Vulnerability to the Misinformation Effect As a Function of Handedness Consistency

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Vulnerability to the Misinformation Effect As a Function of Handedness Consistency Vulnerability to the Misinformation Effect as a Function of Handedness Consistency A Master’s Thesis Stephanie Monroe Marietta College A Thesis Submitted to the Faculty of Marietta College Psychology Department in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree of Master of Arts in Psychology May 2019 Running head: HANDEDNESS AND FALSE MEMORY 1 Vulnerability to the Misinformation Effect as a Function of Handedness Consistency A Master’s Thesis Stephanie Monroe Marietta College A Thesis Submitted to the Faculty of Marietta College Psychology Department in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree of Master of Arts in Psychology May 2019 HANDEDNESS AND FALSE MEMORY 2 Vulnerability to the Misinformation Effect as a Function of Handedness Consistency A Master’s Thesis Stephanie Monroe Marietta College This thesis has been approved for the Master of Arts in Psychology Program by the faculty in the Department of Psychology at Marietta College. HANDEDNESS AND FALSE MEMORY 3 Abstract The current study examined the production of false memory in relation to consistency of handedness. Past research has demonstrated that there are individual differences between left- and right-handed people regarding the accuracy of their memory. Subsequent research has suggested that the differences are likely due to interhemispheric interaction, which is more likely from consistency of handedness (inconsistently handed or consistently handed). The current study examined individual differences in vulnerability to the misinformation effect in relation to consistency of handedness. It was predicted that participants with inconsistent handedness would be less vulnerable to the misinformation effect than participants with consistent handedness. The results of the current study however, suggest that there are no significant differences between consistently and inconsistently handed participants regarding false memory utilizing the misinformation paradigm. Explanations for the results are further described in the discussion section. HANDEDNESS AND FALSE MEMORY 4 Vulnerability to the Misinformation Effect as a Function of Handedness Consistency People often believe that their memories are always, or almost always, accurate. However, a wide array of research has shown otherwise. Memory is malleable in the sense that it can be altered by information received prior to the event (proactive interference), or by information received after the event (retroactive interference) (Loftus & Pickrell, 1995). Inaccurate or completely fabricated memories that are believed to be both accurate and true are referred to as false memories. As time passes, our memories slowly diminish, and our brains automatically fill in the gaps (Loftus & Pickrell, 1995). New information is easily added to memories, altering our memories and making them inaccurate (Roediger & McDermott, 1995). Additionally, memory reconstruction occurs when the brain automatically fills in fuzzy areas of a memory, making the memory seem and feel accurate, but is in fact fabricated in some way (Roediger & McDermott, 1995). Many researchers have explored the underpinnings of false memories and have applied this phenomenon to a multitude of real-world situations including therapy, interrogation methods, and eyewitness testimony (Loftus & Zanni, 1975). Researchers have also been interested in whether there are specific characteristics or individual differences that affect peoples’ performance on memory tests. For example, research has demonstrated that left-handed people tend to exhibit better memory when taking memory tests than right-handed people (Cuzzocreo, Yassa, Verduzco, Honeycutt, Scott, & Bassett, 2009). One characteristic that has not been heavily researched is consistency of handedness in relation to memory. Specifically, consistency of handedness goes further than just identifying as left- or right-handed, but how consistently a person uses the same hand across several tasks. The current study examines consistency of handedness (inconsistently and consistently handed) and vulnerability to false HANDEDNESS AND FALSE MEMORY 5 memory. Specifically, consistency of handedness may be a factor of falling victim to the creation of false memories. False Memory Loftus has conducted seminal work in false memory research. Loftus was interested in how information given after witnessing an event can alter memories as well as prompt the creation of entirely new memories. Research has shown that after witnessing an event, any information given afterward creates a post-information effect that can alter memories of that event, leading to false recall (Loftus, Miller, & Burns, 1978). Participants were shown, via slide show, a car accident. After being exposed to a series of slides, the participants were given a set of questions to answer about the event they had just witnessed. Some participants were given misleading information while other participants were given information consistent with what they had just seen (Loftus et al, 1978). For example, in the slideshow, participants saw a red car stop at a stop sign. Some participants had the question, “what color was the car that stopped at the stop sign?” while other participants had the question, “what color was the car that waited at the yield sign?” The second group had been presented with the misinformation of the type of sign the car stopped at (Loftus et al, 1978). Participants were most likely to fall victim to the misinformation effect and report false details about the event when misleading information was presented after viewing the event (Loftus et al, 1978). Another experiment explored how the amount of time between the presentation of the stimuli and the recall test affects the accuracy of recall. Overall, the researchers concluded that there are no major differences between immediately, 20 minutes, and one day after, but any timeframe after one day decreases accuracy on the recall test (Loftus et al, 1978). HANDEDNESS AND FALSE MEMORY 6 Loftus was interested in furthering these findings by inducing complete memories that never happened to the individual, and so conducted the “Lost in a Shopping Mall” study. With the help of family members, Loftus and Pickrell (1995) created believable (but false) memories and had family members “remind” participants of the events (three true events and one fabricated event). The false memory always involved getting lost in a mall or department store. During interviews, participants were asked to recall each of the four memories (the three true memories and one fabricated memory) in as much detail as possible. At the end, they were asked how clear each memory was and how confident they were of the accuracy of each memory (Loftus & Pickrell, 1995). Overall, by the second interview, confidence levels and clarity for the false memories were relatively high. At the end of the study, the participants were then informed that one of the four memories they were asked to recall was in fact fabricated and they were asked to choose which event was not real. Participants often chose a true event as being false, meaning that they believed the false memory of getting lost in a store was true (Loftus & Pickrell, 1995). Overall, participants remembered the true events 68% of the time and falsely remembered the fabricated event 29% of the time. This research shows that suggestion and planting information can lead to the formation of memories that never actually happened. When asked how confident each participant was in how accurate their recollection of each event was, participants were relatively confident on all of their memories, including the memory that was unknowingly artificial. This shows that confidence levels are not significantly different between true memories and memories where the misinformation effect has been conducted (Loftus & Pickrell, 1995; Mahé, Corson, Verrier, & Payoux, 2015; Roediger & McDermott, 1995). This is important because it demonstrates that confidence does not always mean accuracy. HANDEDNESS AND FALSE MEMORY 7 Roediger and McDermott (1995) have conducted a great deal of research on false memory as well. Roediger and McDermott (1995) developed a recall task that involved being exposed to lists of words by modifying a model originally created by Deese (1959). Each list surrounded one key word that was intentionally left out, called the “critical lure word” (Roediger & McDermott, 1995). For example, a list would include, “bed, pillow, night, alarm clock, pajamas…” and the critical lure word would be “sleep.” Although the word “sleep” was never presented, participants often falsely remembered seeing or hearing the word, thus creating a false memory. This task is called the Deese-Roediger-McDermott Paradigm, or the DRM Paradigm (Roediger & McDermott, 1995). Roediger and McDermott’s 1995 research consisted of two experiments. In experiment one, the researchers utilized the DRM task. Participants listened to six lists, all surrounded by one missing critical lure word. Then after a brief break, participants were asked to write down any words they were fairly certain they had heard. Experiment two was similar, except it involved 16 lists. After the first eight lists, participants took a recall test immediately after. Then after the second set of lists, participants completed math problems for five minutes, followed by a recall test for all 16 lists (Roediger & McDermott, 1995). The participants also completed a Remember-Know task during the overall recall test (Tulving, 1985). The purpose of
Recommended publications
  • Memory Specificity, but Not Perceptual Load, Affects Susceptibility to Misleading Information
    Memory specificity, but not perceptual load, affects susceptibility to misleading information Francesca R. Farina1,2,* & Ciara M. Greene1 1School of Psychology, University College Dublin, Ireland. 2Trinity College Institute of Neuroscience, Trinity College Dublin, Ireland. Note: This paper has not yet been peer-reviewed. Please contact the authors before citing. Correspondence may be sent to Francesca Farina, Trinity College Institute of Neuroscience, Trinity College Dublin, Ireland. Email: [email protected] 1 Abstract The purpose of this study was to examine the role of perceptual load in eyewitness memory accuracy and susceptibility to misinformation at immediate and delayed recall. Despite its relevance to real-world situations, previous research in this area is limited. A secondary aim was to establish whether trait-based memory specificity can protect against susceptibility to misinformation. Participants (n=264) viewed a 1-minute video depicting a crime and completed a memory questionnaire immediately afterwards and one week later. Memory specificity was measured via an online version of the Autobiographical Memory Test (AMT). We found a strong misinformation effect, but no effect of perceptual load on memory accuracy or suggestibility at either timepoint. Memory specificity was a significant predictor of accuracy for both neutrally phrased and leading questions, though the effect was weaker after a one-week delay. Results suggest that specific autobiographical memory, but not perceptual load, enhances eyewitness memory and protects against misinformation. Keywords Perceptual load; memory specificity; eyewitness; misinformation. 2 General Audience Summary The misinformation effect is a memory impairment for a past event that occurs when a person is presented with leading information. Leading information can distort the original details of a memory and produce false memories.
    [Show full text]
  • Can Implicit Post-Event Information Influence Explicit Eyewitness Memory?
    City University of New York (CUNY) CUNY Academic Works Student Theses John Jay College of Criminal Justice Spring 6-2017 Can Implicit Post-Event Information Influence Explicit yE ewitness Memory? Hang Sze Chau CUNY John Jay College, [email protected] How does access to this work benefit ou?y Let us know! More information about this work at: https://academicworks.cuny.edu/jj_etds/15 Discover additional works at: https://academicworks.cuny.edu This work is made publicly available by the City University of New York (CUNY). Contact: [email protected] Running Head: UNCONSCIOUS PROCESSING OF POST-EVENT INFORMATION 1 Can Implicit Post-Event Information Influence Explicit Eyewitness Memory? Hang-Sze Chau John Jay College of Criminal Justice City University of New York UNCONSCIOUS PROCESSING OF POST-EVENT INFORMATION 2 Table of Contents Introduction 4 Misinformation Effect 5 Source Monitoring 7 Implicit Learning 9 Methods 13 Results 19 Discussion 23 Reference 30 Appendix 37 UNCONSCIOUS PROCESSING OF POST-EVENT INFORMATION 3 Abstract This study examines whether unconscious
    [Show full text]
  • Impairing Existing Declarative Memory in Humans by Disrupting Reconsolidation
    Impairing existing declarative memory in humans by disrupting reconsolidation Jason C. K. Chana,b,1 and Jessica A. LaPagliaa aDepartment of Psychology and bNeuroscience Interdisciplinary Program, Iowa State University, Ames, IA 50014 Edited by Robert Desimone, Massachusetts Institute of Technology, Cambridge, MA, and approved April 23, 2013 (received for review October 23, 2012) During the past decade, a large body of research has shown that through a memory test, the results almost unequivocally show memory traces can become labile upon retrieval and must be re- that retrieval actually makes the original memory less, not more, stabilized. Critically, interrupting this reconsolidation process can susceptible to interference (17–20). In sum, scant evidence exists abolish a previously stable memory. Although a large number of to support the idea that declarative memory undergoes recon- studies have demonstrated this reconsolidation associated amne- solidation upon retrieval. sia in nonhuman animals, the evidence for its occurrence in hu- Distinct neural systems subserve the formation and retrieval of mans is far less compelling, especially with regard to declarative fear, motor, and declarative memory (21). Whereas memories memory. In fact, reactivating a declarative memory often makes it acquired via fear conditioning or motor sequence learning can be more robust and less susceptible to subsequent disruptions. Here relatively localized neurologically (22), the encoding and retrieval we show that existing declarative memories can be selectively of declarative memories rely on a more distributed network (23– impaired by using a noninvasive retrieval–relearning technique. In 27). This complexity might be one reason why declarative mem- six experiments, we show that this reconsolidation-associated am- ory is particularly resistant to treatments designed to disrupt re- nesia can be achieved 48 h after formation of the original memory, consolidation.
    [Show full text]
  • Visual Imagery on False Memories
    Running head: VISUAL IMAGERY ON FALSE MEMORIES ___________________________________________________________________________ This is an Author’s Original Manuscript. This article has been accepted for publication (19 Feb 2021) in Memory published by Taylor & Francis. The final version of the article is available online: https://www.tandfonline.com/doi/full/10.1080/09658211.2021.1895221. 50 free online copies of the final version of the article are available at https://www.tandfonline.com/eprint/GZSWHQ5IAKF7SCZCTYMJ/full?target=10.108 ____________________________________________________________________0/09658211.2021.1895221 _______ Effect of visual imagery on false memories in DRM and Misinformation paradigms Frédérique Robin1 Emmanuelle Ménétrier2 Brice Beffara Bret1 1 Laboratoire de Psychologie des Pays de la Loire (LPPL-EA4638), Université de Nantes, Nantes 44000, France 2 Laboratoire de Psychologie des Pays de la Loire (LPPL-EA4638), Université d’Angers, Angers 49045, France Correspondence concerning this manuscript should be adressed to: Frédérique ROBIN, Faculté de Psychologie, LPPL – Laboratoire de Psychologie des Pays de la Loire, Université de Nantes, Chemin de la Censive du Tertre, BP 81227, 44312 Nantes Cedex 3, France. E- Mail: [email protected] 1 Abstract This study is an extension of recent research, which examined the possibility that false memories in the Deese-Roediger-McDermott (DRM) paradigm predict the occurrence of false memories in misinformation paradigm. The purpose was to determine in which extent an imaging instruction reduces false memories in DRM and Misinformation paradigms. A sample of young adults was assigned to the DRM or the misinformation tasks, either in control conditions or in conditions including an imaging instruction. Findings confirm that an imaging instruction decreases false memories in DRM whereas it is not possible to conclude about such a reduction in the misinformation task.
    [Show full text]
  • False Memories in Highly Superior Autobiographical Memory Individuals
    False memories in highly superior autobiographical memory individuals Lawrence Patihisa,1, Steven J. Frendaa, Aurora K. R. LePortb,c, Nicole Petersenb,c, Rebecca M. Nicholsa, Craig E. L. Starkb,c, James L. McGaughb,c, and Elizabeth F. Loftusa aDepartment of Psychology and Social Behavior, bDepartment of Neurobiology and Behavior, and cCenter for the Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, University of California, Irvine, CA 92697 Edited by George Sperling, University of California, Irvine, CA, and approved October 22, 2013 (received for review July 29, 2013) The recent identification of highly superior autobiographical memory and the cellist Jacqueline du Pré died that day.” HSAM indi- (HSAM) raised the possibility that there may be individuals who viduals can remember what happened on a day a decade ago are immune to memory distortions. We measured HSAM partic- better than most people can remember a day a month ago. In ipants’ and age- and sex-matched controls’ susceptibility to false some ways, these abilities seem to be at odds with what we know memories using several research paradigms. HSAM participants about the reconstructive, unreliable, and malleable processes and controls were both susceptible to false recognition of non- underlying memory in people with typical memory. presented critical lure words in an associative word-list task. In a HSAM abilities are distinct from previously described superior- misinformation task, HSAM participants showed higher overall memory individuals (22–25) who typically rely upon practiced false memory compared with that of controls for details in a pho- mnemonics to remember unusually long lists of domain-specific tographic slideshow. HSAM participants were equally as likely as data, yet remain average in their ability to retrieve autobio- controls to mistakenly report they had seen nonexistent footage graphical information.
    [Show full text]
  • Reducing Misinformation Effects in Older Adults with Cognitive Interview Mnemonics
    Psychology and Aging © 2011 American Psychological Association 2012, Vol. 27, No. 4, 1191–1203 0882-7974/11/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0022031 Reducing Misinformation Effects in Older Adults With Cognitive Interview Mnemonics Robyn E. Holliday and Joyce E. Humphries Rebecca Milne University of Leicester University of Portsmouth Amina Memon Lucy Houlder, Amy Lyons, and Ray Bull Royal Holloway, University of London University of Leicester We examined the effect of a prior Modified Cognitive Interview on young and older adults’ recall of a short film of a staged crime and subsequent reporting of misinformation. Participants viewed the film followed the next day by misinformation presented in a postevent summary. They were then interviewed with either a Modified Cognitive Interview or a control interview followed by a recognition memory test. A Modified Cognitive Interview elicited more correct details and improved overall accuracy compared to a control interview in both age groups, although the young adults recollected three times more correct information in a Modified Cognitive Interview than the older adults. In both age groups, correct recollections of person and action details were higher in a Modified Cognitive Interview than a control interview. Importantly, older adults who were interviewed with a Modified Cognitive Interview were not susceptible to misinformation effects. Keywords: aging, Cognitive Interview, misinformation effect, eyewitness, suggestibility Obtaining reliable eyewitness testimony from older witnesses tend to perform worse than young adults when recalling statements has become a key concern to policy makers and professionals in (Nyberg, Baekman, Erngrund, Olofsson, & Nilsson, 1996), prose recent years. Clearly, there is a need for a set of protocols that is (Carlesimo et al., 1998), and long word lists (Cohen, Sandler, & specifically designed to maximize accurate testimony from older Schroeder, 1987).
    [Show full text]
  • A Study Investigating the Factors Affecting the Misinformation Effect with a Preface on the Connections Between Memory and Identity
    Regis University ePublications at Regis University All Regis University Theses Spring 2016 Emotion, Memory, and Identity: A Study Investigating the Factors Affecting the Misinformation Effect with a Preface on the Connections Between Memory and Identity Jamie L. Dinneen Regis University Follow this and additional works at: https://epublications.regis.edu/theses Recommended Citation Dinneen, Jamie L., "Emotion, Memory, and Identity: A Study Investigating the Factors Affecting the Misinformation Effect with a Preface on the Connections Between Memory and Identity" (2016). All Regis University Theses. 705. https://epublications.regis.edu/theses/705 This Thesis - Open Access is brought to you for free and open access by ePublications at Regis University. It has been accepted for inclusion in All Regis University Theses by an authorized administrator of ePublications at Regis University. For more information, please contact [email protected]. Regis University Regis College Honors Theses Disclaimer Use of the materials available in the Regis University Thesis Collection (“Collection”) is limited and restricted to those users who agree to comply with the following terms of use. Regis University reserves the right to deny access to the Collection to any person who violates these terms of use or who seeks to or does alter, avoid or supersede the functional conditions, restrictions and limitations of the Collection. The site may be used only for lawful purposes. The user is solely responsible for knowing and adhering to any and all applicable laws, rules, and regulations relating or pertaining to use of the Collection. All content in this Collection is owned by and subject to the exclusive control of Regis University and the authors of the materials.
    [Show full text]
  • Working Memory and the Continued Influence Effect 1
    Running head: WORKING MEMORY AND THE CONTINUED INFLUENCE EFFECT 1 Working memory capacity predicts ongoing reliance on misinformation: A latent-variable analysis Christopher R. Brydges, Gilles E. Gignac, & Ullrich K. H. Ecker School of Psychological Science, the University of Western Australia ***IN PRESS AT INTELLIGENCE*** Corresponding author: Christopher R. Brydges School of Psychological Science Mailbag M304 University of Western Australia 35 Stirling Highway Crawley WA 6009 Phone: +61 (8) 6488 1404 Email: [email protected] Word count: 3,426 (main text); 210 (Abstract) WORKING MEMORY AND THE CONTINUED INFLUENCE EFFECT 2 Abstract Misinformation often affects inferences and judgments even after it has been retracted and discredited. This is known as the continued influence effect. Memory processes have been theorized to contribute to the continued influence effect, and much previous research has focussed on the role of long-term memory processes at the time misinformation is retrieved during inferential reasoning and judgments. Recently, however, experimental research has focussed upon the role of working memory (WM) processes engaged in the updating and integration of information, when the retraction is encoded. From an individual differences perspective, susceptibility to continued influence effects should be predicted by a person’s WM abilities, if continued reliance on misinformation is influenced, at least in part, by insufficient integration of the initial misinformation and its subsequent retraction. Consequently, we hypothesized that WM capacity would predict susceptibility to continued influence effects uniquely and more substantially than short-term memory (STM) capacity. Participants (N = 216) completed a continued-influence task, as well as a battery of WM and STM capacity tasks.
    [Show full text]
  • Aging and the Seven Sins of Memory Benton H
    Advances in Cell Aging and Gerontology Aging and the seven sins of memory Benton H. Pierce1,*, Jon S. Simons2 and Daniel L. Schacter1,* 1Department of Psychology, Harvard University, William James Hall, 33 Kirkland St., Cambridge, MA 02138 2Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience, University College London, Alexandra House, 17 Queen Square, London WC1N 3AR, UK Contents 1. Sins of omission 2 1.1. Transience 2 1.2. Absent-mindedness 4 1.3. Blocking 8 2. Sins of commission 12 2.1. Misattribution 12 2.2. Suggestibility 18 2.3. Bias 21 2.4. Persistence 24 2.5. Conclusions 26 Acknowledgements 29 References 29 Memory serves many different functions in everyday life, but none is more important than providing a link between the present and the past that allows us to re-visit previously experienced events, people, and places. This link becomes increasingly significant as we age: recollections of past experiences serve as the basis for a process of life review that assumes great importance to many older adults (Coleman, 1986; Schacter, 1996). Not surprising, then, a common worry among older adults is that forgetfulness, which may seem to be increasingly pervasive as years go by, will eventually result in the loss of the precious store of memories that has been built up over a lifetime. While much evidence suggests that various aspects of memory do decline with increasing age, it is clear that performance on some memory tasks remains relatively preserved (Naveh-Benjamin et al., 2001). *Corresponding author. Tel.: 617-495-3855; fax: 617-496-3122. E-mail address: [email protected] (D.L.
    [Show full text]
  • Unit 7 Reading Guide
    UNIT 7: COGNITION 7A: MEMORY KEY QUESTION: What is Memory? CORE CONCEPT: Human memory is an information–processing system that works constructively to encode, stores, and retrieves information Memory: Vague Memory: MEMORY’S THREE BASIC TASKS AKA: Information Processing Model of Memory 1. Encoding: Elaboration: 2. Storage: 3. Retrieval: Eidetic Memory: KEY QUESTION: How do we form memories? CORE CONCEPT: Each of the three stages of memory encodes and stores memories in different ways. But they also work together to transform sensory experience into a lasting record that has a pattern or meaning THREE STAGES OF MEMORY Based upon the Atkinson and Shiffrin Model STAGE ONE: Sensory Memory Capacity: Duration: Visual Stimulation = Iconic Memory Auditory Stimulation = Echoic Memory Tactile (touch) Stimulation = Tactile Sensory Memory Olfactory Stimulation = Olfactory Sensory Memory Gustatory Stimuli = Gustatory Sensory Memory 1 STAGE TWO: Working Memory AKA: Short term memory Capacity: Duration: Magic Number Seven Three Parts of Working Memory Central Executive: directs attention to material retrieved from LTM or to important input from the sensory memory Phonological Loop: Temporarily stores sounds….like someone’s name Sketchpad: Stores and manipulates mental images…like when you can imagine driving a car to school from home Working Memory Aides to Overcome Limited Capacity and Short Duration Chunking: Rehearsal a. Maintenance Rehearsal: b. Elaborate Rehearsal: Acoustic Encoding: The Phonological Loop Visual and Spatial Encoding...the sketchpad Levels of Processing Theory: STAGE THREE: Long-Term Memory Capacity: Duration: Procedural Memory: Declarative Memory: 2 Episodic Memory: Semantic Memory: (SEE CHART p.272) Engram or Memory Trace: Consolidation: Antergrade Amnesia: Retrograde Amnesia: Flashbulb Memories: PARTS OF THE BRAIN ASSOCIATED WITH LONG TERM MEMORY Amygdala: strengthens memories that have strong emotional associations….
    [Show full text]
  • Teaching and Experiencing the Misinformation Effect: a Classroom Exercise John Eric Swenson III & Gregory R
    Teaching and experiencing the misinformation effect: A classroom exercise John Eric Swenson III & Gregory R. Schneller Students from four sections of Introduction to Psychology (N=82) were taught that participating in a classroom exercise may make memories vulnerable to the misinformation effect. All students were shown a short video clip of a car wreck. Students were then asked either ‘leading’ or ‘non-leading’ questions about the video clip. Students were also asked to rate how reliable they believed their memories to be. Responses to the questions were statistically analysed. This classroom exercise was found to be a simple and useful way of allowing students to experience firsthand the way in which memories may be distorted. ESEARCH consistently shows that our remembered as having been a part of the memories are not exact representa- actual incident even though it was not R tions of previous events (e.g. Loftus & present (Dodson & Reisberg, 1991). Other Ketcham, 1991; Kassin, Rigby & Castillo, research examining the misinformation 1991). In this regard, after witnessing an effect indicates that hearing new informa- event, there are numerous factors that can tion about an event can make it more of a cause memories to be inaccurate when a challenge for people when they are trying to person tries to recall an event. For example, retrieve the original memory (Tversksy & when a police officer asks an eyewitness to Tuchin, 1989). There are also occasions recall a crime, if the police officer’s ques- when new information gets integrated in to tions contain new information that was not an old memory, and the new information actually a part of the actual crime, this new and the old memory become so intertwined information can alter the eyewitness’ that they become virtually impossible to memory of the crime (Belli & Loftus, 1996).
    [Show full text]
  • How the Authority of Misinformation Influences Personal Judgments of Others Elizabeth Crosswhite University of North Georgia, [email protected]
    University of North Georgia Nighthawks Open Institutional Repository Honors Theses Honors Program Spring 2018 How the Authority of Misinformation Influences Personal Judgments of Others Elizabeth Crosswhite University of North Georgia, [email protected] Follow this and additional works at: https://digitalcommons.northgeorgia.edu/honors_theses Part of the Psychology Commons Recommended Citation Crosswhite, Elizabeth, "How the Authority of Misinformation Influences Personal Judgments of Others" (2018). Honors Theses. 39. https://digitalcommons.northgeorgia.edu/honors_theses/39 This Honors Thesis is brought to you for free and open access by the Honors Program at Nighthawks Open Institutional Repository. It has been accepted for inclusion in Honors Theses by an authorized administrator of Nighthawks Open Institutional Repository. RUNNING HEAD: AUTHORITY IN MISINFORMATION 1 How the Authority of Misinformation Influences Personal Judgments of Others Elizabeth Crosswhite A Thesis Submitted to the Faculty of the University of North Georgia in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree Bachelor of Science in Psychology with Honors Spring 2018 University of North Georgia MISINFORMATION IN AUTHORITY 2 INTRODUCTION The study of misinformation blossomed with the work of Elizabeth Loftus in the 1970s, looking, for example, at how the presence of information in leading questions overshadowed the existing memory of a car crash. Loftus and Palmer (1974) first used the term misinformation effect and found a connection between language and episodic memory. At first, this term only referred to when participants were asked to recall an event when prompted by a biased question, and their memory of the misinformation was measured. Quickly there grew two types of misinformation effect, one in which the misleading information is presented before the event occurs as ‘priming’ the individual ((Lewandowsky, Ecker, Seifert, Schwarz & Cook, 2012), and one in which the biasing information is presented afterwards, as in the original study.
    [Show full text]