The Mystery of the Rattlesnake 1

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

The Mystery of the Rattlesnake 1 THE MYSTERY OF THE RATTLESNAKE 1 The Mystery of the RattleSnake Geri Glinsek Special Program: Project Dragonfly Miami University of Ohio THE MYSTERY OF THE RATTLESNAKE 2 Communication is used in all living things. Animals, plants, bacteria, and even an organism's cells use communication, but each of them express it differently. In animal communication there is a signaler (sender) and a receiver (Rosenthal & Ryan, 2000). The sender's signal was shaped into an evolved trait because of the effect it had on the receiver's behavior. Most animals use visual or auditory signals in communication. The auditory signals are produced by sound from an animal's organ. The visual signals are considered traits which are continuously expressed despite the signaler's determined state or environmental condition. The signaler and receiver interaction happens between species (interspecies communication), but it is more common within a species (intraspecies communication) (Roger & Kaplan, 2002). Pursuit-deterrent communication is an example of interspecies communication (Clark, 2005). This type is between predators and prey. The prey are the signalers and the predators are the receivers. For example, many pursuit-deterrent communication happens between rodents and rattlesnakes. Most rodents will signal by mobbing and by using visual cues to warn other prey that snakes have been spotted. This form of communication mutually benefits both predators and prey because predators will avoid a difficult costly pursuit, and prey increase their survival rate by living another day. In only studying animals there are many different ways communication can happen. A prime example of pursuit-deterrent communication is a rattlesnake's rattle. The rattle is only seen in Crotalus and Sistrurus (Allf, Durst & Pfennig, 2016). The rattle is made up of keratin which are loosely scaly segments located at the rattlesnake's tail tip. What makes the rattle vibrate is the loosely scaly segments rubbing against each other at 50 cycles per second (Zug & Erst, 2004). THE MYSTERY OF THE RATTLESNAKE 3 One of the most complicated and unique acoustic-producing structures in science is the rattle (Reisner & Schuett, 2016). There is a lot of controversy on how the rattlesnake rattle evolved. Did it evolve because snakes were feeling threatened or was it due to Caudal Luring (CL) where baby lizards mistakenly identify the rattlesnake's rattle for dinner? One scientist held the idea that behavioral plasticity took a part in the evolution of the rattlesnake's rattle. Another alternative to the rattle's beginning Many different hypotheses are on the rattle controversy, but there is one hypothesis that many researchers and scientists agree on. They proposed the hypothesis that the rattlesnake's rattle evolved to warn predators to stay away (Zug & Ernst, 2004). However, not everyone agrees with this popular hypothesis. According to Reiserer and Schuett (2016), the popular hypothesis does not look at selection playing a part in the initial stages of the rattle which include anatomy, physiology, and behavior. Their study suggested two potential behaviors that formed the early selection on the rattle. The first one is Defensive Tail Vibration and Thrashing (DTVT). This behavior is similar to the popular hypothesis which the rattle evolved to warn predators, but also using it to cause distraction from a predator attacking it's head. Caudal luring (CL) is the other potential behavior that may have helped in the evolution of the rattlesnake's rattle. CL can happen when juvenile lizards are feeding. They are attracted by moving tails that resemble their prey (worms, insects), and end up biting snake tails. CL might have led to the early phenotypic change of the rattlesnake's tail so it mimicked a cephalized region of a moth or beetle larva. Behavioral plasticity and the rattle Behavioral plasticity is another possibility of how the rattle evolved. Allf and his colleagues (2016), believed that behavioral plasticity could give rise to morphological evolution. They THE MYSTERY OF THE RATTLESNAKE 4 thought the best way to examine this is by looking at animal communication signals. In the natural world communication signals are combined with both complex behavioral and morphological traits which most scientists have yearned to understand. Communication signals begin when the receiver uses simple behaviors to guess the actor's next actions which expand over time. This leads to simple behaviors preceding to more complex signaling. Allf and his colleagues (2016) decided to study their predictions by testing the evolution of the unique behavior of the rattlesnake and the rattle. Behavioral plasticity study The researchers captured 155 individual snakes from 56 species and measured tail vibration behaviors (Allf, Durst, & Phennig, 2016). They filmed captive snakes so they could control the different environmental factors that could have affected tail vibration. Defensive behavior was expressed by snakes when researchers waved a stuffed animal head in front of them. Then the researchers assessed the strength of the phylogenetic signal, the species phylogenetic distance from rattlesnakes, and the relationship between each response assessed. The assessment helped to determine if tail vibration behavior came before the evolution of the rattle. Their results supported that tail vibration may have given rise to the rattlesnake's rattling behavior. Conclusion Snakes comprise more than 2,800 species, and yet we know little of their physiology (Zug & Ernst, 2004). Humans only represent one species, but much more is known about us compared to snakes. Some species of snakes like the Santa Catalina Island Rattlesnake and the San Lorenzo Island Rattlesnake have a high rate of losing their rattle segments (Rowe, et al., 2002). Why? It could be poor development, direct selection, or mutations. What does that mean for rattlesnakes? THE MYSTERY OF THE RATTLESNAKE 5 References Allf, B. C., Durst, P. A., & Pfennig, D. W. (2016). Behavioral plasticity and the origins of novelty: the evolution of the rattlesnake rattle. The American Naturalist, 188(4), 475-483. Clark, R. W. (2005). Pursuit-deterrent communication between prey animals and timber rattlesnakes (Crotalus horridus): the response of snakes to harassment displays. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 59(2), 258-261. Meik, J. M., & Pires-daSilva, A. (2009). Evolutionary morphology of the rattlesnake style. BMC Evolutionary Biology, 9(1), 1-9. Reiserer, R. S., & Schuett, G. W. (2016). The origin and evolution of the rattlesnake rattle: misdirection, clarification, theory, and process. Rattlesnakes of Arizona, 2, 247-274. Rogers, L. J., & Kaplan, G. T. (2002). Songs, roars, and rituals: Communication in birds, mammals, and other animals. Harvard University Press. Rowe, M. P., Farrell, T. M., & May, P. G. (2002). Rattle loss in Pigmy Rattlesnakes (Sistrurus miliarius): causes, consequences, and implications for rattle function and evolution. Biology of the Vipers, 385, 404. Ernst, C. H., & Zug, G. R. (1996). Snakes in question: The Smithsonian answer book. Washington, DC: Smithsonian Institution Press..
Recommended publications
  • Evidence of Lingual-Luring by an Aquatic Snake
    Journal of Herpetology, Vol. 34 No. 1 pp 67-74, 2000 Copyright 2000 Society for the Study of Amphibians and Reptiles Evidence of Lingual-luring by an Aquatic Snake HARTWELL H. WELSH, JR. AND AMY J. LIND Pacific Southwest Research Station, USDA Forest Service, 1700 Bayview Dr., Arcata, California 95521, USA. E-mail: hwelsh/[email protected] ABSTRACT.-We describe and quantify the components of an unusual snake behavior used to attract fish prey: lingual-luring. Our earlier research on the foraging behavior of the Pacific Coast aquatic garter snake (Thamnophis atratus) indicated that adults are active foragers, feeding primarily on aquatic Pacific giant salamanders (Dicamptodon tenebrosus) in streambed substrates. Juvenile snakes, however, use primarily ambush tactics to capture larval anurans and juvenile salmonids along stream margins, behaviors that include the lingual-luring described here. We found that lingual-luring differed from typical chemosensory tongue-flicking by the position of the snake, contact of the tongue with the water surface, and the length of time the tongue was extended. Luring snakes are in ambush position and extend and hold their tongues out rigid, with the tongue-tips quivering on the water surface, apparently mimicking insects in order to draw young fish within striking range. This behavior is a novel adaptation of the tongue-vomeronasal system by a visually-oriented predator. The luring of prey by snakes has been asso- luring function (Mushinsky, 1987; Ford and ciated primarily with the use of the tail, a be- Burghardt, 1993). However, Lillywhite and Hen- havior termed caudal luring (e.g., Neill, 1960; derson (1993) noted the occurrence of a pro- Greene and Campbell, 1972; Heatwole and Dav- longed extension of the tongue observed in vine ison, 1976; Jackson and Martin, 1980; Schuett et snakes (e.g., Kennedy, 1965; Henderson and al., 1984; Chizar et al., 1990).
    [Show full text]
  • Caudal Distraction by Rat Snakes (Colubridae, Elaphe): a Novel Behavior Used When Capturing Mammalian Prey
    Great Basin Naturalist Volume 59 Number 4 Article 8 10-15-1999 Caudal distraction by rat snakes (Colubridae, Elaphe): a novel behavior used when capturing mammalian prey Stephen J. Mullin University of Memphis, Memphis, Tennessee Follow this and additional works at: https://scholarsarchive.byu.edu/gbn Recommended Citation Mullin, Stephen J. (1999) "Caudal distraction by rat snakes (Colubridae, Elaphe): a novel behavior used when capturing mammalian prey," Great Basin Naturalist: Vol. 59 : No. 4 , Article 8. Available at: https://scholarsarchive.byu.edu/gbn/vol59/iss4/8 This Article is brought to you for free and open access by the Western North American Naturalist Publications at BYU ScholarsArchive. It has been accepted for inclusion in Great Basin Naturalist by an authorized editor of BYU ScholarsArchive. For more information, please contact [email protected], [email protected]. Great Ba....in Naturalist 59(4), ©1999, pp. 361....167 CAUDAL DISTRACTION BY RAT SNAKES (COLUBHIDAE, ELAPHE): A NOVEL BEHAVIOR USED WHEN CAPTURING MAMMALIAN PREY Stephen]. Mullin1 AJ3S11UCT.--el.mthtl movement in snakes trulY serve ei.ther a pl'Cdatory (e.g., caudal luring) or defensive (e.g., rattling, aposem,ttism) fUllction, I descliho n new behavioral pattern of tai.l movement in snakes. Gray rat snakl.'$ (Elaphe OhSO!etd spiloid.es) fi)raging on ~ma11 mmnmnls (Mus d01ne~·ticus) Inoved. their tails in un erratic, whiplike fashion uIter detecting prey in their vidnity. The thrashing movement in the horizontal plfme was audibly and visually obviolls, resulting in dis­ placement of leaf litter around the hlil. All subjects displayed the behavior, hilt not in all foraging episodes.
    [Show full text]
  • Spring 2009 RURAL LIVING in ARIZONA Volume 3, Number 2
    ARIZONA COOPERATIVE E TENSION THE UNIVERSITY OF ARIZONA COLLEGE OF AGRICULTURE AND LIFE SCIENCES Backyards& Beyond Spring 2009 RURAL LIVING IN ARIZONA Volume 3, Number 2 Spring 2009 1 Common Name: Globemallow Scientific Name: Sphaeralcea spp. Globemallow is a common native wildflower found throughout most of Arizona. There are 16 species (and several varieties) occurring in the state, the majority of which are perennials. They are found between 1,000 and 6,000 feet in elevation and grow on a variety of soil types. Depending on the species, globemallows are either herbaceous or slightly woody at the base of the plant and grow between 2-3 feet in height (annual species may only grow to 6 inches). The leaves are three-lobed, and while the shape varies by species, they are similar enough to help identify the plant as a globemallow. The leaves have star-shaped hairs that give the foliage a gray-green color. Flower color Plant Susan Pater varies from apricot (the most common) to red, pink, lavender, pale yellow and white. Many of the globemallows flower in spring and again in summer. Another common name for globemallow is sore-eye poppy (mal de ojos in Spanish), from claims that the plant irritates the eyes. In southern California globemallows are known as plantas muy malas, translated to mean very bad plants. Ironically, the Pima Indian name for globemallow means a cure for sore eyes. The Hopi Indians used the plant for healing certain ailments and the stems as a type of chewing gum, and call the plant kopona.
    [Show full text]
  • 2007 Australasian Society for the Study of Animal Behaviour & the International Union for the Study of Social Insects (Australian Chapter)
    ASSAB 2007 AUSTRALASIAN SOCIETY FOR THE STUDY OF ANIMAL BEHAVIOUR & THE INTERNATIONAL UNION FOR THE STUDY OF SOCIAL INSECTS (AUSTRALIAN CHAPTER) 12-15 April 2007 The Australian National University Canberra Venue Robertson Lecture Theatre Research School of Biological Sciences Building 46E Hosted by the Research School of Biological Sciences 2 Sponsored by the ARC Centre of Excellence in Vision Science (ACEVS) http://www.vision.edu.au/ LOCAL HOSTS: JOCHEN ZEIL Visual Sciences, Research School of Biological Sciences The Australian National University AJAY NARENDRA Visual Sciences, Research School of Biological Sciences The Australian National University ROB HEINSOHN Centre for Resource and Environmental Studies The Australian National University JAN HEMMI Visual Sciences, Research School of Biological Sciences The Australian National University RICHARD PETERS Visual Sciences, Research School of Biological Sciences The Australian National University WITH SPECIAL THANKS TO THE ASSAB TREASURER, XIMENA NELSON & THE ASSAB PRESIDENT PHIL TAYLOR FOR THEIR SUPPORT Thursday, 12 April Friday, 13 April Saturday, 14 April Sunday, 15 April 8:00 Plenary Lecture Tinbergen Centenary Lecture IUSSI Lecture 9:00 ASSAB 2007 Barbara Webb Chris Evans & Jochen Zeil Ryszard Maleszka 12 - 15 April 2007 09:30 09:30 09:30 Session 3: RSBS Session 8: Session 12: 10:00 SENSORY SYSTEMS & SOCIAL INSECTS I COMMUNICATION I HOMING & NAVIGATION Tea/CoffeeTea/Coffee Break Break Tea/Coffee Break 11:00 Session 9: FORAGING, COMPETITION & Session 4: LIFE HISTORIES I Session 13:
    [Show full text]
  • Nelson Et Al No Highlight-1 Edit Changes
    1 Receiver psychology and the design of the deceptive caudal luring signal of the death 2 adder 3 Ximena J. Nelsona,b* Daniel T. Garnettc, 1 Christopher S. Evansb, 2 4 5 aSchool of Biological Sciences, University of Canterbury 6 bCentre for the Integrative Study of Animal Behaviour, Macquarie University 7 cDepartment of Biological Sciences, Macquarie University 8 Received 6 July 2009 9 Initial acceptance 4 September 2009 10 Final acceptance 30 November 2009 11 MS. number: 09-00453R 12 13 *Correspondence: X. J. Nelson, School of Biological Sciences, University of Canterbury, 14 Private Bag 4800, Christchurch, New Zealand. 15 E-mail address: [email protected] (X. J. Nelson). 16 1D. T. Garnett is at the Department of Biological Sciences, Macquarie University, Sydney, 17 NSW 2109 Australia. 18 2C. S. Evans is at the Centre for the Integrative Study of Animal Behaviour, Macquarie 19 University, Sydney, NSW 2109 Australia. 20 1 21 Signal design can reflect the sensory properties of receivers. The death adder, Acanthophis 22 antarcticus, attracts prey by wriggling the distal portion of its tail (caudal luring). To 23 understand the design of this deceptive signal, we explored perceptual processes in a 24 representative prey species: the Jacky dragon, Amphibolurus muricatus. We used 3D 25 animations of fast and slow death adder luring movements against different backgrounds, to 26 test the hypothesis that caudal luring mimics salient aspects of invertebrate prey. Moving 27 stimuli elicited predatory responses, especially against a conspicuous background. To identify 28 putative models for caudal luring, we used an optic flow algorithm to extract velocity values 29 from video sequences of 61 moving invertebrates caught in lizard territories, and compared 30 these to the velocity values of death adder movements.
    [Show full text]
  • Activity, Habitat Use and the First Evidence of Lingual Lure Behavior in a South American Snake
    SALAMANDRA 56(1): 39–47 Activity, habitat use and lingual lure behavior in a South American snakeSALAMANDRA 15 February 2020 ISSN 0036–3375 German Journal of Herpetology A fisherman’s tale: Activity, habitat use and the first evidence of lingual lure behavior in a South American snake Conrado Mario-da-Rosa1, Arthur Diesel Abegg2,3, Leandro Malta-Borges1, Alexandre Ferreira Righi4, Paulo Sérgio Bernarde5, Sonia Zanini Cechin1 & Tiago Gomes dos Santos1,6 1) Programa de Pós-Graduação em Biodiversidade Animal, Universidade Federal de Santa Maria, Av. Roraima, 1000, Santa Maria, Rio Grande do Sul, Brazil 2) Laboratório Especial de Coleções Zoológicas, Instituto Butantan, Av. Vital Brasil, 1500, São Paulo, São Paulo, Brazil 3) Programa de Pós-Graduação em Zoologia, Instituto de Biociências, Universidade de São Paulo, Travessa 14, Rua do Matão, 321, 05508-090, São Paulo, São Paulo, Brazil 4) Museu de História Natural e Jardim Botânico da Universidade Federal de Minas Gerais, R. Gustavo da Silveira, 1035, Belo Horizonte, Minas Gerais, Brazil 5) Laboratório de Herpetologia, Centro Multidisciplinar, Campus Floresta, Universidade Federal do Acre, Cruzeiro do Sul, Acre, Brazil 6) Universidade Federal do Pampa, Campus São Gabriel, Av. Antônio Trilha, 1847, São Gabriel, Rio Grande do Sul, Brazil Corresponding author: Conrado Mario-da-Rosa, e-mail: [email protected] Manuscript received: 23 August 2019 Accepted: 25 November 2019 by Arne Schulze Abstract. Literature data suggests that Thamnodynastes strigatus (Serpentes, Dipsadidae) is a snake that actively forages for anurans near waterbodies, using several microhabitats for this activity (e.g., shrubs, soil, and water). However, herein we present dissonant data previously known to the species, both concerning type of prey and foraging strategy.
    [Show full text]
  • Growth and Age at Reproductive Maturity of the Carolina Pigmy Rattlesnake, Sistrurus M
    Marshall University Marshall Digital Scholar Theses, Dissertations and Capstones 1-1-2010 Growth and Age at Reproductive Maturity of the Carolina Pigmy Rattlesnake, Sistrurus m. miliarius (Reptilia: Serpentes) Kevin Robert Messenger [email protected] Follow this and additional works at: http://mds.marshall.edu/etd Part of the Other Animal Sciences Commons Recommended Citation Messenger, Kevin Robert, "Growth and Age at Reproductive Maturity of the Carolina Pigmy Rattlesnake, Sistrurus m. miliarius (Reptilia: Serpentes)" (2010). Theses, Dissertations and Capstones. Paper 106. This Thesis is brought to you for free and open access by Marshall Digital Scholar. It has been accepted for inclusion in Theses, Dissertations and Capstones by an authorized administrator of Marshall Digital Scholar. For more information, please contact [email protected]. Growth and Age at Reproductive Maturity of the Carolina Pigmy Rattlesnake, Sistrurus m. miliarius (Reptilia: Serpentes) Thesis submitted to the Graduate College of Marshall University In partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Master of Science Biological Sciences by Kevin Robert Messenger Dr. Thomas K. Pauley, Committee Chair Dr. Jayme L. Waldron, Committee Member Dr. Dan K. Evans, Committee Member Marshall University December 2010 ABSTRACT Growth and Age at Reproductive Maturity of the Carolina Pigmy Rattlesnake, Sistrurus m. miliarius (Reptilia: Serpentes) Kevin Robert Messenger Growth and age at reproductive maturity are life history attributes that play an important role in the development of proper management strategies. The Carolina Pigmy Rattlesnake, Sistrurus m. miliarius, is one of two dwarf rattlesnake species. The subspecies ranges from eastern NC to nearly the southern tip of SC and into the northeastern portion of GA.
    [Show full text]
  • Stimulus Control of Caudal Luring and Other Feeding Responses: a Program for Research on Visual Perception in Vipers
    STIMULUS CONTROL OF CAUDAL LURING AND OTHER FEEDING RESPONSES: A PROGRAM FOR RESEARCH ON VISUAL PERCEPTION IN VIPERS RANDALL S. REISERER ABSTRACT: Quantitative studies of perceptual mechanisms in vipers have focused on chemosensory tongue-flicking, in part because the prevalent sit-and-wait foraging habit provides few other clues about pre-strike prey recognition. Snakes that employ caudal luring offer a unique opportunity to quantify visual perception, and to infer cognitive functions when snakes use caudal luring, pursuit, or sit-and-wait predation depending on prey type. Stimulus control of caudal luring and other prey capture tactics were examined in four phylogenetically distant vipers: the Copperhead (Agkistrodon contortrix); the Sidewinder (Crotalus cerastes); the Massasauga (Sistrurus catenatus); and the Horned Adder (Bitis caudalis). While all four species feed on a variety of prey, the luring response of each species is evoked almost exclusively by a certain type of organism, and alternate strategies are used to capture other prey types. Feeding experiments and experimental manipulations using prey-like models suggest that these vipers readily discriminate between different types of prey through visual cues involving shape and/or movement features, and that they have evolved appropriate responses for capturing particular prey taxa. Intraspecific variation in stimulus control between populations of S. catenatus suggests that evolution of perceptual mechanisms for prey recognition and appropriate response operates on a fine scale.
    [Show full text]
  • Caudal Distraction by Rat Snakes (Colubridae, Elaphe): a Novel Behavior Used When Capturing Mammalian Prey
    Stephen F. Austin State University SFA ScholarWorks Faculty Publications Biology 1999 Caudal Distraction by Rat Snakes (Colubridae, Elaphe): a Novel Behavior Used when Capturing Mammalian Prey Stephen J. Mullin Stephen F Austin State University, [email protected] Follow this and additional works at: https://scholarworks.sfasu.edu/biology Part of the Biology Commons Tell us how this article helped you. Repository Citation Mullin, Stephen J., "Caudal Distraction by Rat Snakes (Colubridae, Elaphe): a Novel Behavior Used when Capturing Mammalian Prey" (1999). Faculty Publications. 115. https://scholarworks.sfasu.edu/biology/115 This Article is brought to you for free and open access by the Biology at SFA ScholarWorks. It has been accepted for inclusion in Faculty Publications by an authorized administrator of SFA ScholarWorks. For more information, please contact [email protected]. Great Ba....in Naturalist 59(4), ©1999, pp. 361....167 CAUDAL DISTRACTION BY RAT SNAKES (COLUBHIDAE, ELAPHE): A NOVEL BEHAVIOR USED WHEN CAPTURING MAMMALIAN PREY Stephen]. Mullin1 AJ3S11UCT.--el.mthtl movement in snakes trulY serve ei.ther a pl'Cdatory (e.g., caudal luring) or defensive (e.g., rattling, aposem,ttism) fUllction, I descliho n new behavioral pattern of tai.l movement in snakes. Gray rat snakl.'$ (Elaphe OhSO!etd spiloid.es) fi)raging on ~ma11 mmnmnls (Mus d01ne~·ticus) Inoved. their tails in un erratic, whiplike fashion uIter detecting prey in their vidnity. The thrashing movement in the horizontal plfme was audibly and visually obviolls, resulting in dis­ placement of leaf litter around the hlil. All subjects displayed the behavior, hilt not in all foraging episodes. Shorler durations ofcaudal distraction~1.l1lecl in greater predator SIIt.'t.'eSS during the 1st attempt <It prey c<lpl'ure.
    [Show full text]
  • Deliberate Tail Loss (Pseudoautotomy) in a Viperid Snake
    144 North-Western Journal of Zoology 14(1) / 2018 Pannonian subspecies of the common lizard, Zootoca vivipara pannonica long and slender bodies (Costa et al. 2014, Crnobrnja- (Reptilia: Lacertidae). Herpetology Notes 5: 219–221. Isailović et al. 2016). Korsós, Z., Nagy, Z.T. (2006): Kurzbericht über ein vollständig melanotisches Exemplar der Smaragdeidechse, Lacerta viridis (Laurenti, 1768) in Ungarn. Here we describe the first observation of deliberate tail Die Eidechse 17: 42–46. loss, in the form of pseudoautotomy, in a viperid snake San-Jose, L.M., Gonzales-Jimena, V., Fitze, P.S. (2008): Frequency and species: Vipera ammodytes (the European nose-horned viper). phenotypic differences of melanistic and normally coloured common th lizards, Lacerta (Zootoca) vivipara of the Southern Pyrenees (Spain). The observation was made on the 13 of June, 2013, Herpetological Review 39: 422–425. during a survey of the herpetofauna from the Cerna river Stojanov, A., Tzankov, N.D., Naumov, B. (2011): Die Amphibien und Reptilien valley (south-western Romania; coordinates: 44.890 N, Bulgariens. Chimaira, Frankfurt am Main. Stošić, J. (2014): The first record of a melanistic Eastern Green Lizard, Lacerta 22.425 E) conducted by two of the authors (AS and PCD). viridis Laurenti, 1768 (Squamata, Lacertidae), in Croatia. Hyla 2014: 47–50. The habitat in which the observation took place comprises a Tamar, K., Maza, E., Meiri, S. (2013): Acanthodactylus boskianus (Bosk’s Fringe- westerly exposed slope with deciduous forest along a busy Fingered Lizard). Bifurcation. Herpetological Review 44: 135–136. Vroonen, J., Vervust, B., Van Damme, R. (2013): Melanin-based colouration as a road with rock reinforced sides.
    [Show full text]
  • Serpentes, Viperidae)
    UNIVERSIDADE DE SÃO PAULO INSTITUTO DE PSICOLOGIA Juliana Hiromi Tashiro Estudos comparativos da morfologia da retina de Bothrops jararaca e Crotalus durissus (Serpentes, Viperidae) São Paulo 2020 JULIANA HIROMI TASHIRO Estudos comparativos da morfologia da retina de Bothrops jararaca e Crotalus durissus (Serpentes, Viperidae) Versão Corrigida Dissertação apresentada ao Instituto de Psicologia da Universidade de São Paulo como parte das exigências para obtenção do título de Mestre em Ciências. Orientação: Profª. Dra. Dora Fix Ventura Coorientação: Dra. Einat Hauzman São Paulo 2020 AUTORIZO A REPRODUÇÃO E DIVULGAÇÃO TOTAL OU PARCIAL DESTE TRABALHO, POR QUALQUER MEIO CONVENCIONAL OU ELETRÔNICO, PARA FINS DE ESTUDO E PESQUISA, DESDE QUE CITADA A FONTE. Agradecimentos A conclusão desta dissertação só foi possível devido ao apoio e participação de pessoas e instituições as quais tem meus sinceros agradecimentos. Agradeço imensamente às minhas orientadoras, Dra. Einat Hauzman e Profª. Dra. Dora Fix Ventura, por me acolherem no laboratório, pelos preciosos ensinamentos e também pela dedicação integral, gentileza, simpatia, doçura, paciência, respeito e atenção que têm com seus alunos. Estes aspectos foram a base para que a aprendizagem e desenvolvimento de cada etapa deste projeto seguissem de maneira fluida e com menos momentos de estresse, me dando forças para seguir em frente e concretizar o mestrado, que antes era algo distante e apenas almejado. Agradeço à Fundação de Amparo à Pesquisa do Estado de São Paulo (FAPESP), processo n° 2018/13910-9, pela bolsa concedida. Declaro ainda que as opiniões, hipóteses e conclusões ou recomendações expressas neste material são de responsabilidade dos autores e não necessariamente refletem a visão da FAPESP.
    [Show full text]
  • ABSTRACTS 29 Reptile Ecology I, Highland A, Sunday 15 July 2018
    THE JOINT MEETING OF ASIH SSAR HL lcHTHYOLOGISTS & HERPETOLOGISTS ROCHESTER, NEW YORK 2018 ABSTRACTS 29 Reptile Ecology I, Highland A, Sunday 15 July 2018 Curtis Abney, Glenn Tattersall and Anne Yagi Brock University, St. Catharines, Ontario, Canada Thermal Preference and Habitat Selection of Thamnophis sirtalis sirtalis in a Southern Ontario Peatland Gartersnakes represent the most widespread reptile in North America. Despite occupying vastly different biogeoclimatic zones across their range, evidence suggests that the thermal preferenda (Tset) of gartersnakes has not diverged significantly between populations or different Thamnophis species. The reason behind gartersnake success could lie in their flexible thermoregulatory behaviours and habitat selection. We aimed to investigate this relationship by first identifying the Tset of a common gartersnake species (Thamnophis sirtalis sirtalis) via a thermal gradient. We then used this Tset parameter as a baseline for calculating the thermal quality of an open, mixed, and forested habitat all used by the species. We measured the thermal profiles of these habitats by installing a series of temperature-recording analogues that mimicked the reflectance and morphology of living gartersnakes and recorded environmental temperatures as living snakes experience them. Lastly, we used coverboards to survey the current habitat usage of T. s. sirtalis. Of the three habitats, we found that the open habitat offered the highest thermal quality throughout the snake’s active season. In contrast, we recorded the greatest number of snakes using the mixed habitat which had considerably lower thermal quality. Although the open habitat offered the greatest thermal quality, we regularly recorded temperatures exceeding the upper range of the animals’ thermal preference.
    [Show full text]