Lip-Pellet Positions During Vowels and Labial Consonants

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Lip-Pellet Positions During Vowels and Labial Consonants Journal of Phonetics (1997) 25, 405Ð 419 Lip-pellet positions during vowels and labial consonants John R. Westbury and Michiko Hashi Waisman Center and Department of Communicative Disorders, University of Wisconsin-Madison, 1500 Highland Avenue, Madison, WI 53705Ð2280, U.S.A. Received 15th July 1996, and in revised form 8th April 1997 Sagittal-plane movements of small markers attached to the upper and lower lips were analyzed for ten speakers of American English, and seven speakers of Japanese. Each speaker produced simple utterances containing vowels and labial consonants. The data were analyzed to better understand: (1) patterns of pellet motions associated with labial consonant production; (2) pellet positions at discrete, acoustically- defined moments during selected speech sounds; and (3) the relationship between midline separation between the lip surfaces and inter-lip-pellet distance. Results from the study provide qualitative information about the dynamics of labial gestures for consonants involving lip closure. The data also indicate that the English and Japanese speakers positioned and moved their lips in generally similar ways during the test sounds analyzed. Finally, results suggest that plausible estimates of mid-line inter-lip separation can be derived from the trajectories of two pellets, one on each lip, as long as the possibility of lip-body deformation is taken into account. ( 1997 Academic Press Limited 1. Introduction Houde (1968) introduced the phrase ‘‘point-parameterized’’ to refer to records of speech movement that take the form of trajectories of discrete, point-like ‘‘markers’’ (e.g., pellets, coils, light-emitting diodes, reflecting disks, or even bony landmarks) on an articulator. Several modern techniques for studying speech movement provide data of this type for accessible articulators such as the tongue, lips, jaw, and soft palate. For example, the X-ray microbeam (e.g., Macchi, 1988), electro-magnetic articulometry (e.g., Perkell, Matthies, Svirsky & Jordan, 1993), and certain opto-electrical techniques (e.g., Boyce, 1990), have all been used to examine and describe speech-related motions of the lips in terms of sagittal-plane coordinates of the centers of small markers, one firmly attached in the mid-line at the vermillion border of each lip.1 The lips pucker and spread and rise and fall during speech, as talkers shape the inter-lip cavity to modify the spectrum of sound radiating from the mouth. In the best of all worlds, a complete understanding of these actions might be inferred from a sagittal-plane representation of the motions of two fleshpoints. However, more must be known about the relationship between lip-marker 1 A recent report by Ramsey, Munhall, Gracco & Ostry (1996) describes three-dimensional fleshpoint- kinematic data, recorded from a single talker, using multiple lip markers tracked by means of an optoelectrical system. Those data provide a more complete view of the actions of the lips than the more conventional two-marker, sagittal-plane view considered in this report. However, inferences about size and shape of the lip opening, from both types of data, will probably be constrained in similar ways. 0095Ð4470/97/040405#15 $25.00/0/jp970050 ( 1997 Academic Press Limited 406 J. R. ¼estbury and M. Hashi positions, and the nature and degree of labial constrictions, before broad inferences about lip action can be drawn. A handful of direct-imaging studies of lip function (e.g., Fujimura, 1961; Fromkin, 1964; Linker, 1982; Abry & Bo¬ e, 1986) provide information about time changes in size and shape of the lip opening during speech. However, these studies do not tell us what discrete point motions reveal about labial articulation. An indirect way to begin to address this question is to examine trajectories of lip markers during speech events that limit lip opening. The labial consonants /p b m/ are events of this type. During their closures, no air exits the mouth, because the lips are fully together and form a tight seal. For this reason, we might expect the positions of lip markers, and the distances between them, to vary little during consonantal closure intervals. The analysis of lip-marker kinematics described in this report was designed, in part, to address these simple expectations, and in general, to provide an improved understanding of two broader topics. The first relates to how midline labial fleshpoints move as lip closures are formed and released for the labial consonants /p b m/. The second relates to the question of whether reliable conclusions about labial constrictions are possible from something so simple as the sagittal-plane trajectories of one marker on each lip. A practical benefit associated with a better understanding of the articulatory signifi- cance of lip-marker positions has to do with representing vocal tract postures for vowels. It is common to think of the vocal tract as a flexible tube, and to define the articulatory posture at any moment during speech in terms of an area function which represents the cross-sectional area of the tube as a function of its length, from the glottis to the lips. A plausible labial termination can easily be ‘‘attached’’ to a tube approximation of the Figure 1. A stylized tracing of a midsagittal section of the vocal tract. Hypothetical pellets are represented by small solid circles ‘‘attached’’ to the outlines of both lips. Letter symbols and lines and defined in the text. ¸ip-pellet positions during selected sounds 407 vocal tract if length and degree of the lip constriction are simply related to the measured positions of upper and lower-lip markers. A sketch shown in Fig. 1 suggests a scheme for doing this. To a first approximation, constriction length might be represented by the length of a line segment such as A, drawn tangent to the lower-most edges of the maxillary incisors, and perpendicular to the segment connecting upper and lower-lip marker positions. Constriction degree reflected by the midsagittal separation between the lip surfaces, analogous to B, might then be represented by the distance C between the two markers, minus some reference measure of the lips’ combined thickness. A ‘‘candi- date’’ reference measure might be some distance between markers when the opposing lip surfaces are in contact (e.g., during closure for a labial stop). 2. Methods and materials Acoustic and labial fleshpoint kinematic data from a sample of 10 speakers of American English, and seven speakers of Japanese, were analyzed. Data from these speakers were not collected specifically for this analysis. Instead, they were available from existing corpora, in which the sound pressure wave had been sampled 21,739 times/s, while each of the speakers produced single examples of /p b m/ in isolated /" Ð "/ frames, with primary stress on the second vowel; and, single, isolated examples of the five vowels /i 2"ou/2. During each brief speech task, sagittal-plane positions of upper-lip (UL) and lower-lip (LL) markers (gold pellets, 2.5 mm in diameter, attached in the midline at the vermillion border), were recorded at 40 and 80 times/s, respectively, using the X-ray microbeam (XRMB) system at the University of Wisconsin3. Pellet positions were expressed relative to cranial axes, defined relative to each speaker’s maxillary occlusal plane (MaxOP) and central maxillary incisors (CMI), according to conventions de- scribed elsewhere (Westbury, 1994a). Materials for the English speakers were drawn from the publicly available XRMB Speech Production Database (Westbury, 1994b) [speakers E29 (female), E31(f), E34(f), E35(f), E41 (male), E44(m), E53(m), E54(f), E59(m), E61(m)].4 Materials for the Japanese 2 Throughout the text of this report, the mid-front, low-back, and high-back vowels of Japanese are transcribed phonemically as /2"u/, respectively. These symbols are correct for the English vowels, but possibly misleading for Japanese. Sources cited by Vance (1987) suggest that the mid-front vowel in Japanese may be phonetically ‘‘midway’’ between [e] and [2]; the low-back vowel midway between [a] and ["]; and the high-back vowel closer to [ɯ]. In Fig. 5 the symbols e and a are used to represent the mid-front and low-back vowels produced by both sets of speakers. This usage is due to graphical limitations imposed by plotting software. 3 The XRMB system sometimes fails to track one or more pellets for some or all time samples spanning a speech task. Examples of /m b/ were lost to mistracking for one Japanese speaker (J5). Consequently, only 49 VCV utterances were available for analysis, rather than the 51 that would be expected given one example each of/pbm/inan["Ð"] frame, for seventeen speakers. Two time samples of UL pellet position were also lost to mistracking for one English speaker (E61), during the release gesture for /m/. However, su¦cient residual information was available in the latter case to approximate the two missing time samples inthe trajectory, using a simple linear interpolation scheme. 4 Identification numbers for English speakers are the same as in the publicly available XRMB Speech Production Database, while the speech tasks correspond to vcv and vowel records from that corpus. Waveforms analyzed in the current study were processed and filtered in a slightly di¤erent way from those in the public release of the XRMB corpus, though these di¤erences have no material e¤ect on relative magnitudes of positional measures. Materials from these XRMB Database speakers, tasks, and/or waveforms may be used for similar or other purposes, in future work by other investigators, and it may become important to know which of the relevant materials were included in the current analysis. The 17 English and Japanese speakers included in this study are the same group also described in an independent analysis of vowel configurations (Hashi, Westbury & Honda, 1994).
Recommended publications
  • A Perceptual Correlate of the Labial-Coronal Effect Marc Sato, Nathalie Vallée, Jean-Luc Schwartz, Isabelle Rousset
    A perceptual correlate of the labial-coronal effect Marc Sato, Nathalie Vallée, Jean-Luc Schwartz, Isabelle Rousset To cite this version: Marc Sato, Nathalie Vallée, Jean-Luc Schwartz, Isabelle Rousset. A perceptual correlate of the labial- coronal effect. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, American Speech-Language- Hearing Association, 2007, 50 (6), pp.1466-1480. 10.1044/1092-4388(2007/101). hal-00194046 HAL Id: hal-00194046 https://hal.archives-ouvertes.fr/hal-00194046 Submitted on 5 Dec 2007 HAL is a multi-disciplinary open access L’archive ouverte pluridisciplinaire HAL, est archive for the deposit and dissemination of sci- destinée au dépôt et à la diffusion de documents entific research documents, whether they are pub- scientifiques de niveau recherche, publiés ou non, lished or not. The documents may come from émanant des établissements d’enseignement et de teaching and research institutions in France or recherche français ou étrangers, des laboratoires abroad, or from public or private research centers. publics ou privés. Author manuscript, published in "Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 50, 6 (2007) 1466-1480" DOI : 10.1044/1092-4388(2007/101) A Perceptual correlate of the Labial-Coronal Effect 1 A perceptual correlate of the Labial-Coronal Effect Marc Sato 1,2, Nathalie Vallée 1,*, Jean-Luc Schwartz 1, Isabelle Rousset 1 1 Institut de la Communication Parlée, CNRS UMR 5009, Institut National Polytechnique de Grenoble, Université Stendhal 2 Departimento di Neuroscienze, Sezione di Fisiologia, Universita
    [Show full text]
  • Phonological Degrees of Labiality
    Phonological degrees of labiality February 5, 2018 Abstract A [+round] or [labial] feature is traditionally viewed as an elementary phonological unit that has different phonetic realizations depending on the height and backness of the segment that realizes it (Clements and Hume 1995, McCarthy 1988, Halle 1995, Kaun 1997 among oth- ers). In this paper, I make two claims: a) qualitatively different lip-gestures are phonological in some languages, and b) there is more faithfulness to more extreme lip gestures. 1 Introduction While it has long been known that rounding is not realized uniformly across vowel heights, this fact has been considered purely phonetic. This paper proposes an analysis of a phonological phenomenon involving a transfer of labiality from a vowel to a consonant under certain conditions. I argue that the Karata data support the idea that differences in rounding can be phonological1. Karata (kir¯Ïi maţ¯’i, Russian karatinskij jazyk)2 is an understudied Nakh-Daghestanian lan- guage originally spoken in 10 ‘auls’ (i.e. mountain-top villages) in western Daghestan.3 The available literature on Karata consists of a grammatical sketch [Magomedbekova, 1971] , and a dictionary (Karata to Russian) [Magomedova and Khalidova, 2001]. The data presented in this paper come from fieldwork that I carried out in June 2011 and July 2012 and from the above mentioned sources. The structure of the paper is the following. Section 2 gives an overview of the phenomenon investigated and its proposed analysis. Section 3 provides background information on the phonol- ogy of Karata. Section 4 is dedicated to the OT analysis of the Karata.
    [Show full text]
  • Input, Intake, and Phonological Development; the Case Of
    Input, Intake, and Phonological Development; The case of Consonant Harmony 0. Introduction A puzzling observation about Consonant Harmony in Dutch- and English child language is that while for English both Labial and Dorsal Harmony is reported, Dutch data exhibit almost exclusively Labial Harmony. Consonant Harmony (CH) is usually described as a process in which two non-adjacent consonants within a word come to share one or more phonological features. In the most frequently occurring – and most commented on - type of CH the two non-adjacent consonants share their primary Place of Articulation (PoA) feature. Examples from PoA-Harmony in English and Dutch child language are in (1). (1) PoA Consonant Harmony a. duck /dѩk/ [gѩk] Dorsal Harmony b. take /teik/ [geik] Dorsal Harmony c. soep /sup/ soup [fup] Labial Harmony d. slapen /slapђ/ sleep [fapђ] Labial Harmony From a purely theoretical, in casu, Optimality theoretical perspective, this inter- linguistic difference is not expected; If child language data reflect an initial Markedness >> Faithfulness ordering of universal constraints, and CH forms are considered to be unmarked output forms, resulting from some highly ranked universal Markedness constraint, then we would expect similar CH data for both types of language learners, at least in a particular developmental stage. However, it is not the case that Dutch children either come from, or go to a grammatical stage in which Dorsal Harmony is present, nor do English children start out with only Labial Harmony, and add Dorsal Harmony later. Our explanation appears simple: the observed difference results from different distributions of Place of Articulation features in the two languages.
    [Show full text]
  • Studies in African Linguistics Volume 21, Number 3, December 1990
    Studies in African Linguistics Volume 21, Number 3, December 1990 CONTEXTUAL LABIALIZATION IN NA WURI* Roderic F. Casali Ghana Institute of Linguistics Literacy and Bible Translation and UCLA A spectrographic investigation into the non-contrastive labialization of consonants before round vowels in Nawuri (a Kwa language of Ghana) sup­ ports the notion that this labialization is the result of a phonological, feature­ spreading rule and not simply an automatic transitional process. This as­ sumption is further warranted in that it allows for a more natural treatment of some other phonological processes in the language. The fact that labial­ ization before round vowels is generally not very audible is explained in terms of a principle of speech perception. A final topic addressed is the question of why (both in Nawuri and apparently in a number of other Ghanaian languages as well) contextual labialization does tend to be more perceptible in certain restricted environments. o. Introduction This paper deals with the allophonic labialization of consonants before round vowels in Nawuri, a Kwa language of Ghana.! While such labialization is gener­ ally not very audible, spectrographic evidence suggests that it is strongly present, * The spectrograms in this study were produced at the phonetics lab of the University of Texas at Arlington using equipment provided through a grant of the Permanent University Fund of the University of Texas system. I would like to thank the following people for their valuable comments and suggestions: Joan Baart, Don Burquest, Mike Cahill, Jerry Edmondson, Norris McKinney, Bob Mugele, Tony Naden, and Keith Snider. I would also like to express my appreciation to Russell Schuh and an anonymous referee for this journal for their helpful criticism of an earlier version, and to Mary Steele for some helpful discussion concerning labialization in Konkomba.
    [Show full text]
  • Neutralization of Vowels / Ɨ/ and /U/ After a Labial Consonant in Korean
    3 Phonetics and Speech Sciences Vol. 6 No. 1 (March 31, 2014) www.speechsciences.or.kr ISSN 2005-8063, pp. 3~10 http://dx.doi.org/10.13064/KSSS.2014.6.1.003 Neutralization of Vowels /ɨ/ and /u/ after a Labial Consonant in Korean: A Cross-generational Study Kang, Hyunsook1) ABSTRACT This study investigated whether Korean vowels, /ɨ/ and /u/, are distinctively perceived after a labial consonant given the fact that native and Sino-Korean nouns showed only vowel /u/ after a labial consonant while this pattern was massively broken by the recent introduction of loanwords. For this purpose, a perception experiment was conducted with V1C1V2 sequences in which different vowels /a, i, u/ and consonants /p, t, k/ occurred in V1 and C1 before the target V2, /ɨ/ and /u/. The data was produced by six speakers each from two different age groups, Age20 and Age40/50 in the read speech style. The results showed that consonant /p/ attracted significantly more responses of /u/ from /VCɨ/ sequences and significantly less responses of /u/ from /VCu/ sequence than the other consonants did in both age groups. Furthermore, Age20 group showed significantly less percentage of /u/ responses than Age40 group when the preceding consonant was /p/ regardless of the target vowel. We suggest therefore that unlike the traditional belief of labial assimilation, there is neutralization after a labial consonant in which vowels /ɨ/ and /u/ are often realized as any sound between two vowels, /ɨ/ and /u/. That is, this vowel change is not categorial but it rather produces an ambiguous stimulus which attracts different responses from different listeners.
    [Show full text]
  • Diverse Repairs for Multiple Labial Consonants
    Nat Lang Linguist Theory (2009) 27: 197–224 DOI 10.1007/s11049-008-9061-1 ORIGINAL PAPER Diverse repairs for multiple labial consonants Kie Zuraw · Yu-An Lu Received: 19 February 2008 / Accepted: 30 November 2008 / Published online: 17 February 2009 © The Author(s) 2009. This article is published with open access at Springerlink.com Abstract The relationship between constraints on surface forms and operations that alter representations is of central interest in phonological theory. This squib presents a case of diverse “repairs” in response to a marked structure—labial . labial sequences—created by um-infixation in stems beginning with (or, in some cases, merely containing) labial consonants in Austronesian languages. We review several strategies, which for the most part do not cluster according to subfamilies: tolerance, gaps, loss of stem consonant, loss of infix nasality, stem dissimilation, infix dissimila- tion, prefixation, and non-realization of infix. The evidence indicates that avoidance of these sequences applies only within the root-and-infix domain, and only in de- rived environments. This diversity of repairs seems unexpected if changes should be perceptually minimal; we suggest possible explanations. Keywords Infixation · Austronesian · Obligatory Contour Principle · Dissimilation, heterogeneity of process · Too-many-solutions problem · Phonological typology 1 Diverse repairs for multiple labial consonants The relationship between constraints on surface forms and operations that alter rep- resentations is of central interest in phonological theory. Classic Optimality Theory (OT; Prince and Smolensky 1993/2004) predicts that, cross-linguistically, multifari- ous “repairs” can occur in response to a markedness constraint. For example, Pater (1999—though see Pater 2001) describes diverse ways in which languages avoid a K.
    [Show full text]
  • American Indian Languages (Abbreviated A), and the Alphabet of the Dialect Atlas of New England (Abbreviated D)
    CONCORDANCE OF PHONETIC ALPHABETS Robert C. Hollow, Jr. University of North Carolina, Chapel Hill A concordance of 3 major phonetic alphabets used in North America is presented and discussed. Those alphabets consid- ered are one used by the International Phonetic Association, one used for American dialectology and one used for American Indian languages. Comparisons are made in terms of vowel symbols, -consonant symbols, secondary segmental- symbols, and diacritic marks. Typewriter equivalents of standard symbols are also given. [phonetics, linguistics, North A.merica, American Indians, phonetic symbols] This paper is a brief concordance of the major phonetic alphabets currently in use by linguists and anthropologists in North America. The alphabets included are the International Phonetic Alphabet (abbreviated I in this paper), the Americanist alphabet used in the transcription of American Indian Languages (abbreviated A), and the alphabet of the Dialect Atlas of New England (abbreviated D). For convenience I have divided the concordance into five sections: 1) Primary Vowel Symbols, 2) Primary Consonant Symbols, 3) Secondary Segmental Symbols, 4) Diacritic Marks, and 5) Typewriter Symbols. The form of I used in this paper is the 1951 revision as fully presented in The Principles of the International Phonetic Association (International Phonetic Association 1957). D is presented and discussed in the Handbook of the Linguistic Geography of New England (Kurath, Bloch and Hansen 1939). This alphabet is based onI, but includes certain modifications made to facilitate the transcription of American English dialect material. There is no single phonetic alphabet currently in use by students of American Indian Languages, for this reason I have consulted several alternate formu- lations of phonetic alphabets given by scholars in the field, most notably 42 Bloch and Trager (1942), Pike (1947), Trager (1958), and Shipley (1965).
    [Show full text]
  • CHINESE PRONUNCIATION of PLOSIVES and FRICATIVES What
    Running head: CHINESE PRONUNCIATION OF PLOSIVES AND FRICATIVES What does the pronunciation of plosives and fricatives by Chinese learners of Portuguese teach us? Adelina Castelo Macao Polytechnic Institute (Portuguese Language Teaching and Research Centre) & Centre of Linguistics of University of Lisbon Taipa Campus, Macao Polytechnic Institute, Avenida Padre Tomás Pereira, Taipa, Macau – China 00853 8399 8629 [email protected] Author’s biography Adelina Castelo is an Associate Professor in Macao Polytechnic Institute (Macao SAR, China) since 2015, where she teaches courses of Portuguese Language, Language Laboratory and where she gives training sessions to teachers of Portuguese in mainland China. Prior to teaching in Macao, she earned her bachelor’s degree in Modern Languages and Literatures - Portuguese and French Studies (1999), master’s degree in Portuguese Linguistics (2004) and doctoral degree in Educational Linguistics (2012), all from University of Lisbon, and also taught Linguistics, Language Acquisition, Psycholinguistics, Mother Tongue Education and Portuguese as a Foreign Language in different colleges in Portugal. Her areas of research encompass phonology, phonological awareness, teaching of oral skills and pronunciation, and she has collaborated in several research projects CHINESE PRONUNCIATION OF PLOSIVES AND FRICATIVES 2 at the Center of Linguistics of the University of Lisbon. Adelina Castelo has also presented several communications in academic meetings, as well as published papers and books, namely a series of materials to teach Portuguese pronunciation through songs ( Sons da fala e sons do canto: música para ensinar Fonética do Português. Volume 1: Música de Portugal and Volume 2: Música do Brasil , 2017, Macao, MPI). CHINESE PRONUNCIATION OF PLOSIVES AND FRICATIVES 3 Abstract Producing didactic materials for teaching the Portuguese pronunciation to Chinese learners should take into account their specific difficulties.
    [Show full text]
  • Labial-Coronal Vs. Labial-Velar Consonant Sequences: an Articulatory Study
    Labial-Coronal vs. Labial-Velar Consonant Sequences: An Articulatory Study Manon Carrissimo-Bertola1, Nathalie Vallée1, Ioana Chitoran2 (1) Gipsa-Lab, (2) CLLILAC-ARP [email protected], nathalie.vallee@gipsa- lab.grenoble-inp.fr, [email protected] Studies on universal trends in languages, both from different genetic and geographic origins, have shown favored combinations of non-adjacent consonants in words across various syllabic structures. Regarding place of articulation, sound sequences are more often organized with a labial consonant followed by a coronal one, rather than the opposite order. McNeilage & Davis (2000) called this trend the Labial Coronal (LC) Effect, and since then, it has been observed in twenty or so language lexicons (Vallée & al. 2009, G-ULSID database). This preferred organization is attested (i) between heterosyllabic onsets of two consecutive open syllables - CVCV, (ii) between tautosyllabic onset and coda - CVC (Table1), and (iii) in children productions at the first stage of words (McNeilage & Davis, 2000). Carrissimo- Bertola (2010) looked for other favored consonant place orders, in particular a potential labial-velar (LV) trend (Table2). A labial-velar effect was hypothesized because it shares with the labial-coronal order an anterior-posterior order of constrictions, and the use of two independent articulators. No labial- velar preference was found. We therefore propose to compare the articulatory strategies for both consonant sequences according to the places of articulation in order to understand why some of them are more widespread than others. Table 1: Mean LC/CL ratios estimated in four studies (* means no data). Table 2: Mean LV/VL ratios for the 19 G-ULSID languages, and number of languages by trend (* means no data).
    [Show full text]
  • Information to Users
    INFORMATION TO USERS This manuscript has been reproduced from the microfilm master. UMI films the text directly from the original or copy submitted. Thus, some thesis and dissertation copies are in typewriter face, while others may be from any type of computer printer. The quality of this reproduction is dependent upon the quality of the copy submitted. Broken or indistinct print, colored or poor quality illustrations and photographs, print bleedthrough, substandard margins, and improper alignment can adversely affect reproduction. In the unlikely event that the author did not send UMI a complete manuscript and there are missing pages, these will be noted. Also, if unauthorized copyright material had to be removed, a note will indicate the deletion. Oversize materials (e.g., maps, drawings, charts) are reproduced by sectioning the original, beginning at the upper left-hand comer and continuing from left to right in equal sections with small overlaps. Photographs included in the original manuscript have been reproduced xerographicaily in this copy. Higher quality 6” x 9” black and white photographic prints are available for any photographs or illustrations appearing in this copy for an additional charge. Contact UMI directly to order. ProQuest Information and Learning 300 North Zeeb Road, Ann Arbor, Ml 48106-1346 USA 800-521-0600 UMI* GROUNDING JUl’HOANSI ROOT PHONOTACTICS: THE PHONETICS OF THE GUTTURAL OCP AND OTHER ACOUSTIC MODULATIONS DISSERTATION Presented in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree Doctor of Philosophy in the Graduate School of the Ohio State University By Amanda Miller-Ockhuizen, M.A. The Ohio State University 2001 Dissertation Committee: Approved by Mary.
    [Show full text]
  • Introductory Phonology
    9781405184120_1_pre.qxd 06/06/2008 09:47 AM Page iii Introductory Phonology Bruce Hayes A John Wiley & Sons, Ltd., Publication 9781405184120_4_C04.qxd 06/06/2008 09:50 AM Page 70 4 Features 4.1 Introduction to Features: Representations Feature theory is part of a general approach in cognitive science which hypo- thesizes formal representations of mental phenomena. A representation is an abstract formal object that characterizes the essential properties of a mental entity. To begin with an example, most readers of this book are familiar with the words and music of the song “Happy Birthday to You.” The question is: what is it that they know? Or, to put it very literally, what information is embodied in their neurons that distinguishes a knower of “Happy Birthday” from a hypothetical person who is identical in every other respect but does not know the song? Much of this knowledge must be abstract. People can recognize “Happy Birth- day” when it is sung in a novel key, or by an unfamiliar voice, or using a different tempo or form of musical expression. Somehow, they can ignore (or cope in some other way with) inessential traits and attend to the essential ones. The latter include the linguistic text, the (relative) pitch sequences of the notes, the relative note dura- tions, and the musical harmonies that (often tacitly) accompany the tune. Cognitive science posits that humans possess mental representations, that is, formal mental objects depicting the structure of things we know or do. A typical claim is that we are capable of singing “Happy Birthday” because we have (during childhood) internalized a mental representation, fairly abstract in character, that embodies the structure of this song.
    [Show full text]
  • Palatalization As Overlap of Articulatory Gestures: Crosslinguistic Evidence
    Job Name: -- /337544t BLS 34, No 1 2008. DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.3765/bls.v34i1.3554 (published by the Berkeley Linguistics Society and the Linguistic Society of America) Palatalization as Overlap of Articulatory Gestures: Crosslinguistic Evidence NICOLETA BATEMAN California State University, San Marcos 0. Introduction This paper addresses an asymmetry in the behavior of labial consonants with respect to two types of palatalization, full and secondary. Full palatalization involves consonants being articulated within the palatal region of the vocal tract when adjacent to a palatal vocoid such as i, e, j, as in (1). Secondary palataliza- tion involves consonants acquiring a secondary palatal articulation in the same context, as in (2): (1) Full palatalization: >NIW@ ‘open!’ (masc. sg. Imperative) (Amharic, Ethiopia) >NIL@ ‘open!’ (fem. sg. Imperative) (2) Secondary palatalization: /jamati/ >MDPDWL@ ‘a person’ (Watjarri, Australia) Palatalization has been reported to affect consonants at the three major places of articulation, labial, coronal, and dorsal, in asymmetrical ways. For example Bhat (1978), whose crosslinguistic survey presents 120 instances of various types of palatalization, finds that secondary palatalization is common for consonants at all three places of articulation, while what I call full palatalization is common for coronal and dorsal consonants, but rare for labial consonants. He reports only five languages with labial full palatalization: the Moldavian dialect of Romanian (Romance, Romania), Tswana (Southern Bantu, Botswana), Lumaasaba (Narrow Bantu, Uganda), Fula (Niger Congo, Cameroon), and Chontal (Hokan, Mexico). This suggests that labials are more resistant than coronals and dorsals to full palatalization, or shifting to coronal articulations. In addressing the issue of labial to coronal shifts, particularly those in Southern Bantu, Ohala (1978) proposes a perceptual explanation: labials followed by a palatal glide can be directly per- ceived as coronals.
    [Show full text]