Caudal Distraction by Rat Snakes (Colubridae, Elaphe): a Novel Behavior Used When Capturing Mammalian Prey

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Caudal Distraction by Rat Snakes (Colubridae, Elaphe): a Novel Behavior Used When Capturing Mammalian Prey Stephen F. Austin State University SFA ScholarWorks Faculty Publications Biology 1999 Caudal Distraction by Rat Snakes (Colubridae, Elaphe): a Novel Behavior Used when Capturing Mammalian Prey Stephen J. Mullin Stephen F Austin State University, [email protected] Follow this and additional works at: https://scholarworks.sfasu.edu/biology Part of the Biology Commons Tell us how this article helped you. Repository Citation Mullin, Stephen J., "Caudal Distraction by Rat Snakes (Colubridae, Elaphe): a Novel Behavior Used when Capturing Mammalian Prey" (1999). Faculty Publications. 115. https://scholarworks.sfasu.edu/biology/115 This Article is brought to you for free and open access by the Biology at SFA ScholarWorks. It has been accepted for inclusion in Faculty Publications by an authorized administrator of SFA ScholarWorks. For more information, please contact [email protected]. Great Ba....in Naturalist 59(4), ©1999, pp. 361....167 CAUDAL DISTRACTION BY RAT SNAKES (COLUBHIDAE, ELAPHE): A NOVEL BEHAVIOR USED WHEN CAPTURING MAMMALIAN PREY Stephen]. Mullin1 AJ3S11UCT.--el.mthtl movement in snakes trulY serve ei.ther a pl'Cdatory (e.g., caudal luring) or defensive (e.g., rattling, aposem,ttism) fUllction, I descliho n new behavioral pattern of tai.l movement in snakes. Gray rat snakl.'$ (Elaphe OhSO!etd spiloid.es) fi)raging on ~ma11 mmnmnls (Mus d01ne~·ticus) Inoved. their tails in un erratic, whiplike fashion uIter detecting prey in their vidnity. The thrashing movement in the horizontal plfme was audibly and visually obviolls, resulting in dis­ placement of leaf litter around the hlil. All subjects displayed the behavior, hilt not in all foraging episodes. Shorler durations ofcaudal distraction~1.l1lecl in greater predator SIIt.'t.'eSS during the 1st attempt <It prey c<lpl'ure. Caudal dis­ traction may facilitate prey capture by gray rat snakes by directing tbe attention of prey away fmm lhe approHching head of the snake. Key words: caudal diJplalj, pre<!awry behaviol; deceit, Elaphe obsolela, flUUnrludian p'-ey, rat snake, Cnlul.1t'hlae. Visual communication by snakes (Carpenter 1993, Rahatsky and Farrell 1996) and at least 1 1977, Carpenter and Ferguson 1977) occurs in lizard specics (Morray et at. 1991). It is typi­ the absence of appendages used by many cally displayed by snakes foraging from lin other vertebrate taxa (CuUen 1972, Enquist et ambush posture (Chiszar et al. 1990). al. 1985). Limblessness necessitates the use of Both defensive tail vibration and caudal lur­ head, body, or tail for visual display. Snake de­ ing have been observed in rat snakes (Elaphe fens:ive displays frequently il1volve tail move­ obsolet<I; TIebout 1997; personal observation). ment or posturing (Greene 1988, Sazima and While observing a gray rat snake (E. o. spiloides) Abe 1991). In particnl",; members of the sub­ foraging for a small mammal (Mus dorne.1icus), family Crotalinae have evolved a specialized 1 recorded a tail display unlike that of either morphology at the taU tip, the rattle. Greene loring (Chiszar et aI. 1990) or defensive rat­ (1992) suggested that the incipient structure tling or tail vibration (Greene 1988). During serves us a warning device to snake predators. subsequent observations, several individuals a conclusion that is the subject of continued displayed similar taU movement when approach­ discussion (Sisk and Jackson 1997, Tiehout ing their prey. The purpose of this study is to 1997). Several species ofcolubrid snakes (Greene descrihe a heretofore undocurnented prellu­ 1988) and other elongate reptiles (Greene 1973) tory behavior of gray rat snakes that was dis­ also perform defensive tail displays. played only in tl,e presence of mammalian Another fonn of taU movement, termed prey. Frequency and duration of this behavior caudal/tIring, fucilitates prey capture. This be­ and its facilitation of prey capture arc also havior is described as a slow, undulatory or reported. Becaose this behavior parallels cau­ vermiform movement of the tail while it is dal luring and defensive tail vibration, I term held upright in close proximity to the snake's the novel behavior caudal distraction.. head (Heatwole ,uld Davison 1976). Caudallur­ ing is presumably mimetiC, in that the move­ MATERIALS AND METHODS ment is thought to resemble an insect huva OT worm and thus attracts potential prey to within r first noticcd caudal disb'action in a 113­ striking dLstance of the otherwise motionless em snout-vent length (SVL) female gray rat snake. Caudal luring has been documcnted in snake foraging for a mixed-sb'ain mouse (M. nearly aU fiunilies of snakes (Carpenter et at. domesticus). The snake was in an enclosure 1978, Radcliffe et al. 1980, Sazima and Puorto tllat simulated a bottomland hardwood forest 1Dellurtll1<:nt of 13inJO/,.'Y. University of Me,nphi~> Mell1phl~, TN 38152. l'r~enl (u::Idre$~' [kplll'tO\onl of 13iolOjl;1cul Scf«llOO&. EiI.I!I,,·n lllilmis UIlive-!'!'It.v. Charleston, IL 61920. 361 362 GREAT BASiN NATURALIST [Volume 59 habitat (see below). Behavior was documented Adult male lllic-e were placed in the enclo­ on videotape to permit detailed description. sure for a minimum of 10 min for acclimatiza­ Several other adult snakes displayed this tion. Only adult maJe mice having dark brown behavior with IitUe departure from the oligi­ pelage were used as prey duling the observa­ nal pattern. To examine possible differences in tions to minimize visual and vomeronasal dif­ capture success and latency to prey capture ferences among prey available to snakes (J ~oop that might be dependent on the use of this 1970) and provide prey visually similar to behavior, I recorded adult gray rat snakes (n those which sn.\kes encounter in the field. All =15) involved in {(>raging episodes under sim­ mice had similar mass (30.7 + 0.5 g, n = 30), ilar conditions. although the ratio of prey mass to snake mass Subjects (10 males and 5 females. >100 cm was not fixed during the study. A gray rat snake SVL) were obtained fi·OOl forested and semi· was placed in thc corner of the enclosure far­ rural areas within Shelby County, Tennessee, thest removed from the prey. Because observer and maintained 'in captivity at temperature presence may influence snake behavior (Drum­ and photopeliod regimes of 26-29°C and mond 198.3), behaviors were recorded by a 14:10 h light:dark, respectively. Suhjects had video carner.! on a tripod elevated over the spent between 1 and 16 months in captivity enclosure floor. The camera was always Oli­ prior to examination. Snakes were housed jn­ ented toward the snake, meauing that possible uividually in cages measuring a minimum of differences in prey behavior were not recorded. 30 x 60 x 30 ern, provided with water ad libi­ Occasionallv,, caudal distraction was initiated tum, and fed either KorUlem Bobwhite Quail with the b,il outside the field of view; bow­ (Colinus vir{"rinianlls) eggs or mixed-strain mice ever, tail movement was audibly discernible as weekly. Excepting; o<..:ca.~iona1 cage cleaning and leaf litte.- in the immediate vicinity was dis­ monthly SVL (±O.5 Col) measurements, sub­ turbed and could thus be recorded from its jects were handled as little as possihle to mini.· initial occurrence. mize any hehavioral modifications resulting The latency to suc(.--essfiIl mouse capture may from prolonged captivity (Warwick 1990, Ford lwve depended, in part, on initial distances 1995). Because recently fed snakes may exhibit separating the snake and its prey. Howevel; shilts in behavioral pattern (Beck 1996) or the colJlined, relatively sm,Jl euclosure proba­ alteTation of locomotor pClformance (Martin bly limited the influence of initial separation 1996), individuals were placed on a restricted distance. Missed attempts at capturing the diet (water alone) for 3 wk prior to experimen­ mouse and frequency and duration of caudal llJ trials. Between 23 April 1995 and 25 March distraction were recorded from videotaped 1996, snakes were allowed to forage indiVidu­ observations. A repeated-measures analysis of ally for mice on at least 2. different occasions, variance (ANOVAR; each mbject was recorded each separated by a 3-wk resmeted diet. on 2 occasions) was used to determine An enclosure (2.25 m2 and 2 m in height), whether those snakes exhibiting caudill dis­ c'Onstructed to simulate bottomland hardwood traction required fewer attempts to Sllccess­ forest ofthe wildlile m'magement area ofnearby /llily capture mke. l'aramemc statistics (Scheffe Mecman-Shelby Forest State Park (MSFSP), 1959, Cohen 1965) were also used to detect Tennessee, ,;vas maintained at the photoperiod any relationships between frequency and/or and temperature regimes described above. duration of caudal distraction and snake gen­ ArtHicial vegetation simulated mean recorded der (ANOVAR) or sile (Pearson's regression). level of vegetation density al MSFSP (mea­ Statistical tests were conducted using Super­ sured in June 19<J4; ,. + 1 Sf = 64 + 2.3 plants ANOVA~ software (Abacus Conccpts) at an 01-2, n = 144). Leaves, obtained at field siles accepted sil,'Dificanee level ofa = 0.05. where snakes were collected, pro,idee! a nat­ ural substrate on the ellclosure floor. Light RESULTS intensity on the enclosure Hoor approximated Description levcls measured at MSFSP during UlC vegeta­ tion sampling period (for IllJiher details on Caudal distraction is the use of tail move­ hahitat sampling and enclosure construction, ments by an elongate predator which serve to see Mullin 1998, Mullin et al. 1998). hold the attention of a prey animal while the 1999J CAUDAL BEHAVIOR IN El..1I'H£ 363 predator's bead is moved to within slrildng distraction dJsplay; instances ofstationary t:au­ range ofthe prey. dal distraction were frequently accompanied In E. o. spiloides c-audal disb1lction displaL-ed by some time displaying the hehavior while leaves near the blil. was visually conspicnous advancing toward the mouse. There was no when unobscurcd by emergent vegetation, and difference in the amount of time a snake dis­ was audible from a distance ofseveral meters.
Recommended publications
  • Evidence of Lingual-Luring by an Aquatic Snake
    Journal of Herpetology, Vol. 34 No. 1 pp 67-74, 2000 Copyright 2000 Society for the Study of Amphibians and Reptiles Evidence of Lingual-luring by an Aquatic Snake HARTWELL H. WELSH, JR. AND AMY J. LIND Pacific Southwest Research Station, USDA Forest Service, 1700 Bayview Dr., Arcata, California 95521, USA. E-mail: hwelsh/[email protected] ABSTRACT.-We describe and quantify the components of an unusual snake behavior used to attract fish prey: lingual-luring. Our earlier research on the foraging behavior of the Pacific Coast aquatic garter snake (Thamnophis atratus) indicated that adults are active foragers, feeding primarily on aquatic Pacific giant salamanders (Dicamptodon tenebrosus) in streambed substrates. Juvenile snakes, however, use primarily ambush tactics to capture larval anurans and juvenile salmonids along stream margins, behaviors that include the lingual-luring described here. We found that lingual-luring differed from typical chemosensory tongue-flicking by the position of the snake, contact of the tongue with the water surface, and the length of time the tongue was extended. Luring snakes are in ambush position and extend and hold their tongues out rigid, with the tongue-tips quivering on the water surface, apparently mimicking insects in order to draw young fish within striking range. This behavior is a novel adaptation of the tongue-vomeronasal system by a visually-oriented predator. The luring of prey by snakes has been asso- luring function (Mushinsky, 1987; Ford and ciated primarily with the use of the tail, a be- Burghardt, 1993). However, Lillywhite and Hen- havior termed caudal luring (e.g., Neill, 1960; derson (1993) noted the occurrence of a pro- Greene and Campbell, 1972; Heatwole and Dav- longed extension of the tongue observed in vine ison, 1976; Jackson and Martin, 1980; Schuett et snakes (e.g., Kennedy, 1965; Henderson and al., 1984; Chizar et al., 1990).
    [Show full text]
  • Caudal Distraction by Rat Snakes (Colubridae, Elaphe): a Novel Behavior Used When Capturing Mammalian Prey
    Great Basin Naturalist Volume 59 Number 4 Article 8 10-15-1999 Caudal distraction by rat snakes (Colubridae, Elaphe): a novel behavior used when capturing mammalian prey Stephen J. Mullin University of Memphis, Memphis, Tennessee Follow this and additional works at: https://scholarsarchive.byu.edu/gbn Recommended Citation Mullin, Stephen J. (1999) "Caudal distraction by rat snakes (Colubridae, Elaphe): a novel behavior used when capturing mammalian prey," Great Basin Naturalist: Vol. 59 : No. 4 , Article 8. Available at: https://scholarsarchive.byu.edu/gbn/vol59/iss4/8 This Article is brought to you for free and open access by the Western North American Naturalist Publications at BYU ScholarsArchive. It has been accepted for inclusion in Great Basin Naturalist by an authorized editor of BYU ScholarsArchive. For more information, please contact [email protected], [email protected]. Great Ba....in Naturalist 59(4), ©1999, pp. 361....167 CAUDAL DISTRACTION BY RAT SNAKES (COLUBHIDAE, ELAPHE): A NOVEL BEHAVIOR USED WHEN CAPTURING MAMMALIAN PREY Stephen]. Mullin1 AJ3S11UCT.--el.mthtl movement in snakes trulY serve ei.ther a pl'Cdatory (e.g., caudal luring) or defensive (e.g., rattling, aposem,ttism) fUllction, I descliho n new behavioral pattern of tai.l movement in snakes. Gray rat snakl.'$ (Elaphe OhSO!etd spiloid.es) fi)raging on ~ma11 mmnmnls (Mus d01ne~·ticus) Inoved. their tails in un erratic, whiplike fashion uIter detecting prey in their vidnity. The thrashing movement in the horizontal plfme was audibly and visually obviolls, resulting in dis­ placement of leaf litter around the hlil. All subjects displayed the behavior, hilt not in all foraging episodes.
    [Show full text]
  • Spring 2009 RURAL LIVING in ARIZONA Volume 3, Number 2
    ARIZONA COOPERATIVE E TENSION THE UNIVERSITY OF ARIZONA COLLEGE OF AGRICULTURE AND LIFE SCIENCES Backyards& Beyond Spring 2009 RURAL LIVING IN ARIZONA Volume 3, Number 2 Spring 2009 1 Common Name: Globemallow Scientific Name: Sphaeralcea spp. Globemallow is a common native wildflower found throughout most of Arizona. There are 16 species (and several varieties) occurring in the state, the majority of which are perennials. They are found between 1,000 and 6,000 feet in elevation and grow on a variety of soil types. Depending on the species, globemallows are either herbaceous or slightly woody at the base of the plant and grow between 2-3 feet in height (annual species may only grow to 6 inches). The leaves are three-lobed, and while the shape varies by species, they are similar enough to help identify the plant as a globemallow. The leaves have star-shaped hairs that give the foliage a gray-green color. Flower color Plant Susan Pater varies from apricot (the most common) to red, pink, lavender, pale yellow and white. Many of the globemallows flower in spring and again in summer. Another common name for globemallow is sore-eye poppy (mal de ojos in Spanish), from claims that the plant irritates the eyes. In southern California globemallows are known as plantas muy malas, translated to mean very bad plants. Ironically, the Pima Indian name for globemallow means a cure for sore eyes. The Hopi Indians used the plant for healing certain ailments and the stems as a type of chewing gum, and call the plant kopona.
    [Show full text]
  • 2007 Australasian Society for the Study of Animal Behaviour & the International Union for the Study of Social Insects (Australian Chapter)
    ASSAB 2007 AUSTRALASIAN SOCIETY FOR THE STUDY OF ANIMAL BEHAVIOUR & THE INTERNATIONAL UNION FOR THE STUDY OF SOCIAL INSECTS (AUSTRALIAN CHAPTER) 12-15 April 2007 The Australian National University Canberra Venue Robertson Lecture Theatre Research School of Biological Sciences Building 46E Hosted by the Research School of Biological Sciences 2 Sponsored by the ARC Centre of Excellence in Vision Science (ACEVS) http://www.vision.edu.au/ LOCAL HOSTS: JOCHEN ZEIL Visual Sciences, Research School of Biological Sciences The Australian National University AJAY NARENDRA Visual Sciences, Research School of Biological Sciences The Australian National University ROB HEINSOHN Centre for Resource and Environmental Studies The Australian National University JAN HEMMI Visual Sciences, Research School of Biological Sciences The Australian National University RICHARD PETERS Visual Sciences, Research School of Biological Sciences The Australian National University WITH SPECIAL THANKS TO THE ASSAB TREASURER, XIMENA NELSON & THE ASSAB PRESIDENT PHIL TAYLOR FOR THEIR SUPPORT Thursday, 12 April Friday, 13 April Saturday, 14 April Sunday, 15 April 8:00 Plenary Lecture Tinbergen Centenary Lecture IUSSI Lecture 9:00 ASSAB 2007 Barbara Webb Chris Evans & Jochen Zeil Ryszard Maleszka 12 - 15 April 2007 09:30 09:30 09:30 Session 3: RSBS Session 8: Session 12: 10:00 SENSORY SYSTEMS & SOCIAL INSECTS I COMMUNICATION I HOMING & NAVIGATION Tea/CoffeeTea/Coffee Break Break Tea/Coffee Break 11:00 Session 9: FORAGING, COMPETITION & Session 4: LIFE HISTORIES I Session 13:
    [Show full text]
  • Nelson Et Al No Highlight-1 Edit Changes
    1 Receiver psychology and the design of the deceptive caudal luring signal of the death 2 adder 3 Ximena J. Nelsona,b* Daniel T. Garnettc, 1 Christopher S. Evansb, 2 4 5 aSchool of Biological Sciences, University of Canterbury 6 bCentre for the Integrative Study of Animal Behaviour, Macquarie University 7 cDepartment of Biological Sciences, Macquarie University 8 Received 6 July 2009 9 Initial acceptance 4 September 2009 10 Final acceptance 30 November 2009 11 MS. number: 09-00453R 12 13 *Correspondence: X. J. Nelson, School of Biological Sciences, University of Canterbury, 14 Private Bag 4800, Christchurch, New Zealand. 15 E-mail address: [email protected] (X. J. Nelson). 16 1D. T. Garnett is at the Department of Biological Sciences, Macquarie University, Sydney, 17 NSW 2109 Australia. 18 2C. S. Evans is at the Centre for the Integrative Study of Animal Behaviour, Macquarie 19 University, Sydney, NSW 2109 Australia. 20 1 21 Signal design can reflect the sensory properties of receivers. The death adder, Acanthophis 22 antarcticus, attracts prey by wriggling the distal portion of its tail (caudal luring). To 23 understand the design of this deceptive signal, we explored perceptual processes in a 24 representative prey species: the Jacky dragon, Amphibolurus muricatus. We used 3D 25 animations of fast and slow death adder luring movements against different backgrounds, to 26 test the hypothesis that caudal luring mimics salient aspects of invertebrate prey. Moving 27 stimuli elicited predatory responses, especially against a conspicuous background. To identify 28 putative models for caudal luring, we used an optic flow algorithm to extract velocity values 29 from video sequences of 61 moving invertebrates caught in lizard territories, and compared 30 these to the velocity values of death adder movements.
    [Show full text]
  • Activity, Habitat Use and the First Evidence of Lingual Lure Behavior in a South American Snake
    SALAMANDRA 56(1): 39–47 Activity, habitat use and lingual lure behavior in a South American snakeSALAMANDRA 15 February 2020 ISSN 0036–3375 German Journal of Herpetology A fisherman’s tale: Activity, habitat use and the first evidence of lingual lure behavior in a South American snake Conrado Mario-da-Rosa1, Arthur Diesel Abegg2,3, Leandro Malta-Borges1, Alexandre Ferreira Righi4, Paulo Sérgio Bernarde5, Sonia Zanini Cechin1 & Tiago Gomes dos Santos1,6 1) Programa de Pós-Graduação em Biodiversidade Animal, Universidade Federal de Santa Maria, Av. Roraima, 1000, Santa Maria, Rio Grande do Sul, Brazil 2) Laboratório Especial de Coleções Zoológicas, Instituto Butantan, Av. Vital Brasil, 1500, São Paulo, São Paulo, Brazil 3) Programa de Pós-Graduação em Zoologia, Instituto de Biociências, Universidade de São Paulo, Travessa 14, Rua do Matão, 321, 05508-090, São Paulo, São Paulo, Brazil 4) Museu de História Natural e Jardim Botânico da Universidade Federal de Minas Gerais, R. Gustavo da Silveira, 1035, Belo Horizonte, Minas Gerais, Brazil 5) Laboratório de Herpetologia, Centro Multidisciplinar, Campus Floresta, Universidade Federal do Acre, Cruzeiro do Sul, Acre, Brazil 6) Universidade Federal do Pampa, Campus São Gabriel, Av. Antônio Trilha, 1847, São Gabriel, Rio Grande do Sul, Brazil Corresponding author: Conrado Mario-da-Rosa, e-mail: [email protected] Manuscript received: 23 August 2019 Accepted: 25 November 2019 by Arne Schulze Abstract. Literature data suggests that Thamnodynastes strigatus (Serpentes, Dipsadidae) is a snake that actively forages for anurans near waterbodies, using several microhabitats for this activity (e.g., shrubs, soil, and water). However, herein we present dissonant data previously known to the species, both concerning type of prey and foraging strategy.
    [Show full text]
  • The Mystery of the Rattlesnake 1
    THE MYSTERY OF THE RATTLESNAKE 1 The Mystery of the RattleSnake Geri Glinsek Special Program: Project Dragonfly Miami University of Ohio THE MYSTERY OF THE RATTLESNAKE 2 Communication is used in all living things. Animals, plants, bacteria, and even an organism's cells use communication, but each of them express it differently. In animal communication there is a signaler (sender) and a receiver (Rosenthal & Ryan, 2000). The sender's signal was shaped into an evolved trait because of the effect it had on the receiver's behavior. Most animals use visual or auditory signals in communication. The auditory signals are produced by sound from an animal's organ. The visual signals are considered traits which are continuously expressed despite the signaler's determined state or environmental condition. The signaler and receiver interaction happens between species (interspecies communication), but it is more common within a species (intraspecies communication) (Roger & Kaplan, 2002). Pursuit-deterrent communication is an example of interspecies communication (Clark, 2005). This type is between predators and prey. The prey are the signalers and the predators are the receivers. For example, many pursuit-deterrent communication happens between rodents and rattlesnakes. Most rodents will signal by mobbing and by using visual cues to warn other prey that snakes have been spotted. This form of communication mutually benefits both predators and prey because predators will avoid a difficult costly pursuit, and prey increase their survival rate by living another day. In only studying animals there are many different ways communication can happen. A prime example of pursuit-deterrent communication is a rattlesnake's rattle.
    [Show full text]
  • Growth and Age at Reproductive Maturity of the Carolina Pigmy Rattlesnake, Sistrurus M
    Marshall University Marshall Digital Scholar Theses, Dissertations and Capstones 1-1-2010 Growth and Age at Reproductive Maturity of the Carolina Pigmy Rattlesnake, Sistrurus m. miliarius (Reptilia: Serpentes) Kevin Robert Messenger [email protected] Follow this and additional works at: http://mds.marshall.edu/etd Part of the Other Animal Sciences Commons Recommended Citation Messenger, Kevin Robert, "Growth and Age at Reproductive Maturity of the Carolina Pigmy Rattlesnake, Sistrurus m. miliarius (Reptilia: Serpentes)" (2010). Theses, Dissertations and Capstones. Paper 106. This Thesis is brought to you for free and open access by Marshall Digital Scholar. It has been accepted for inclusion in Theses, Dissertations and Capstones by an authorized administrator of Marshall Digital Scholar. For more information, please contact [email protected]. Growth and Age at Reproductive Maturity of the Carolina Pigmy Rattlesnake, Sistrurus m. miliarius (Reptilia: Serpentes) Thesis submitted to the Graduate College of Marshall University In partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Master of Science Biological Sciences by Kevin Robert Messenger Dr. Thomas K. Pauley, Committee Chair Dr. Jayme L. Waldron, Committee Member Dr. Dan K. Evans, Committee Member Marshall University December 2010 ABSTRACT Growth and Age at Reproductive Maturity of the Carolina Pigmy Rattlesnake, Sistrurus m. miliarius (Reptilia: Serpentes) Kevin Robert Messenger Growth and age at reproductive maturity are life history attributes that play an important role in the development of proper management strategies. The Carolina Pigmy Rattlesnake, Sistrurus m. miliarius, is one of two dwarf rattlesnake species. The subspecies ranges from eastern NC to nearly the southern tip of SC and into the northeastern portion of GA.
    [Show full text]
  • Stimulus Control of Caudal Luring and Other Feeding Responses: a Program for Research on Visual Perception in Vipers
    STIMULUS CONTROL OF CAUDAL LURING AND OTHER FEEDING RESPONSES: A PROGRAM FOR RESEARCH ON VISUAL PERCEPTION IN VIPERS RANDALL S. REISERER ABSTRACT: Quantitative studies of perceptual mechanisms in vipers have focused on chemosensory tongue-flicking, in part because the prevalent sit-and-wait foraging habit provides few other clues about pre-strike prey recognition. Snakes that employ caudal luring offer a unique opportunity to quantify visual perception, and to infer cognitive functions when snakes use caudal luring, pursuit, or sit-and-wait predation depending on prey type. Stimulus control of caudal luring and other prey capture tactics were examined in four phylogenetically distant vipers: the Copperhead (Agkistrodon contortrix); the Sidewinder (Crotalus cerastes); the Massasauga (Sistrurus catenatus); and the Horned Adder (Bitis caudalis). While all four species feed on a variety of prey, the luring response of each species is evoked almost exclusively by a certain type of organism, and alternate strategies are used to capture other prey types. Feeding experiments and experimental manipulations using prey-like models suggest that these vipers readily discriminate between different types of prey through visual cues involving shape and/or movement features, and that they have evolved appropriate responses for capturing particular prey taxa. Intraspecific variation in stimulus control between populations of S. catenatus suggests that evolution of perceptual mechanisms for prey recognition and appropriate response operates on a fine scale.
    [Show full text]
  • Deliberate Tail Loss (Pseudoautotomy) in a Viperid Snake
    144 North-Western Journal of Zoology 14(1) / 2018 Pannonian subspecies of the common lizard, Zootoca vivipara pannonica long and slender bodies (Costa et al. 2014, Crnobrnja- (Reptilia: Lacertidae). Herpetology Notes 5: 219–221. Isailović et al. 2016). Korsós, Z., Nagy, Z.T. (2006): Kurzbericht über ein vollständig melanotisches Exemplar der Smaragdeidechse, Lacerta viridis (Laurenti, 1768) in Ungarn. Here we describe the first observation of deliberate tail Die Eidechse 17: 42–46. loss, in the form of pseudoautotomy, in a viperid snake San-Jose, L.M., Gonzales-Jimena, V., Fitze, P.S. (2008): Frequency and species: Vipera ammodytes (the European nose-horned viper). phenotypic differences of melanistic and normally coloured common th lizards, Lacerta (Zootoca) vivipara of the Southern Pyrenees (Spain). The observation was made on the 13 of June, 2013, Herpetological Review 39: 422–425. during a survey of the herpetofauna from the Cerna river Stojanov, A., Tzankov, N.D., Naumov, B. (2011): Die Amphibien und Reptilien valley (south-western Romania; coordinates: 44.890 N, Bulgariens. Chimaira, Frankfurt am Main. Stošić, J. (2014): The first record of a melanistic Eastern Green Lizard, Lacerta 22.425 E) conducted by two of the authors (AS and PCD). viridis Laurenti, 1768 (Squamata, Lacertidae), in Croatia. Hyla 2014: 47–50. The habitat in which the observation took place comprises a Tamar, K., Maza, E., Meiri, S. (2013): Acanthodactylus boskianus (Bosk’s Fringe- westerly exposed slope with deciduous forest along a busy Fingered Lizard). Bifurcation. Herpetological Review 44: 135–136. Vroonen, J., Vervust, B., Van Damme, R. (2013): Melanin-based colouration as a road with rock reinforced sides.
    [Show full text]
  • Serpentes, Viperidae)
    UNIVERSIDADE DE SÃO PAULO INSTITUTO DE PSICOLOGIA Juliana Hiromi Tashiro Estudos comparativos da morfologia da retina de Bothrops jararaca e Crotalus durissus (Serpentes, Viperidae) São Paulo 2020 JULIANA HIROMI TASHIRO Estudos comparativos da morfologia da retina de Bothrops jararaca e Crotalus durissus (Serpentes, Viperidae) Versão Corrigida Dissertação apresentada ao Instituto de Psicologia da Universidade de São Paulo como parte das exigências para obtenção do título de Mestre em Ciências. Orientação: Profª. Dra. Dora Fix Ventura Coorientação: Dra. Einat Hauzman São Paulo 2020 AUTORIZO A REPRODUÇÃO E DIVULGAÇÃO TOTAL OU PARCIAL DESTE TRABALHO, POR QUALQUER MEIO CONVENCIONAL OU ELETRÔNICO, PARA FINS DE ESTUDO E PESQUISA, DESDE QUE CITADA A FONTE. Agradecimentos A conclusão desta dissertação só foi possível devido ao apoio e participação de pessoas e instituições as quais tem meus sinceros agradecimentos. Agradeço imensamente às minhas orientadoras, Dra. Einat Hauzman e Profª. Dra. Dora Fix Ventura, por me acolherem no laboratório, pelos preciosos ensinamentos e também pela dedicação integral, gentileza, simpatia, doçura, paciência, respeito e atenção que têm com seus alunos. Estes aspectos foram a base para que a aprendizagem e desenvolvimento de cada etapa deste projeto seguissem de maneira fluida e com menos momentos de estresse, me dando forças para seguir em frente e concretizar o mestrado, que antes era algo distante e apenas almejado. Agradeço à Fundação de Amparo à Pesquisa do Estado de São Paulo (FAPESP), processo n° 2018/13910-9, pela bolsa concedida. Declaro ainda que as opiniões, hipóteses e conclusões ou recomendações expressas neste material são de responsabilidade dos autores e não necessariamente refletem a visão da FAPESP.
    [Show full text]
  • ABSTRACTS 29 Reptile Ecology I, Highland A, Sunday 15 July 2018
    THE JOINT MEETING OF ASIH SSAR HL lcHTHYOLOGISTS & HERPETOLOGISTS ROCHESTER, NEW YORK 2018 ABSTRACTS 29 Reptile Ecology I, Highland A, Sunday 15 July 2018 Curtis Abney, Glenn Tattersall and Anne Yagi Brock University, St. Catharines, Ontario, Canada Thermal Preference and Habitat Selection of Thamnophis sirtalis sirtalis in a Southern Ontario Peatland Gartersnakes represent the most widespread reptile in North America. Despite occupying vastly different biogeoclimatic zones across their range, evidence suggests that the thermal preferenda (Tset) of gartersnakes has not diverged significantly between populations or different Thamnophis species. The reason behind gartersnake success could lie in their flexible thermoregulatory behaviours and habitat selection. We aimed to investigate this relationship by first identifying the Tset of a common gartersnake species (Thamnophis sirtalis sirtalis) via a thermal gradient. We then used this Tset parameter as a baseline for calculating the thermal quality of an open, mixed, and forested habitat all used by the species. We measured the thermal profiles of these habitats by installing a series of temperature-recording analogues that mimicked the reflectance and morphology of living gartersnakes and recorded environmental temperatures as living snakes experience them. Lastly, we used coverboards to survey the current habitat usage of T. s. sirtalis. Of the three habitats, we found that the open habitat offered the highest thermal quality throughout the snake’s active season. In contrast, we recorded the greatest number of snakes using the mixed habitat which had considerably lower thermal quality. Although the open habitat offered the greatest thermal quality, we regularly recorded temperatures exceeding the upper range of the animals’ thermal preference.
    [Show full text]