Activity, Habitat Use and the First Evidence of Lingual Lure Behavior in a South American Snake

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Activity, Habitat Use and the First Evidence of Lingual Lure Behavior in a South American Snake SALAMANDRA 56(1): 39–47 Activity, habitat use and lingual lure behavior in a South American snakeSALAMANDRA 15 February 2020 ISSN 0036–3375 German Journal of Herpetology A fisherman’s tale: Activity, habitat use and the first evidence of lingual lure behavior in a South American snake Conrado Mario-da-Rosa1, Arthur Diesel Abegg2,3, Leandro Malta-Borges1, Alexandre Ferreira Righi4, Paulo Sérgio Bernarde5, Sonia Zanini Cechin1 & Tiago Gomes dos Santos1,6 1) Programa de Pós-Graduação em Biodiversidade Animal, Universidade Federal de Santa Maria, Av. Roraima, 1000, Santa Maria, Rio Grande do Sul, Brazil 2) Laboratório Especial de Coleções Zoológicas, Instituto Butantan, Av. Vital Brasil, 1500, São Paulo, São Paulo, Brazil 3) Programa de Pós-Graduação em Zoologia, Instituto de Biociências, Universidade de São Paulo, Travessa 14, Rua do Matão, 321, 05508-090, São Paulo, São Paulo, Brazil 4) Museu de História Natural e Jardim Botânico da Universidade Federal de Minas Gerais, R. Gustavo da Silveira, 1035, Belo Horizonte, Minas Gerais, Brazil 5) Laboratório de Herpetologia, Centro Multidisciplinar, Campus Floresta, Universidade Federal do Acre, Cruzeiro do Sul, Acre, Brazil 6) Universidade Federal do Pampa, Campus São Gabriel, Av. Antônio Trilha, 1847, São Gabriel, Rio Grande do Sul, Brazil Corresponding author: Conrado Mario-da-Rosa, e-mail: [email protected] Manuscript received: 23 August 2019 Accepted: 25 November 2019 by Arne Schulze Abstract. Literature data suggests that Thamnodynastes strigatus (Serpentes, Dipsadidae) is a snake that actively forages for anurans near waterbodies, using several microhabitats for this activity (e.g., shrubs, soil, and water). However, herein we present dissonant data previously known to the species, both concerning type of prey and foraging strategy. A total of 72 observations were performed exclusively at night, when snakes were in vegetation near streams in 93% of the cases. Among these observations, 41 were active snakes, and most of them (97%) were in an ambush position on the vegetation, peering at fishes. On two occasions, the snakes used a lingual lure behavior in order to attract fishes. This is only the sixth species in which this behavior has been observed, and the first in South America. Therefore, we provide additional data on T. strigatus habitat activity and habitat use, as well as unpublished data on ambush and lingual lure behavior for the Neo- tropical genus Thamnodynastes. Key words. Thamnodynastes strigatus, nocturnal activity, ambush behavior, sub-arboreal, Uruguayan savannah. Introduction evolutionarily plastic and can be shaped by prey abundance and behavior (Vitt & Caldwell 2014). Some snakes, in Data on the feeding ecology of any species aids in the un- association to the ambush-hunting method, use attractive derstanding of its natural history, ecological requirements exhibitionist displays to maximize prey capture success and the diverse approaches involved in the search for food (Sazima 1991, Welsh & Lind 2000, Hansknecht 2008), and habitat use (Al-Sadoon & Parray 2016). In snakes, the also termed mimetic-aggressive behaviors (Glaudas & most commonly observed criteria for habitat selection and Alexander 2017). However, although visible growth in use are the selection of better hibernation sites (Reinert recent decades is noted (Bernarde et al. 2000b, Hart- 1993), safer refuges for predator avoidance (Martins 1993, mann & Marques 2005), studies on snake feeding ecology Webb & Whiting 2005), or prey distribution and availabil- are still scarce in the Neotropical region (Andrade et al. ity (Madsen & Shine 1996). Notably, the foraging environ- 2010, Braz & Marques, 2016), when compared to temper- ment is strongly related to the availability and abundance ate northern hemisphere regions and the Australian conti- of potential prey (Bernarde et al. 2000a). Concerning ba- nent (Shine 1988, Welsh & Lind 2000, Gibbons & Dor- sic hunting strategies, snakes may use the ambush meth- cas 2004, Hansknecht 2008). od (Glaudas & Alexander 2017), the active search (Ber- The genus Thamnodynastes Wagler, 1830 comprises 20 narde et al. 2000a) or even both, in different situations species distributed throughout South America (Franco et (Martins & Oliveira 1998). These foraging strategies are al. 2017). Thamnodynastes strigatus (Günther, 1858) is a © 2020 Deutsche Gesellschaft für Herpetologie und Terrarienkunde e.V. (DGHT), Mannheim, Germany Open access at http://www.salamandra-journal.com 39 Conrado Mario-da-Rosa et al. medium-sized snake that reaches about 800 mm in length etation; and height (cm) of the specimens perched on the (Giraudo 2001) and occurs in Argentina, Paraguay, Uru- substrate in relation to the water or soil surface, measured guay, and southern and southeastern portions of Brazil with a millimeter measuring tape); (2) activity pattern (ac- (Borges-Martins et al. 2007). Its diet consists of fishes, tive = specimens hanging on branches, perpendicular to lizards, mammals and, predominantly, anuran amphibians the water surface (ambush position) or in motion; inac- (Bernarde et al. 2000b, Ruffato et al. 2003). The only in tive = specimens that remained static on a substrate, with situ feeding ecology study concerning T. strigatus report- no signs of activity for at least 15 minutes) (adapted from ed some anuran predation events and concluded that the Di-Bernardo et al. 2007); (3) air temperature (°C), deter- species is an active forager that exploits different types of mined using a digital thermo-hygrometer. Monthly aver- substrates at night (ground, vegetation, water body surface age maximum and minimum temperatures were obtained and interior) and uses undergrowth vegetation for daytime from the Instituto Nacional de Meteorologia (INMET) rest (Bernarde et al. 2000a). However, we found a T. stri­ (http://www.inmet.gov.br/portal/) for the Alegrete mete- gatus population in the Uruguayan Savannah ecoregion orological station, 78 km distant from the study area. These (sensu Olson et al. 2001), southern Brazil, which differs averages were used to test the relationship between the remarkably concerning previously known hunting strate- abundance of snake encounters and temperature extremes gies and food items. Therefore, the aim of the present study (minimum and maximum). is to describe this population’s hunting strategies, habitat In order to describe T. strigatus ambush-hunting behav- use and activity. ior, we opted for ad libitum observations (Altmann 1974), of active individuals and better observation conditions at each sampling (i.e. allowing for a nearby observation site Material and methods with the least possible visual obstruction). Video record- Study site ings were made in order to document and characterize the duration of the displayed behaviors, using a Nikon Coolpix The fieldwork was performed in the Serra do Caverá re- p510 camera. Additionally, the snout–vent length (SVL, to gion, in the east portion of the geomorphological unit the nearest mm) and sex of the individuals (determined named Cuesta de Haedo (Pilau 2011). This area is locat- with eversion of hemipenis) observed in predation events ed between the municipalities of Rosário do Sul and San- were also recorded. tana do Livramento, in southwestern state of Rio Grande do Sul (Brazil), in the Uruguayan Savannah ecoregion (30°21’41.16” S 55°15’53.15” W). The region is composed of Statistical analyses a natural mosaic that includes steep elevations with flat or round ridges and flooded lowlands, both with the predom- Student’s t-test was applied to assess possible differenc- inance of grasslands, besides arboreal-shrub vegetation re- es in the mean values of the behaviors exhibited by the stricted to stream banks and some hill slopes (Robaina et snakes. Linear regressions were used to evaluate the re- al. 2015). The climatic seasonality of the region is marked, lationship between snake abundance and air tempera- with the four well defined seasons. The thermal ampli- ture (mean monthly minimum and maximum). An alpha tude is accentuated, ranging from -3°C during the winter < 0.05 was established for all statistical tests. All statisti- to 39°C during the hottest months of the year (Wrege et cal analyses were carried out with the R software (R Core al. 2012). Although the rainfall in the region is high and Team 2015). constant throughout the year (1400–1700 mm; Rossato 2011), a drop-in surface water availability (rainfall-eva- potranspiration) is noted in the summer months (Wrege Results et al. 2012). Activity and habitat use A total of 72 T. strigatus specimen observations were made Sampling and data collection throughout the samplings, distributed during the hottest months of the year (November to April). A positive and sig- Monthly samplings were carried out during one year, from nificant relationship between the number of observations August 2017 to July 2018 (except in June, due to logistical and the average minimum and maximum temperature was problems), with each field trip lasting 5–6 days. Search- observed (R²aj = 0.45; p <0.05; R²aj = 0.45; p <0.05, respec- es for specimens were performed using the visual search tively). A significant difference (t = 2.49, p <0.05) regarding method (McDiarmid et al. 2012). After sighted, the snakes temperature and the respective snake activity pattern was were monitored at a minimum distance of 1.5 m, in order noted, since active individuals were found in higher tem- to minimize possible disturbances due to the presence of peratures (x = 22.93±0.43; n = 45) compared
Recommended publications
  • Evidence of Lingual-Luring by an Aquatic Snake
    Journal of Herpetology, Vol. 34 No. 1 pp 67-74, 2000 Copyright 2000 Society for the Study of Amphibians and Reptiles Evidence of Lingual-luring by an Aquatic Snake HARTWELL H. WELSH, JR. AND AMY J. LIND Pacific Southwest Research Station, USDA Forest Service, 1700 Bayview Dr., Arcata, California 95521, USA. E-mail: hwelsh/[email protected] ABSTRACT.-We describe and quantify the components of an unusual snake behavior used to attract fish prey: lingual-luring. Our earlier research on the foraging behavior of the Pacific Coast aquatic garter snake (Thamnophis atratus) indicated that adults are active foragers, feeding primarily on aquatic Pacific giant salamanders (Dicamptodon tenebrosus) in streambed substrates. Juvenile snakes, however, use primarily ambush tactics to capture larval anurans and juvenile salmonids along stream margins, behaviors that include the lingual-luring described here. We found that lingual-luring differed from typical chemosensory tongue-flicking by the position of the snake, contact of the tongue with the water surface, and the length of time the tongue was extended. Luring snakes are in ambush position and extend and hold their tongues out rigid, with the tongue-tips quivering on the water surface, apparently mimicking insects in order to draw young fish within striking range. This behavior is a novel adaptation of the tongue-vomeronasal system by a visually-oriented predator. The luring of prey by snakes has been asso- luring function (Mushinsky, 1987; Ford and ciated primarily with the use of the tail, a be- Burghardt, 1993). However, Lillywhite and Hen- havior termed caudal luring (e.g., Neill, 1960; derson (1993) noted the occurrence of a pro- Greene and Campbell, 1972; Heatwole and Dav- longed extension of the tongue observed in vine ison, 1976; Jackson and Martin, 1980; Schuett et snakes (e.g., Kennedy, 1965; Henderson and al., 1984; Chizar et al., 1990).
    [Show full text]
  • Caudal Distraction by Rat Snakes (Colubridae, Elaphe): a Novel Behavior Used When Capturing Mammalian Prey
    Great Basin Naturalist Volume 59 Number 4 Article 8 10-15-1999 Caudal distraction by rat snakes (Colubridae, Elaphe): a novel behavior used when capturing mammalian prey Stephen J. Mullin University of Memphis, Memphis, Tennessee Follow this and additional works at: https://scholarsarchive.byu.edu/gbn Recommended Citation Mullin, Stephen J. (1999) "Caudal distraction by rat snakes (Colubridae, Elaphe): a novel behavior used when capturing mammalian prey," Great Basin Naturalist: Vol. 59 : No. 4 , Article 8. Available at: https://scholarsarchive.byu.edu/gbn/vol59/iss4/8 This Article is brought to you for free and open access by the Western North American Naturalist Publications at BYU ScholarsArchive. It has been accepted for inclusion in Great Basin Naturalist by an authorized editor of BYU ScholarsArchive. For more information, please contact [email protected], [email protected]. Great Ba....in Naturalist 59(4), ©1999, pp. 361....167 CAUDAL DISTRACTION BY RAT SNAKES (COLUBHIDAE, ELAPHE): A NOVEL BEHAVIOR USED WHEN CAPTURING MAMMALIAN PREY Stephen]. Mullin1 AJ3S11UCT.--el.mthtl movement in snakes trulY serve ei.ther a pl'Cdatory (e.g., caudal luring) or defensive (e.g., rattling, aposem,ttism) fUllction, I descliho n new behavioral pattern of tai.l movement in snakes. Gray rat snakl.'$ (Elaphe OhSO!etd spiloid.es) fi)raging on ~ma11 mmnmnls (Mus d01ne~·ticus) Inoved. their tails in un erratic, whiplike fashion uIter detecting prey in their vidnity. The thrashing movement in the horizontal plfme was audibly and visually obviolls, resulting in dis­ placement of leaf litter around the hlil. All subjects displayed the behavior, hilt not in all foraging episodes.
    [Show full text]
  • Spring 2009 RURAL LIVING in ARIZONA Volume 3, Number 2
    ARIZONA COOPERATIVE E TENSION THE UNIVERSITY OF ARIZONA COLLEGE OF AGRICULTURE AND LIFE SCIENCES Backyards& Beyond Spring 2009 RURAL LIVING IN ARIZONA Volume 3, Number 2 Spring 2009 1 Common Name: Globemallow Scientific Name: Sphaeralcea spp. Globemallow is a common native wildflower found throughout most of Arizona. There are 16 species (and several varieties) occurring in the state, the majority of which are perennials. They are found between 1,000 and 6,000 feet in elevation and grow on a variety of soil types. Depending on the species, globemallows are either herbaceous or slightly woody at the base of the plant and grow between 2-3 feet in height (annual species may only grow to 6 inches). The leaves are three-lobed, and while the shape varies by species, they are similar enough to help identify the plant as a globemallow. The leaves have star-shaped hairs that give the foliage a gray-green color. Flower color Plant Susan Pater varies from apricot (the most common) to red, pink, lavender, pale yellow and white. Many of the globemallows flower in spring and again in summer. Another common name for globemallow is sore-eye poppy (mal de ojos in Spanish), from claims that the plant irritates the eyes. In southern California globemallows are known as plantas muy malas, translated to mean very bad plants. Ironically, the Pima Indian name for globemallow means a cure for sore eyes. The Hopi Indians used the plant for healing certain ailments and the stems as a type of chewing gum, and call the plant kopona.
    [Show full text]
  • 2007 Australasian Society for the Study of Animal Behaviour & the International Union for the Study of Social Insects (Australian Chapter)
    ASSAB 2007 AUSTRALASIAN SOCIETY FOR THE STUDY OF ANIMAL BEHAVIOUR & THE INTERNATIONAL UNION FOR THE STUDY OF SOCIAL INSECTS (AUSTRALIAN CHAPTER) 12-15 April 2007 The Australian National University Canberra Venue Robertson Lecture Theatre Research School of Biological Sciences Building 46E Hosted by the Research School of Biological Sciences 2 Sponsored by the ARC Centre of Excellence in Vision Science (ACEVS) http://www.vision.edu.au/ LOCAL HOSTS: JOCHEN ZEIL Visual Sciences, Research School of Biological Sciences The Australian National University AJAY NARENDRA Visual Sciences, Research School of Biological Sciences The Australian National University ROB HEINSOHN Centre for Resource and Environmental Studies The Australian National University JAN HEMMI Visual Sciences, Research School of Biological Sciences The Australian National University RICHARD PETERS Visual Sciences, Research School of Biological Sciences The Australian National University WITH SPECIAL THANKS TO THE ASSAB TREASURER, XIMENA NELSON & THE ASSAB PRESIDENT PHIL TAYLOR FOR THEIR SUPPORT Thursday, 12 April Friday, 13 April Saturday, 14 April Sunday, 15 April 8:00 Plenary Lecture Tinbergen Centenary Lecture IUSSI Lecture 9:00 ASSAB 2007 Barbara Webb Chris Evans & Jochen Zeil Ryszard Maleszka 12 - 15 April 2007 09:30 09:30 09:30 Session 3: RSBS Session 8: Session 12: 10:00 SENSORY SYSTEMS & SOCIAL INSECTS I COMMUNICATION I HOMING & NAVIGATION Tea/CoffeeTea/Coffee Break Break Tea/Coffee Break 11:00 Session 9: FORAGING, COMPETITION & Session 4: LIFE HISTORIES I Session 13:
    [Show full text]
  • Abstract Book JMIH 2011
    Abstract Book JMIH 2011 Abstracts for the 2011 Joint Meeting of Ichthyologists & Herpetologists AES – American Elasmobranch Society ASIH - American Society of Ichthyologists & Herpetologists HL – Herpetologists’ League NIA – Neotropical Ichthyological Association SSAR – Society for the Study of Amphibians & Reptiles Minneapolis, Minnesota 6-11 July 2011 Edited by Martha L. Crump & Maureen A. Donnelly 0165 Fish Biogeography & Phylogeography, Symphony III, Saturday 9 July 2011 Amanda Ackiss1, Shinta Pardede2, Eric Crandall3, Paul Barber4, Kent Carpenter1 1Old Dominion University, Norfolk, VA, USA, 2Wildlife Conservation Society, Jakarta, Java, Indonesia, 3Fisheries Ecology Division; Southwest Fisheries Science Center, Santa Cruz, CA, USA, 4University of California, Los Angeles, CA, USA Corroborated Phylogeographic Breaks Across the Coral Triangle: Population Structure in the Redbelly Fusilier, Caesio cuning The redbelly yellowtail fusilier, Caesio cuning, has a tropical Indo-West Pacific range that straddles the Coral Triangle, a region of dynamic geological history and the highest marine biodiversity on the planet. Caesio cuning is a reef-associated artisanal fishery, making it an ideal species for assessing regional patterns of gene flow for evidence of speciation mechanisms as well as for regional management purposes. We evaluated the genetic population structure of Caesio cuning using a 382bp segment of the mitochondrial control region amplified from over 620 fish sampled from 33 localities across the Philippines and Indonesia. Phylogeographic
    [Show full text]
  • Nelson Et Al No Highlight-1 Edit Changes
    1 Receiver psychology and the design of the deceptive caudal luring signal of the death 2 adder 3 Ximena J. Nelsona,b* Daniel T. Garnettc, 1 Christopher S. Evansb, 2 4 5 aSchool of Biological Sciences, University of Canterbury 6 bCentre for the Integrative Study of Animal Behaviour, Macquarie University 7 cDepartment of Biological Sciences, Macquarie University 8 Received 6 July 2009 9 Initial acceptance 4 September 2009 10 Final acceptance 30 November 2009 11 MS. number: 09-00453R 12 13 *Correspondence: X. J. Nelson, School of Biological Sciences, University of Canterbury, 14 Private Bag 4800, Christchurch, New Zealand. 15 E-mail address: [email protected] (X. J. Nelson). 16 1D. T. Garnett is at the Department of Biological Sciences, Macquarie University, Sydney, 17 NSW 2109 Australia. 18 2C. S. Evans is at the Centre for the Integrative Study of Animal Behaviour, Macquarie 19 University, Sydney, NSW 2109 Australia. 20 1 21 Signal design can reflect the sensory properties of receivers. The death adder, Acanthophis 22 antarcticus, attracts prey by wriggling the distal portion of its tail (caudal luring). To 23 understand the design of this deceptive signal, we explored perceptual processes in a 24 representative prey species: the Jacky dragon, Amphibolurus muricatus. We used 3D 25 animations of fast and slow death adder luring movements against different backgrounds, to 26 test the hypothesis that caudal luring mimics salient aspects of invertebrate prey. Moving 27 stimuli elicited predatory responses, especially against a conspicuous background. To identify 28 putative models for caudal luring, we used an optic flow algorithm to extract velocity values 29 from video sequences of 61 moving invertebrates caught in lizard territories, and compared 30 these to the velocity values of death adder movements.
    [Show full text]
  • Hypersonic Hyperstitions
    HYPERSONIC HYPERSTITIONS Šum #11 HYPERSONIC HYPERSTITIONS Šum #11 HYPERSONIC HYPERSTITIONS Šum #11 Šum #11 HYPERSONIC HYPERSTITIONS There used to be supersonics and superstitions. Then hyper came along. Marko Peljhan invited Šum journal to produce a special issue that would accompany his exhibition Here we go again… SYSTEM 317 in the Slovenian Pavilion at the Venice Biennale. The invitation included, among a lot of referential vectors, this short description: “The exhibition will feature a full-scale model of a hypersonic aircraft system and will appropriate, combine and hack actual US, Russian and Chinese blueprints. The cockpit will be done in the style of a West Coast lowrider … how to light a match and keep it lit during a hurricane.” Šum in turn approached theory-fiction and hard sci-fi, that is, mobilised their agents, to provide overlaps with Peljhan’s exhibition and his work in general. War. Evolution. Tech. Escape/Exit. The Arctic. Conversion/Convergence. Transpiercement. Surrationality. Shadow-subsuming. Intelligent Battlespace. UGO. Godspeed. 10.851735 1447 1448 10.851735 1451 SURRATIONAL FUGITIVES LUCIANA PARISI 1467 ETERNAL PHENOTYPE MIROSLAV GRIŠKO 1499 AN EXEGETICAL INCURSION OF THE EMERGENT “INTELLIGENT BATTLESPACE” MANABRATA GUHA 1531 CAPITAL, WAR AND LOVE PRIMOŽ KRAŠOVEC 1563 THE WISDOM OF CROWDS PETER WATTS 1595 DIRECTORATE OF CELESTIAL SURVEILLANCE, 1665 ANDREJ TOMAŽIN 1627 UNIDENTIFIED GLIDING OBJECT: THE DAY THE EARTH WAS UNMOORED REZA NEGARESTANI Appendix: 1659 YAMAL EVENTS REPORT 2071 EDMUND BERGER, KEVIN ROGAN 10.851735 1449 1450 10.851735 Surrational Fugitives LUCIANA PARISI 10.851735 1451 SURRATIONAL FUGITIVES 1452 10.851735 In an attempt to contain the human exodus from planet earth, today at 09:09 am, the Planetary Security Council has ordered a shutdown of celestial borders: gateways of hypersonic vessels that pierce the terrestrial atmosphere and fly 9 million light years away from planetary existence.
    [Show full text]
  • The Mystery of the Rattlesnake 1
    THE MYSTERY OF THE RATTLESNAKE 1 The Mystery of the RattleSnake Geri Glinsek Special Program: Project Dragonfly Miami University of Ohio THE MYSTERY OF THE RATTLESNAKE 2 Communication is used in all living things. Animals, plants, bacteria, and even an organism's cells use communication, but each of them express it differently. In animal communication there is a signaler (sender) and a receiver (Rosenthal & Ryan, 2000). The sender's signal was shaped into an evolved trait because of the effect it had on the receiver's behavior. Most animals use visual or auditory signals in communication. The auditory signals are produced by sound from an animal's organ. The visual signals are considered traits which are continuously expressed despite the signaler's determined state or environmental condition. The signaler and receiver interaction happens between species (interspecies communication), but it is more common within a species (intraspecies communication) (Roger & Kaplan, 2002). Pursuit-deterrent communication is an example of interspecies communication (Clark, 2005). This type is between predators and prey. The prey are the signalers and the predators are the receivers. For example, many pursuit-deterrent communication happens between rodents and rattlesnakes. Most rodents will signal by mobbing and by using visual cues to warn other prey that snakes have been spotted. This form of communication mutually benefits both predators and prey because predators will avoid a difficult costly pursuit, and prey increase their survival rate by living another day. In only studying animals there are many different ways communication can happen. A prime example of pursuit-deterrent communication is a rattlesnake's rattle.
    [Show full text]
  • Growth and Age at Reproductive Maturity of the Carolina Pigmy Rattlesnake, Sistrurus M
    Marshall University Marshall Digital Scholar Theses, Dissertations and Capstones 1-1-2010 Growth and Age at Reproductive Maturity of the Carolina Pigmy Rattlesnake, Sistrurus m. miliarius (Reptilia: Serpentes) Kevin Robert Messenger [email protected] Follow this and additional works at: http://mds.marshall.edu/etd Part of the Other Animal Sciences Commons Recommended Citation Messenger, Kevin Robert, "Growth and Age at Reproductive Maturity of the Carolina Pigmy Rattlesnake, Sistrurus m. miliarius (Reptilia: Serpentes)" (2010). Theses, Dissertations and Capstones. Paper 106. This Thesis is brought to you for free and open access by Marshall Digital Scholar. It has been accepted for inclusion in Theses, Dissertations and Capstones by an authorized administrator of Marshall Digital Scholar. For more information, please contact [email protected]. Growth and Age at Reproductive Maturity of the Carolina Pigmy Rattlesnake, Sistrurus m. miliarius (Reptilia: Serpentes) Thesis submitted to the Graduate College of Marshall University In partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Master of Science Biological Sciences by Kevin Robert Messenger Dr. Thomas K. Pauley, Committee Chair Dr. Jayme L. Waldron, Committee Member Dr. Dan K. Evans, Committee Member Marshall University December 2010 ABSTRACT Growth and Age at Reproductive Maturity of the Carolina Pigmy Rattlesnake, Sistrurus m. miliarius (Reptilia: Serpentes) Kevin Robert Messenger Growth and age at reproductive maturity are life history attributes that play an important role in the development of proper management strategies. The Carolina Pigmy Rattlesnake, Sistrurus m. miliarius, is one of two dwarf rattlesnake species. The subspecies ranges from eastern NC to nearly the southern tip of SC and into the northeastern portion of GA.
    [Show full text]
  • Stimulus Control of Caudal Luring and Other Feeding Responses: a Program for Research on Visual Perception in Vipers
    STIMULUS CONTROL OF CAUDAL LURING AND OTHER FEEDING RESPONSES: A PROGRAM FOR RESEARCH ON VISUAL PERCEPTION IN VIPERS RANDALL S. REISERER ABSTRACT: Quantitative studies of perceptual mechanisms in vipers have focused on chemosensory tongue-flicking, in part because the prevalent sit-and-wait foraging habit provides few other clues about pre-strike prey recognition. Snakes that employ caudal luring offer a unique opportunity to quantify visual perception, and to infer cognitive functions when snakes use caudal luring, pursuit, or sit-and-wait predation depending on prey type. Stimulus control of caudal luring and other prey capture tactics were examined in four phylogenetically distant vipers: the Copperhead (Agkistrodon contortrix); the Sidewinder (Crotalus cerastes); the Massasauga (Sistrurus catenatus); and the Horned Adder (Bitis caudalis). While all four species feed on a variety of prey, the luring response of each species is evoked almost exclusively by a certain type of organism, and alternate strategies are used to capture other prey types. Feeding experiments and experimental manipulations using prey-like models suggest that these vipers readily discriminate between different types of prey through visual cues involving shape and/or movement features, and that they have evolved appropriate responses for capturing particular prey taxa. Intraspecific variation in stimulus control between populations of S. catenatus suggests that evolution of perceptual mechanisms for prey recognition and appropriate response operates on a fine scale.
    [Show full text]
  • TOWARDS a SEMIOTIC BIOLOGY Life Is the Action of Signs
    • TOWARDS [ ASEMIOTIC ' BIOLOGY Life is the Action of Signs Imperial College Press TOWARDS A SEMIOTIC BIOLOGY Life is the Action of Signs P771tp.indd 1 5/3/11 5:22 PM TOWARDS A SEMIOTIC BIOLOGY Life is the Action of Signs Sense world s si interpretant io m e s Receptor Sense net Carrier of a feature sign Internal world Object Opposite structure Effect net Carrier of an effect representamen object Effector Effect world Editors Claus Emmeche University of Copenhagen, Denmark Kalevi Kull University of Tartu, Estonia Imperial College Press ICP P771tp.indd 2 5/3/11 5:22 PM This page is intentionally left blank Published by Imperial College Press 57 Shelton Street Covent Garden London WC2H 9HE Distributed by World Scientific Publishing Co. Pte. Ltd. 5 Toh Tuck Link, Singapore 596224 USA office: 27 Warren Street, Suite 401-402, Hackensack, NJ 07601 UK office: 57 Shelton Street, Covent Garden, London WC2H 9HE British Library Cataloguing-in-Publication Data A catalogue record for this book is available from the British Library. TOWARDS A SEMIOTIC BIOLOGY Life is the Action of Signs Copyright © 2011 by Imperial College Press All rights reserved. This book, or parts thereof, may not be reproduced in any form or by any means, electronic or mechanical, including photocopying, recording or any information storage and retrieval system now known or to be invented, without written permission from the Publisher. For photocopying of material in this volume, please pay a copying fee through the Copyright Clearance Center, Inc., 222 Rosewood Drive, Danvers, MA 01923, USA. In this case permission to photocopy is not required from the publisher.
    [Show full text]
  • Caudal Distraction by Rat Snakes (Colubridae, Elaphe): a Novel Behavior Used When Capturing Mammalian Prey
    Stephen F. Austin State University SFA ScholarWorks Faculty Publications Biology 1999 Caudal Distraction by Rat Snakes (Colubridae, Elaphe): a Novel Behavior Used when Capturing Mammalian Prey Stephen J. Mullin Stephen F Austin State University, [email protected] Follow this and additional works at: https://scholarworks.sfasu.edu/biology Part of the Biology Commons Tell us how this article helped you. Repository Citation Mullin, Stephen J., "Caudal Distraction by Rat Snakes (Colubridae, Elaphe): a Novel Behavior Used when Capturing Mammalian Prey" (1999). Faculty Publications. 115. https://scholarworks.sfasu.edu/biology/115 This Article is brought to you for free and open access by the Biology at SFA ScholarWorks. It has been accepted for inclusion in Faculty Publications by an authorized administrator of SFA ScholarWorks. For more information, please contact [email protected]. Great Ba....in Naturalist 59(4), ©1999, pp. 361....167 CAUDAL DISTRACTION BY RAT SNAKES (COLUBHIDAE, ELAPHE): A NOVEL BEHAVIOR USED WHEN CAPTURING MAMMALIAN PREY Stephen]. Mullin1 AJ3S11UCT.--el.mthtl movement in snakes trulY serve ei.ther a pl'Cdatory (e.g., caudal luring) or defensive (e.g., rattling, aposem,ttism) fUllction, I descliho n new behavioral pattern of tai.l movement in snakes. Gray rat snakl.'$ (Elaphe OhSO!etd spiloid.es) fi)raging on ~ma11 mmnmnls (Mus d01ne~·ticus) Inoved. their tails in un erratic, whiplike fashion uIter detecting prey in their vidnity. The thrashing movement in the horizontal plfme was audibly and visually obviolls, resulting in dis­ placement of leaf litter around the hlil. All subjects displayed the behavior, hilt not in all foraging episodes. Shorler durations ofcaudal distraction~1.l1lecl in greater predator SIIt.'t.'eSS during the 1st attempt <It prey c<lpl'ure.
    [Show full text]