Bade/Ngizim Vowels and Syllable Structure*

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Bade/Ngizim Vowels and Syllable Structure* Studies in African Linguistics 247 Volume 9, Number 3, December 1978 BADE/NGIZIM VOWELS AND SYLLABLE STRUCTURE* Russell G. Schuh University of California, Los Angeles Among the vowels of Bade and Ngizim, the short high vowels playa functional role different from the other vowels. Al­ though word final i and u are full-fledged phonemes (non­ predictable and contrastive), both the position of occurrence and the quality of the phonetic high vowels [i, a, uJ is pre­ dictable in medial position: the quality is determined by other segments in the environment; the position is determined by restrictions against certain groupings of consonants. Bade and Ngizim differ in one important respect in the placement of non-final short, high vowels, viz. through a change called PROTHESIS, original initial sequences of the type *ClaC2 ... (still realized as such in Ngizim) are now real­ ized as aC l C2 ... in Bade if Cl C2 is not an impermissible sequence. This is true for all words with no more than two consonants, though the situation is somewhat more complicated in longer words; tone of the .initial syllable is also seen to playa role. The permissible sequences of consonants, and as a consequence the environment for PROTHESIS, are discussed in the light of universal hierarchies of consonantal strength and principles of syllabification in conjunction with a restriction in Bade/Ngizim against two consonants occurring at a syllable margin. 1. Introduction 1.1. The languages. Bade and Ngizim are two closely related languages of the West Chadic branch of the Chadic family spoken in northeastern Nigeria. l *Research on Ngizim was conducted mainly from 1969-70 in Potiskum, Nigeria under the auspices of NSF grant no. GS-2279 (Paul Newman, Prin­ cipal Investigator). Research on Bade was conducted from 1973-75 in Gashua, Nigeria while I was employed as a Research Fellow in the Centre for the Study of Nigerian Languages, Bayero University College, Kano. My thanks to Paul Newman for useful comments on an earlier draft of this paper. lThe most recent classification of Chadic languages [Newman 1977J divides the Chadic family into four major branches: West Chadic, spoken almost entirely in northeastern Nigeria; Biu-Mandara, spoken in northern Cameroon and adjacent areas of Nigeria and Chad; East Chadic, spoken in Western and central Chad; and Masa, spoken in Cameroon and Chad south of N'Djamena. 248 Ngizim is spoken in an area fanning out to the east from Potiskum. Bade is spoken north of Ngizim in an area from Gashua to the west and south. Bade and Ngizim are no longer in contact, but the migration of the Ngizims southward and the ultimate separation of the two language com­ munities has taken place only in the last few hundred years. Ngizim has virtually no dialect differentiation other than a few lexical differences among villages. Bade, on the other hand, has con­ siderable differentiation at all linguistic levels (see Schuh [1977aJ for many examples in the determiner system and associative noun phrases). The dialect used here is that of Gashua and adjacent villages. Although there are a number of phonological differences between this dialect and others, all dialects are virtually identical in the respects relevant to this study. 1.2. Scope of the present study. One of the main areas of interest in Bade and Ngizim phonology is the distribution of high vowels. In fact, high vowels in both languages function primarily as epenthetic vowels to break up impermissible consonant sequences. The paper begins with a summary of the vowel pLonology of this group, concentrating on the dis­ tribution of high vowels. Following this is a description of a phonological difference between Bade and Ngizim, which is discussed and illustrated in detail. Briefly stated, this difference is the following: under certain conditions, in an initial sequence C[+~ighJ C Bade has dropped the vowel and added a prothetic high vowel to give a sequence [+~igh] CC, whereas Ngizim has not done this, e.g. Ngizim guzap 'slave' but Bade ugzaf 'slave'. Finally, certain universal principles of syllabification which have been proposed are cited as bearing on the distribution patterns of Bade and Ngizim high vowels. 2. Bade/Ngizim Vowels 2.1. Vowel nuclei. The simple vowel nuclei of Bade and Ngizim are given in (1): 249 i , i i [a] u, uu ee 00 a, aa Doubled vowels represent long vowels; the symbol a represents phonetic [~J and hence is one of the set of high vowels. The mid vowels are of secondary origin, having entered the languages through borrowing and monophthongization of diphthongs. Long aa and short a contrast in all environments, though long aa is extremely rare in word final position. Some minimal or near minimal pairs are gi ven in (2): 2 (2) aa a Ngizim Bade Ngizim Bade d:'aasu I &aasu I 'pour' dc'asu I &asu I 'finish' gaadu kaadu 'bite' gadu kadu 'break' aadau aadau 'south' adan adan 'crying' No high vowels can be reconstructed word initial for proto-Bade/ Ngizim,3 though in Bade the sound change mentioned above and discussed in detail below has produced initial [uuJ « *\~u), [iiJ « *yi), CuCJ « *Cu), and [aC] « *Ca). 2Tone marks are acute accent (') for high tone, grave accent (') for low, circumflex accent (A) for falling, and tick (') for downstep. The following transcription conventions should also be noted: c and j are voiceless and voiced. palatal affricates respectively; sh and zh are voicless and voiced palatal fricatives respectively; ny is a palatal nasal, analyzed as a unit phoneme, not a sequence; 'y is a glottalized palatal semivowel; tl and jl are voicless and voiced lateral fricatives respectively; kw and gw 8.l'e labialized velar stops, not stop-semivowel sequences. 3This statement is true for nouns. There may be one or two sur­ vivals of initial *i in verbs (see 'rabIe, i'n. 15). If we move one historical step back, we must reconstruct initial *i in both nmms and verbs. Dtlw",i, the mo::;t clc::;::oly related language to Bade and Ngizim, has initial i, which is phonetically CiiJ but does not con­ trast with [iJ in this position, e.g. )ida 'eye' (cf. Bade and Ngizim da), 1 ije 'dog' (cf. Bade and Ngizim ja). 250 Medially the long high vowels, ii and uu contrast with each other and with other vowels. There are no minimal ii/uu pairs, but there is no way to predict which vowel will be used on the basis of phonological environment. As we will see below, the short high vowels i/u/a are not in contrast medially--the quality of a medial short high vowel is phonologically predictable. Some minimal or near minimal sets showing medial contrast between ii and uu and between these vowels and the medial short high vowel(s) are given in (3): (3) long high vowels short high vowels z) idu s)idu 'slaughter' 'zadu aZdu 'six' v(ida f(ida 'hare' veda avda 'open space' duuzh) duuz) 'owl' dazh f dez ( 'vein' , , , , ruunu luunu 'spread to dry' rnu rnu « *ranu) 'fornicate' Long high vowels do not occur underlyingly in word final position. There is, however, a monophthongization rule shared by Bade and Ngizim which changes the word final diphthong -ai to [ii] and -au to [uu] when the word occurs in the middle of a phrase, e.g. (4) (Ng) rakf i c"""; } 'bed' bA;} 'it's not a bed' (Ba) laka i lakf i bai I , (Ng and Ba) sesau sasuu bai 'it's not a hut' The short high vowels and u are in contrast with each other and with a and aa (also with the mid vowels ee and 00) in word final position. There are no lexical min'imals pairs distinguished only by final or u,4 but choice of i, u, or the absence of a vowel cannot be predicted phonologically. The vowel [a] does not occur under­ lyingly word final, but word final Iii and lui change to [a] medially in a phrase under the appropriate conditions (see below). 4~linimal sets can be constructed in the verbal system where dif­ ferent verb forms are marked by final vowel changes, among other things, e.g. Ngizim ja karu 'we stole', ja kar) 'that we steal', ja kara 'we should steal'. 251 (5) Final Final u 'fat' kunamu kynamu 'fan palm' 'beard' aatar~ atal~ 'ridgerow' 'rope' kwayu a' d"Iyu , (Ng) 'jujube' (Ba) 'gourd' gabaabu daabu (Ng) 'billy-goat' (Ba) 'middle' dukshl duksl 'heavy' k'utasu J;'uksu , (Ng) 'shea-nut' (Ba) type of weed avj I 'monkey' zadu aZdu 'six' Final C kwam 'bull' zager 'foot' guzai 'pubic hair' 'slave' 'sorrel' 'ashes' 2.2. Quality of medial short high vowels. The quality of medial short high vowels is determined by phonological environment. The rule for determining the choice of [iJ, [uJ, or [aJ is given in (6).5 (6) in the env. of y +h~gh] in the env. of w or labialized velar r,--long {~ elsewhere Conditions: (1) Vowel does not precede pause. (2) When the env. for both [iJ and [uJ are met, the env. following the vowel overrides the env. preceding the vowel. 5Phonetically a wider range of vowels than these exists. Thus, following palatals the vowel tends to be somewhat fronted, e.g. Ng jajem [JI.JLmJ 'thorn', and following labial fricatives the vowel tends to be somewhat rounded, e.g. Ng varu [voruJ Igo out'. Also, the following environment generally exercises a stronger influence than preceding. Thus, the vowel following. w is not as strongly rounded as when preceding w, and in my field notes I have often transcribed a a instead of a u following w but never preceding w, e.g.
Recommended publications
  • Livret Des Résumés Booklet of Abstracts
    34èmes Journées de Linguistique d’Asie Orientale JLAO34 34th Paris Meeting on East Asian Linguistics 7–9 juillet 2021 / July, 7th–9th 2021 Colloque en ligne / Online Conference LIVRET DES RÉSUMÉS BOOKLET OF ABSTRACTS Comité d’organisation/Organizing committee Raoul BLIN, Ludovica LENA, Xin LI, Lin XIAO [email protected] *** Table des matières / Table of contents *** Van Hiep NGUYEN (Keynote speaker): On the study of grammar in Vietnam Julien ANTUNES: Description et analyse de l’accent des composés de type NOM-GENITIF-NOM en japonais moderne Giorgio Francesco ARCODIA: On ‘structural particles’ in Sinitic languages: typology and diachrony Huba BARTOS: Mandarin Chinese post-nuclear glides under -er suffixation Bianca BASCIANO: Degree achievements in Mandarin Chinese: A comparison between 加 jiā+ADJ and 弄 nòng+ADJ verbs Etienne BAUDEL: Chinese and Sino-Japanese lexical items in the Hachijō language of Japan Françoise BOTTERO: Xu Shen’s graphic analysis revisited Tsan Tsai CHAN: Cartographic fieldwork on sentence-final particles – Three challenges and some ways around them Hanzhu CHEN & Meng CHENG: Corrélation entre l’absence d’article et la divergence lexicale Shunting CHEN, Yiming LIANG & Pascal AMSILI: Chinese Inter-clausal Anaphora in Conditionals: A Linear Regression Study Zhuo CHEN: Differentiating two types of Mandarin unconditionals: Their internal and external syntax Katia CHIRKOVA: Aspect, Evidentiality, and Modality in Shuhi Anastasia DURYMANOVA: Nouns and verbs’ syntactic shift: some evidences against Old Chinese parts-of- speech
    [Show full text]
  • A Grammar of Gyeli
    A Grammar of Gyeli Dissertation zur Erlangung des akademischen Grades doctor philosophiae (Dr. phil.) eingereicht an der Kultur-, Sozial- und Bildungswissenschaftlichen Fakultät der Humboldt-Universität zu Berlin von M.A. Nadine Grimm, geb. Borchardt geboren am 28.01.1982 in Rheda-Wiedenbrück Präsident der Humboldt-Universität zu Berlin Prof. Dr. Jan-Hendrik Olbertz Dekanin der Kultur-, Sozial- und Bildungswissenschaftlichen Fakultät Prof. Dr. Julia von Blumenthal Gutachter: 1. 2. Tag der mündlichen Prüfung: Table of Contents List of Tables xi List of Figures xii Abbreviations xiii Acknowledgments xv 1 Introduction 1 1.1 The Gyeli Language . 1 1.1.1 The Language’s Name . 2 1.1.2 Classification . 4 1.1.3 Language Contact . 9 1.1.4 Dialects . 14 1.1.5 Language Endangerment . 16 1.1.6 Special Features of Gyeli . 18 1.1.7 Previous Literature . 19 1.2 The Gyeli Speakers . 21 1.2.1 Environment . 21 1.2.2 Subsistence and Culture . 23 1.3 Methodology . 26 1.3.1 The Project . 27 1.3.2 The Construction of a Speech Community . 27 1.3.3 Data . 28 1.4 Structure of the Grammar . 30 2 Phonology 32 2.1 Consonants . 33 2.1.1 Phonemic Inventory . 34 i Nadine Grimm A Grammar of Gyeli 2.1.2 Realization Rules . 42 2.1.2.1 Labial Velars . 43 2.1.2.2 Allophones . 44 2.1.2.3 Pre-glottalization of Labial and Alveolar Stops and the Issue of Implosives . 47 2.1.2.4 Voicing and Devoicing of Stops . 51 2.1.3 Consonant Clusters .
    [Show full text]
  • Linguistic Nature of Prenasalization
    City University of New York (CUNY) CUNY Academic Works All Dissertations, Theses, and Capstone Projects Dissertations, Theses, and Capstone Projects 1977 Linguistic Nature of Prenasalization Mark H. Feinstein The Graduate Center, City University of New York How does access to this work benefit ou?y Let us know! More information about this work at: https://academicworks.cuny.edu/gc_etds/2207 Discover additional works at: https://academicworks.cuny.edu This work is made publicly available by the City University of New York (CUNY). Contact: [email protected] INFORMATION TO USERS This material was produced from a microfilm copy of the original document. While the most advanced technological means to photograph and reproduce this document have been used, the quality is heavily dependent upon the quality of the original submitted. The following explanation of techniques is provided to help you understand markings or patterns which may appear on this reproduction. 1. The sign or "target" for pages apparently lacking from the document photographed is "Missing Page(s)". If it was possible to obtain the missing page(s) or section, they are spliced into the film along with adjacent pages. This may have necessitated cutting thru an image and duplicating adjacent pages to insure you complete continuity. 2. When an image on the film is obliterated w ith a large round black mark, it is an indication that the photographer suspected that the copy may have moved during exposure and thus cause a blurred image. You will find a good image of the page in the adjacent frame. 3. When a map, drawing or chart, etc., was part o f the material being photographed the photographer followed a definite method in "sectioning" the material.
    [Show full text]
  • Proposal for Superscript Diacritics for Prenasalization, Preglottalization, and Preaspiration
    1 Proposal for superscript diacritics for prenasalization, preglottalization, and preaspiration Patricia Keating Department of Linguistics, UCLA [email protected] Daniel Wymark Department of Linguistics, UCLA [email protected] Ryan Sharif Department of Linguistics, UCLA [email protected] ABSTRACT The IPA currently does not specify how to represent prenasalization, preglottalization, or preaspiration. We first review some current transcription practices, and phonetic and phonological literature bearing on the unitary status of prenasalized, preglottalized and preaspirated segments. We then propose that the IPA adopt superscript diacritics placed before a base symbol for these three phenomena. We also suggest how the current IPA Diacritics chart can be modified to allow these diacritics to be fit within the chart. 2 1 Introduction The IPA provides a variety of diacritics which can be added to base symbols in various positions: above ([a͂ ]), below ([n̥ ]), through ([ɫ]), superscript after ([tʰ]), or centered after ([a˞]). Currently, IPA diacritics which modify base symbols are never shown preceding them; the only diacritics which precede are the stress marks, i.e. primary ([ˈ]) and secondary ([ˌ]) stress. Yet, in practice, superscript diacritics are often used preceding base symbols; specifically, they are often used to notate prenasalization, preglottalization and preaspiration. These terms are very common in phonetics and phonology, each having thousands of Google hits. However, none of these phonetic phenomena is included on the IPA chart or mentioned in Part I of the Handbook of the International Phonetic Association (IPA 1999), and thus there is currently no guidance given to users about transcribing them. In this note we review these phenomena, and propose that the Association’s alphabet include superscript diacritics preceding the base symbol for prenasalization, preglottalization and preaspiration, in accord with one common way of transcribing them.
    [Show full text]
  • O. Introduction
    Studies in African Linguisticss Volume 26, Number 2, Fall 1997 TWO KINDS OF MORAIC NASAL IN CIYAO* Larry M. Hyman University of California, Berkeley and Annindo S. A. Ngunga University of California, Berkeley Eduardo Mondlane University A problematic issue in a number of Bantu languages concerns the phonological analysis of "preconsonantal nasality", i.e., the question of whether NC entities should be analyzed as single prenasalized consonants or as sequences of nasal + (homorganic) consonant. In this paper, the authors examine two kinds of moraic nasal---one syllabic, one not-in Ciyao, a Bantu language spoken in East Africa. They further demonstrate that there is a third type of preconsonantal nasality in Ciyao which is neither moraic nor syllabic. o. Introduction Among the few problematic issues surrounding the syllabification of consonants in Bantu languages is the proper interpretation of "preconsonantal nasality": For each relevant language of the family, one must first ask whether the sequencing of [+nasal] [-nasal] in forms such as Proto-Bantu *ba-ntu 'people' is properly viewed as a consonant cluster or as a single prenasalized consonant. Noting that * Research on Ciyao has been conducted in the context of the Comparative Bantu On-Line Dictionary (CBOLD) project, supported in part by National Science Foundation Grants #SBR93- 19415 and #SBR96-16330. Earlier versions of this paper were given at D.C. Santa Cruz, Universite Lumiere Lyon2, Universite d' Aix-en-Provence, and the University of Edinburgh. We would like to thank the participants at these presentations as well as Robert Botne and two anonymous reviewers for helpful comments on the paper.
    [Show full text]
  • Using Native Lexical Resources to Create Technical Neologisms for Minority Languages James A
    0 Using native lexical resources to create technical neologisms for minority languages James A. Matisoff University of California, Berkeley [email protected] Department of Linguistics 1203 Dwinelle Hall UC Berkeley Berkeley, CA 94720-2650 1 Using native lexical resources to create technical neologisms for minority languages 2 Several young Lahu speakers have been receiving graduate training in linguistics at Payap University in Chiang Mai, Thailand. However, the language of instruction in their linguistics courses is English or Thai, since like most other minority languages, Lahu lacks a technical vocabulary in which to discuss scientific subjects. Faced with such a problem, languages typically resort to borrowing technical terminology from majority languages. In the case of Lahu, a language dispersed over several countries, borrowings have been flooding in from Chinese, Burmese, Shan, Northern Thai, Standard Thai, Lao, and Vietnamese. This makes it difficult to decide which languages to borrow technical terms from. It therefore seems to me that the best solution would be to invent such terms using native Lahu morphemes as much as possible. In order to achieve this goal, I have been collaborating with a Lahu student at Payap to develop such a linguistic vocabulary. So far several hundred terms have been proposed, mostly in the realm of phonology. As this effort develops, we shall concentrate more on morphosyntactic and historical linguistic terminology. This paper discusses the issues involved in creating technical neologisms, and lists the terms so far proposed. * 近年來有好幾位拉祜族學生在泰國清邁西北大學 (Payap University) 進修語言學碩 士。但由於拉祜語(像多數其他的少數民族語言)缺乏語言學的技術詞彙,討論語言學的 時候通常用英語或泰語。 面對著這種問題,很多語言以借貸多數語言的技術詞彙為解決方法。分佈在好幾個 國家的拉祜語已吸收了從漢、緬、掸、北泰、標準泰、老撾和越南語言的借詞。在這個複 * This material is based upon work supported by the National Science Foundation under Grant Nos.
    [Show full text]
  • Looking Into Segments Sharon Inkelas and Stephanie S Shih University of California, Berkeley & University of California, Merced
    Looking into Segments Sharon Inkelas and Stephanie S Shih University of California, Berkeley & University of California, Merced 1 Introduction It is well-known that the phonological ‘segment’ (consonant, vowel) is internally dynamic. Complex segments, such as affricates or prenasalized stops, have sequenced internal phases; coarticulation induces change over time even within apparently uniform segments. Autosegmental Phonology (e.g., Goldsmith 1976, Sagey 1986) captured the internal phasing of complex segments using feature values ordered sequentially on a given tier and ‘linked’ to the same timing unit. Articulatory Phonology (e.g., Browman & Goldstein 1992, Gafos 2002, Goldstein et al. 2009) captures internal phases through the use of coordinated gestures, which overlap in time with one another but are aligned to temporal landmarks. Segments can be internally dynamic in a contrastive way. Affricates differ from plain stops or plain fricatives in being sequentially complex; the same difference obtains between prenasalized vs. plain stops, between contour and level tones, and so forth. Segments can also be dynamic in a noncontrastive way, due to coarticulation with surrounding segments. To the extent that phonological patterns are sensitive to contrastive or noncontrastive segment-internal phasing, the phasing needs to be represented in a manner that is legible to phonological grammar. However, contemporary phonological analysis couched in Optimality Theory, Harmonic Grammar and similar approaches is very highly segment-oriented. For example, Agreement by Correspondence theory, or ABC (Hansson 2001, 2010; Rose & Walker 2004; Bennett 2013; inter alia) and other surface correspondence theories of harmony and disharmony are theories of segmental correspondence. The constraints in these theories refer to segments as featurally uniform units, and do not have a way of referencing their internal phases.
    [Show full text]
  • Rethinking the Proto-Tibeto-Burman *A- Prefix: Glottal and Nasal Complications 31
    Journal of Asian and African Studies, No.96, 2018 Article Rethinking the Proto-Tibeto-Burman *a- prefix:* Glottal and Nasal Complications MATISOFF, James A. There has always been something rather anomalous about the PTB prefix conventionally reconstructed as *a-. While all the other prefixes set up for the proto-language (*s-, *m-, *b-, *d-, *g-, *r-) are consonantal, *a- looks as if it consisted of a simple vowel. My contention is that the phonetics and morphophonemics of this prefix were considerably more complicated than that: · The prefix should be reconstructed with a glottal stop preceding the vowel (i.e. *ʔa-), bringing it in line with the other consonantal prefixes. · Both stressed and unstressed variants should be recognized (i.e. *ʔa- vs. *ʔə-). · A nasal increment to the prefix seems to have arisen at an early date, via the mechanism of rhinoglottophilia (see Section IV), leading to forms like *ʔaɴ (stressed) and *ʔə̃- (unstressed). · Some languages, notably Mikir (Karbi), Lotha, and Akha, developed a palatalized as well as a non-palatalized variant of the nasalized prefix (i.e. *ʔaɴ- ~ *yaɴ-). · Certain languages (Tibetan, Proto-Lolo-Burmese) underwent loss (apheresis) of the vowel of the nasalized prefix, yielding prenasalized monosyllabic forms. · Lahu (and perhaps other languages) have somehow developed a stop-finalled variant in addition to the nasal-finalled one (i.e., *ʔaŋ > *ʔak-). See Section VII. The prefix occurs in dozens of languages with a wide variety of interrelated semantic functions, most of them apparently having to do with the notion of inalienable possession: kinship terms, body-parts, adjectives, genitives, 3rd person pronouns.
    [Show full text]
  • Reconstruction of Nasality and Other Aspects of A'ingae Phonology
    Reconstruction of nasality and other aspects of A’ingae phonology∗ Chelsea Sanker and Scott AnderBois Abstract: We propose a reconstruction of several sound changes in the history of A’ingae, a language isolate spoken in Ecuador and Colombia, using evidence from the phonology and lexicon of the modern language. Crucial to this reconstruction process is distinguishing ordinary inherited lexical items from borrowed forms, ideophones, and forms which are morphologically complex, each of which may sometimes exhibit different patterns. The major element of the reconstruction is a set of related changes in nasality. We derive the prenasalized series of stops and affricates from the voiceless unaspirated series, based on reanalysis of preceding nasal codas, which would have been the only codas permitted other than glottal stops, as part of the following consonant. The voiceless unaspirated and prenasalized series became contrastive word-initially as the result of deletion of certain word-initial nasal vowels. Coda nasals also coalesced with following glides, producing a new palatal nasal phoneme. Remaining nasal codas later developed into nasality on vowels. We also propose a set of more speculative palatalization processes, alterations to the velar approximant, and developments of diphthongs. All of these changes have substantially altered the phonological appearance of A’ingae; improving our understanding of the form it had further in the past may facilitate identification of its affiliation with a language family or identification of loanwords. Keywords: Sound change, internal reconstruction, A’ingae, language isolates, nasality, loan- words ∗Our heartfelt thanks to the A’ingae speakers who have generously share their language and time on aspects of this project: Shen Aguinda, Jorge Mendua, Raúl Queta, and Martín Criollo.
    [Show full text]
  • Liko Phonology and Grammar : a Bantu Language of the Democratic Republic of the Congo Issue Date: 2015-11-05
    Cover Page The handle http://hdl.handle.net/1887/36066 holds various files of this Leiden University dissertation Author: Wit, Gerrit de Title: Liko phonology and grammar : a Bantu language of the Democratic Republic of the Congo Issue Date: 2015-11-05 2 Phonological Structure 2.1 Introduction This chapter describes the phonological structure of the Liko language, its consonant and vowel systems, syllable structure and word structure. Syllable structure will be presented after the interpretation of surface glides and sequences of two identical vowels. The inventory of Liko consonants comprises the places and manners of articulation common in Bantu languages and includes prenasalized plosives and fricatives, implosives and plosives with double articulation. The consonants are presented and contrasted, with remarks about their distribution in roots. An analysis of complex consonants is given after the contrasts. I will describe syllable structure after consonants and vowels, because it depends on the analysis of sequences of identical vowels in the section on vowels. Syllable structures within roots in Liko are: CV, V and CGV, where CV and V are the most common. The presentation of these structures is followed by the analysis of the phonetic nature of "G" in CGV structures. A description of glides and an account of the changes imposed on loanwords are given at the end of the section on syllable structure. The structure of words in terms of sequences of syllables is presented in the section on Word Structure, for nouns and verbs separately. Liko has few consonant constraints as far as the position in the word is concerned.
    [Show full text]
  • Universals of Prenasalized Consonants: Phonemic Or Derived, Single Or Complex?*
    Jin-young Tak 127 Journal of Universal Language 12-2 September 2011, 127-158 Universals of Prenasalized Consonants: * Phonemic or Derived, Single or Complex? Jin-young Tak Sejong University 1 Abstract Most previous research on prenasalized consonants (Herbert 1975, 1977, 1986; Hayes 1991; Hyman 1992) focuses mainly on those derived from two underlying segments and functioning as a unitary segment. However, by examining further data from various languages, this paper proposes that in fact there are four types of prenasalized consonants: phonemic prenasalized consonants as in Fijian, derived prenasalized consonants functioning as a unitary segment as in Kikuyu, derived prenasalized consonants that behave as a complex segment as in Japanese, and phonetically enhanced prenasalized consonants as in Southern Barasano. Additionally, this paper compares phonologically driven prenasalized consonants to morphologically driven ones, arguing that in most cases phonologically * I would like to express my genuine gratitude to the three anonymous reviewers of this journal for their valuable comments. All errors and misrepresentations are the author’s responsibility. Jin-young Tak Department of English Language and Literature, Sejong University 98 Gunja-dong, Gwangjin-gu, Seoul 143-747 Korea Phone +82-(0)2-3408-3637; Email: [email protected] Received July 10, 2011; Revised August 31, 2011; Accepted September 9, 2011. 128 Universals of Prenasalized Consonants driven consonants are developed into a single segment, while in a number of the languages the morphologically driven nasal-plus- consonant sequences turn out to be sequences of a syllabic nasal and a consonant (i.e., NC). Furthermore, based on the typology of prenasalized consonants, this paper argues that phonetic adjustments, such as vowel lengthening or duration of prenasalized consonants, cannot be a cue in deciding their phonemic or phonetic status since some Bantu languages, wherein prenasalized consonants are derived from underlying two segments, behave as a singleton and do not lengthen a preceding vowel.
    [Show full text]
  • Highly Complex Syllable Structure
    Highly complex syllable structure A typological and diachronic study Shelece Easterday language Studies in Laboratory Phonology 9 science press Studies in Laboratory Phonology Chief Editor: Martine Grice Editors: Doris Mücke, Taehong Cho In this series: 1. Cangemi, Francesco. Prosodic detail in Neapolitan Italian. 2. Drager, Katie. Linguistic variation, identity construction, and cognition. 3. Roettger, Timo B. Tonal placement in Tashlhiyt: How an intonation system accommodates to adverse phonological environments. 4. Mücke, Doris. Dynamische Modellierung von Artikulation und prosodischer Struktur: Eine Einführung in die Artikulatorische Phonologie. 5. Bergmann, Pia. Morphologisch komplexe Wörter im Deutschen: Prosodische Struktur und phonetische Realisierung. 6. Feldhausen, Ingo & Fliessbach, Jan & Maria del Mar Vanrell. Methods in prosody: A Romance language perspective. 7. Tilsen, Sam. Syntax with oscillators and energy levels. 8. Ben Hedia, Sonia. Gemination and degemination in English affixation: Investigating the interplay between morphology, phonology and phonetics. 9. Easterday, Shelece. Highly complex syllable structure: A typological and diachronic study. ISSN: 2363-5576 Highly complex syllable structure A typological and diachronic study Shelece Easterday language science press Easterday, Shelece. 2019. Highly complex syllable structure: A typological and diachronic study (Studies in Laboratory Phonology 9). Berlin: Language Science Press. This title can be downloaded at: http://langsci-press.org/catalog/book/249 © 2019, Shelece
    [Show full text]