Phonological and Phonetic Asymmetries of Cw Combinations A

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Phonological and Phonetic Asymmetries of Cw Combinations A Phonological and Phonetic Asymmetries of Cw Combinations A Dissertation Presented by Yunju Suh to The Graduate School in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree of Doctor of Philosophy in Linguistics Stony Brook University August 2009 Stony Brook University The Graduate School Yunju Suh We, the dissertation committee for the above candidate for the Doctor of Philosophy degree, hereby recommend acceptance of this dissertation. Marie K. Huffman – Dissertation Advisor Associate Professor, Department of Linguistics Ellen Broselow Professor, Department of Linguistics Christina Y. Bethin Professor, Department of Linguistics Edward Flemming Associate Professor, Department of Linguistics and Philosophy, Massachusetts Institute of Technology This dissertation is accepted by the Graduate School ______________________ Lawrence Martin Dean of the Graduate School ii Abstract of the Dissertation Phonological and Phonetic Asymmetries of Cw Combinations by Yunju Suh Doctor of Philosophy in Linguistics Stony Brook University 2009 This thesis investigates the relationship between the phonological distribution of Cw combinations, and the acoustic/perceptual distinctiveness between syllables with plain C onsets and with Cw combination onsets. Distributional asymmetries of Cw combinations discussed in this thesis include the avoidance of Cw combinations in the labial consonant context and before rounded or back vowels, and the preference for contrastive labialization on velar consonants. Following the claim that the phonetic salience of phonological contrasts should be taken into account in the explanation of typologically common co-occurrence restrictions (Kawasaki 1982, Flemming 1995, et seq.), I focus on showing that the salience of the contrast between CV and CwV syllables is a major factor in the asymmetric distribution of Cw combinations, with supporting evidence from acoustic and perception experiments. The results of a study on the C-Cw comparison in Korean and Spanish show that the acoustic difference between plain C and Cw combination is weaker in contexts where Cw iii combinations are avoided. In addition, cross-linguistic comparison reveals that the phonetic status of the /w/ component—a glide consonant or a secondary labialization— affects the acoustic distinctiveness of a Cw combination relative to the plain C counterpart. Perception experiment results confirm that the degree of acoustic distinctiveness between CV and CwV syllables in different consonant and vowel contexts is reflected in the strength of perceptual cues to the contrast. Based on the findings from the experimental studies, I propose an analysis of the Cw asymmetries within the Dispersion Theory of Contrast (Flemming 1995, et seq.). The asymmetric distribution of Cw combinations emerges because the contrast between CV and CwV is allowed only when they are separated by sufficient perceptual distance. Optimality-Theoretic interaction between such requirements on perceptual distance (minimal distance constraints) and constraints governing the preservation of contrast gives rise to the asymmetries in the distribution of Cw combinations observed in languages like Korean, Mandarin, Cantonese and Dan. I compare the current analysis with an alternative identity avoidance (OCP) analysis, and argue that the former is superior in terms of both descriptive and explanatory adequacies. I also show the parallel between the distributional asymmetries of Cw and Cj combinations, and extend the Dispersion-Theoretic analysis to the Cj distribution for languages like Ukrainian. iv Table of Contents List of Figures ………………………………………………………………………….. vii List of Tables ……………………………….……………………………………….…… ix Acknowledgements ..…………………………………………………………………….. x Chapter 1. Phonological and phonetic aspects of Cw asymmetries ..………………... 1 1.1 Phonological asymmetries of Cw combinations …………………………………….. 2 1.1.1 Consonant place and vowel asymmetries ………………………………….. 2 1.1.2 Consonant place and Cw status asymmetries ……………………………… 9 1.2 Relationship between phonetic salience of /w/ and Cw phonological asymmetries .. 11 1.2.1 Consonant place and vowel ……………………………….……...…..…….. 11 1.2.2 Consonant place and the status of /w/ …………………………………….. 13 1.3 Theoretical background …………………………….……………………….………… 13 1.3.1 Dispersion Theory of Contrast …………………………………………… 13 1.3.2 Dispersion-Theoretic analysis of the distribution of Cw combinations ….. 15 Chapter 2. Acoustics of Cw combinations and the C-Cw contrast ….……………... 19 2.1 Acoustic cues to the C-Cw contrast ……………………………………………….... 19 2.2 Methods …………………………………………………………………………….. 21 2.2.1 Language choice …………………………………………………….…….. 21 2.2.2 Recording and stimuli …………………………………………………….. 21 2.2.3 Analysis ………………………………………………………………….... 23 2.3 Results ……………………………………………………………………………… 24 2.3.1 Duration of vocoid ………………………………………………………... 26 2.3.2 Formants ……………………………………………………….………….. 27 2.3.2.1 F1 at vocoid onset ………………………………………………. 28 2.3.2.2 F2 at vocoid onset ………………………………………………. 30 2.3.2.3 F2 change ………………………………………………………. 34 2.3.3 Consonant release noise spectral properties ....…………………………… 40 2.3.3.1 Stop burst spectral peak location frequency ……………………. 43 2.3.3.2 Intensity difference between mid- and high-frequency in stop burst spectra …………………………………………………………… 47 2.3.3.3 Fricative noise spectral center of gravity ……………….………. 51 2.4 Summary and discussion …………………………………………………………… 54 2.4.1 Consonant place effect on the CV-CwV contrast ………………………… 54 2.4.2 Vowel effect on the CV-CwV contrast …………………………………… 55 2.4.3 Language effect on the CV-CwV contrast ……………………………….. 56 Chapter 3. Perception of the labialization contrast ………………………………… 59 3.1 Perceptual correlates of prevocalic glide and the implications for the CV-CwV contrast ……………………………………………………………………………. 59 v 3.2 Methods ……………………………………………………….……………..……... 61 3.2.1 Stimuli ……………………………………………………………………. 61 3.2.2 Participants ……………………………………………………………….. 62 3.2.3 Procedure ………………………………………………………………….. 62 3.3 Results …………………………………………………………………………...…. 63 3.3.1 Vocoid stimuli ……………………………………………………………. 64 3.3.1.1 d prime ………………………………………………………….. 64 3.3.1.2 Response time …………………………………………………… 67 3.3.2 Burst stimuli ……………………………………………………………… 68 3.3.2.1 d prime ………………………………………………………….. 68 3.3.2.2 Response time …………………………………………………… 69 3.3.3 Summary and discussion ………………………………………………… 70 3.4 Relative strength of formant transition and burst frequency cues to the CV-CwV contrast ………………………………………………………………….…………. 71 3.4.1 Formant transition cues for the labialization contrast ……………………. 72 3.4.2 Burst noise frequency cue for the labialization contrast …………………. 73 Chapter 4. A Dispersion-Theoretic analysis of the distribution of Cw combinations …………………………………………………………………………….... 76 4.1 Contrast dispersion and the asymmetries in labialization …………..……………… 76 4.1.1 Perceptibility scales of labialization contrast …………………………….. 77 4.1.2 Korean ……………………………………………………………………. 78 4.1.3 Mandarin ………………………………………………………………….. 81 4.1.4 Cantonese ……………………………………………….………………… 84 4.2 An alternative analysis: feature identity avoidance ………………………………… 85 4.2.1 Korean ……………………………………………………………………. 86 4.2.2 Mandarin ………………………………………………………………….. 90 4.3 Interaction of contrast dispersion and articulatory gesture coordination in Cw combinations ………………………………………………………………………. 91 4.4 Interaction of contrast dispersion and articulatory gesture coordination in Cj combinations ………………………………………………………………………. 97 4.5 An additional complex case: Ponapean labialized labials ………………………… 104 4.6 The role of distinctiveness scales in the loanword adaptation of Cw clusters in Korean ……………………………………………………………………………………... 105 4.7 Summary …………………………………………………………………………… 107 Chapter 5. Conclusion .…………………………………………………….…………. 109 References ……………………………………………………………………………. 111 vi List of Figures Figure 2.1 Sample spectrograms of the Korean and the Spanish Ca and Cwa syllables ….. 24 Figure 2.2 Mean duration of vocoids of the Korean and the Spanish CV and CwV syllables …………………………………………………………………….………. 27 Figure 2.3 Stylized F2 trajectories of Ka and Kwa, and the estimation of F2 difference between them in lenis and fortis contexts ……………………………………… 28 Figure 2.4 Mean F1 at vocoid onset of the Korean CV and CwV syllables ………….... 29 Figure 2.5 Mean F1 at vocoid onset of the Spanish Ce and Cwe syllables ….…………. 29 Figure 2.6 Mean F1 at vocoid onset of the Spanish Ca and Cwa syllables ….…………. 30 Figure 2.7 Mean F2 at vocoid onset of the Korean fortis-initial Ce and Cwe syllables ..... 31 Figure 2.8 Mean F2 at vocoid onset of the Korean fortis-initial Ca and Cwa syllables …. 31 Figure 2.9 Mean F2 at vocoid onset of the Korean fortis-initial Cʌ and Cwʌ syllables .… 32 Figure 2.10 Mean F2 at vocoid onset of the Spanish Ce and Cwe syllables ….………… 32 Figure 2.11 Mean F2 at vocoid onset of the Spanish Ca and Cwa syllables …….……… 33 Figure 2.12 Mean F2 at vocoid onset of the Korean and the Spanish Cwa and Cwe syllables ………………………………………………………………………… 34 Figure 2.13 Sample spectrogram of Spanish twe …………….………………………… 35 Figure 2.14 Sample spectrogram of Korean t'we …….………………………………… 35 Figure 2.15 Mean F2 change in frequency, from the onset to a later point in the Korean fortis-initial Ce and Cwe syllables ……………………………………………... 37 Figure 2.16 Mean F2 change in frequency, from the onset to a later point in the Korean fortis-initial Ca and Cwa syllables ……………………………………………... 37 Figure 2.17 Mean F2 change in frequency, from the onset to a later point in the Korean fortis-initial Cʌ and Cwʌ syllables ……………………………………………... 38
Recommended publications
  • Phonation Types of Korean Fricatives and Affricates
    ISSN 2005-8063 2017. 12. 31. 말소리와 음성과학 Vol.9 No.4 pp. 51-57 http://dx.doi.org/10.13064/KSSS.2017.9.4.051 Phonation types of Korean fricatives and affricates Goun Lee* Abstract The current study compared the acoustic features of the two phonation types for Korean fricatives (plain: /s/, fortis : /s’/) and the three types for affricates (aspirated : /tsʰ/, lenis : /ts/, and fortis : /ts’/) in order to determine the phonetic status of the plain fricative /s/. Considering the different manners of articulation between fricatives and affricates, we examined four acoustic parameters (rise time, intensity, fundamental frequency, and Cepstral Peak Prominence (CPP) values) of the 20 Korean native speakers’ productions. The results showed that unlike Korean affricates, F0 cannot distinguish two fricatives, and voice quality (CPP values) only distinguishes phonation types of Korean fricatives and affricates by grouping non-fortis sibilants together. Therefore, based on the similarity found in /tsʰ/ and /ts/ and the idiosyncratic pattern found in /s/, this research concludes that non-fortis fricative /s/ cannot be categorized as belonging to either phonation type. Keywords: Korean fricatives, Korean affricates, phonation type, acoustic characteristics, breathy voice 1. Introduction aspirated-like characteristic by remaining voiceless in intervocalic position. Whereas the lenis stops become voiced in intervocalic Korean has very well established three-way contrasts (e.g., position (e.g., /pata/ → / [pada] ‘sea’), the aspirated stops do not aspirated, lenis, and fortis) both in stops and affricates in three undergo intervocalic voicing (e.g., /patʰaŋ/ → [patʰaŋ] places of articulation (bilabial, alveolar, velar). However, this ‘foundation’). Since the plain fricative /s/, like the aspirated stops, distinction does not occur in fricatives, leaving only a two-way does not undergo intervocalic voicing, categorizing the plain contrast between fortis fricative /s’/ and non-fortis (hereafter, plain) fricative as aspirated is also possible.
    [Show full text]
  • Mehri Ejective Fricatives: an Acoustic Study Rachid Ridouane, Cédric Gendrot, Rajesh Khatiwada
    Mehri ejective fricatives: an acoustic study Rachid Ridouane, Cédric Gendrot, Rajesh Khatiwada To cite this version: Rachid Ridouane, Cédric Gendrot, Rajesh Khatiwada. Mehri ejective fricatives: an acoustic study. 18th International Congress of Phonetic Sciences, Aug 2015, Glasgow, United Kingdom. halshs- 01287685 HAL Id: halshs-01287685 https://halshs.archives-ouvertes.fr/halshs-01287685 Submitted on 15 Mar 2016 HAL is a multi-disciplinary open access L’archive ouverte pluridisciplinaire HAL, est archive for the deposit and dissemination of sci- destinée au dépôt et à la diffusion de documents entific research documents, whether they are pub- scientifiques de niveau recherche, publiés ou non, lished or not. The documents may come from émanant des établissements d’enseignement et de teaching and research institutions in France or recherche français ou étrangers, des laboratoires abroad, or from public or private research centers. publics ou privés. Mehri ejective fricatives: an acoustic study Rachid Ridouane, Cédric Gendrot, Rajesh Khatiwada Laboratoire de Phonétique et Phonologie (CNRS/Sorbonne Nouvelle), Paris ABSTRACT sequence of a pulmonic fricative followed by a glottal stop [3]. The second strategy, observed in Ejective consonants are not very common cross- Tlingit, was to produce ejective fricatives with a linguistically. Even less common is the occurrence much narrower constriction than was used in their of ejective fricatives. This infrequency is generally pulmonic counterparts [4]. This allowed for glottal attributed to the incompatibility of two aerodynamic closure to overlap the entire frication duration while requirements: a continuing flow of air to create noise producing high intra-oral pressure, suggesting that frication and an increasing intraoral air pressure to they were indeed ejective fricatives.
    [Show full text]
  • An Acoustic Comparison of Russian and English Sibilant Fricatives
    An acoustic comparison of Russian and English sibilant fricatives Russian is known to have a 4-way place and secondary articulation contrast in voiceless sibilant fricatives, as, for example, in [sok] <сок> ‘juice’ vs. [sjok] <сёк> ‘whipped’ vs. [ʂok] <шок> ‘shock’ vs. [ʃjok] <щёк> ‘cheeks (gen.)’ (Avanesov 1972; Timberlake 2004). These consonants have been previously noted to be different from the corresponding English fricatives /s/ and /ʃ/. The classical phonetic descriptive account of Russian sounds by Jones & Ward (1969; pp. 125- 134) mentions that the Russian non-palatalized anterior /s/ has a “slightly lower pitch” compared to the English /s/, likely reflecting some constriction differences (laminal dental vs. apical alveolar) and presence or absence of secondary velarization. The Russian palatalized anterior /sj/ is noted to be similar to the (British) English /s/ + /j/ sequence (as in assume), yet also showing some differences in “pitch” (higher than in English). The Russian /ʂ/, according to the authors, exhibits a “characteristic ‘dark’ or ‘hollow’ property”, an apparent result of the raised tongue tip, flattened tongue body, and rounded lips. This makes the sound particularly different from English /ʃ/, which has a “somewhat palatalized” quality resulting from a moderate raising of the tongue front. The degree of palatalization of the English /ʃ/, according to Jones & Ward (1969), however, is substantially smaller than for the strongly palatalized Russian /ʃj/. This auditorily- based comparison is undoubtedly useful for learners of Russian, yet it is not clear which specific acoustic properties these description represents. For example, Jones & Ward’s the non-standard use of the term ‘pitch’ could refer to differences in spectral means of fricative noise or differences in the formants of an adjacent vowel.
    [Show full text]
  • A Corpus Study of the 3 Tone Sandhi in Standard Chinese
    A Corpus Study of the 3 rd Tone Sandhi in Standard Chinese Yiya Chen 1, Jiahong Yuan 2 1 Department of Linguistics, Radboud University Nijmegen 2 Department of Linguistics, University of Pennsylvania [email protected], [email protected] (Zhang 1988, Shih 1997, M. Chen 2000, Chen 2003, Chen Abstract 2004). Speer et al. (1989) show that listeners are indeed In Standard Chinese, a Low tone (Tone3) is often realized sensitive to a constituent’s phrasal structure in judging the application of the 3 rd tone Sandhi to constituents which could with a rising F0 contour before another Low tone, known as the 3rd tone Sandhi. This study investigates the acoustic be ambiguous between an underlying Rising tone and a characteristics of the 3rd tone Sandhi in Standard Chinese Sandhi Rising tone. Their results suggest the possibility that the higher linguistic boundary it is between two Low tones, using a large telephone conversation speech corpus. Sandhi rd Rising was found to be different from the underlying Rising the less likely the 3 tone sandhi rule is applied. With regard tone (Tone2) in bi-syllabic words in two measures: the to the difference between the underlying Rising tone and the Sandhi Rising tone, Peng (2000) show that the F0 maximum magnitude of the F 0 rising and the time span of the F 0 rising. We also found different effects of word frequency on Sandhi of SR is lower than R. Furthermore, in fast speech, a Sandhi Rising and the underlying Rising tones. Finally, for tri- Rising tone may flatten and show no apparent F0 rise (Kuo, syllabic constituents with Low tone only, constituent Xu, and Yip, to appear).
    [Show full text]
  • A Brief Description of Consonants in Modern Standard Arabic
    Linguistics and Literature Studies 2(7): 185-189, 2014 http://www.hrpub.org DOI: 10.13189/lls.2014.020702 A Brief Description of Consonants in Modern Standard Arabic Iram Sabir*, Nora Alsaeed Al-Jouf University, Sakaka, KSA *Corresponding Author: [email protected] Copyright © 2014 Horizon Research Publishing All rights reserved. Abstract The present study deals with “A brief Modern Standard Arabic. This study starts from an description of consonants in Modern Standard Arabic”. This elucidation of the phonetic bases of sounds classification. At study tries to give some information about the production of this point shows the first limit of the study that is basically Arabic sounds, the classification and description of phonetic rather than phonological description of sounds. consonants in Standard Arabic, then the definition of the This attempt of classification is followed by lists of the word consonant. In the present study we also investigate the consonant sounds in Standard Arabic with a key word for place of articulation in Arabic consonants we describe each consonant. The criteria of description are place and sounds according to: bilabial, labio-dental, alveolar, palatal, manner of articulation and voicing. The attempt of velar, uvular, and glottal. Then the manner of articulation, description has been made to lead to the drawing of some the characteristics such as phonation, nasal, curved, and trill. fundamental conclusion at the end of the paper. The aim of this study is to investigate consonant in MSA taking into consideration that all 28 consonants of Arabic alphabets. As a language Arabic is one of the most 2.
    [Show full text]
  • LINGUISTICS 221 LECTURE #3 the BASIC SOUNDS of ENGLISH 1. STOPS a Stop Consonant Is Produced with a Complete Closure of Airflow
    LINGUISTICS 221 LECTURE #3 Introduction to Phonetics and Phonology THE BASIC SOUNDS OF ENGLISH 1. STOPS A stop consonant is produced with a complete closure of airflow in the vocal tract; the air pressure has built up behind the closure; the air rushes out with an explosive sound when released. The term plosive is also used for oral stops. ORAL STOPS: e.g., [b] [t] (= plosives) NASAL STOPS: e.g., [m] [n] (= nasals) There are three phases of stop articulation: i. CLOSING PHASE (approach or shutting phase) The articulators are moving from an open state to a closed state; ii. CLOSURE PHASE (= occlusion) Blockage of the airflow in the oral tract; iii. RELEASE PHASE Sudden reopening; it may be accompanied by a burst of air. ORAL STOPS IN ENGLISH a. BILABIAL STOPS: The blockage is made with the two lips. spot [p] voiceless baby [b] voiced 1 b. ALVEOLAR STOPS: The blade (or the tip) of the tongue makes a closure with the alveolar ridge; the sides of the tongue are along the upper teeth. lamino-alveolar stops or Check your apico-alveolar stops pronunciation! stake [t] voiceless deep [d] voiced c. VELAR STOPS: The closure is between the back of the tongue (= dorsum) and the velum. dorso-velar stops scar [k] voiceless goose [g] voiced 2. NASALS (= nasal stops) The air is stopped in the oral tract, but the velum is lowered so that the airflow can go through the nasal tract. All nasals are voiced. NASALS IN ENGLISH a. BILABIAL NASAL: made [m] b. ALVEOLAR NASAL: need [n] c.
    [Show full text]
  • Phonological Processes
    Phonological Processes Phonological processes are patterns of articulation that are developmentally appropriate in children learning to speak up until the ages listed below. PHONOLOGICAL PROCESS DESCRIPTION AGE ACQUIRED Initial Consonant Deletion Omitting first consonant (hat → at) Consonant Cluster Deletion Omitting both consonants of a consonant cluster (stop → op) 2 yrs. Reduplication Repeating syllables (water → wawa) Final Consonant Deletion Omitting a singleton consonant at the end of a word (nose → no) Unstressed Syllable Deletion Omitting a weak syllable (banana → nana) 3 yrs. Affrication Substituting an affricate for a nonaffricate (sheep → cheep) Stopping /f/ Substituting a stop for /f/ (fish → tish) Assimilation Changing a phoneme so it takes on a characteristic of another sound (bed → beb, yellow → lellow) 3 - 4 yrs. Velar Fronting Substituting a front sound for a back sound (cat → tat, gum → dum) Backing Substituting a back sound for a front sound (tap → cap) 4 - 5 yrs. Deaffrication Substituting an affricate with a continuant or stop (chip → sip) 4 yrs. Consonant Cluster Reduction (without /s/) Omitting one or more consonants in a sequence of consonants (grape → gape) Depalatalization of Final Singles Substituting a nonpalatal for a palatal sound at the end of a word (dish → dit) 4 - 6 yrs. Stopping of /s/ Substituting a stop sound for /s/ (sap → tap) 3 ½ - 5 yrs. Depalatalization of Initial Singles Substituting a nonpalatal for a palatal sound at the beginning of a word (shy → ty) Consonant Cluster Reduction (with /s/) Omitting one or more consonants in a sequence of consonants (step → tep) Alveolarization Substituting an alveolar for a nonalveolar sound (chew → too) 5 yrs.
    [Show full text]
  • Phonological Use of the Larynx: a Tutorial Jacqueline Vaissière
    Phonological use of the larynx: a tutorial Jacqueline Vaissière To cite this version: Jacqueline Vaissière. Phonological use of the larynx: a tutorial. Larynx 97, 1994, Marseille, France. pp.115-126. halshs-00703584 HAL Id: halshs-00703584 https://halshs.archives-ouvertes.fr/halshs-00703584 Submitted on 3 Jun 2012 HAL is a multi-disciplinary open access L’archive ouverte pluridisciplinaire HAL, est archive for the deposit and dissemination of sci- destinée au dépôt et à la diffusion de documents entific research documents, whether they are pub- scientifiques de niveau recherche, publiés ou non, lished or not. The documents may come from émanant des établissements d’enseignement et de teaching and research institutions in France or recherche français ou étrangers, des laboratoires abroad, or from public or private research centers. publics ou privés. Vaissière, J., (1997), "Phonological use of the larynx: a tutorial", Larynx 97, Marseille, 115-126. PHONOLOGICAL USE OF THE LARYNX J. Vaissière UPRESA-CNRS 1027, Institut de Phonétique, Paris, France larynx used as a carrier of paralinguistic information . RÉSUMÉ THE PRIMARY FUNCTION OF THE LARYNX Cette communication concerne le rôle du IS PROTECTIVE larynx dans l'acte de communication. Toutes As stated by Sapir, 1923, les langues du monde utilisent des physiologically, "speech is an overlaid configurations caractéristiques du larynx, aux function, or to be more precise, a group of niveaux segmental, lexical, et supralexical. Nous présentons d'abord l'utilisation des différents types de phonation pour distinguer entre les consonnes et les voyelles dans les overlaid functions. It gets what service it can langues du monde, et également du larynx out of organs and functions, nervous and comme lieu d'articulation des glottales, et la muscular, that come into being and are production des éjectives et des implosives.
    [Show full text]
  • Vowel Acoustics Reliably Differentiate Three Coronal Stops of Wubuy Across Prosodic Contexts
    Vowel acoustics reliably differentiate three coronal stops of Wubuy across prosodic contexts Rikke L. BundgaaRd-nieLsena, BRett J. BakeRb, ChRistian kRoosa, MaRk haRveyc and CatheRine t. Besta,d aMARCS Auditory Laboratories, University of Western Sydney bSchool of Languages and Linguistics, University of Melbourne cSchool of Humanities and Social Science, University of Newcastle dHaskins Laboratories, New Haven Abstract The present study investigates the acoustic differentiation of three coronal stops in the indigenous Australian language Wubuy. We test independent claims that only VC (vowel-into-consonant) transitions provide robust acoustic cues for retroflex as compared to alveolar and dental coronal stops, with no differentiating cues among these three coronal stops evident in CV (consonant-into-vowel) transitions. The four-way stop distinction /t, t̪ , ʈ, c/ in Wubuy is contrastive word-initially (Heath 1984) and by implication utterance-initially, i.e., in CV-only contexts, which suggests that acoustic differentiation should be expected to occur in the CV transitions of this language, including in initial positions. Therefore, we examined both VC and CV formant transition information in the three target coronal stops across VCV (word-internal), V#CV (word-initial but utterance-medial) and ##CV (word- and utterance-initial), for /a / vowel contexts, which provide the optimal environment for investigating formant transitions. Results confirm that these coro- nal contrasts are maintained in the CVs in this vowel context, and in all three posi- tions. The patterns of acoustic differences across the three syllable contexts also provide some support for a systematic role of prosodic boundaries in influencing the degree of coronal stop differentiation evident in the vowel formant transitions.
    [Show full text]
  • Part 1: Introduction to The
    PREVIEW OF THE IPA HANDBOOK Handbook of the International Phonetic Association: A guide to the use of the International Phonetic Alphabet PARTI Introduction to the IPA 1. What is the International Phonetic Alphabet? The aim of the International Phonetic Association is to promote the scientific study of phonetics and the various practical applications of that science. For both these it is necessary to have a consistent way of representing the sounds of language in written form. From its foundation in 1886 the Association has been concerned to develop a system of notation which would be convenient to use, but comprehensive enough to cope with the wide variety of sounds found in the languages of the world; and to encourage the use of thjs notation as widely as possible among those concerned with language. The system is generally known as the International Phonetic Alphabet. Both the Association and its Alphabet are widely referred to by the abbreviation IPA, but here 'IPA' will be used only for the Alphabet. The IPA is based on the Roman alphabet, which has the advantage of being widely familiar, but also includes letters and additional symbols from a variety of other sources. These additions are necessary because the variety of sounds in languages is much greater than the number of letters in the Roman alphabet. The use of sequences of phonetic symbols to represent speech is known as transcription. The IPA can be used for many different purposes. For instance, it can be used as a way to show pronunciation in a dictionary, to record a language in linguistic fieldwork, to form the basis of a writing system for a language, or to annotate acoustic and other displays in the analysis of speech.
    [Show full text]
  • SSC: the Science of Talking
    SSC: The Science of Talking (for year 1 students of medicine) Week 3: Sounds of the World’s Languages (vowels and consonants) Michael Ashby, Senior Lecturer in Phonetics, UCL PLIN1101 Introduction to Phonetics and Phonology A Lecture 4 page 1 Vowel Description Essential reading: Ashby & Maidment, Chapter 5 4.1 Aim: To introduce the basics of vowel description and the main characteristics of the vowels of RP English. 4.2 Definition of vowel: Vowels are produced without any major obstruction of the airflow; the intra-oral pressure stays low, and vowels are therefore sonorant sounds. Vowels are normally voiced. Vowels are articulated by raising some part of the tongue body (that is the front or the back of the tongue notnot the tip or blade) towards the roof of the oral cavity (see Figure 1). 4.3 Front vowels are produced by raising the front of the tongue towards the hard palate. Back vowels are produced by raising the back of the tongue towards the soft palate. Central vowels are produced by raising the centre part of the tongue towards the junction of the hard and soft palates. 4.4 The height of a vowel refers to the degree of raising of the relevant part of the tongue. If the tongue is raised so as to be close to the roof of the oral cavity then a close or high vowel is produced. If the tongue is only slightly raised, so that there is a wide gap between its highest point and the roof of the oral cavity, then an open or lowlowlow vowel results.
    [Show full text]
  • Issues in the Distribution of the Glottal Stop
    Theoretical Issues in the Representation of the Glottal Stop in Blackfoot* Tyler Peterson University of British Columbia 1.0 Introduction This working paper examines the phonemic and phonetic status and distribution of the glottal stop in Blackfoot.1 Observe the phonemic distribution (1), four phonological operations (2), and three surface realizations (3) of the glottal stop: (1) a. otsi ‘to swim’ c. otsi/ ‘to splash with water’ b. o/tsi ‘to take’ (2) a. Metathesis: V/VC ® VV/C nitáó/mai/takiwa nit-á/-omai/takiwa 1-INCHOAT-believe ‘now I believe’ b. Metathesis ® Deletion: V/VùC ® VVù/C ® VVùC kátaookaawaatsi káta/-ookaa-waatsi INTERROG-sponsor.sundance-3s.NONAFFIRM ‘Did she sponsor a sundance?’ (Frantz, 1997: 154) c. Metathesis ® Degemination: V/V/C ® VV//C ® VV/C áó/tooyiniki á/-o/too-yiniki INCHOAT-arrive(AI)-1s/2s ‘when I/you arrive’ d. Metathesis ® Deletion ® V-lengthening: V/VC ® VV/C ® VVùC kátaoottakiwaatsi káta/-ottaki-waatsi INTEROG-bartender-3s.NONAFFIRM ‘Is he a bartender?’ (Frantz, 1997) (3) Surface realizations: [V/C] ~ [VV0C] ~ [VùC] aikai/ni ‘He dies’ a. [aIkaI/ni] b. [aIkaII0ni] c. [aIkajni] The glottal stop appears to have a unique status within the Blackfoot consonant inventory. The examples in (1) (and §2.1 below) suggest that it appears as a fully contrastive phoneme in the language. However, the glottal stop is put through variety of phonological processes (metathesis, syncope and degemination) that no other consonant in the language is subject to. Also, it has a variety of surfaces realizations in the form of glottalization on an adjacent vowel (cf.
    [Show full text]