a Continuum of Tunings
    Invariance of Controller Fingerings across a Continuum of Tunings Andrew Milne∗∗, William Setharesyy, and James Plamondonzz May 26, 2007 Introduction On the standard piano-style keyboard, intervals and chords have different shapes in each key. For example, the geometric pattern of the major third C–E is different from the geometric pattern of the major third D–F]. Similarly, the major scale is fingered differently in each of the twelve keys (in this usage, fingerings are specified without regard to which digits of the hand press which keys). Other playing surfaces, such as the keyboards of [Bosanquet, 1877] and [Wicki, 1896], have the property that each interval, chord, and scale type have the same geometric shape in every key. Such keyboards are said to be transpositionally invariant [Keisler, 1988]. There are many possible ways to tune musical intervals and scales, and the introduction of computer and software synthesizers makes it possible to realize any sound in any tuning [Carlos, 1987]. Typically, however, keyboard controllers are designed to play in a single tuning, such as the familiar 12-tone equal temperament (12-TET) which divides the octave into twelve perceptually equal pieces. Is it possible to create a keyboard surface that is capable of supporting many possible tunings? Is it possible to do so in a way that analogous musical intervals are fingered the same throughout the various tunings, so that (for example) the 12-TET fifth is fingered the same as the Just fifth and as the 17-TET fifth? (Just tunings ∗∗Corresponding author. Email: [email protected] yyDepartment of Electrical and Computer Engineering, University of Wisconsin-Madison, Madison, WI 53706 USA.
    [Show full text]
  • Invariant Fingering Over a Tuning Continuum
    Open Research Online The Open University’s repository of research publications and other research outputs Isomorphic controllers and Dynamic Tuning: invariant fingering over a tuning continuum Journal Item How to cite: Milne, Andrew; Sethares, William and Plamondon, James (2007). Isomorphic controllers and Dynamic Tuning: invariant fingering over a tuning continuum. Computer Music Journal, 31(4) pp. 15–32. For guidance on citations see FAQs. c 2007 Massachusetts Institute of Technology Version: Version of Record Link(s) to article on publisher’s website: http://dx.doi.org/doi:10.1162/comj.2007.31.4.15 http://www.mitpressjournals.org/loi/comj Copyright and Moral Rights for the articles on this site are retained by the individual authors and/or other copyright owners. For more information on Open Research Online’s data policy on reuse of materials please consult the policies page. oro.open.ac.uk Andrew Milne,* William Sethares,** and James Isomorphic Controllers and Plamondon† *Department of Music Dynamic Tuning: Invariant University of Jyväskylä Finland Fingering over a Tuning [email protected] **Department of Electrical and Computer Engineering Continuum University of Wisconsin-Madison Madison, WI 53706 USA [email protected] †Thumtronics Inc. 6911 Thistle Hill Way Austin, TX 78754 USA [email protected] In the Western musical tradition, two pitches are all within the time-honored framework of tonality. generally considered the “same” if they have nearly Such novel musical effects are discussed briefly in equal fundamental frequencies. Likewise, two the section on dynamic tuning, but the bulk of this pitches are in the “same” pitch class if the frequency article deals with the mathematical and perceptual of one is a power-of-two multiple of the other.
    [Show full text]
  • Beyond the Spectrum of Music: an Exploration
    BEYOND THE SPECTRUM OF MUSIC: AN EXPLORATION THROUGH SPECTRAL ANALYSIS OF SOUND COLOR IN THE ALBAN BERG VIOLIN CONCERTO by Diego Bañuelos A written project submitted in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Musical Arts (Music) at the UNIVERSITY OF WISCONSIN-MADISON 2005 © Copyright by Diego Banuelos 2005 All Rights Reserved To Cathy Ann Elias ii CONTENTS LIST OF ILLUSTRATIONS . iii SOUND EXAMPLES . vi PART I: INTRODUCTION . 1 PART II: ANALYTICAL PROCEDURES. 14 Spectrum and Spectrogram . 14 Noise and Peaks . 20 Plotting Single Attributes of Sound . 27 Dimensions in Sound Color and the Purpose of Spectral Analysis. 27 Sensory Roughness (SR) . 30 Registral and Timbral Brightness (RB and TB) . 39 Peak Variance (PV) . 42 Noise to Signal Ratio (NSR) . 44 Summary . 47 PART III: SPECTRUM-BASED ANALYSIS OF ALBAN BERG’S VIOLIN CONCERTO (SECOND MOVEMENT) . 48 Micro-Structure . 53 Macro-Structure: Movement II in Continuity . 69 Conclusion . 80 APPENDIX A: SR, PV, NSR, RB, and TB graphs of the Berg Violin Concerto (movement II) . 84 APPENDIX B: Spectrograms of the Berg Violin Concerto (movement II) . 89 WORKS CITED . 110 iii ILLUSTRATIONS Figure Page 2.1 Spectrogram of the first six measures of Alban Berg’s Violin Concerto, movement II . 14 2.2 Spectrum of a violin open A-string analyzed over a period of 0.19 seconds 17 2.3 Stages in the construction of the spectrogram . 19 2.4 Spectrum and noise floor of a violin A-string . 22 2.5 Two spectral representations of a violin A-string. 26 2.6 Succinct representation of a Plomp and Levelt curve.
    [Show full text]
  • Kraig Grady [Personal Data Omitted] [email protected]
    Kraig Grady [personal data omitted] [email protected] EDUCATION MCA-R Visual arts. Faculty of Law, Humanities and the Arts, University of Wollongong, 2014. John Cage mentions in one of his books of a student who went from school to school, finding the best teachers, taking their classes and then going on to other schools. This had a great impact on my young idealist mind and led me through a nine-year excursion through the following institutions: Los Angeles City College, California State Northridge, University of California Los Angeles, and finally Immaculate Heart College. Privately, I studied briefly with composers Nicolas Slonimsky, Dorrance Stalvey and more extensively with Byong-Kon Kim. This was in conjunction with years spent in libraries (downtown and UCLA). My studies with tuning theorist Erv Wilson were the longest, investigating microtonality, both in historical and cultural contexts, as well as the potential of unique scale designs and structures. EMPLOYMENT Casual lecturer in performance, Faculty of Creative Arts, University of Wollongong, 2008-2011. Subjects taught: THEA390, SCMP321, SCMP322. Scenic artist for various television stations, scenic companies and movie studios in Hollywood, including ABC, PBS, KTTV, KHJ, KTLA, Professional Scenery, etc, 1974-2005. Journeyman working on films such as Francis Ford Coppola’s One From The Heart, Brian De Palma’s Body Double, as well as various PBS productions, 1982-2005. ARTISTIC ACTIVITIES Composer/Performer/Instrument-Builder/Sound Artist All my music involves various microtonal tunings, 1975-present. Shadow Theatre Director Directed, produced, wrote, and scored eight shadow plays. I also made and operated 28 puppets that run the gamut from traditional sources to original designs.
    [Show full text]
  • Module 4/Melody,Harmony,Texture
    Module 4- Melody, Harmony and Texture Pitch in music refers to vibrations of sound waves. These vibrations are measured in hertz (cycles per second). Therefore a musical pitch is a sound produced at a certain number of cycles per second (Wade, 2013). The faster the vibration, the higher the resulting pitch. Likewise, the slower the rate of vibration, the lower the pitch. Musical tones can be divided into two cateGories: determinate and indeterminate pitch. Musical pitches contain a mixture of sound waves. The one that dominates the sound is referred to as the “fundamental” pitch. All of the other waves that are produced by a pitch are referred to (in the music world) as overtones. When a pitch has a set of overtones that allow a fundamental note to dominate the sound it is a determinate pitch. Indeterminate pitch happens when the overtones of a note are not in aliGnment or there are conflictinG fundamentals and therefore no “one” vibration dominates the sound. Another way to think about this concept is to know that instruments that have determinate pitch play notes that are typically Given names (letter, number or solfeGe). Determinate pitch instruments include (but are not limited to): voice, piano, Guitar, marimba, woodwinds, brass, chordophones, etc.… Indeterminate pitch instruments are instruments like Gamelan GonGs, snare drums, cymbals, trianGle, etc… These instruments are Generally used to keep a rhythm, to accent, or to add color. When a pitch has a set of overtones that vibrate alonG with the fundamental in simple ratios (see Figure 2) then it makes a harmonic pitch.
    [Show full text]