SUBA‑SIMBITI ORTHOGRAPHY STATEMENT

SIL International Uganda- Branch Suba‑Simbiti Orthography Statement

Approved Orthography Edition

Acknowledgements

Many individuals contributed to this document by formatting the structure, contributing the language data, organizing the data, writing the document and by giving advice for editing the document. This document was authored by Rebekah Overton and John B. Walker, and it is an updated and expanded version of the Suba‑Simbiti Orthography Sketch. Other SIL members who have been involved in the development of the Suba-Simbiti orthography include Dave Brewerton, Alison Compton, Leila Schroeder and Oliver Stegen, as well as Holly Robinson and Brigitte Niederseer, who reviewed and helped to improve this document. Contributors from the Suba‑Simbiti language community include Albinus Waynse and Robert Mago (Simbiti Bible translators), as well as Julius Kishërë Mwita, Wilfred Matesi, Grace Barasa, Jackson Mangarai, Stephen Bwana, Joyce Kazi, Peter Mwita Nyakamande, Dorka Kimera, Vincent Marwa, and Stephen Shirati.

© SIL International Uganda-Tanzania Branch

P.O. Box 1235 Musoma, Tanzania

P.O. Box 60368 Dar Es Salaam, Tanzania

P.O. Box 750 Entebbe, Uganda

P.O. Box 44456 00100 Nairobi,

Approved Orthography Edition: July 2018

2

Table of Contents

Abbreviations ...... 5 1. Introduction ...... 6 1.1 Classification ...... 6 1.2 Data ...... 6 1.3 Dialects ...... 6 1.4 Format and Purpose of this Orthography Statement ...... 8 2. Phonology ...... 8 2.1 Vowels ...... 8 2.1.1 Phonemic Vowel Length ...... 9 2.1.2 Vowel Length in Conditioned Environments ...... 10 2.1.3 Vowel Length in Noun Class Prefixes ...... 13 2.2 Consonants ...... 14 2.2.1 Palatalisation ...... 16 2.2.2 Labialisation ...... 18 2.2.3 Prenasalisation ...... 18 2.3 Tone ...... 19 3. Morphophonology ...... 22 3.1 Vowels ...... 22 3.1.1 Vowel Harmony Processes ...... 22 3.1.2 Vowel Adjacency at Morpheme Boundaries ...... 28 3.1.3 Word-Initial ...... 35 3.2 Consonants ...... 38 3.2.1 Dahl’s Law ...... 38 3.2.2 Elision of /ɾ/ ...... 38 3.2.3 1SG Nasal Prefix ...... 39 3.3 Morphophonological Process between Verb Suffixes ...... 40 3.3.1 Creation of the Trill ...... 40 3.3.2 Applicative with Anterior ...... 42 3.3.3 Causative with Anterior ...... 43 4. Word Boundaries ...... 43 4.1 Copula ...... 43 4.2 Locatives ...... 47 4.3 Associatives ...... 49

3

4.4 Conjunction ...... 51 4.5 Negative Clitics ...... 51 4.6 Question Words...... 53 4.7 Adverbial ...... 54 4.8 Reduplication ...... 54 4.9 Compound Words ...... 57 5. Loan Words ...... 58 6. Punctuation ...... 59 6.1 Sentence-Final Punctuation...... 59 6.2 Comma ...... 59 6.3 Colon ...... 60 6.4 Quotation Marks ...... 60 7. Capitalisation ...... 61 References ...... 63 Appendix A: Suba‑Simbiti Noun Class Chart ...... 64 Appendix B: Noun Class Pairings ...... 67 Appendix C: The History of Suba-Simbiti Orthography Development ...... 68

4

Abbreviations

1 1st person, or Noun Class 1 FOC Focus Marker 2 2nd person, or Noun Class 2 FV Final Vowel 3 3rd person, or Noun Class 3 HAB Habitual 4 Noun Class 4 HYP Hypothetical 5 Noun Class 5 INF Infinitive 6 Noun Class 6 N Nasal 7 Noun Class 7 NAR Narrative 8 Noun Class 8 NEG Negative 9 Noun Class 9 P2 Remote Past (before today) 10 Noun Class 10 P3 Untimed Past 11 Noun Class 11 PL Plural 12 Noun Class 12 (diminutive) PRS Present 14 Noun Class 14 PST Past 15 Noun Class 15 REFL Reflexive 16 Noun Class 16 SBJV Subjunctive 17 Noun Class 17 SG Singular 18 Noun Class 18 V Vowel ANT Anterior1 APPLIC Applicative ATR Advanced Tongue Root C Consonant CAUS Causative F1 Immediate Future F2 Today’s Future

1 This term is taken from Walker’s (2013) analysis. 5

1. Introduction

1.1 Classification Suba‑Simbiti is a Bantu language spoken in the Mara Region of Tanzania. The ISO 639-3 code for Suba‑Simbiti is [ssc] (Simons and Fennig, 2018), and it should not be confused with the Suba language of Kenya [sxb], classed as JE403 by Maho (2009). It is part of the Lacustrine languages, a name given because of the proximity to . The language name Suba‑Simbiti includes a number of language varieties (see §1.3 below).

1.2 Data The data in this study comes from a variety of sources, including the Suba‑Simbiti people who attended numerous orthography development and orthography testing workshops in the Mara Region between 2006 and 2009. There has also been ongoing research conducted by SIL linguists and Suba-Simbiti Bible translators. Trial orthography status was granted to Suba‑Simbiti in 2009, as described in the “Suba‑Simbiti Orthography Sketch” (Compton and Walker, 2010). Additional orthography refinement and testing was carried out among Suba‑Simbiti speakers over the years, and changes and additions based on that research are included in this document. The SIL linguists are very grateful to all the Suba‑Simbiti people who have participated in this process.

1.3 Dialects The name “Suba” is an umbrella term which includes several related ethnolinguistic groups. Table 1.3 below lists each of these groups, including Maho’s (2009) classification (which exists for some groups but not others), as well as population figures (Simons and Fennig, 2018).

Table 1.3: Suba language varieties Language variety Population (2011 estimate) Simbiti (JE431) 33,000 Hacha (JE432) 4,200 Surwa (JE433) 4,200 Sweta (JE434) 12,800 Kine 14,500

6

Language variety Population (2011 estimate) Rieri2 unknown

Simbiti is by far the largest of these groups. It is the reference dialect chosen for written materials intended to serve the Suba people, thus the name “Suba-Simbiti”. In Swahili, the language name is “Kisimbiti”, and in Simbiti it is “Ikisimbëtë”. A 2011 sociolinguistic survey confirmed close ties between these Suba groups, a high level of mutual intelligibility, and a general willingness for each of them to use Simbiti written materials (Mitterhofer and Robinson, 2012). However, the survey also confirmed some significant linguistic differences between some of the language varieties, including different vowel inventories, regular sound changes, and some vocabulary differences. Therefore, current language development and translation activities continue to target the Suba group as a whole, while also acknowledging that some of the Suba groups might be better served in the future by materials in their own language varieties, dependant on community interest and resources. Simbiti itself has some dialectal differences, with some speakers influenced by proximity to the area, and others influenced by Sweta. The most notable dialect difference throughout the Simbiti language area is pronunciation of the class 10 prefix. The form used in the standardised orthography is /sɛ-/, realised as , which represents the speech of the Makutano/Irienyi/Komuge area, which is in the centre of the Simbiti language area.3 There is also great similarity between Simbiti and the languages of the nearby Kiroba and Iregi people. Kiroba and Iregi are currently listed under the same [ssc] language code, and they are indeed linguistically similar to Simbiti. They do not, however, consider themselves to be part of the “Suba” group, so for sociolinguistic reasons these two groups might be most ideally served by separate written materials.

2 The Rieri are ethnically Suba, but most Rieri have adopted the Luo language as their mother tongue, so they have not been significantly involved in Suba-Simbiti orthography development activities. 3 Note that this is different to the recommendation received from some Simbiti speakers during the 2011 survey. At that time, some suggested following the more northern dialect spoken in the Kisumwa area, which has the prefix pronunciation instead of . However, further research and discussion has shown that is also a very acceptable option which actually represents the speech of more Simbiti speakers. 7

1.4 Format and Purpose of this Orthography Statement The purpose of this orthography statement is to show the approved orthography of Suba‑Simbiti and the reasons behind the orthographic decisions that have been made. Although there is phonological and grammatical information in this paper, the primary intent is to give necessary documentation for orthography decisions. All data is written using the orthography itself, unless otherwise stated. Orthographic brackets <> are used when necessary, but any data not in brackets should be assumed to be in orthographic form. Slashes // are used to indicate underlying phonemes, and square brackets [] the phonetic form. All glosses are in both Swahili, the national language of Tanzania, and English. The following sections focus on phonology (§2), morphophonology (§3), word-boundary rules (§4), loan words (§5), punctuation (§6), and capitalisation (§7).

2. Phonology

2.1 Vowels Suba‑Simbiti has seven vowel phonemes plus contrastive length. However, different vowel systems are displayed in nouns and verbs. Nouns have a seven-vowel system, with one set of high-vowels, two sets of mid-vowels, and one low central vowel: , phonemically /i, u, e, o, ɛ, ɔ, a/ respectively. Verbs, on the other hand, show a five-vowel system in stems, , with allophonic variations of the mid-vowels to when certain verbal suffixes are added (see §3.1.1.3 on vowel harmony in verbal suffixes). Evidence of all vowel phonemes, not regarding length or tone, is found in the following bisyllabic roots with identical vowels (i.e. V1=V2), shown in Table 2.1 below.

Table 2.1: Vowel phoneme inventory Phoneme Grapheme Orthography4 Swahili English i i urusiri kamba rope e ë ëngërë ngiri boar ɛ e engere aina ya samaki type of fish a a enkara kiroboto flea

4 The stem of each word in highlighted in bold. 8

Phoneme Grapheme Orthography4 Swahili English ɔ o rikongo ugali mgumu stiff ugali (maize meal) o ö riköngö nchi kame barren land u u umukungu mzee wa kike elder woman

There are minimal pairs for the mid-vowel pairs and , as shown in the table above. See §3.1.1.2 concerning vowel harmony in prefixes. The vowel combination also appears root internally in twelve words in the data set, including the word ‘mamba’ ‘crocodile’. It seems to be a diphthong, functioning as a single phoneme in a way comparable to long vowels (Overton, 2018). (See §2.3 and §3.1.2.1 for details of when the vowel sequence is written.)

2.1.1 Phonemic Vowel Length

Vowel length is contrastive in Suba‑Simbiti, and there are minimal pairs which are distinguished only by length (or near minimal pairs where the roots are contrastive but are in different classes). The following table gives evidence of this contrastive vowel length. Long vowels are represented by writing the vowel grapheme twice.

Table 2.1.1a: Vowel length minimal pairs Vowel Short Vowel Swahili /English Long Vowel Swahili /English i /i/ ukusika kuangamia /to disappear ukusiika kufunga /to close ugonjwa wa mnyama kiwete /crippled ë /e/ öröbhëru ömöbhëëru /type of animal disease person aina ya mtego wa e /ɛ/ ritera pembe /horn riteera samaki /type of fish trap a /a/ okobhasha kuvamia /to attack okobhaasha kuchonga /to carve o /ɔ/ okoghota kuzeeka /to age okoghoota kushika /to grasp ö /o/ righöti shingo /neck righööti kodi /tax laika (ya wanyama) upande wa bega u /u/ ribhuri ibhuuri /body hair (animal) /side of the shoulder

9

Table 2.1.1b below gives examples of phonemic long vowels in different syllable structures.5 All syllables in Suba‑Simbiti are open, as is normal for . Long vowels can occur in adjacent syllables but never word finally.

Table 2.1.1b: Vowel length in different syllable structures Stem syllable Stem syllable Simbiti Swahili English count structure 1 CV öröme umande dew V.CV umwibhi mwizi thief 2 CV.CV ribhina muziki music CVV.CV ikimiira pokeo tradition CV.CV.CV ikibhisirë methali proverb CVV.CV.CV obhoreenero ngazi ladder 3 CV.CVV.CV ikisukaare ndizi banana CVV.CVV.CV ëkësëëkaani chemichemi spring of water CV.CV.CV.CV ikibhurughusi aina ya mjusi type of lizard CVV.CV.CV.CV etaabharanda kivunjavunja praying mantis 4 aina ya mmea type of CV.CVV.CV.CV ritaraatora wa dawa medicinal plant CVV.CV.CVV.CV ëkëkëërikëëri aina ya mmea type of plant

2.1.2 Vowel Length in Conditioned Environments

Suba‑Simbiti also has conditioned long vowels, which are found after labialised or palatalised consonants, before prenasalised consonants, and before the alveolar trill. In these environments, phonetically the vowels sound longer; this means that the vowel length contrast is neutralised in these environments. Therefore, within roots (of both nouns and verbs), even though these conditioned vowels are phonetically lengthened to some degree, they are not phonemically long and are written short. Table 2.1.2a gives examples of each of these environments, showing the words written according to the aforementioned rule.

5 For the purpose of this description, I have analysed prenasalised consonants as units, as opposed to sequential consonant clusters. 10

Table 2.1.2a: Vowel length in conditioned environments within roots Orthography Phonetic Phonemic Swahili English Prenasalisation ekeghambo [ɛkɛɣaːmbɔ] /ɛkɛ-ɣambɔ/ lugha language tobacco pipe Labialisation ikikwabhë [ikikwaːβe] /ɛkɛ-kwaβe/ kiko /elbow Palatalisation umubhyara [umuβjaːɾa] /ɔmɔ-βjaɾa/ binamu cousin Alveolar trill6 omorro [ɔmɔːrɔ] /ɔmɔ-rɔ/ moto fire

Exceptions to this rule are across morpheme boundaries in inflected verbs: for example, if a vowel-initial stem has a prenasalised consonant, the vowel of a subject prefix might join with the stem-initial vowel to create a derived long vowel. It is desirable to preserve the vowels of the underlying morphemes wherever possible, since vowel length in verbs often carries grammatical meaning, sometimes distinguishing tense or aspect. In verbs therefore, multiple vowels are written if there are multiple underlying morphemes, even in a seemingly conditioned environment. Some examples are shown in Table 2.1.2b below. (See §2.3 for examples of where writing derived long vowels can differentiate tenses orthographically.)

Table 2.1.2b: Long vowels written in conditioned environments Orthography Phonetic Phonemic Swahili English araanga [aɾaːŋga] /a-ɾa-aŋg-a/ anakataa he is refusing 3SG-PRS-refuse-FV bhaandeke [βaːndɛkɛ] /βa-andɛk-ɛ/ waandike they should 3PL-write-SBJV write tweeghya [tweːɣja] /tɔ-a-ɛɣ-i-a/ tulifundisha we taught (P2) 1PL-PST-teach-CAUS-FV

Certain verb roots have a phonemic long vowel followed by /ɾ/, and very often the addition of certain suffixes (e.g. Applicative <‑er>, Anterior <‑ire>) causes a trill /r/ to be formed. Since the long vowel is underlying present, not present only phonetically because of the conditioned environment before the trill, the long vowel is written even in forms with the trill:

6 The trill is often formed from the earlier elision of a vowel between two taps . This can be clearly seen by comparing the word omorro with the same stem in other Mara languages: Kabwa omururo; Ikizu omurero; Kwaya, Zanaki and Jita omuriro; Ikoma omororo. Nurse and Philippson (2006) give the Proto-Bantu as *mʊ-lɪlo. 11

Table 2.1.2c: Long vowels before trill Orthography Phonetic Phonemic Swahili English ukwinaara [ukwinaːɾa] /ɔkɔ-inaːɾ-a/ kuzunguka to go around INF-go.around-FV bhainaarrë [βainaːre] /βa-inaːɾ-iɾe/ wamezunguka they have gone 3PL-go.around-ANT around ukukuura [ukukuːɾa] /uku-kuːɾ-a/ kulia to cry INF-cry-FV bhakuurrë [βakuːre] /βa-kuːɾ-iɾe/ wamelia they have cried 3PL-cry-ANT

Usually no more than two adjacent vowels are written, since, even if there are underlyingly more than two vowels, the resulting vowel is still recognised as a normal long vowel – Suba‑Simbiti does not have phonemically ‘extra-long’ vowels. More than two adjacent vowels are written only in the combination ; see §2.3 for an explanation of this. Table 2.1.2d below shows examples of where two vowels are written for a long vowel, but there are three vowels underlyingly:

Table 2.1.2d: Absence of ‘extra-long’ vowels Orthography Phonetic Phonemic Swahili English bhaandëkirë [βaːndekiɾe] /βa-a-andɛk-iɾe/ waliandika they wrote (P2) 3PL-PST-write-ANT βaangirë [βaːŋgiɾe] /βa-a-aŋg-iɾe/ walikataa they refused (P2) 3PL-PST-refuse-ANT

Across morpheme boundaries in noun-related words it was decided not to break the rule, because there are no known instances where vowel length in a seemingly conditioned environment would distinguish between different meanings in these types of words. Thus, in nouns and adjectives one vowel is written in a conditioned environment at a morpheme boundary, even if there is more than one vowel underlyingly, for example:

Table 2.1.2e: Conditioned environments at morpheme boundaries in nouns Orthography Phonetic Phonemic Swahili English abhenga [aβɛːŋga] /aβa-ɛŋga/ bibi harusi bride 2-bride bhonswe [bhɔːnswɛ] /βa-ɔnswɛ/ wao pia they also 2-also

12

2.1.3 Vowel Length in Noun Class Prefixes The class 5 nominal prefix surfaces as long with monosyllabic noun stems, and in these cases it is written long . With stems with two or more syllables, the prefix is short and is written short. Some examples of the Class 5 prefix with stems of different lengths are given in Table 2.1.3a below:

Table 2.1.3a: Class 5 prefix with different length stems Class 5 noun Swahili English riibhu jivu ash 1-syllable stem riihë vita war ribhina muziki music 2-syllable stem rireebhwa mhogo cassava ribhaabhayö papai papaya 3-syllable stem riwensero madhabahu altar

The class 10 nominal prefix also shows a variant with a long vowel when occurring before monosyllabic stems. This variation is shown in the orthography. Before monosyllabic stems the prefix is clearly perceived as long, and therefore in this environment it is written long. Before stems of two or more syllables, the prefix is perceived as short and is written short.7 Table 2.1.3b below shows examples of the class 10 prefix with nouns of different stem lengths.

Table 2.1.3b: Class 10 prefix with different length stems Class 10 noun Swahili English seeka koo tribe, families 1-syllable stem seesa masaa hours sehagho matunda fruits 2-syllable stem sembaara pande parts sehakëri akili mind 3-syllable stem sembeehera makaburi graves

7 There is at least one exception to this, with the bisyllabic stem seehera ‘hela’ ‘money’ in which the prefix is clearly perceived as long. It is not yet understood why this is an exception, but it is written with a long vowel in the prefix. 13

2.2 Consonants Suba‑Simbiti contains the following consonant phonemes, shown in Table 2.2a according to their place and manner of articulation (the graphemes are given below, with the phonetic realisation as necessary):

Table 2.2a: Suba‑Simbiti consonant phonemes Voicing velar Manner of articulation labial alveolar palatal /nasalisation /glottal Stops and affricates voiceless p t k voiced bh [β] d j [dʒ] gh [ɣ] prenasalised8 mb nd ng [ŋg]

Fricatives voiceless f s sh [ʃ] h

Sonorants nasal m n ny [ɲ] ng’ [ŋ] r [ɾ] oral w y [j] rr [r] The fricatives /β/ and /ɣ/ become stops when prenasalised, and in those cases they are written and respectively. When palatalised or labialised these consonants are pronounced as fricatives, and written as , , and . Simbiti has a phonemic contrast between the alveolar tap /ɾ/ and the trill /r/ , as shown by the following minimal pairs:9

Table 2.2b: Alveolar tap and trill minimal pairs10 Orthography Phonetic Phonemic Swahili English omooro [ɔmɔːɾɔ] /ɔmɔ-ɔɾɔ/ mto river omorro [ɔmɔːrɔ] /ɔmɔ-rɔ/ moto fire okoghora [ɔkɔɣɔɾa] /ɔkɔ-ɣɔɾ-a/ kununua to buy okoghorra [ɔkɔɣɔːra] /ɔkɔ-ɣɔr-a/ kunyoosha to straighten

8 These prenasalised consonants are included in the table of consonant phonemes because the nasal changes the nature of the following consonant; see explanations following the table. 9 Note that the vowel preceding the alveolar trill is compensatorily lengthened, see §2.1.2. 10 Note than the first pair are a minimal pair phonetically, and the second pair are a minimal pair phonemically. 14

The consonants /p/, /f/, /d/ and /dʒ/ occur almost exclusively in Swahili loan words and are written

, , and respectively, following the Swahili orthography.11 Other phonemes are written following the Swahili orthography as well, including /ʃ/ as , /ɲ/ as , /ŋ/ as , /ŋg/ as , and /j/ as . The only consonants that are usually restricted to stem-medial position are prenasalised consonants. Class 9/10 nouns stems often appear to begin with prenasalised consonants, but underlyingly the nasal is actually part of the prefix. This can be seen in the following example, where the Suba‑Simbiti word has a class 11 singular prefix and a class 10 plural prefix. These prefixes make the root evident, showing that the is underlyingly an at the beginning of the root:

Table 2.2c: is underlying /Nɾ/ Orthography Phonetic Phonemic Swahili English ororara [ɔɾɔɾaɾa] /ɔɾɔ-ɾaɾa/ aina ya mti type of tree sendara [sɛndaɾa] /sɛN-ɾaɾa/ aina ya mti (wingi) type of tree (plural)

The common exceptions, which do have stem-initial prenasalised consonants, are ‘maji’ ‘water’, and the /nto/ noun stem, found in words including ‘mtu’ ‘person’ and ‘kitu’ ‘thing’. Evidence of each phoneme is available in the following table, showing if they occur both stem-initially and stem-medially. The phoneme is in the leftmost column, while the orthographic representation is shown in the stem-initial and stem-medial columns:

Table 2.2d: Occurrences of Suba‑Simbiti consonant phonemes Stem-initial Swahili English Stem-medial Swahili English /p/ ripeera pera guava ekekapo kikapu basket aina ya /mb/ - - - emamba type of fish samaki garbage /f/ umufuko mfuko pocket ubhushafu takataka dump

11 Note that when the tap is prenasalised it becomes , which occurs in non-loanwords. 15

Stem-initial Swahili English Stem-medial Swahili English /β/ ribhaara kipande slice ukwisaabha kuoga to bathe /m/ okomanya kujua to know ekenama paja thigh /w/ ikiwembe wembe razor ekesaawa chamvua corn husk /t/ okotanga kuanza to begin engata twisho headpad aina ya /d/ indighi ukuta wall domodomo type of fish samaki praying /nd/ abhandë wengine others etaabharanda kivunjavunja mantis /s/ omosaani rafiki friend ikyasamötë aina ya mti type of tree /n/ okonaghya kulinda to herd ekeghanana aina ya mti type of tree /ɾ/ obhorabhu nuru light ikibhisirë methali proverb to be strong /r/ öbhörrë kitanda bed okohangarra kukakawana (physically) yoke (for /dʒ/ amajöki12 nira edaraja daraja bridge oxen) /ʃ/ umushaasha mwanaume man okobhaasha kuchonga carve /ɲ/ inyaamu paka cat okomanya kujua to know /j/ bhayö hao those mayaayi njano yellow /k/ okokebha kukata to cut nyakarare pilipili hot pepper nape of /ŋg/ ëkëngötiira ukosi enkangaaru -ekundu red neck /h/ umuhyö panga machete righuha mfupa bone /ɣ/ okoghenda kutembea to walk okoghegha kubeba to carry aina ya /ŋ/ eng’ana neno word nyaang'anda type of bird ndege

2.2.1 Palatalisation Within nouns and verbs, palatalisation13 occurs stem-initially and stem-medially (see §3.1.2.2 on glide formation, a common cause of palatalised consonants). According

12 This is one of the few apparently non-loanword examples of /dʒ/ in Simbiti. 16

to the data set, three consonant phonemes do not display palatalisation within roots: . (Also, and are never palatalised.) Table 2.2.1a shows examples of palatalisation in all three root positions.

Table 2.2.1a: Palatalisation Palatalisation Orthography Phonetic Phonemic Swahili English Stem-initial isyaghara [isjaːɣaɾa] /ɛ-sjaɣaɾa/ mjusi lizard Stem-medial rikumyö [ɾikumjo] /ɾi-kumjo/ sifa praise

Palatalisation occurs across morpheme boundaries, commonly root-finally in verbs when the root is followed by the Causative suffix (see §3.1.1.3.1 for further details about the Causative suffix). Usually the Causative suffix is written <‑y>:

Table 2.2.1b: Palatalisation with the Causative suffix Orthography Phonetic Phonemic Swahili English okotema [ɔkɔtɛma] /ɔkɔ-tɛm-a/ kupiga to hit INF-hit-FV ökötëmya [okotemja] /ɔkɔ-tɛm-i-a/ kupigisha to cause to hit INF-hit-CAUS-FV

When the Causative follows verb roots ending with an alveolar or palatal nasal, the Causative suffix is written with an <-i>. In these cases there is a syllable break before the final vowel <-a>. For example:

Table 2.2.1c: Causative suffix with alveolar and palatal nasals Orthography Phonetic Phonemic Swahili English okomeena [ɔkɔmɛːna] /ɔkɔ-mɛːn-a/ kulamba to lick INF-lick-FV ökömëënia [okomeːnia] /ɔkɔ-mɛːn-i-a/ kulambisha to cause to lick INF-lick-CAUS-FV okokenya [ɔkɔkɛɲa] /ɔkɔ-kɛɲ-a/ kukimbia to run INF-run-FV ökökënyia [okokeɲia] /ɔkɔ-kɛɲ-i-a/ kukimbiza to cause to run; to INF-run-CAUS-FV chase

13 Within this paper the use of the term ‘palatalisation’ does not mean that we are claiming that a separate set of palatalised consonants are formed, but rather that a sequence is formed of a consonant followed by a palatal glide /j/. 17

This issue is especially important because the palatal nasal is already written . The palatalised alveolar nasal is differentiated from the palatal nasal by being written . The palatal nasal followed by the Causative is written .

2.2.2 Labialisation Within nouns and verbs, labialisation14 occurs stem-initially and stem-medially (see §3.1.2.2 on glide formation, a common cause of labialised consonants). All true phonemes (i.e. those which do not only occur in Swahili loanwords) display labialisation (apart from and , which are never labialised). Also, like palatalisation, labialisation is prevalent across morpheme boundaries (particularly with the Passive verbal suffix) and can occur with consonants which had no examples of stem-medial labialisation. Table 2.2.2 shows examples of labialisation. Table 2.2.2: Labialisation

Suba‑Simbiti Swahili English Stem-initial labialisation ikimwensi aina ya kifutu type of viper irinyamwiti bundi owl Stem-medial labialisation okong’entwa kusama to choke

2.2.3 Prenasalisation Prenasalisation occurs in Suba‑Simbiti root-internally in both nouns and verbs. Table 2.2.3a shows examples of stem-internal prenasalisation at many different places and with different manners of articulation.

Table 2.2.3a: Prenasalisation Prenasalised Consonants Suba‑Simbiti Swahili English mb okwighomba kutapia to be eager nd okokandora kubandua to strip off ns okosensa kupembua to winnow nt okonantera kunata to stick nsh okotanshora kuponyoka to escape

14 Within this paper the use of the term ‘labialisation’ does not mean that we are claiming that a separate set of labialised consonants are formed, but rather that a sequence is formed of a consonant followed by a labio-velar glide /w/. 18

Prenasalised Consonants Suba‑Simbiti Swahili English ng okohongora kutapika to vomit nk engonkone mjusi kafiri gecko

Prenasalisation is also common across morpheme boundaries, especially with noun classes 9 and 10. As mentioned above (§2.2), when is prenasalised, it becomes . Prenasalised also occurs with the 1sg subject and object verb prefix (see §3.2.3 for further details). There are some Suba‑Simbiti words which have prenasalised consonants in a root-initial position. The following words from classes 1, 6, 7, 14 and 15 were found.

Table 2.2.3b: Prenasalisation in root-initial position Orthography Phonetic Phonemic Swahili English ömöntö [omoːnto] /ɔmɔ-nto/ mtu person amanshë [amaːɲʃe] /ama-ɲʃe/ maji water ëkëntö [ekeːnto] /ɛkɛ-nto/ kitu thing ëkëngötiira [ekeːŋgotiːɾa] /ɛkɛ-ŋgotiːɾa/ ukosi nape of neck ubhunsha [uβuːnʃa] /ɔβɔ-ɲʃa/ ua la maboga pumpkin flower okongerya [ɔkɔŋgɛɾja] /ɔkɔ-ŋgɛɾja/ kujumlisha to add

2.3 Tone There is no lexical tone in Suba‑Simbiti. There is grammatical tone on verbs, and two verb tenses in particular sometimes form tonal minimal pairs. The minimal pairs occur between the Anterior and the Remote Past with certain vowel-initial verb roots. Below are firstly examples of these tenses when they are not ambiguous, followed by examples illustrating ambiguous forms. With consonant-initial roots, the Anterior and Remote Past are distinguished by the presence of the /a-/ past tense prefix in the Remote Past, shown in the examples below:

19

Table 2.3a: Anterior and Remote Past with consonant-initial roots Orthography Phonetic Phonemic Swahili English bhatöbhërëkirë [βatóβeɾekíɾe] /βa-tɔ-βɛɾɛk-iɾe/ wametubeba they have 3PL-1PL-carry-ANT carried us bhaatöbhërëkirë [βaːtoβeɾékiɾe] /βa-a-tɔ-βɛɾɛk-iɾe/ walitubeba they carried 3PL-PST-1PL-carry-ANT us (P2) ötëmirë [otemíɾe] /ɔ-tɛm-iɾe/ umepiga you have hit 1PL-hit-ANT watëmirë [watemíɾe] /ɔ-a-tɛm-iɾe/ ulipiga you hit (P2) 1PL-PST-hit-ANT

The distinct tone patterns for these two tense-aspect forms can be most clearly seen with longer verb roots, as shown in the first two examples above: the Anterior has a high tone underlyingly on the first and fourth moras of the macrostem, and the Remote Past has a high tone underlyingly on the third mora of the macrostem (see Walker and Aunio, 2016, for further details on Simbiti melodic tone pattens).

With vowel-initial roots, it is possible to distinguish the two tenses orthographically when the subject prefix ends in a vowel other than , as shown in the examples below:

Table 2.3b: Anterior and Remote Past distinguished orthographically Orthography Phonetic Phonemic Swahili English twëbhirë [tweːβíɾe] /tɔ-ɛβ-iɾe/ tumesahau we have forgotten 1PL-forget-ANT twëëbhirë [tweːβíɾe] /tɔ-a-ɛβ-iɾe/ tulisahau we forgot (P2) 1PL-PST-forget-ANT kyöbhöhirë [kjóːβohíɾe] /kɛ-ɔβɔh-iɾe/ kimeogopa it has feared 7-fear-ANT kyööbhöhirë [kjoːβohíɾe] /kɛ-a-ɔβɔh-iɾe/ kiliogopa it was afraid (P2) 7-PST-fear-ANT

In the examples above, when the vowel of the subject prefix desyllabifies, the following vowel is compensatorily lengthened; this means that the Anterior also has a phonetically long vowel before the root, as well as the Remote Past, meaning that the two tenses are identical in terms of vowel length. Also, they are sometimes completely phonetically identical, even in tone pattern, especially with short verb roots in which the tone patterns cannot be so clearly distinguished (as in the first two 20

examples above). However, it is possible to distinguish the two tenses orthographically in this environment by representing the underlying morphemes: only a short vowel is written in the Anterior (representing the root-initial vowel), but a long vowel is written for the Remote Past (representing the past tense prefix and the root-initial vowel). With /i/-initial roots, it is also possible to differentiate the two tenses orthographically, regardless of the vowel of the subject prefix. When the subject prefix ends in , the two tenses are distinguished by writing the underlying morphemes; this leads to writing a sequence of three vowels in the Remote Past, as shown in the table below:15

Table 2.3c: Anterior and Remote Past distinguish by writing Orthography Phonetic Phonemic Swahili English bhaighuurë [βaíɣúːɾe] /βa-iɣw-iɾe/ wamesikia they have heard 3PL-hear-ANT bhaaighuurë [βaiɣúːɾe] /βa-a-iɣw-iɾe/ walisikia they heard (P2) 3PL-PST-hear-ANT

However, when the subject prefix ends in , and the vowel-initial root is other than /i/-initial, it is not possible to differentiate the two tenses orthographically. Sometimes, often with longer verb stems, the two forms are tonally distinct, but sometimes with short verb roots they are completely phonetically identical.

Table 2.3d: Anterior and Remote Past identical orthographically Orthography Phonetic Phonemic Swahili English bhaandëkirë [βáːndekíɾe] /βa-andɛk-iɾe/ wameandika they have written 3PL-write-ANT bhaandëkirë [βaːndekíɾe] /βa-a-andɛk-iɾe/ waliandika they wrote (P2) /3PL-PST-write-ANT/ bhëëghirë [βéːɣíɾe] /βa-ɛɣ-iɾe/ wamejifunza they have learnt 3PL-learn-ANT bhëëghirë [βeːɣíɾe] /βa-a-ɛɣ-iɾe/ walijifunza they learnt (P2) 3PL-PST-learn-ANT

15 The sequence is also written with /i/-initial roots with the Immediate Future (SBJ‑raa‑R‑e), with the Hypothetical (SBJ‑kaa‑R‑irë) and with the Negative Hypothetical (SBJ‑ta-kaa‑R‑irë) (see §3.1.2.1 for further details about ). 21

As all of the examples above have shown, it has been decided to distinguish the Anterior and Remote Past orthographically wherever possible, by representing the underlying morphemes. But for those few cases where it is not possible to distinguish them orthographically, context will make it clear which tense is required. For further details of Suba‑Simbiti grammatical tone contrasts, see Walker and Aunio (2016).

3. Morphophonology

3.1 Vowels

Morphophonological phenomena on vowels are found frequently in Suba‑Simbiti. Vowel harmony will be looked at in section 3.1.1. First, vowel harmony within noun roots will be examined, followed by nominal and verbal prefixes, and then verbal suffixes. Section 3.1.2 looks at the various processes that happen when vowels are adjacent at morpheme boundaries, including the diphthong , glide formation, assimilation, and intervocalic glide insertion. Section 3.1.3 describes how a glide is sometimes inserted word-initially, creating word-initial .

3.1.1 Vowel Harmony Processes Suba‑Simbiti has extensive vowel height harmony, affecting roots, prefixes and suffixes, which harmonise with other vowels in the word. There are four vowel heights: high /i, u/ , high-mid /e, o/ , low-mid /ɛ, ɔ/ , and low /a/ . The low vowel /a/ is neutral, not participating in the vowel-harmony processes, and opaque, blocking vowel harmony processes. Vowel harmony operates from right-to-left, moving leftward from suffixes or roots all the way to prefixes. All of the vowel harmony processes described in this section are at the level of mother-tongue speaker awareness, and the overarching orthography rule is to write the vowels as they are pronounced, even in cases of root vowel alternations. First, we describe vowel harmony within noun roots (§3.1.1.1), followed by vowel harmony in both nominal and verbal prefixes (§3.1.1.2), followed by vowel harmony in verb stems and suffixes (§3.1.1.3).

3.1.1.1 Vowel Harmony in Noun Roots First, regarding vowel harmony in noun roots, a look at vowel distribution in Table 3.1.1.3 is helpful.

22

Table 3.1.1.1: Vowel distribution in bisyllabic noun roots16

V1 V2 urusiri ubhubhimbu isiirë urusikö ingighe umurigho ribhina ‘kamba’ ‘uvimbe’ ‘deni’ ‘siku’ ‘nzige’ ‘mzigo’ ‘muziki’ ‘rope’ ‘swelling’ ‘debt’ ‘day’ ‘locust’ ‘burden’ ‘music’ umuturi ikighutu rihutë ingubhö amarure rituho umumura ‘mfuachuma’ ‘kibanda’ ‘jipu’ ‘kiboko’ ‘maziwa ‘zawadi’ ‘mvulana’ ‘blacksmith’ ‘hut’ ‘abcess’ ‘rhino’ mgando’ ‘gift ‘boy’ ‘yoghurt’ ömöhëti öröbhëru örörëmë mwëtö X X righësa ‘mpita njia’ ‘ugonjwa ‘ulimi’ ‘ndugu wa ‘majira ya ‘passerby’ (mnyama)’ ‘tongue’ ukoo’ mavuno’ ‘disease ‘clan ‘harvest (animal)’ relative’ season’ ömösöri ömöghötu inshökë ököghörö X X ëhörya ‘mchuzi’ ‘kikongwe’ ‘nyuki’ ‘mguu’ ‘aina ya mti’ ‘broth’ ‘very old ‘bee’ ‘foot’ ‘type of tree’ person’ X X X X umushere ekegheso omokera ‘mchele’ ‘kisu’ ‘mkia’ ‘uncooked ‘knife’ ‘tail’ rice’ X X X X omobhohe ekenono ikyoma ‘mfungwa’ ‘kilele’ ‘chuma’ ‘prisoner’ ‘summit’ ‘iron’ ritati ekesaku ensasë omobhasö ekesare orobhagho ikyara ‘tambara’ ‘mlango’ ‘cheche’ ‘kiangazi’ ‘pacha’ ‘ua’ ‘kidole’ ‘rag’ ‘door’ ‘spark’ ‘dry season’ ‘twin’ ‘fence’ ‘finger’

The low-mid vowels cannot co-occur in stems with high-mid . This is important for writers to understand, and it should prevent any words with stems like , etc. The low-mid vowels can occur in stems with high vowels if the high vowels are to the left of the low-mid vowels. However, cannot occur within a stem to the left of , because then they are raised to a high-mid vowel . (All of the combinations that cannot occur are symbolised by X in the table above.) It should also be noted that, since blocks vowel harmony, it is possible for and to co-occur in a word if there is an intervening . This

16 In the chart, X symbolises combinations that cannot occur because of vowel harmony. The noun roots are highlighted in bold. 23

is seen in the examples given above, and . The following section explains the vowel harmony processes occuring in prefixes.

3.1.1.2 Vowel Harmony in Prefixes Many Suba‑Simbiti prefixes (both nominal and verbal) alternate as a result of height harmony. These alternations are written in the orthography. For example, Table 3.1.1.2a shows the possible allomorphs of noun class prefixes. Only alternating prefixes are shown.17

Table 3.1.1.2a: Noun prefix vowel harmony V1 of root Class 1 umu- ömö- omo- 3 umu- ömö- omo- 4 imi- ëmë- eme- 7 iki- ëkë- eke- 8 ibhi- ëbhë- ebhe- 9 i(N)- ë(N)- e(N)- 10 si(N)- së(N)- se(N)- 11 uru- örö- oro- 14 ubhu- öbhö- obho- 15 uku- ökö- oko-

As the chart above shows, the height of V1 in a consonant-initial root determines the height of the vowels in the prefix. The height feature spreads leftward from the root of the noun. In verbs, subject agreement markers, object agreement markers and TAM prefixes are susceptible to the same vowel harmony process as noun class prefixes. Harmony processes move leftwards from the root, through one prefix to another, except when /a/ is part of a prefix, in which case vowel harmony is blocked.

17 The class 5 noun prefix is an exception to the normal vowel harmony processes: as a noun-class prefix is appears in only the high vowel form, <(i)ri->, apart from a couple of exceptional forms, e.g. ‘jiwe’ ‘rock’. However, the verbal subject agreement prefix for class 5 does show vowel harmony, with three alternations: . 24

Labialisation and palatalisation do affect vowel harmony in prefixes, with subject prefixes interpreting the glides and as their high vowel counterparts, /u/ and /i/. Also, the palatal nasal stimulates leftward vowel raising to high vowels. The following table illustrates changes to subject and object prefixes due to vowel harmony and vowel hiatus resolution (see §3.1.2 about adjacent vowels at morpheme boundaries) in various stem-initial environments. All verbs are in the Subjunctive mood <-e> (note that only five phonemic vowels occur in verb roots underlyingly, ):

Table 3.1.1.2b: Vowel harmony in verbal prefixes Low root V1 Mid root V1 High root V1 V-initial root INF → okobhara okoghora ukuhiita ukwanga /ɔkɔ-βaɾ-a/ /ɔkɔ-ɣɔɾ-a/ /ɔkɔ-hiːt-a/ /ɔkɔ-aŋg-a/ ‘kuhesabu’ ‘kununua’ ‘kukumbuka’ ‘kukataa’ Prefix  ‘to count’ ‘to buy’ ‘to remember’ ‘to refuse’ obhare oghore uhiite wange 2SG /ɔ-/ /ɔ-βaɾ-ɛ/ /ɔ-ɣɔɾ-ɛ/ /ɔ-hiːt-ɛ/ /ɔ-ang-ɛ/ kebhare keghore kihiite kyange NC7 /kɛ-/ /kɛ-βaɾ-ɛ/ /kɛ-ɣɔɾ-ɛ/ /kɛ-hiːt-ɛ/ /kɛ-ang-ɛ/ bhabhare bhaghore bhahiite bhaange 3PL /βa-/ /βa-βaɾ-ɛ/ /βa-ɣɔɾ-ɛ/ /βa-hiːt-ɛ/ /βa-ang-ɛ/

3.1.1.3 Vowel Harmony in Verb Stems and Suffixes Simbiti shows both root vowel raising (of low-mid vowels to high-mid vowels), caused by the Causative and Anterior verb suffixes, and root vowel lowering (of high vowels to low-mid vowels), caused by the Applicative and Inversive verb suffixes. Each of these will be looked at in turn.

3.1.1.3.1 Root Vowel Raising The Causative /-i/ causes raising of all phonemic /ɛ/ and /ɔ/ in roots and prefixes to [e] and [o] . This height harmony is reflected in the orthography, as illustrated in the table below:

25

Table 3.1.1.3.1a: Vowel harmony with the Causative suffix Underlying Orthography Phonetic Phonemic Swahili English root vowel to cause to /a/ okobharya [ɔkɔβaɾja] /ɔkɔ-βaɾ-i-a/ kuhesabisha count /ɛ/ ökötëmya [okotemja] /ɔkɔ-tɛm-i-a/ kupigisha to cause to hit /ɔ/ ököghörya [okoɣoɾja] /ɔkɔ-ɣɔɾ-i-a/ kuuza to sell /i/ ukuhiitya [ukuhiːtja] /ɔkɔ-hiːt-i-a/ kukumbusha to remind /u/ ukurutya [ukuɾutja] /ɔkɔ-ɾut-i-a/ kuvutisha to cause to pull

As the examples above show, after most consonants the Causative /-i/ desyllabifies, forming a sequence of a consonant plus a palatal glide, and the Causative is written <-y>. When the Causative follows verb roots ending with an alveolar or palatal nasal, it will be written with an <-i> (see examples and further details in §2.2.1). Root vowel raising is also caused by the Anterior suffix /-iɾe/. Like the Causative, this suffix raises all phonemic /ɛ/ and /ɔ/ in the root and prefixes to [e] and [o] . As with the cases above, this height harmony is reflected in the orthography.

Table 3.1.1.3.1b: Vowel harmony with the Anterior suffix Underlying Orthography Phonetic Phonemic Swahili English root vowel /a/ tohakirë [tɔhakiɾe] /tɔ-hak-iɾe/ tumelipa we have paid /ɛ/ tötëmirë [totemiɾe] /tɔ-tɛm-iɾe/ tumepiga we have hit /ɔ/ tötömirë [totomiɾe] /tɔ-tɔm-iɾe/ tumetuma we have sent we have /i/ tuhiitirë [tuhiːtiɾe] /tɔ-hiːt-iɾe/ tumekumbuka reminded /u/ turutirë [tuɾutiɾe] /tɔ-ɾut-iɾe/ tumevuta we have pulled

3.1.1.3.2 Root Vowel Lowering

The Applicative suffix <-er> lowers all and / i, u/ in the root and prefixes to match its height as and /ɛ, ɔ/. These root changes are reflected in the orthography, as shown in the table below:

26

Table 3.1.1.3.2a: Vowel harmony caused by Applicative suffix Root Derivative Applicative Phonetic Phonemic Swahili English vowel of Orthography to love /a/ okohansha okohanshera [ɔkɔhanʃɛɾa] /ɔkɔ-hanʃ-ɛɾ-a/ kupendea for /ɛ/ okotema okotemera [ɔkɔtɛmɛɾa] /ɔkɔ-tɛm-ɛɾ-a/ kupigia to hit for to close /ɔ/ okobhoha okobhohera [ɔkɔβɔhɛɾa] /ɔkɔ-βɔh-ɛɾ-a/ kufungia for to leave /i/ ukutigha okoteghera [ɔkɔtɛɣɛɾa] /ɔkɔ-tiɣ-ɛɾ-a/ kuachia for to cook /u/ ukurugha okoroghera [ɔkɔɾɔɣɛɾa] /ɔkɔ-ɾɔɣ-ɛɾ-a/ kupikia for

It is important to note that this root vowel lowering causes quite a few homophones of Applicative verbs in the language. For example:

Table 3.1.1.3.2b: Homophones caused by vowel harmony Orthography Phonetic Phonemic Swahili English ukutigha [ukutiɣa] /ɔkɔ-tiɣ-a/ kuacha to leave okoteghera [ɔkɔtɛɣɛɾa] /ɔkɔ-tiɣ-ɛɾ-a/ kuachia to leave for okotegha [ɔkɔtɛɣa] /ɔkɔ-tɛɣ-a/ kuvua (samaki) to fish okoteghera [ɔkɔtɛɣɛɾa] /ɔkɔ-tɛɣ-ɛɾ-a/ kuvulia (samaki) to fish for

Like the Applicative, the Inversive extension lowers all and /i, u/ in the root and prefixes to match its height as and /ɛ, ɔ/. These vowel harmony changes will be reflected in the orthography, as shown in the table below:

Table 3.1.1.3.2c: Vowel harmony with the Inversive suffix Root Derivative Applicative Phonetic Phonemic Swahili /English vowel of Orthography /i/ ukuribha okorebhora [ɔkɔɾɛβɔɾa] /ɔkɔ-ɾiβ-ɔɾ-a/ kuzibua /to unblock kuchimbua /to fill in /u/ ukutuka okotokora [ɔkɔtɔkɔɾa] /ɔkɔ-tuk-ɔɾ-a/ (a hole that was dug)

27

3.1.2 Vowel Adjacency at Morpheme Boundaries Generally in Suba‑Simbiti, when two vowels occur together at morpheme boundaries, there is either glide formation (§3.1.2.2) or assimilation (§3.1.2.3 and §3.1.2.4). There is also one permitted combination of vowels, , which seems to be a diphthong (see §3.1.2.1). In certain morphophonological environments there is also intervocalic glide epenthesis (§3.1.2.5). Also, at word boundaries there is often vowel elision, which is commented on in the relevant subsections of §4. The following two tables give an overview of the results of vowel combinations at morpheme boundaries, Table 3.1.2a showing the vowel resolutions, and Table 3.1.2b showing an overview of the processes involved:18

Table 3.1.2a: Vowel combinations at morpheme boundaries19 V2→ Front vowels [-low] [+low] Back vowels [-low] V1 i ë e a o ö

i

low] ë ii yë ye ya yo yö

-

Front [ e [+low] a ai ëë ee aa oo öö

o

low] ö wi wë we wa wo / oo wö / öö

-

Back [ u

18 Note that the underlying form of prefixes with a [-low] vowel are the low-mid vowels . Vowel harmony causes prefixes to raise to high-mid or high vowels (see §3.1.1). 19 V2 is not included in the table because there is no known /u/-initial root or morpheme. 28

Table 3.1.2b: Vowel processes at morpheme boundaries V2→ Front vowels [-low] [+low] Back vowels [-low] V1 i ë e a o ö

20

i

low] ë ASSIM. GLIDE FORMATION

-

Front [ e [+low] a DIPHTH. ASSIMILATION

o GLIDE

low] ö GLIDE FORMATION FORMATION OR

-

Back [ u ASSIMILATION

The tables above show that, generally, the low vowel assimilates to a following vowel, except when followed by the high front vowel . Also, non- low vowels desyllabify before a following vowel, except when front vowels are followed by , and when back vowels are followed by back vowels there is an alternative pronunciation with assimilation.

3.1.2.1 Permitted Diphthong One combination of non-identical vowels, , is found both word-internally and at morpheme boundaries. The sequence seems to be a diphthong, behaving in a way similar to long vowels as a single syllable, rather than a permitted bisyllabic vowel sequence.21 In Table 3.1.2.1a below are some of the word-internal examples of :

Table 3.1.2.1a: Morpheme-internal Orthography Phonetic Swahili English ëng’waina [eŋwaina] mamba crocodile abhang’aini [aβaŋaini] hekima wisdom hai [hai] wapi where kai [kai] hasa, sana very

20 When V1 is the Reflexive morpheme or the negative prefix , glide epenthesis of occurs before all V2; further details are given in §3.1.2.5. 21 See Overton (2018) for further details on the status of as a diphthong. 29

The sequence occurs frequently at morpheme boundaries in both nouns and verbs, and some examples are given in Table 3.1.2.1b below:

Table 3.1.2.1b: Morpheme boundary examples of Orthography Phonemic Swahili English amaino /ama-ino/ meno teeth 6-tooth amaiso /ama-iso/ macho eyes 6-eye amaitegherra /ama-itɛɣɛra/ mawazo thoughts 6-thought abhaibhöri /aβa-iβoɾi/ wazazi parents 2-parent aitegherre /a-itɛɣɛr-ɛ/ asikilize he should listen 3SG-listen-SBJV bhaishirë /βa-iʃ-iɾe/ wamekuja they have come 3PL-come-ANT tokaikara /tɔ-ka-ikaɾ-a/ tukakaa then they stayed 1PL-NAR-stay-FV

This permitted vowel hiatus means that occasionally a combination of three vowels is written . Writing this vowel sequence in some verbs represents underlying morphemes, which sometimes enables certain tonally-distinct verb forms to be distinguished orthographically. Note that the sequence written is not “extra-long”, but the same length as the normal /ai/ written . (See §2.3 for further details about grammatical tone minimal pairs).

Table 3.1.2.1c: Examples of Orthography Phonetic Phonemic Swahili English bhaaighuurë [βaiɣuːɾe] /βa-a-iɣw-iɾe/ walisikia they heard (P2) 3PL-PST-hear-ANT araaighwe [aɾaiɣwɛ] /a-ɾaː-iɣw-ɛ/ atasikia he will hear 3SG-F1-hear-FV (soon) akaaighuurë [akaiɣuːɾe] /a-kaː-iɣw-iɾe/ angesikia he would hear 3SG-HYP-hear-ANT atakaaighuur [atakaiɣuːɾe] /a-ta-kaː-iɣw-iɾe/ asisikie he should not ë 3SG-NEG-HYP-hear-ANT hear

30

3.1.2.2 Glide Formation Glide formation occurs when a front vowel or a back vowel is followed by another vowel.22 It happens both word-initially and word-internally. Front vowels desyllabify to form a palatal glide /j/, written , and back vowels desyllabify to form a labio-velar glide /w/, written . When vowels desyllabify, the following vowel is compensatorily lengthened; this length is not written, except in some cases across morpheme boundaries in verbs (see §2.1.2 about vowel length in conditioned environments). In nouns, glide formation frequently occurs when noun class prefixes desyllabify before a vowel-initial noun root. These class prefixes can also desyllabify in combination with noun modifiers, such as possessives, adjectives, etc. Some examples of glide formation in noun-related words are given in Table 3.1.2.2a below:

Table 3.1.2.2a: Glide formation in noun-related words Orthography Phonetic Phonemic Swahili English umwikari [umwiːkaɾi] /ɔmɔ-ikaɾi/ mwenyeji host imyëri [imjeːɾi] /ɛmɛ-eɾi/ miezi months iryobha [iɾjɔːβa] /ɛɾɛ-ɔβa/ jua sun ikyara [ikjaːɾa] /ɛkɛ-aɾa/ kidole finger ibhyeyo [iβjɛːjɔ] /ɛβɛ-ɛjɔ/ fagio brooms ubhwato [uβwaːtɔ] /ɔβɔ-atɔ/ mtumbwi canoe umwene [umwɛːnɛ] /ɔmɔ-ɛnɛ/ mwenyewe himself ghyonswe [ɣjɔːnswɛ] /ɣɔ-ɔnswɛ/ pia also (class 4) ibhyëndë [iβjende] /ɛβɛ-ende/ vingine other (things) ikyeng’e [ikjɛːŋɛ] /ɛkɛ-ɛŋɛ/ kifupi short (thing)

It can be seen in the table above that, when a glide is formed at the end of an agreement prefix, vowel harmony processes interpret it as its high vowel counterpart, triggering vowel raising of the augment vowel to its high vowel form. In verbs, glide formation often occurs between a subject prefix and a TAM prefix, or with a vowel-initial root; Table 3.1.2.2b gives some examples of this:

22 There are some exceptions; see §3.1.2.4 on sequences of identical vowels. 31

Table 3.1.2.2b: Glide formation in verbs Orthography Phonetic Phonemic Swahili English wahakirë [waːhakiɾe] /ɔ-a-hak-iɾe/ ulilipa you (sg.) paid 2SG-PST-pay-ANT (P2) mwibhirë [mwiːtɛɣɛːrɛ] /mɔ-itɛɣɛr-ɛ/ msikilize you (pl.) 2PL-listen-SBJV should listen twakyobhoha [twaːkjɔːβoha] /tɔ-a-kɛ-ɔβɔh-a/ tulikiogopa we feared it 1PL-PST-7-fear-FV (P2) bhaghwibhirë [βaɣwiːβiɾe] /βa-ghɔ-iβ-iɾe/ wameuiba they have 3PL-3-steal-ANT stolen it

3.1.2.3 Assimilation The low vowel always assimilates to the quality of the following vowel (apart from , in which case a diphthong is formed, see §3.1.2.1 on ). This occurs in both nouns and verbs; the following two tables give examples of these:23

Table 3.1.2.3a: Assimilation of in noun-related words Orthography Phonetic Phonemic Swahili English abhaame [aβaːmɛ] /aβa-amɛ/ tajiri rich person amaato [amaːtɔ] /ama-atɔ/ mtumbwi canoe abheehwa [aβɛːhwa] /aβa-ɛhwa/ wapwa nephews akeene [akɛːnɛ] /aka-ɛnɛ/ kenyewe itself abhoobha [aβɔːβa] /aβa-ɔβa/ waoga cowards amoobha [amɔːβa] /ama-ɔβa/ yoga mushrooms

For ease of comparison, all the verbal examples in the following table use the 3PL subject prefix, in the Subjunctive mood, with different verb stems:24

Table 3.1.2.3b: Assimilation of in verbs Orthography Phonetic Phonemic Swahili English bhaibhe [βaiβɛ] /βa-iβ-ɛ/ waibe they should steal bheebhe [βɛːβɛ] /βa-ɛβ-ɛ/ wasahau they should forget

23 Thus far no /u/-initial roots have been found in Suba‑Simbiti. 24 As above. 32

Orthography Phonetic Phonemic Swahili English bhëëghi [βeːɣi]25 /βa-ɛɣ-i-ɛ/ wafundishe they should teach bhaange [βaːŋgɛ] /βa-aŋg-ɛ/ wakatae they should refuse bhoobhohe [βɔːβɔhɛ] /βa-ɔβɔh-ɛ/ waogope they should fear bhöömi [βoːmi]26 /βa-ɔm-i-ɛ/ wakaushe they should cause to dry

3.1.2.4 Sequences of identical vowels Sometimes, when vowels are followed by an identical vowel at a morpheme boundary, they remain the same and are pronounced long. As seen in the previous section (§3.1.2.3) this happens when the low vowel is followed by an identical vowel. This also happens when a front vowel prefix is followed by a high front vowel : the second vowel in the sequence causes the prefix to be raised to a high vowel, and then the two front high vowels combine to form a long vowel.27 Some examples are show in the table below:

Table 3.1.2.4a: Sequence of identical vowels at morpheme boundaries Orthography Phonetic Phonemic Swahili English abhaame [aβaːmɛ] /aβa-amɛ/ matajiri rich people 2-rich.person akaandeka [akaːndɛka] /a-ka-andɛk-a/ akaandika then he wrote 3SG-NAR-write-FV ikiinyaku [ikiːɲaku] /ɛkɛ-iɲaku/ aina ya samaki type of fish 7-type.of.fish akiitegherre [akiːtɛɣɛːrɛ] /a-kɛ-itɛɣɛr-ɛ/ akisikilize he should listen 3SG-7-listen-SBJV to it

For mid back vowels , there are two alternative pronunciations for a sequence of identical vowels: either they remain the same and form a long vowel, or the first vowel forms a glide (and the second vowel is compensatorily lengthened, as in §3.1.2.2). Generally, at least in verbs and nouns, the

25 The Subjunctive suffix does not surface after the Causative. 26 The Subjunctive suffix does not surface after the Causative. 27 An exception to this can when the sequence is the Reflexive prefix /i-/ followed by an /i/-initial verb root. There are two alternative pronunciations of this: either formation of a long vowel, or if the speaker wishes to emphasise the Reflexive nature of the form, it can be pronounced bisyllabically with a glide insertion of . See §3.1.2.5 for further details about glide insertion. 33

pronounciation with the long vowel is regarded as the ‘proper’ pronunciation, therefore in nouns and verbs it is written this way. In certain pronominal forms both pronunciations are regarded as equal alternatives. It should be noted that in the form with glide formation, the glide triggers the high vowel form of the prefix. Some examples of combinations of mid back vowels are given below:28

Table 3.1.2.4b: Realisations of /ɔ+ɔ/ Orthography Phonetic Phonemic Swahili English omooshokoro [ɔmɔːʃɔkɔɾɔ] /ɔmɔ-ɔʃɔkɔɾɔ/ mjukuu grandchild 1-grandchild okoobhoha [ɔkɔːβɔha] /ɔkɔ-ɔβɔ-ha/ kuogopa to fear INF-fear-FV öghöndë [oɣoːnde] /ɔɣɔ-Vnde/29 mwingine other (thing) ughwöndë [uɣwoːnde] 3-other ronswe [ɾɔːnswɛ] /ɾɔ-ɔnswɛ/ pia also (class 11) rwonswe [ɾwɔːnswɛ] 11-also

3.1.2.5 Intervocalic Glide Insertion of In certain morphological environments, when a high front vowel is followed by another vowel, the vowel hiatus is resolved by the insertion of a glide between the two vowels, separating the two vowels into separate syllables. The main environments in which this occurs are after the Reflexive verbal prefix , and after the negative prefix (when it is followed by the 3SG prefix /a-/). Some examples of both of these environments are given below:

28 It should be noted that, in noun-related words, vowels are written short in compensatorily lengthened environments even when there is more than one vowel underlyingly at a morpheme boundary, as can be seen in the table in the words and (see §2.1.2 about vowels in conditioned environments). 29 The stem for ‘other’ seems to be /-Vnde/, in that the /V/ is an unspecified vowel and assimilates to whatever the previous vowel (i.e. the prefix vowel) is. 34

Table 3.1.2.5a: Inter-vocalic glide insertion with the Reflexive Orthography Phonetic Phonemic Swahili /English bhaiyahööyë30 [βaijahoːje] /βa-i-ahor-iɾe/ wamejitenga /they have 3PL-REFL-separate-ANT separated themselves ukwiyörökya [ukwiːjoɾokja] /uku-i-ɔɾɔk-i-a/ kujionyesha /to show INF-REFL-show-CAUS-FV oneself aiyitaahe [aijitaːhɛ] /a-i-itaah-ɛ/ ajichunguze /he should 3SG-REFL-examine-SBJV examine himself twaiyobhoha [twaijɔβɔha] /tɔ-a-i-ɔβɔh-a/ tulijiogopa /we were 1PL-PST-REFL-fear-FV afraid of ourselves bhaaiyëghëri [βaijeɣeɾi] /βa-a-i-ɛɣ-i-iɾe/ walijifundisha /they 3PL-PST-REFL-learn-CAUS-ANT taught themselves

Table 3.1.2.5b: Inter-vocalic glide insertion in negative verbs Swahili Orthography Phonetic Phonemic /English tiyëëghyanga hë [tijeːɣjaːŋga /ti-a-ɛɣ-i-aŋg-a=he/ hakuwa he] NEG-PST-learn-CAUS-HAB-FV=NEG akifundisha /he wasn’t teaching tiyatigha hë [tijatiɣa he] /ti-a-tiɣ-a=he/ hakuacha /he NEG-PST-stop-FV=NEG didn’t stop tiyaikërëëyi hë [tijaikeɾeːji he] /ti-a-ikɛɾ-i-iɾi=he/ hakukubali /he NEG-PST-agree-CAUS-ANT=NEG didn’t agree tiyööbhöhërrëëyi [tijoːβoheːreːji /ti-a-ɔβɔh-ɛɾ-i-iɾe=he/ hakuogopeshea hë he] NEG-3SG-fear-APPLIC-CAUS-ANT=NEG /he didn’t cause to fear about

3.1.3 Word-Initial

In a number of verb tense-aspect forms, before the 3SG subject prefix /a-/, the glide is inserted word-initially. It is compulsory for certain tense-aspects, and

30 This form, and many other examples in this and the following table, involves imbrication of the Anterior suffix; see §3.3.2 for further details. 35

optional for others. Some examples, together with alternative forms and other forms for comparison, are given in the tables below.31 In the Remote Past, Untimed Past and Today’s Future forms, the word-initial glide is compulsory, and it seems as though the functions to prevent word- initial long vowels:

Table 3.1.3a: Compulsory word-initial glide insertion TAM Orthography Phonetic32 Phonemic Swahili English /a-a-aβɛɾ-iɾe/ he forgave yaabhëëyë [jaːβéːje] alisamehe 3SG-PST-forgive-ANT (P2) P2 /a-a-tuk-iɾe/ yaatukirë [jaːtukiɾe] alichimba he dug (P2) 3SG-PST-dig-ANT /a-a-aβɛɾ-a/ yaabhera [jaːβɛɾa] alisamehe he forgave 3SG-PST-forgive-FV P3 /a-a-tuk-a/ yaatuka [jaːtuka] alichimba he dug 3SG-PST-dig-FV /a-aka-tuk-ɛ/ he will dig F2 yaakatuke [jaːkatukɛ] atachimba 3SG-F2-dig-FV (later today)

However, preventing word-initial long vowels cannot be the (only) reason, since word-initial long vowels are permitted in the Anterior and Imperative, and the variant is just an alternative form for these:33

31 The following abbreviations for TAM forms are used in the tables below: P2 ‘Remote Past’ (occurring yesterday or before); P3 ‘Untimed Past’; F2 ‘Today’s Future’; ANT ‘Anterior/Recent Past’; IMP ‘Imperative’. 32 Tone is included in the phonetic transcription of only those forms that are distinguished by tone alone. 33 Alternative forms for the same underlying morphemes are divided by a dotted line. Forms with different underlying morphemes are divided by a solid line. Forms in italics would be understood, but are not generally acceptable. 36

Table 3.1.3b: Optional word-initial glide insertion TAM Orthography Phonetic34 Phonemic Swahili English

aabhëëyë [áːβeːje] /a-aβɛɾ-iɾe/ he has amesamehe yaabhëëyë [jáːβeːje] 3SG-forgive-ANT forgiven /a-tuk-iɾe/ ANT atukirë [atukiɾe] 3SG-dig-ANT amechimba he has dug /n-a-tuk-iɾe/ natukirë [natukiɾe] FOC-3SG-dig-ANT

abhera [aːβɛɾa] /aβɛɾ-a/ yabhera [jaβɛɾa] forgive-FV samehe! forgive! /t-aβɛɾ-a/ tabhera [taβɛɾa] FOC-forgive-FV anga [aːŋga] /aŋg-a/ yanga [jaːŋga] refuse-FV IMP kata! refuse! /t-aŋg-a/ tanga [taːŋga] FOC-refuse-FV ëmya [eːmja] /ɛm.i-a/35 panda! climb! yëmya [jemja] climb.CAUS-FV /ɛmɛr-a/ emerra [ɛmɛːra] simama! stand! stand-FV

It is possible that preventing word-initial long vowels in specific tense-aspects could have been the original function of the , but over time it has been extended optionally to other forms. The word-initial could also be an optional alternative focus marker (in addition to the normal and unusual focus markers used for certain forms). But it is not clear if there is any difference in meaning between the alternative forms with and without the . Since the function of the word-initial is not clear, it has been decided to write it wherever it occurs as the most suitable pronunciation of a form.

34 Tone is included in the phonetic transcription of only those forms that are distinguished by tone alone. 35 The verb seems to contain a lexicalised Causative. 37

3.2 Consonants

3.2.1 Dahl’s Law Prefixes of noun classes 7, 12 and 15 (including the Infinitive prefix) underlyingly have the consonant /k/. However, for some Suba‑Simbiti speakers, this prefix consonant is voiced when the next consonant in the word is voiceless. This is the result of a common Bantu consonant dissimilation process known as Dahl’s Law. This voicing process is optional in Suba‑Simbiti, and research indicates that it is still always considered correct to pronounce these prefixes with , even if the voiced alternation is also possible. Therefore these prefixes are written with only. Some examples are given in Table 3.2.1 below:

Table 3.2.1: Inter-vocalic glide insertion Orthography Phonetic Phonemic Swahili English [ɛkɛsaka] /ɛkɛ-saka/ ekesaka kichaka bush or [ɛɣɛsaka] 7-bush [akatɔmɔ] /aka-tɔmɔ/ akatomo kangoma small drum or [aɣatɔmɔ] 12-small.drum [okotwe] /ɔkɔ-twe/ ökötwë sikio ear or [oɣotwe] 15-ear

3.2.2 Elision of /ɾ/ The consonant /ɾ/ elides in various environments across morpheme boundaries. One of these environments is in the class 5 noun prefix /ɾi-/ when preceded by a locative clitic /kɔ-/ or /mɔ-/. An optional pronunciation is with elision of the /ɾ/ causing desyllabification of the vowel of the locative clitic, producing the sequence /kwi/ or /mwi/. However, because this elision does not consistently occur, the form is written in full (with a hyphen separating the locative clitic from the prefix, and with the /ɾ/ unelided). (See §4.2 for further details on locatives.) Some examples are given in Table 3.2.2a below:

Table 3.2.2: Class 5 /ɾ/ elision with locatives Orthography Phonetic Phonemic Swahili English [kuɾisaːɾo] /kɔ-ɾisaːɾo/ ku-risaarö kwenye wingu in the clouds or [kwiːsaːɾo] 17-cloud [kuɾitaɾɛ] /kɔ-ɾitaɾɛ/ ku-ritare kwenye mwamba on the rock or [kwiːtaɾɛ] 17-rock

38

Orthography Phonetic Phonemic Swahili English [muɾikoːŋgo] /mɔ-ɾikongo/ in the mu-riköngö jangwani or [mwiːkoːŋgo] 18-wilderness wilderness [muɾiɣaɾi] /mɔ-ɾiɣaɾi/ in the mu-righari kwenye uwanja or [mwi:ɣaɾi] 18-courtyard courtyard

Elision of /ɾ/ also occurs when various verb suffixes combine; further details of these environments are given below in §3.3.2.

3.2.3 1SG Nasal Prefix

The 1SG subject and object verbal prefix is underlyingly a nasal /n-/, and is usually written as such, as in ‘kunipiga’ ‘to hit me’. But before bilabial roots it is ,36 and before it is phonetically /ŋ/ but is written ; before both the bilabial and velar fricatives , it causes the fricative to become a stop, giving the sequences and . When the 1SG prefix occurs before , the /ɾ/ strengths to become /d/, and the combination is written as it sounds . Before vowels the 1SG prefix surfaces as /ɲ/ and is written . However, in certain environments there are alternative pronunciations: with nasal-initial stems, sometimes the nasal prefix can be heard, but often the preceding vowel is heard lengthened instead. However, the 1SG prefix is still written as because it adds important meaning, and the phonetic vowel length is not written; this results in consonant sequences such as , , and . There are also alternative pronunciations with /h/-initial stems: often the combination of 1SG prefix with the /h/ surfaces as a velar nasal /ŋ/. However, with most verb stems this pronunciation is sporadic, and therefore the 1SG prefix with most /h/-initial stems is written . However, there is one verb stem with which the 1SG prefix is always pronounced as a velar nasal, that is the verb ‘to give’, ; therefore, with this stem alone the 1SG prefix and root-initial /h/ are written : ‘kunipa’ ‘to give me’. Examples of all the forms with alterntiave pronunciations can be seen in Table 3.2.3 below:

36 Before an the sequence is written as a double nasal , 39

Table 3.2.3: 1SG subject and object prefix

1SG Orthography Phonetic Phonemic Swahili /English [aɾanːaːɾja] /a-ɾa-N-naːɾj-a/ ananizingira /he is nn arannaarya / [aɾaːnaːɾja] 3SG-PRS-1SG-go.round-FV going around me [ɲːɔːɾɛ] /N-ɲɔːɾ-ɛ/ nny nnyoore nipate /I should get / [ɲɔːɾɛ] 1SG-get-SBJV [βɛkaŋːaina] /βɛ-ka-N-ŋain-a/ zikanidanganya /then nng’ bhekanng’aina / [βɛkaːŋaina] 8-NAR-1SG-deceive-FV it deceived me [ŋːgeɾi] /N-ŋgɛɾ.i-ɛ/37 nijumlishe /I should nng nngëri / [ŋgeɾi] 1SG-count.up.CAUS-SBJV count up [βanhiːtɛ] /βa-N-hiːt-ɛ/ wanikumbuke /they nh bhanhiite / [βaːhiːtɛ] 3PL-1SG-remember-SBJV should remember me /ɔkɔ-N-haan-a/ ng’ okong’aana [ɔkɔŋa:na] kunipa /to give me INF-1SG-give-FV

3.3 Morphophonological Process between Verb Suffixes There are a number of processes which occur between various verb suffixes, some of which will be mentioned in this section.

3.3.1 Creation of the Trill Care must be taken when writing a tap or a trill in verb suffixes, because often a trill can be created when certain suffixes combine with certain verb roots. When either the Applicative suffix /-ɛɾ/ or the Anterior suffix /-iɾe/ occur after a vowel root ending in /-Vɾ/, there is elision of the first vowel of the suffix, and the taps of the root and the suffix combine to form a trill /r/ . Care must also be taken when writing vowel length in such forms, since vowels are phonetically lengthened before a trill, but this conditioned vowel length is not written (see §2.1.2 concerning vowel length in conditioned environments). Table 3.1.1 below shows a number of verb roots together with various suffixes in order to illustrate the environments in which special care must be taken. The first two roots, and , demonstrate how the suffixes are written with a root that doesn’t end in , with both a short and a long vowel in the root. The other roots, all ending in , demonstrate the morphophonological changes that take place when certain suffixes interact with the

37 The verb seems to contain a lexicalised Causative. 40

root-final . Three of these roots, , and , show how roots with short vowels appear, while the other two, and , illustrate the process for roots with long vowels. Special care must be taken concerning roots for which there is a minimal pair for vowel length in the root, for example: ‘kuweza / to be able’ and ‘kuweka / to put’. Vowel length is written before a trill when there is an underlying long vowel in the root (as for and below).38

Table 3.3.1: Combinations of suffixes with or APPLIC + INF APPLIC /-ɛɾ/ SBJV /-ɛ/ ANT /-iɾe/ SBJV /-ɛɾ-ɛ/ okotema okotemera toteme totemere tötëmirë ‘kupiga /to ‘kupigia /to ‘tupige /we ‘tupigie /we should ‘tumepiga /we hit’ hit for’ should hit’ hit for’ have hit’ okobhooka okobhookera tobhooke tobhookere töbhöökirë ‘kuamka /to ‘kuamkia /to ‘tuamke /we ‘tuamkie /we ‘tumeamka /we wake up’ wake for’ should wake’ should wake for’ have woken’ okomera okomerra tomere tomerre tömërrë ‘kumeza /to ‘kumezea /to ‘tumeze /we ‘tumezee /we ‘tumemeza /we swallow’ swallow for’ should swallow’ should swallow for’ have swallowed’ okoghora okoghorra toghore toghorre töghörrë ‘kununua ‘kununulia ‘tununue /we ‘tununulie /we ‘tumenunua /we /to buy’ /to buy for’ should buy’ should buy for’ have bought’ okotora okotorra totore totorre tötörrë ‘kuweza /to ‘kuwezea /to ‘tuweze /we ‘tuwezee /we ‘tumeweza /we be able’ be able for’ should be able’ should be able for’ have been able’ okotoora okotoorra totoore totoorre totoorrë ‘kuweka /to ‘kuwekea /to ‘tuweke /we ‘tuwekee /we ‘tumeweka /we put’ put for’ should put’ should put for’ have put’ okonyoora okonyoorra tonyoore tonyoorre tönyöörrë ‘kupata /to ‘kupatia /to ‘tupate /we ‘tupatie /we should ‘tumepata /we get’ get for’ should get’ get for’ have got’

38 The combined suffixes are highlighted in bold to aid comparison. 41

3.3.2 Applicative with Anterior

When the Anterior suffix /-iɾe/ follows the Applicative suffix /-ɛɾ/, or a verb root ending with /Vɾ/, there is elision of the /ɾ/ of both suffixes, resulting in a suffix with the glide . This process is called “imbrication” and is not an uncommon process in many Bantu languages; the full analysis of this complex process is beyond the scope of this document. These verb endings are simply written as they are pronounced, and some examples are given in the table below:

Table 3.3.2a: Elision of /ɾ/ with Anterior suffix Word Orthography Phonetic Phonemic Swahili /English ending /a-a-andɛk-ɛɾ-iɾe/ aliandikia /he wrote <-ëëyë> yaandëkëëyë [jaːndekeːje] 3SG-PST-write-APPLIC-ANT to (P2) wamejitenga /they /βa-i-ahɔɾ-iɾe/ <-ööyë> bhaiyahööyë [βaijahoːje] have separated 3PL-REFL-separate-ANT themselves /a-tiɣaɾ-iɾe/ amebaki /he has atighaayë [aitiɣaːje] 2SG-remain-ANT remained <-aayë> /a-ikaɾ-iɾe/ aikaayë [aikaːje] amekaa /he has sat 3SG-sit-ANT

When the Applicative /-ɛɾ/ (or verb root ending in /Vɾ/) and Anterior /-iɾe/ are also combined with the Causative suffix /i-/, a similar suffix with the glide surfaces, this time with word-final <-i> because of the Causative. Some examples are given in the table below:

Table 3.3.2b: Elision of /ɾ/ with Anterior suffix and Causative Word Orthography Phonetic Phonemic Swahili /English ending /a-kundikir-i-iɾe/ amefunika /he has <-iiyi> akundikiiyi [akundikiːji] 3SG-cover-CAUS-ANT covered /ti-a-ikɛɾ-i-iɾe/ hakukubali /he <-ëëyi> tiyaikërëëyi [tijaikeɾeːji] NEG-3SG-agree-CAUS-ANT didn’t agree (P2) /N-βa-a-iʃɔɾ-i-iɾe/ walijaza /they <-ööyi> mbaaishööyi [mbaiʃoːji] FOC-3PL-PST-fill-CAUS-ANT filled up (P2) /a-a-saɣaɾ-i-iɾe/ alikodisha /he <-aayi> yaasaghaayi [jaːsaɣaːji] 3SG-PST-tax-CAUS-ANT taxed (P2)

42

3.3.3 Causative with Anterior

When the Causative suffix /-i/ is followed by the Anterior /-iɾe/, the suffixes combine through imbrication. The combined suffix shows two forms, dependent on the vowels of the root. After /i, u/ in the root, the Causative-Anterior combination is <-iri>, and after in the verb root, the ending is <-ëri>. The root vowels are also raised to their high-mid counterpart <ë, ö>, as per the normal leftward vowel harmony caused by the Anterior and/or Causative. Examples are given in the table below:

Table 3.3.3: Causative with Anterior suffix combinations Root CAUS + Derivative Example Phonemic Swahili /English Vowel ANT of /a/ -ëri okohansha /a-ɣamb-i-iɾe/ amesemesha /he has [aɣambeɾi] caused to say /ɛ/ -ëri okotema /a-tɛm-i-iɾe/ amepigisha /he has [atemeɾi] caused to hit /ɔ/ -ëri okobhoha /a-βɔh-i-iɾe/ amefungisha /he has [aβoheɾi] caused to close /i/ -iri ukutigha /a-tiɣ-i-iɾe/ ameachisha /he has [atiɣiɾi] caused to leave /u/ -iri ukurugha /a-ruɣ-i-iɾe/ amepikisha /he has [aɾuɣiɾi] caused to cook

4. Word Boundaries

4.1 Copula The copula in Suba‑Simbiti is underlyingly a nasal /n-/ and is separated from the following word with a hyphen. When the word following the copula begins with an augment vowel, as in the case of most nouns, the nasal attaches directly to the augment vowel. For ease of readability, the hyphen is placed after the vowel, at the syllable boundary rather than at the morpheme boundary.39 When the following word is consonant-initial the nasal attaches directly to the following consonant, with the hyphen written between the nasal and the consonant. This happens with class 10

39 This decision was confirmed as the preferred choice by the Suba‑Simbiti participants at the Orthography Testing in March 2009. 43

nouns since they don’t have an augment vowel. Table 4.1a below gives examples of the copula with each of the noun classes.

Table 4.1a: Copula with each noun class Class Noun With copula Swahili English 1 ömöntö nö-möntö ni mtu is a person 2 abhantö na-bhantö ni watu is people 3 umuki nu-muki ni mshipa is a vein 4 ëmëtë në-mëtë ni miti is trees 5 ritamboka n-ditamboka ni hatua is a step 6 amabhëri na-mabhëri ni marobota is piles 7 ekesaka ne-kesaka ni kichaka is a bush 8 ibhighutu ni-bhighutu ni mabanda is huts 9 ehagho ne-hagho ni tunda is a fruit 10 sinsukubhë n-sinsukubhë ni nundu is bumps 11 örötë nö-rötë ni ufito is a rod 12 akatomo na-katomo ni kangoma is a small drum 14 obhogharë no-bhogharë ni upana is width 15 ökötwë nö-kötwë ni sikio is an ear 16 ahasë na-hasë ni mahali is a place 17 ko-kebhara nko-kebhara ni kwenye dunia is on earth 18 mo-kabhira mmo-kabhira ni katika kabila is in the tribe 20 oghotomo no-ghotomo ni ngoma kubwa is a big drum 23 yeeka n-yeeka ni nyumbani is home

When the copula precedes a class 5 noun beginning with the class prefix , the strengthens and together they form a prenasalised stop . Originally, the Suba‑Simbiti tried writing this as in a desire to keep the syllable together, putting the hyphen at the syllable boundary. However this was later changed to use the hyphen to show the two separate words, . When the copula occurs before a locative form (classes 17 and 18), the copula is written conjunctively with the locative prefix without an additional hyphen. For a time it was written with an additional hyphen, i.e. ; however, the 44

Suba‑Simbiti found the multiple hyphens quite difficult to read, so it was decided to omit the first hyphen. Therefore, only one hyphen is written, after the locative prefix. When the following word begins with a nasal, the nasal of the copula completely assimilates to the nasal of the root (i.e. by place of articulation). Sometimes the nasal does not sound lengthened, and the presence of the copula is only understood from context. Even when the extra nasal sound is not pronounced, it is written anyway in order to distinguish the meaning in writing. When the nasal is velar or palatal, the entire digraph or trigraph is not written twice. Instead, alone is written. For example:

Table 4.1b: Copula with nasal-initial words Noun With copula Swahili English mwarimu m-mwarimu ni mwalimu is a teacher nyamobhwe n-nyamobhwe ni bweha is a jackal ng’ööng’ö n-ng’ööng’ö ni aina ya paa is a dik dik

In some cases, there is more than one correct pronunciation of a word with the copula. An example of this is with a class 2 noun or adjective: if the augment elides, then the copula causes the fricative to form a stop, and it is written as pronounced; but sometimes the augment does not elide, in which case it is written .40 In pronunciations where the augment elides, the copular nasal should be written as pronounced, i.e. or , not or . Table 4.1c gives examples of some of the possible alternative forms:

Table 4.1c: Copula with and without the augment vowel Class Noun With copula Swahili English no-moona 1 omoona ni mtoto is a child m-moona na-bhaiya 2 abhaiya ni wazuri are good m-baiya

40 The form without the augment seems to be the more common choice in questions. The augment thus seems to hold a sense of definiteness. 45

Class Noun With copula Swahili English na-mang’ana 6 amang’ana ni mambo is news m-mang’ana në-këntö 7 ëkëntö ni kitu is a thing n-këntö

There is a set of vowel-initial words in which the initial vowel is not an augment vowel. When the copula occur with these words the initial vowel is written twice: once with the copular nasal before the hyphen, and again after the hyphen with the rest of the word stem. (The vowel of the first syllable of these words is also usually pronounced long.) This aids reading and word recognition, especially because many of these words are very short. Some examples are given in Table 4.1d below:

Table 4.1d: Copula with non-augment vowel-initial words Without copula With copula Swahili English öni nö-öni ni mimi is me uwe nu-uwe ni wewe is you (singular) igha ni-igha ni kwamba is that /because /thus igho ni-igho ni hivyo is thus ikömi ni-ikömi ni kumi is ten isho ni-isho ni kesho is tomorrow Eramu ne-Eramu ni Elamu is Elam Erasito ne-Erasito ni Erasto is Erastus Anderea na-Anderea ni Andrea is Andrew

There are also some short vowel-initial words in which the initial vowel is an augment vowel (such as associatives). Because these words are so short, and because most Suba-Simbiti speakers find it very difficult to discern the difference between an augment and non-augment word-initial vowel, when the copula occurs with these words the vowel is written twice: once with the copular nasal before the hyphen, and again after the hyphen with the rest of the word stem. (The vowel of the first syllable of these words is also usually pronounced long.) This means that the full word is written after the hyphen, which greatly increases ease of reading and comprehension. Some examples are given in Table 4.1e below: 46

Table 4.1e: Copula with short vowel-initial words Without copula With copula Swahili English ono no-ono ni huyu is this (class 2) ömwë nö-ömwë ni mmoja is one (class 2) ëëhë në-ëëhë ni ipi is which (class 9)? ike ni-ike ni ndogo is small (class 7) ikya ni-ikya ni cha is of (class 7) abha na-abha ni wa is of (class 2)

Note that there is a focus marker /n-/, which looks identical to the copula, occurring on many verb forms (for some tense-aspect forms it is optional, and for others it is compulsory). It is written conjunctively with the verb, without a hyphen, because it functions as part of the verb form and is not perceived as a separate component. Some examples are given in Table 4.1f below:

Table 4.1f: Verbal focus marker Orthography Phonetic Phonemic Swahili English /N-tɔ-ɾaː-βin-ɛ/ we will (soon) ntoraabhine [ntɔɾaːβinɛ] tutacheza FOC-1PL-F1-dance-FV dance /N-tɔ-kaː-βin-iɾe/ we would ntokaabhinirë [ntɔkaːβiniɾe] tungecheza FOC-1PL-HYP-dance-ANT dance /N-tɔ-kaː-βin-a/ we might ntokaabhina [ntɔkaːβina] tukicheza FOC-1PL-HYP-dance-FV dance

4.2 Locatives Nominal locatives in Suba‑Simbiti are shown by attaching a class 17 or 18 prefix to the beginning of a noun, before the noun’s usual noun class prefix. The class 17 locative tends to add the meaning of ‘in the area of’ the noun, and the class 18 locative usually adds the meaning of ‘inside’ or ‘among’ the noun. The locative prefix is written with a hyphen and with vowel harmony alternations.41 After the locative, the augment of the noun elides, and the vowel of the locative is raised or lowered according to vowel height harmony. See Table 4.2a for a few examples of how locatives are written with nouns.

41 This decision was confirmed in the Orthography Testing of March 2009. 47

Table 4.2a: Locatives with nouns Class Noun With locative Swahili English ku-mukungu kwa mzee wa kike in the area of the old lady 1 umukungu mu-mukungu katika mzee wa kike in the old lady ko-bhantö kwa watu in the area of people 2 abhantö mo-bhantö mwa watu among people kö-mëtë kwenye miti at the trees 4 ëmëtë mö-mëtë katika miti in the trees ku-kishishi kwenye kijiji at the village 7 ikishishi mu-kishishi katika kijiji in the village ko-katughi kwenye kiti kadogo at the small chair 12 akatughi mo-katughi katika kiti kadogo in the small chair

An exception occurs in Class 5. The noun class prefix is but sometimes, especially in quick speech, the elides (as mentioned above in §3.2.2). This causes the vowel of the locative to desyllabify to a labio-velar glide /w/ before the /i/. However, the orthographic decision is to write the form of the noun, since the elision is inconsistent, and the unelided form is attested as proper pronunciation in slow speech:

Table 4.2b: Locatives with class 5 nouns Orthography Phonetic Phonemic Swahili English [kwiːtambɔka] or ku-ritamboka /kɔ-ɾi-tambɔka/ kwenye hatua with a step [kuɾitambɔka] [mwiːsisi] or mu-risisi /mɔ-ɾi-sisi/ mwituni in the wild [muɾisisi]

Locatives are also written with a hyphen when attached to proper nouns, with the proper noun capitalised after the hyphen, for example:

Table 4.2c: Locatives with proper nouns Orthography Phonetic Phonemic Swahili English ku-Waryobha [kuwaɾjɔβa] /kɔ-waɾjɔβa/ mbele za Mungu before God ku-Ibhurahimu [kwiβuɾahimu] /kɔ-iβuɾahimu/ kwa Ibrahimu from Abraham

48

As mentioned above (§4.1), when the copula occurs with a locatival nominal form, the copula is written conjuctively with the locative prefix (without a hyphen), and a hyphen is written between the locative prefix and the noun-class agreement prefix as normal (see Table 4.1a). The class 17 locative is sometimes used with an adverbial meaning not related to location in space or time, and in these instances it tends to be translated as ‘kwa’ ‘by, with, for’. In these adverbial cases the locative is written disjunctively (i.e. as a separate word). The locative vowel still harmonises with the vowels of the noun, and if the following word begins with an augment then in pronunciation the augment vowel elides (however the augment vowel is still written):

Table 4.2d: Disjunctive adverbial locative Orthography Phonetic Phonemic Swahili English ku iriina [kuɾiːna] /kɔ#iri-ina/ kwa jina by the name kö bhëënyu [koβeːɲu] /kɔ#βa-enyu/ kwa ninyi as for you ko bhoora [kɔβɔːɾa] /kɔ#βɔːɾa/ kwa maana for the reason

Classes 16, 17, 18, and 23 locative suffixes can also be attached to verbs. As suffixes, they are <-ho>, <-ko>, <-mo>, and <-yo>. The suffixes do not seem to undergo vowel harmony. These locatives are written with hyphenation, just as the nominal locatives are written. See examples below:

Table 4.2e: Locatives with verbs Class Orthography Swahili English 16 akaasha-ho akaja (hapo) then he came (there) 17 aranyoora-ko anapata (juu ya kitu) he finds (on something) 18 yaarë-mo alikuwemo she was in there 23 akaasha-yo akaja (huko) then he came (to that area)

4.3 Associatives Associative constructions in Suba‑Simbiti are predictable, with the associative being declined by the first noun in the phrase. The underlying vowel of the associative is an <-a>, based on its form before consonant-initial nouns in Class 1a, 5, and 10. Although in quick speech the associative vowel elides, slow speech shows that the associative vowel is an <-a> and is followed by the augment vowel of the following noun. Also, the associative itself has an augment vowel at the start of it. Associatives are written disjunctively, in underlying form with its own final <-a> 49

and with the augment vowel written on the following noun (e.g. ). Table 4.3a below shows the associative for each noun class (except class 17 and 18 locatives, since there are not associatives which agree with these classes).

Table 4.3a: Suba‑Simbiti associatives Class Associative Class Associative 1 uwa 10 isya 2 abha 11 urwa 3 ughwa 12 aka 4 ighya 14 ubhwa 5 irya 15 ukwa 6 agha 16 aha 7 ikya 20 ughwa 8 ibhya 23 wa 9 iya

Table 4.3b below shows associatives in certain noun phrases, showing how forms are written in full with vowels unelided.

Table 4.3b: Associatives in noun phrases Class of noun Suba‑Simbiti Swahili English first second mtoto wa mzee wa the child of the old 1 1 omoona uwa umukungu kike woman mzee wa kike wa the old woman of 1 3 umukungu uwa ömötë mti the tree the tree of the 3 1a ömötë ughwa maayö mti wa mama mother the footstep of the 5 4 ritamboka irya ëmëtë hatua ya mti tree the donkey of the 9 7 itikërë iya ikibhisirë punda wa methali proverb the face of the 14 1 ubhusyö ubhwa ömöntö uso wa mtu person

50

4.4 Conjunction The conjunction ‘na’ ‘and’ is written disjunctively, just like the associative. In quick speech the vowel of the conjunction elides before a vowel-initial word, but in slow speech the full form is preserved, and it is written this way every time. The following examples show the conjunction written as part of a noun phrase:

Table 4.4: Conjunction Suba‑Simbiti Swahili English ömöntö na umukungu mtu na mzee wa kike the person and the old woman ömöntö na ömöbhëëru mtu na kiwete the person and the cripple ömöntö na omobhohe mtu na mfungwa the person and the prisoner ömöntö na maayö mtu na mama the person and the mother ömöntö na imiki mtu na mishipa the person and the veins

4.5 Negative Clitics Suba‑Simbiti has a negative phrasal circumclitic of the shape /tɛ= he/.42 This circumclitic can negate both nouns and verbs. With nouns, it functions as a negative copula. With verbs, the circumclitic can negate a variety of different tense-aspect forms. In both cases, the clitic is always written as a separate word.43 The proclitic /tɛ=/, however, is written differently in different contexts, as described below. When the negative circumfix is negating a noun, the augment vowel of the noun elides, and the prefix participates in vowel harmony. The prefix is written with a hyphen, similarly to how the positive copula form is written. The is not affected by vowel harmony, and it is written as a separate word. See the following examples:

42 More research is needed to determine the exact nature of these morphemes, specifically whether the initial morpheme is best described as a clitic or an affix, and whether the final morpheme is a clitic or an independent word. For ease of reference to this pair of morphemes, we refer to them here as a “circumclitic”. 43 Based on research done in preparation for the Orthography Testing in March 2009 and the opinions from the participants in that workshop, it was decided to always write disjunctively as a separate word since it never changes form. This is the same as what is done in the related language Kuria with their negative clitic “hai”. 51

Table 4.5a: Noun phrases with negative circumfix Noun Noun Swahili English It is a..... It is not a...... class 1 omoona mtoto child no-moona te-moona hë 2 abhantö watu people na-bhantö te-bhantö hë 3 umuki mshipa (wa damu) vein nu-muki ti-muki hë 4 ëmëtë miti trees në-mëtë të-mëtë hë

With verbs, the clitic always occurs at the very beginning of the verb, and it is written conjunctively (without a hyphen). If the subject prefix of the verb starts with a vowel, then the vowel of the clitic usually elides, and the is followed by the vowel of the subject marker. There also seems to be an alternative pronunciation in certain forms: the vowel of the clitic does not elide, but rather a glide is inserted between the clitic and the vowel of the subject prefix; the glide raises the prefix vowel to .

Table 4.5b: Negative circumfix with verbs Orthography Phonemic Swahili English /tɛ-βa-tɛm-iɾe=he/ hawajapig tebhatëmirë hë they haven’t hit NEG-3PL-hit-ANT=NEG a /tɛ-a-tɛm-iɾe=he/ tatëmirë hë hajapiga he hasn’t hit NEG-3SG-hit-ANT=NEG taibhirë hë /tɛ-a-iβ-iɾe=he/ hajaiba he hasn’t stolen tiyaibhirë hë NEG-3SG-steal-ANT=NEG /tɛ-a-kaː-mɔ-tɛm-a=he/ takaamotema hë asimpige he shouldn’t hit him NEG-3SG-HYP-3SG-hit-FV=NEG /tɛ-ɔ-kaː-mɔ-tɛm-a=he/ tokaamotema hë usimpige you shouldn’t hit him NEG-2SG-HYP-3SG-hit-FV=NEG

When the circumfix surrounds a whole phrase, the is placed at the end of the whole negated phrase. Compare the affirmative and negative sentences below.

‘Waliangusha miti yote.’ ‘They felled all the trees.’ ‘Hawakuangusha miti yoyote.’ ‘They did not fell any trees.’

52

4.6 Question Words

There is an interrogrative clitic in Suba-Simbiti, , which means ‘gani, nini’, ‘which, what’. When it has the meaning of ‘gani’ ‘which, what sort of’, it is written disjunctively, as in the following examples: Table 4.6a: Disjuctive clitic Suba‑Simbiti Swahili English M-meremo kë okokora? Ni kazi gani unafanya? What kind of work are you doing? N-kyaro kë urwërëëyë? Ni nchi gani umetoka? Which country are you from? Mighiro kë? Amri zipi? Which command? Ne-ng’ana kë oraakore? Ni jambo gani utafanya? What will you do? Obhoheene n-këntö kë? Ukweli ni kitu gani? What is truth?

The clitic also appears in some short lexicalised words where it is written conjuctively: Table 4.6b: Conjunctive clitic Suba‑Simbiti Swahili English N-kë44 Ni nini? What is it? Iyakë Vipi? How? N-kwakë? Kwa nini? Why?

Suba-Simbiti also has a set of question words with the stem /-Vhe/ ‘ipi’ ‘which’. This word takes agreement from different noun classes, and is always written as a single word. A selection of examples are given below: Table 4.6c: Conjuctive clitic /-Vhe/ Class Suba‑Simbiti Swahili English 1 ööhë? yupi? who? 2 m-baahë? ni nani? who is it? 6 ghaahë? yapi? which? 7 n-këëhë? ni nini? what it is? 9 ëëhë? ipi? which?

44 The hyphen is because of the copula , not because of the clitic. 53

4.7 Adverbial Suba-Simbiti has an adverbial clitic, ‘hivi, kwamba, tu’ ‘thus, just’, which is written disjunctively. It can be difficult for readers to remember to write this word disjunctively, since in quick speech it is pronounced together with the previous word. Also, there are a number of forms where the addition of creates an idiomatic phrase; the most common of these is with the pronoun <-onswe>, which independently means ‘pia’ ‘also’, but together with means ‘wote’ ‘all’. However, because occurs with so many different words, which all occur independently, it has been decided to always write disjunctively (even in those phrases with idiomatic meaning). Table 4.7a: Adverbial Orthography Fast speech Swahili English araakore igho [aɾaːkɔɾiːɣɔ] akifanya hivyo if anyone does this möghömërri igho [moɣomeriːɣɔ] mvumilie tu you should just calm down kya igho [kjaiɣɔ] kama hivyo in this way bhoono igho [βɔːnwiːɣɔ] sasa tu, sasa hivi right now kamwë igho [kamwiːɣɔ] mara moja straight-away bhonswe igho [βɔnswiːɣɔ] wote all

Also, there are two common examples of which are written with a hyphen, since they involve the copula and negative copula: Table 4.7b: with a hyphen Orthography Swahili English ni-igho ni hivyo is thus ti-igho hë sivyo is not thus

4.8 Reduplication Various types of words can undergo reduplication, including: verbs, noun, demonstratives, and adverbs. Reduplicated stems are generally written conjunctively as a single word, as shown in the following paragraphs. Reduplicated verbs tend to imply a continuous or persistent action, as shown in the examples below:

54

Table 4.8a: Reduplicated verbs Original Swahili English Reduplicated stem Swahili English stem ghenda tembea walk okoghendaghenda kuzurura to wander cause to tighya achisha ukutighyatighya kutekenya to tickle leave to keep itegherra kufikiri think ukwitegherrategherra kufikirifikiri thinking about mwenya tabasamu smile ukumwenyamwenya kutabasamu to smile to beg sabha omba ask okosabhasabha kuombaomba (habitually ) cause to kupepesa mitya songoza press ukumityamitya to blink macho together kupepesa tëmya pigisha spank ökötëmyatëmya to blink macho to make hoora pura thresh okohoorahoora kulainisha smooth to move hooya cheza play kuhooyahooya kuchezacheza around

A few nouns also show reduplicated stems, but an unreduplicated form does not always exist: Table 4.8b: Reduplicated nouns Original Swahili English Reduplicated form Swahili English stem - - - sehoorohooro faida gain ng’aini hekima wisdom obhong’aining’aini kiujanjaujanja very shrewd

Demonstratives can be reduplicated and, as in Swahili, the reduplicated form has an emphatic meaning, ‘the very one ...’. Reduplication is possible with all forms of demonstratives (proximal, referential and distal), and reduplicated forms are written conjuctively. A few examples are given in the table below:

55

Table 4.8c: Reduplicated demonstratives Class Proximal Referential Distal 1/1a onoono uyöuyö ooraoora ‘huyuhuyu’ ‘huyohuyo’ ‘yuleyule’ ‘this very one’ ‘that very one (close to listener)’ ‘that very one’ 2/2a bhanobhano bhayöbhayö bhaarabhaara ‘hawahawa’ ‘haohao’ ‘walewale’ ‘these very ones’ ‘those very ones (close to listener)’ ‘those very ones’ 9/9a enoeno iyöiyö eeraeera ‘hiihii’ ‘hiyohiyo’ ‘ileile’ this very one’ ‘that very one (close to listener)’ ‘those very ones’ 16 hanohano hayöhayö haarahaara ‘hapahapa’ ‘hapohapo’ ‘palepale’ ‘this very place’ ‘that very place (close to listener)’ ‘that very place

Certain adverbs and adjectives can also be reduplicated. When the meaning of the reduplicated form is emphatic, it is written conjunctively as one word: Table 4.8d: Reduplicated adverbs and adjectives Suba‑Simbiti Swahili English këmwëkëmwë sanasana very much bhöngöbhöngö harakaharaka very quickly bhöndëbhöndë vinginevingine many others ighoigho hivyohiyvo in this very way umwenemwene45 mwenyewe mwenyewe completely himself

However, when the meaning of a reduplicated form is to do with there being various groups (usually words to do with size and number), rather than being emphatic, then it is written disjunctively. The difference between the two types of reduplication seem very clear to the Suba‑Simbiti. Some examples are given below:

Table 4.8e: Disjuctive reduplication Suba‑Simbiti Swahili English ömwë ömwë mmoja mmoja one each

45 It is worth noting that the augment vowel of the second half of the reduplication elides. 56

Suba‑Simbiti Swahili English ighana ighana mia mia one-hundred each amakomo amakomo makundi makundi (to) each group

4.9 Compound Words There are a number of compound forms in Suba‑Simbiti, in which a new lexical form is created from a combination of two (or more) separate word stems (or sometimes partial word stems). Because the new word has a separate new meaning, more than the meaning of the two original words, it is a new word in its own right, and is written as a single word. Some examples are given in the table below:

Table 4.9a: Compound words Compound word Word 1 Word 2 ikibhuranshöghu ikibhuura inshöghu ‘aina ya mti’ ‘kishinda’ ‘tembo’ ‘type of tree’ ‘residue’ ‘elephant’ risandökëramanyëërö risandokera amanyëërö ‘aina ya mdudu’ ‘kurukia ghafla’ ‘pua (wingi)’ ‘type of insect’ ‘to jump up suddenly’ ‘noses’ kuuryabhësariirö kuurya bhësariirö ‘mwezi wa kumi’ ‘kuliza’ ‘mwewe’ ‘October’ ‘to make cry’ ‘kite (bird)’ omokaamoona omokari wa omoona ‘mke wa mwana’ ‘mke’ ‘wa’ ‘mtoto’ ‘daughter in law’ ‘wife’ ‘of’ ‘child’

Suba-Simbiti also has a compound present-continuous verb form composed of the Infinitive followed by an auxiliary verb which is written disjunctively. Although many Suba-Simbiti speakers naturally write these as a single word, when reading tests were conducted all speakers easily understood the compound form written disjunctively. Some examples are given below:

Table 4.9b: Compound verbs Orthography Swahili English nkorora ërë inafanya kazi it is working nkutuna örë unatafuta you are looking

57

Orthography Swahili English nkutwa arë yeye huchuma he gathers

5. Loan Words Many words, which are borrowed into Suba‑Simbiti from other languages, are adjusted to fit Suba‑Simbiti phonology, morphology and syllable structure. This is done by adapting consonants (for example, from the table below) and vowel quality or length to fit Suba‑Simbiti phonology (for example, ), adding or adjusting appropriate noun class prefixes (for example, and ), or adapting the syllable structure (for example, and ). Some known loan words are listed below:

Table 5a: Loan words Loan word Swahili Gloss English Gloss Origin seehera46 hela money Swahili ekekapo kikapu basket Swahili ekeraatö kiatu shoe Swahili ikisima kisima well Swahili eketambaara kitambaa piece of cloth Swahili ridirisha dirisha window Swahili okokaranga kukaanga to fry Swahili ukusuka kusuka to braid Swahili umushumaari msumari nail Swahili umusumeeno msumeno saw Swahili rishambi jamvi mat Swahili

However, one instance where borrowed words are not adjusted to fit Suba‑Simbiti phonological and syllable structure, is vowel clusters in words borrowed from Swahili, especially Biblical names. In these names other adjustments are made to fit Suba‑Simbiti phonology (such as to for /β/) but they have decided to keep the vowel clusters written as in Swahili, and in most cases not

46 This form is an exception to the usual pattern of vowel length in the class 10 suffix (see §2.1.3). 58

to insert a semivowel, in order to keep the name easily recognisable for readers. Some examples are given below:

Table 5b: Borrowed Biblical names Simbiti Orthography Swahili English Not to write: Yeremia Yeremia Jeremiah Yeremiya Ikiëbhuraania Kiebrania Hebrew Ikiyëbhuraaniya Arimatea Arimatea Arimathea Arimateya Antiökia Antiokia Antioch Antiyökiya

6. Punctuation

6.1 Sentence-Final Punctuation A period (full stop) is used after every declarative sentence. For example:

Naarëmirë ömöghöndö. ‘Nililima shamba.’ ‘I farmed a field.’

A question mark is used after every interrogative sentence. Interrogatives are often marked by question words like ‘where’ or ‘why’. For example:

N-dööhe yaasha? ‘Alikuja lini?’ ‘When did he come?’

Wamötëëbhëri? ‘Ulimwambia?’

‘Did you tell him?’ An exclamation mark is used after every exclamatory sentence to emphasise the information contained within the sentence. For example:

Nkörrë eng’ana! ‘Nimefanya jambo!’ ‘I have done something!’

6.2 Comma Commas should be used every time there is a pause in spoken communication.

Hano niishirë isho, tënamunyöörë hë. ‘Nilipokuja jana, sikumkuta.’ ‘When I came yesterday, I did not find him.’

Commas are also used to separate a quotation from the rest of a sentence:

59

Waryobha yaaghambirë okohetera ko ömörööti Isaaya igha, “Tamaaha, ono we ömököri uwaane...” ‘Mungu alisema kupitia kwa nabii Isaya kwamba, “Tazama, huyu ndiye mtumishi wangu...”’ ‘God said through the prophet Isaiah thus, “Look, this is indeed my servant...”’

Whenever a person’s name is used in direct reference, a comma should follow. For example:

Simööni, neena engʼana eno nkutuna nköbhööri. ‘Simoni, nina jambo ambalo ninataka nikuulize.’ ‘Simon, I have something that I want to ask you.’

Commas are also used to separate various items in a list, as shown in the example below:

Nena ekaramu, amanshë, ikitumbë, na umusi. ‘Nina kalamu, maji, kiti, na muwa.’ ‘I have a pen, water, a chair, and sugarcane.’

6.3 Colon A colon is used in separating the chapter and verse numbers of a Biblical passage. For example:

Mataayo 13:24 ‘Mathayo 13:24’ ‘Matthew 13:24’

Note that, in Suba-Simbiti, colons are not usually used at the beginning of a list; Suba-Simbiti writers prefer the use of a comma in this environment.47

6.4 Quotation Marks Quotation marks are used in Suba‑Simbiti for the quotation of speech. For example:

Wanshara yaantëëbhëri, “Ni-isho ndiisha.” ‘Wanshara aliniambia, “Kesho nitakuja.”’ ‘Wanshara told me, “Tomorrow I will come.”

47 A reason given for this is that some other languages in the Mara region use the colon as a grammatical tense marking; therefore, the Suba-Simbiti wish to avoid using the colon as punctuation to avoid confusion. 60

Hano yaamörööshë, akaghamba, “Nenkotema!” ‘Alipomwona, akasema, “Nitakupiga!”’ ‘When he saw him, he said, “I will hit you!”’

As in English and Swahili, the speech introducer is followed by a comma immediately prior to the quotation. The first word of the quotation is also capitalised, and the final sentence of the quotation is closed with a period, exclamation mark or question mark, before the closing quotation mark <”>. If there is a quotation inside another quotation, the single quotation marks <‘’> should be used to highlight this embedded quotation. Note that spaces are not used to separate the different quotation marks. For example:

Mwarimu Weghesa yaaghambirë igha, “Ikikaro ikya bA hasimbhëtë n-deero keraahwe, na Mwarimu Daudi akantëëbhya, ‘Nkiiya kyare.’” ‘Mwalimu Wegesa alisema kwamba, “Kikao cha Wasimbiti kitaisha leo, na Mwalimu David akaniambia, ‘Kilikuwa kizuri.’”’ ‘Teacher Wegesa said, “The Simbiti meeting will end today, and Teacher David told me, ‘It was good.’”’

7. Capitalisation The first letter of a word should be capitalised if it starts a sentence or a quotation:

Isho naasikaini na Omokarabhëëri Shasha. ‘Jana nilikutana na Mchungaji Shasha.’ ‘Yesterday I met with Pastor Shasha.’

Bhöökë yaantëëbhëri, “Ni-isho ndiisha.” ‘Booke aliniambia, “Kesho nitakuja.”’ ‘Booke told me, “Tomorrow I will come.”’

Also, proper names should have a capitalised first letter, e.g. Mariamu, Yohana, Tanzania.

Omotangati wa Bitiiryö arabherekerwa Masaka. ‘Kiongozi wa Bitiryo anaitwa Masaka.’ ‘The leader of Bitiryo is called Masaka.’

For capitalisation of proper nouns, the first letter of the word (not including clitics) should be capitalised, not the first letter of the root. Thus, we see the following:

61

After a regular verb: toraghya Ubhusimbëtë ‘tunaenda Usimbiti’ ‘we are going to Simbiti land’ After an associative: ekesaku ikya Abhasimbëtë ‘ukoo wa Wasimbiti’ ‘a clan of Simbiti’ After a conjunction: Abhakabhwa na Abhasimbëtë ‘Wakabwa na Wasimbiti’ ‘Kabwa and Simbiti people’ After a copula: bhano m-Basimbëtë ‘hawa ni Wasimbiti’ ‘these are Simbiti people’ After a locative: ku-Waryobha ‘mbele za Mungu’ ‘before God’

62

References Compton, Alison and John B. Walker. 2010. Suba‑Simbiti Orthography Sketch. Unpublished manuscript. SIL International.

Hill, Dustin, Anna-Lena Lindfors, Louise Nagler, Mark Woodward and Richard Yalonde. 2007. A Sociolinguistic Survey of the Bantu Languages in Mara Region, Tanzania. Dodoma, Tanzania: SIL International.

Maho, Jouni Filip. 2009. “NUGL Online: The Online Version of the New Updated Guthrie List, a Referential Classification of the Bantu Languages.” http://goto.glocalnet.net/mahopapers/nuglonline.pdf.

Mitterhofer, Bernadette and Holly Robinson. 2012. Short report of Suba‑Simbiti Dialect Survey. Manuscript. SIL

Nurse, Derek and Gérard Philippson. 2006. The Bantu Languages. 1st edition. London: Routledge.

Overton, Rebekah. 2017. Imbrication of the Anterior Suffix in Kabwa. Unpublished Manuscript. Redcliffe College, Univeristy of Gloucester.

Overton, Rebekah. 2018. Vowel Hiatus Resolution in Simbiti and the Orthographic Implications. MA Dissertation. Redcliffe College, University of Gloucester.

Simons, Gary F. & Charles D. Fennig (eds). 2018. Ethnologue: Languages of the world, Twenty-first edition. Dallas, Texas: SIL International. Online version: www.ethnologue.com.

Walker, John B. 2013. “Comparative Tense and Aspect in the Mara Bantu Languages: Towards a Linguistic History.” MA Thesis, Trinity Western University.

Walker, John B. and Lotta Aunio. 2016. Simbiti (JE431) melodic tones. Poster session presented at the 6th International Conference on Bantu Languages (Bantu 6), Helsinki.

63

Appendix A: Suba‑Simbiti Noun Class Chart

Class Prefix Example Swahili English 1 omo- omobhohe mfungwa prisoner ömö- ömörëmi mkulima farmer umu- umukungu mzee wa kike old woman 1a - maayö mama mother 2 abha- abhakungu wazee wa kike old women 2a bha- bhamaayö akina mama mothers 3 omo- omoghango mwiko large spoon ömö- ömöghöndö shamba field umu- umurigho mzigo burden 4 eme- emeghango miiko large spoons ëmë- ëmëghöndö mashamba fields imi- imirigho mizigo burdens 548 re- reghena jiwe rock rë- rëntö jitu giant ri- riköngö nchi kame barren land iri- iriihiimo49 laana curse 6 ama- amaköngö nchi kame (wingi) barren lands 7 eke- ekeghokora kiwiko elbow ëkë- ëkëntö kitu thing iki- ikighutu kibanda shelter

48 Examples of the and variants of the class 5 prefix are extremely rare; generally, the class 5 noun prefix doesn’t adhere to the usual vowel harmony process, and is found with all root vowels. 49 The prefix seems long , but this is because the root is /i-/initial, as can be seen from the plural, . 64

Class Prefix Example Swahili English 8 ebhe- ebheghokora viwiko elbows ëbhë- ëbhëntö vitu things ibhi- ibhighutu vibanda shelters 9 e- ebhamia bamia okra ë- ëhabhëri mfugo jike female herd animal i- ihuko fuko mole (rodent) 9a eN- engere aina ya samaki type of fish ëN- ëmbëëshë nguruwe pig iN- ingiho figo kidney 10 se- sebhamia bamia (nyingi) okra (plural) së- sëhabhëri mfugo jike (wingi) female herd animals si- sihuko mafuko moles 10a seN- sengere aina ya samaki (wingi) type sëN- sëmbëëshë nguruwe (wingi) pigs siN- singiho figo kidneys 11 oro- oroghendo safari journey örö- örökömö kucha claw, fingernail uru- urutu vumbi dust 12 aka- akatughi kiti kidogo small stool 14 obho- obhohoro kiporo leftovers öbhö- öbhöghöri bei price ubhu- ubhusyo uso face 15 oko- okotwe sikio ear ökö- ököghörö mguu leg uku- ukubhisha kuficha to hide 16 aha- ahase mahali place

65

Class Prefix Example Swahili English 17 ko- ko-kotwe kwenye sikio on the ear kö- kö-möghöndö kwenye shamba at the farm ku- ku-murigho kwenye mzigo on the luggage 18 mo- mo-kotwe ndani ya sikio in the ear mö- mö-möghöndö ndani ya shamba in the farm mu- mu-murigho ndani ya mzigo in the luggage 20 ogho- oghotomo ngoma kubwa large drum öghö- öghösëësë mbwa kubwa large dog ughu- ughutughi kiti kikubwa large stool 23 i- (y-) yeeka nyumbani home

66

Appendix B: Noun Class Pairings Class Sing. prefix Class Plural Prefix 1 omo-, ömö-, umu- 2 abha- 1a - 2a bha- 3 omo-, ömö-, umu- 4 eme-, ëmë-, imi 5 (e)re-, (ë)rë-, (i)ri- 6 ama- 7 eke-, ëkë-, iki- 8 ebhe-, ëbhë-, ibhi- 6 ama- 9 e(N)-, ë(N)-, i(N)- 10 se(N)-, së(N)-, si(N)- 6 ama- 11 oro-, örö-, uru 10 se(N)-, së(N)-, si(N)- 6 ama- 12 aka- 8 ebhe-, ëbhë-, ibhi- 14 obho-, öbhö-, ubhu- 6 ama- 15 oko-, ökö-, uku- 6 ama- 16 aha- - 20 ogho-, öghö-, ughu- 6 ama- 23 i- (y-) -

67

Appendix C: The History of Suba-Simbiti Orthography Development

History of pre-Trial Orthography Status events:

Date Event or Orthography Issues Tested or Changed 2005-2006 Language survey in the Mara Region 2006 Suba-Simbiti word collection workshop 2007 First alphabet creation workshops 2008 Orthography testing and Writers’ Awareness workshops Narrative discourse analysis workshop and the beginning of Bible 2009 translation into Simbiti

History of testing events and orthography changes and additions during the Trial Orthography period:

Year Event or Orthography Issues Tested or Changed Orthography testing was conducted during Simbiti literacy workshops at the SIL offive. Changes made: • Associatives should be written with underlying vowel 2010 <-a> rather than with phrase-level changes of the final vowel; if the associative has an augment vowel, it is written. • Negative verbal prefix should be written with a hyphen. (Later revised, see September 2011) Testing conducted during literacy workshop at the SIL office. Changes made: • Capitalise the first letter in proper nouns, not the first letter September 2011 of the stem. • The negative prefix should not be written with a hyphen. The negative postclitic will continue to be written disjunctively. Dialect survey of Simbiti, Kiroba, Iregi, Sweta, Hacha, Kine October 2011 and Surwa, to test the extensibility of Simbiti materials. Testing was done with the Suba-Simbiti language committee, January 2012 and after receiving clear feedback, two graphemes were changed: now writing rather than .

68

Year Event or Orthography Issues Tested or Changed Formation of Simbiti Reading Group in the village of Baraki, 2014 with a series of reading and writing workshops, in which the orthography continued to be informally tested. During an orthography testing workshop with the Baraki reading group, the following changes and additions were made: • Write the copula with class 5 nouns as rather than . • Write the 1SG object prefix /n-/ before all /h/-initial roots, November 2014 even when not pronounced (except with ). • Confirmed the acceptability of writing for the trill and other specific vowel length and trill spelling conventions in various verb form. • Write fricatives as stops (as pronounced) after the nasal copula, e.g. . Series of meetings held at the SIL office to evaluate and discuss remaining orthography questions. Clarifications and additions: • Write all reduplications conjunctively, unless the reduplication is distinguishing separate groups. • The vowel /a/ blocks vowel harmony, including in locatives and negatives. • Class 5 nouns should be written with a long prefix only for monosyllabic roots. • Vowel length in verb forms: o When there are two vowels underlyingly, write a October 2016 long vowel, even if it is not pronounced very long (e.g. in word-initial syllables, with Anterior and Subjunctive verbs). o Write when necessary (e.g. in P2). o Long vowels in verb roots should always be written long, even in inflected forms. • When the copula is written with short vowel-initial words, repeat the vowel before and after the hyphen. • In Biblical names borrowed from Swahili, do not insert a in vowel clusters, except when present in Swahili.

69

Year Event or Orthography Issues Tested or Changed Formation of second Simbiti Reading Group in the village of 2016-2017 Kukona, with a series of reading and writing workshops, in which the orthography continued to be informally tested. Workshop with Kiroba and Iregi speakers to test their ability to August 2017 use Suba-Simbiti materials. The Suba-Simbiti Language Committee gave their approval of March 2018 the Simbiti orthography.

70