Physical Cognition in Great Apes

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Physical Cognition in Great Apes PHYSICAL COGNITION IN GREAT APES: PLANNING & OBJECT COMPLIANCE Emma Carolan Tecwyn A thesis submitted to The University of Birmingham For the degree of DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY School of Biosciences College of Life and Environmental Sciences University of Birmingham December 2013 University of Birmingham Research Archive e-theses repository This unpublished thesis/dissertation is copyright of the author and/or third parties. The intellectual property rights of the author or third parties in respect of this work are as defined by The Copyright Designs and Patents Act 1988 or as modified by any successor legislation. Any use made of information contained in this thesis/dissertation must be in accordance with that legislation and must be properly acknowledged. Further distribution or reproduction in any format is prohibited without the permission of the copyright holder. \An Orang climbs so slowly and cautiously... Even when closely pursued, his circumspection is amazing: he shakes the branches to see if they will bear him, and then bending an overhanging bough down by throwing his weight gradually along it, he makes a bridge from the tree he wishes to quit to the next." - T.H. Huxley From: Evidence as to man's place in nature (1863) Abstract It has been suggested that great ape cognitive abilities evolved in response to challenges in the physical environment related to large body size. As wild orangutans travel through the forest canopy compliant branches deform under their weight, which increases the size of gaps to be crossed and poses issues of safety and stability; yet they achieve safe and effiecient locomotion. Wild orangutans also build structurally complex nests and select branches for nest-building on the basis of their compliant properties. Both of these behaviours suggest that orangutans (1) are able to consider alternative possible actions or to plan and execute appropriate sequences of actions; and (2) possess some knowledge of object compliance (flexibility). This thesis investigated these cognitive abilities in captive great apes (orangutans and bonobos) as well as human adults and children, by presenting individuals with novel problem-solving tasks (puzzle-boxes) and novel objects (locomotor supports) and observing their behavioural responses. Attempts were made to address previous issues associated with studying physical cognition in animals, and particular attention was paid to individual differences and the role of exploration when interpreting results. Studying cognitive abilities related to wild orangutan behaviour may have implications for the evolution of cognition in great apes. i Acknowledgments First and foremost I would like to thank my supervisors Jackie Chappell and Susannah Thorpe for providing me with the incredible opportunity to work with some of our closest living relatives. I am particularly grateful to them for allowing me to develop independence and forge my own path throughout the course of my PhD, but still always being there when I needed guidance, encouragement and support. Thank you both for lots of inspiring brainstorming sessions, always getting comments on manuscripts back to me so quickly, and most of all for being approachable friendly faces. I couldn't have hoped for better supervisors. I was lucky enough to be part of two research groups: Jackie's Cognitive Adaptations Research Group and Susannah's Locomotor Ecology & Biomechanics Lab. It was great to have two quite different perspectives on my work, and I'd like to thank members of both groups { past and present { for useful discussions and practical help over the past four years: Abi, Emily, Jolyon, Julia, Kirsty, Nardie, Sam and Zoe. Special thanks to Jolyon Troscianko and Sam Coward for lending me tools to build the paddle-box; Sam's engineering prowess also enabled me to make the paddle-box ape-proof, which was no mean feat! Sam also took the time to teach me about beam biomechanics. Zoe Demery was always immensely generous with her time when I needed help, but deserves very special thanks for assisting in data collection at schools; it wouldn't have been possible (or half as fun!) without you. Abi Phillips gave me lots of tips about the orangutans in Dutch zoos with whom she had previously worked and Julia Myatt helped me identify some very bizarre orangutan positional behaviour! Interactions with researchers in other disciplines at the University of Birmingham have instilled in me of the importance of an interdisciplinary approach, and I am particularly grateful to Sarah Beck and Ian Apperly in Psychology for advice on cognitive research with children, and Aaron Sloman in Computer Sciences for challenging and inspiring discussions ii about information processing and AI. Organising an interdisciplinary workshop two years in a row with Nicola Cutting and Zoe Demery was a fantastic experience. This thesis would not have been possible without the zoos and schools that permitted me to conduct research. Many staff need to be thanked for granting permission for research, generously giving their time and sharing their knowledge, and being so enthusiastic and welcoming: Frank Rietkerk, Rudy Berends, Bianca Klein, Leo Hulsker, Cora Schout and Wilma at Apenheul Primate Park, Netherlands; Dirk-Jan van der Kolk, Jord de Meier, Olaf van Leeuwen, Monique Mul, Dorothe and Natasja at Ouwehands Dierenpark Rhenen, Netherlands; Kevin Caley, Charlotte MacDonald, John Buchan, Donna Smithson, Amber Cocks and Emma at Twycross Zoo, UK; all involved in the orangutan beam project at Chester Zoo, UK; Kate Tilley at ARK Tindal Primary Academy; Tricia Myatt at Stoneydelph Primary; and Nikki Devey at Clifton Primary. Constanze Mager-Melicharek deserves a special mention for giving me a place to stay during two separate stints at Apenheul. On a personal note, I would like to thank my parents Pam and Tex for being endlessly supportive and interested in my work; my very good friends Row and Kaat for always being there, and last but not least Paul: for keeping me sane during my time in the Netherlands and being an all round awesome person. This research was funded by the Natural Environment Research Council. iii List of publications Chapter 2 (from page 26): Tecwyn, E.C., Thorpe, S.K.S., Chappell, J. (2012) What cognitive strategies do orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus) use to solve a trial-unique puzzle-tube task incorporating multiple obstacles? Animal Cognition 15: 121-133 Chapter 3 (from page 54): Tecwyn, E.C., Thorpe, S.K.S., Chappell, J. (2013) A novel test of planning ability: great apes can plan step-by-step but not in advance of action. Behavioural Processes 100: 74-84 Chapter 4 (from page 84): Tecwyn, E.C., Thorpe, S.K.S., Chappell, J. (under review) Development of planning in 4- to 10-year-old children: reducing inhibitory demands does not improve performance. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology iv Declaration of author’s contribution Chapter 1: Entirely my own work Chapter 2: ECT designed the experiment, with input from JC and SKST. ECT collected the data, analysed the data and wrote the manuscript. JC and SKST assisted with discussion of the analysis and writing of the manuscript. Chapter 3: ECT designed the experiment, with input from JC and SKST. ECT collected the data, analysed the data and wrote the manuscript. JC and SKST assisted with discussion of the analysis and writing of the manuscript. Chapter 4: ECT designed the experiment, with input from JC and SKST. ECT collected the data, with the assistance of Zoe Demery. ECT analysed the data and wrote the manuscript. JC and SKST assisted with discussion of the analysis and writing of the manuscript. Chapter 5: The experiment was run by Emma-Louise Bryant as part of her undergraduate research project, under the supervision of ECT. The study was designed by ECT and EB with input from JC and SKST. ECT collected the data (from video footage), analysed the data and wrote the manuscript. Chapter 6: ECT designed the experiment, with input from JC and SKST. ECT collected the data, analysed the data and wrote the manuscript. JC and SKST assisted with discussion of the analysis and writing of the manuscript. Chapter 7: The study formed part of a wider project run by SKST and Sam Coward. ECT collected the data, analysed the data and wrote the manuscript. JC and SKST assisted with discussion of the analysis and writing of the manuscript. Chapter 8: Entirely my own work v Contents List of Figures xi List of Tables xii 1 General Introduction1 1.1 Animal cognition and cognitive adaptations.................2 1.2 Evolution of primate cognition........................3 1.3 Great ape cognition: driven by unique physical challenges?........5 1.4 Orangutans as a focal species.........................8 1.5 Planning in the context of physical problem-solving.............9 1.6 Compliance in the context of physical problem-solving........... 15 1.7 Studying physical cognition: considerations and challenges for researchers 18 1.7.1 Captive vs. wild studies........................ 18 1.7.2 Designing appropriate tasks...................... 20 1.7.3 Dealing with the findings: small sample sizes, negative results and individual variation.......................... 22 1.8 Aims, objectives and structure of this thesis................. 23 2 What cognitive strategies do orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus) use to solve a trial-unique puzzle-tube task incorporating multiple obstacles? 26 2.1 Introduction.................................. 28 2.2 Materials and methods............................ 34 2.2.1 Subjects and housing......................... 34 2.2.2 Apparatus: the puzzle-tube...................... 34 2.2.3 General procedure........................... 36 2.2.4 Data scoring and analysis....................... 39 2.3 Results..................................... 40 2.3.1 Initial choice of direction....................... 40 2.3.2 Directional preferences......................... 41 2.3.3 Self-correction............................. 42 2.3.4 Rule simulations............................ 42 2.3.5 GLMMs................................. 44 2.4 Discussion................................... 45 3 A novel test of planning ability: great apes can plan step-by-step but not in advance of action 54 3.1 Introduction..................................
Recommended publications
  • EAZA Best Practice Guidelines Bonobo (Pan Paniscus)
    EAZA Best Practice Guidelines Bonobo (Pan paniscus) Editors: Dr Jeroen Stevens Contact information: Royal Zoological Society of Antwerp – K. Astridplein 26 – B 2018 Antwerp, Belgium Email: [email protected] Name of TAG: Great Ape TAG TAG Chair: Dr. María Teresa Abelló Poveda – Barcelona Zoo [email protected] Edition: First edition - 2020 1 2 EAZA Best Practice Guidelines disclaimer Copyright (February 2020) by EAZA Executive Office, Amsterdam. All rights reserved. No part of this publication may be reproduced in hard copy, machine-readable or other forms without advance written permission from the European Association of Zoos and Aquaria (EAZA). Members of the European Association of Zoos and Aquaria (EAZA) may copy this information for their own use as needed. The information contained in these EAZA Best Practice Guidelines has been obtained from numerous sources believed to be reliable. EAZA and the EAZA APE TAG make a diligent effort to provide a complete and accurate representation of the data in its reports, publications, and services. However, EAZA does not guarantee the accuracy, adequacy, or completeness of any information. EAZA disclaims all liability for errors or omissions that may exist and shall not be liable for any incidental, consequential, or other damages (whether resulting from negligence or otherwise) including, without limitation, exemplary damages or lost profits arising out of or in connection with the use of this publication. Because the technical information provided in the EAZA Best Practice Guidelines can easily be misread or misinterpreted unless properly analysed, EAZA strongly recommends that users of this information consult with the editors in all matters related to data analysis and interpretation.
    [Show full text]
  • Spatial Representation of Magnitude in Gorillas and Orangutans ⇑ Regina Paxton Gazes A,B, , Rachel F.L
    Cognition 168 (2017) 312–319 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Cognition journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/COGNIT Original Articles Spatial representation of magnitude in gorillas and orangutans ⇑ Regina Paxton Gazes a,b, , Rachel F.L. Diamond c,g, Jasmine M. Hope d, Damien Caillaud e,f, Tara S. Stoinski a,e, Robert R. Hampton c,g a Zoo Atlanta, Atlanta, GA, United States b Bucknell University, Lewisburg, PA, United States c Department of Psychology, Emory University, Atlanta, GA, United States d Neuroscience Graduate Program, Emory University, Atlanta, GA, United States e Dian Fossey Gorilla Fund International, Atlanta, GA, United States f Department of Anthropology, University of California Davis, Davis, CA, United States g Yerkes National Primate Research Center, Atlanta, GA, United States article info abstract Article history: Humans mentally represent magnitudes spatially; we respond faster to one side of space when process- Received 1 March 2017 ing small quantities and to the other side of space when processing large quantities. We determined Revised 24 July 2017 whether spatial representation of magnitude is a fundamental feature of primate cognition by testing Accepted 25 July 2017 for such space-magnitude correspondence in gorillas and orangutans. Subjects picked the larger quantity in a pair of dot arrays in one condition, and the smaller in another. Response latencies to the left and right sides of the screen were compared across the magnitude range. Apes showed evidence of spatial repre- Keywords: sentation of magnitude. While all subjects did not adopt the same orientation, apes showed consistent Space tendencies for spatial representations within individuals and systematically reversed these orientations SNARC Ape in response to reversal of the task instruction.
    [Show full text]
  • Jahresbericht 2017 – Sehr Gute Besuchszahlen, Konstante Entwicklung I
    Tiergarten Nürnberg Das Jahr 2017 im Tiergarten Nürnberg: sehr gute Besuchszahlen, konstante Entwicklung 1 Inhaltsverzeichnis Das Jahr 2017 im Tiergarten Nürnberg: sehr gute Besuchszahlen, konstante Entwicklung Vorworte 2 Dr. Dag Encke, Leitender Direktor, Tiergarten Nürnberg 2 Christian Vogel, Bürgermeister der Stadt Nürnberg 3 Teil 1 | Part 1 5 I. Bildung | Education 5 I.1 Teilnehmende | Participants 5 I.2 Programme | Programms 5 I.3 Weitere Bildungsarbeit | Further educational projects 7 II. Forschung | Research 11 II.1 Forschungsprojekte | Research projects 11 II.2 Kooperationen und Treffen | Co-operation and meetings 12 II.3 Yaqu Pacha 14 III. Tierhaltung | Keeping of animals 17 III.1 Tierbestand | Animal population 17 III.2 Arterhalt | Species conservation 18 III.3 Schlaglichter der Tierpflege 20 III.4 Schlaglichter der Tiermedizin | Veterinary 21 IV. Gesellschaftliche Relevanz | Social relevance 23 IV.1 Kommunikation und Öffentlichkeitsarbeit | 23 Communication and Public relations IV.2 Lobbying und Verbände | Lobbying and associations 27 IV.3 Verein der Tiergartenfreunde Nürnberg e.V. mit Tierpaten | 28 Association of the Friends pf Nuremberg Zoo and Godfathers for animals V. Freizeiteinrichtung | Recreational facility 31 V.1 Besuche und Kundenbindung | Visits and customer loyalty 31 V.2 Baumaßnahmen/Investitionen | Building measures and investments 34 VI. Funktionalität | Functionality 37 Impressum VI.1 Verwaltung | Administration 37 Herausgeber Tiergarten Nürnberg, Am Tiergarten 30, 90480 Nürnberg VI.2 Personal | Staff 37 Telefon (0911) 54 54 6 / Fax (0911) 54 54 802 • www.tiergarten.nuernberg.de VI.3 Konsumtion | Consumption 37 Gestaltung hills&trees design, [email protected] VI.4 Wirtschaftlichkeit (Einnahmen/Ausgaben) | Economics 41 Redaktion Dr. Nicola A. Mögel Teil 2 | Part 2 45 Text Dr.
    [Show full text]
  • (Pan Troglodytes Verus) in Guinea-Bissau, West Africa
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
    [Show full text]
  • Laboratory Primate Newsletter
    LABORATORY PRIMATE NEWSLETTER Vol. 44, No. 2 April 2005 JUDITH E. SCHRIER, EDITOR JAMES S. HARPER, GORDON J. HANKINSON AND LARRY HULSEBOS, ASSOCIATE EDITORS MORRIS L. POVAR, CONSULTING EDITOR ELVA MATHIESEN, ASSISTANT EDITOR ALLAN M. SCHRIER, FOUNDING EDITOR, 1962-1987 Published Quarterly by the Schrier Research Laboratory Psychology Department, Brown University Providence, Rhode Island ISSN 0023-6861 POLICY STATEMENT The Laboratory Primate Newsletter provides a central source of information about nonhuman primates and re- lated matters to scientists who use these animals in their research and those whose work supports such research. The Newsletter (1) provides information on care and breeding of nonhuman primates for laboratory research, (2) dis- seminates general information and news about the world of primate research (such as announcements of meetings, research projects, sources of information, nomenclature changes), (3) helps meet the special research needs of indi- vidual investigators by publishing requests for research material or for information related to specific research prob- lems, and (4) serves the cause of conservation of nonhuman primates by publishing information on that topic. As a rule, research articles or summaries accepted for the Newsletter have some practical implications or provide general information likely to be of interest to investigators in a variety of areas of primate research. However, special con- sideration will be given to articles containing data on primates not conveniently publishable elsewhere. General descriptions of current research projects on primates will also be welcome. The Newsletter appears quarterly and is intended primarily for persons doing research with nonhuman primates. Back issues may be purchased for $5.00 each.
    [Show full text]
  • And Chimpanzees (P
    eScholarship International Journal of Comparative Psychology Title Monitoring Spatial Transpositions by Bonobos (Pan paniscus) and Chimpanzees (P. troglodytes) Permalink https://escholarship.org/uc/item/5099j6v4 Journal International Journal of Comparative Psychology, 13(1) ISSN 0889-3675 Authors Beran, Michael J. Minahan, Mary F. Publication Date 2000 License https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ 4.0 Peer reviewed eScholarship.org Powered by the California Digital Library University of California - 1 - International Journal of Comparative Psychology, 2000, 13, 1-15. Copyright 2000 by the International Society for Comparative Psychology Monitoring Spatial Transpositions by Bonobos (Pan paniscus) and Chimpanzees (P. troglodytes) Michael J. Beran and Mary F. Minahan Georgia State University, U.S.A. Two bonobos (Pan paniscus) and three chimpanzees (P. troglodytes) monitored spatial transpositions, or the simultaneous movement of multiple items in an array, so as to select a specific item from the array. In the initial condition of Experiment 1, food reward was hidden beneath one of four cups, and the apes were required to select the cup containing the reward in order to receive it. In the second condition, the test board on which the cups were located was rotated 180 degrees after placement of the food reward. In the third condition, two of the three cups switched locations with one another after placement of the food reward. All five apes performed at very high levels for these conditions. Ex- periment 2 was a computerized simulation of the tasks with the cups in which the apes had to track one of four simultaneously moving stimuli on a computer monitor.
    [Show full text]
  • Registrationsnowopen! March24- 26,2020
    EAZA ANIMAL WELFARE FORUM March 24 - 26, 2020 Hosted by Apenheul Primate Park Foundation Bridging the Gap Bringing together animal welfare research and application, to improve evidence-based animal welfare best practice Registrations Now Open! THE PROGRAMME MONDAY 23 MARCH Pre-conference Day Academy Course Side meetings Icebreaker TUESDAY 24 MARCH Conference Day 1 Session 1: Bridging the Gap Session 2: The Physical Domains - Health and Behaviour WEDNESDAY 25 MARCH Conference Day 2 Sessions 3: Session by WAZA: Animal - Visitor Interactions Session 4: The Physical Domains - Nutrition and Environment THURSDAY 26 MARCH Conference Day 3 Session 5: Welfare and Ethics Session 6: The Affective Experience (Mental Domain) Farewell Dinner FRIDAY 27 MARCH Apenheul Primate Park tour Stichting AAP Tour Any questions relating to conference logistics and registration, please contact [email protected] Any questions relating to the programme, please contact [email protected] T H E M E Bridging the Gap: Bringing together animal welfare research and application, to improve evidence-based animal welfare best practice. With new advances in animal welfare research and understanding, it is important that we are able to employ this science in our collections to ensure we are doing everything we can to promote positive animal welfare. The first session is solely dedicated to this theme with a direct focus on animal welfare assessment. The programme is then structured to reflect Mellor and Beausoleil’s (2015) extended Five Domains of Animal Welfare model. The four Physical/Functional Domains: Nutrition, Environment, Health & Behaviour and the one Affective Experience Domain: Mental State. Each domain is explored with the lens of the bridging the gap theme, focusing on how we can implement the research in each of these areas within the our zoos and aquariums.
    [Show full text]
  • 2Nd Congress of the European Federation for Primatology
    Abstracts Folia Primatol 2008;79:305–401 Published online: June 13, 2008 DOI: 10.1159/000137690 2nd Congress of the European Federation for Primatology Prague, September 3–7, 2007 Editors: Vaclav Vancata and Marina Vancatova, Prague, Czech Republic Do Capuchin Monkeys (Cebus apella) Deal with Tokens as They Do with Real Food? Elsa Addessia , Alessandra Mancini a, b , Lara Crescimbenea , b , Elisabetta Visalberghi a a Unit of Cognitive Primatology and Primate Centre, Institute of Cognitive Sciences and Technologies, CNR, Rome, b Università La Sapienza, Rome, Italy E-Mail: [email protected] Key Words: Transitivity ؒ Cebus apella ؒ T o k e n s Recent studies on the use of tokens (i.e., inherently non-valuable objects that acquire an associated value upon exchange for food with an experimenter) in non-human primates did not investigate whether individuals use tokens as symbols. Therefore, we evaluated this topic in capuchin monkeys. We trained 10 capuchins to associate two types of tokens (A and B) with different amounts of food. Then, we assessed performance in relative numerousness judgment tasks with food (Experiment 1) and with tokens A (Experiment 2). In both experiments, all ca- puchins chose the highest quantity regardless of the type of item presented. Then, in Experi- ment 3 one token B was presented against 1–5 tokens A. Four capuchins used a flexible strategy, maximizing their payoff. Experiment 4 required the capuchins to choose between 1 and 2 to- kens B, and 3 and 6 tokens A. Only one subject always maximized his payoff in this task.
    [Show full text]
  • 1. Would Humans Without Language Be Apes?
    In J. Valsiner (Series Ed.) & A. Toomela (Vol. Ed.) (2003). Cultural guidance in the development of the human mind: Vol. 7. Advances in Child Development within Culturally Structured Environments (pp. 9-26). Greenwich, CT: Ablex Publishing Corporation. 1. Would Humans Without Language Be Apes? Jacques Vauclair THE POSTULATE OF MENTAL CONTINUITY The bedrock of comparative psychology of cognition, especially where non- human primates are concerned, rests on Darwin's famous account according to which continuity would be the main trait leading from the animal to the human mind. This idea was popularized through the statement in which Darwin postu- lated only quantitative differences between humans and the other species, namely "the difference in mind between man and the higher animals, great as it is, certainly is one of degree and not of kind" (Darwin, 1871, p. 128). We can only agree with Darwin's continuity position as concerns the existence of some kind of mental organizations in animals, in particular in nonhuman primates, as a necessary part of the perception of objects and their localization and interrelationships in space and time (Walker, 1983) and in many adaptive functions, including problem solving and memory (e.g., Vauclair, 1996). In effect, human and animal brain functions show sufficient similarity to allow comparisons if one assumes that animal brains are devices for selecting and organizing per- ceived information, and that the neural systems that accomplish perception and memory exhibit evolutionary continuity. It thus appears that these global func- tions are performed by the animal in ways that are basically similar to human performance, that is, through the construction and use of representations of various degrees of schematization and abstraction (Roitblat, 1982).
    [Show full text]
  • The Assessment of Cognitive Bias in Capuchin Monkeys Using a Computerized Task
    Georgia State University ScholarWorks @ Georgia State University Psychology Theses Department of Psychology 5-8-2020 The Assessment of Cognitive Bias in Capuchin Monkeys Using a Computerized Task Kristin French Follow this and additional works at: https://scholarworks.gsu.edu/psych_theses Recommended Citation French, Kristin, "The Assessment of Cognitive Bias in Capuchin Monkeys Using a Computerized Task." Thesis, Georgia State University, 2020. https://scholarworks.gsu.edu/psych_theses/216 This Thesis is brought to you for free and open access by the Department of Psychology at ScholarWorks @ Georgia State University. It has been accepted for inclusion in Psychology Theses by an authorized administrator of ScholarWorks @ Georgia State University. For more information, please contact [email protected]. THE ASSESSMENT OF COGNITIVE BIAS IN CAPUCHIN MONKEYS USING A COMPUTERIZED TASK by KRISTIN A. FRENCH Under the Direction of Michael J. Beran, PhD ABSTRACT Cognitive bias refers to the influence of affective state on the interpretation of ambiguous stimuli and has been used to assess emotional state in nonhuman animals. The current study assessed cognitive bias in 12 brown-tufted capuchin monkeys using three distinct computerized psychophysical tasks and a novel manipulation to affect that involved giving moneys gelatin foods that tasted either pleasant or unpleasant. In addition, monkeys were trained on several positive and negative training cues. Results showed that food type was not a factor in monkeys’ responses to ambiguous stimuli. Behavioral observation during test sessions revealed the unpleasant food may have acted as a form of enrichment, thereby providing the monkeys with two pleasant activities prior to assessments of their emotional states.
    [Show full text]
  • Laboratory Primate Newsletter
    LABORATORY PRIMATE NEWSLETTER Vol. 44, No. 4 October 2005 JUDITH E. SCHRIER, EDITOR JAMES S. HARPER, GORDON J. HANKINSON AND LARRY HULSEBOS, ASSOCIATE EDITORS MORRIS L. POVAR, CONSULTING EDITOR ELVA MATHIESEN, ASSISTANT EDITOR ALLAN M. SCHRIER, FOUNDING EDITOR, 1962-1987 Published Quarterly by the Schrier Research Laboratory Psychology Department, Brown University Providence, Rhode Island ISSN 0023-6861 POLICY STATEMENT The Laboratory Primate Newsletter provides a central source of information about nonhuman primates and re- lated matters to scientists who use these animals in their research and those whose work supports such research. The Newsletter (1) provides information on care and breeding of nonhuman primates for laboratory research, (2) dis- seminates general information and news about the world of primate research (such as announcements of meetings, research projects, sources of information, nomenclature changes), (3) helps meet the special research needs of indi- vidual investigators by publishing requests for research material or for information related to specific research prob- lems, and (4) serves the cause of conservation of nonhuman primates by publishing information on that topic. As a rule, research articles or summaries accepted for the Newsletter have some practical implications or provide general information likely to be of interest to investigators in a variety of areas of primate research. However, special con- sideration will be given to articles containing data on primates not conveniently publishable elsewhere. General descriptions of current research projects on primates will also be welcome. The Newsletter appears quarterly and is intended primarily for persons doing research with nonhuman primates. Back issues may be purchased for $5.00 each.
    [Show full text]
  • What Is an Animal Emotion?
    Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci. ISSN 0077-8923 ANNALS OF THE NEW YORK ACADEMY OF SCIENCES Issue: The Year in Cognitive Neuroscience What is an animal emotion? Frans B.M. de Waal Living Links, Yerkes National Primate Research Center, and Psychology Department, Emory University, Atlanta, Georgia Address for correspondence: Frans B.M. de Waal, Ph.D., Living Links, Yerkes Primate Center, Emory University, 954 N. Gatewood Road Atlanta, GA 30322. [email protected] Emotions suffuse much of the language employed by students of animal behavior—from “social bonding” to “alarm calls”— yet are carefully avoided as an explicit topic in scientific discourse. Given the increasing interest in human emotional intelligence and the explicit attention in neuroscience to the emotions, both human and nonhuman, the taboo that has reigned for so long in animal behavior research seems outdated. The present review seeks to recall the history of our field in which emotions and instincts were mentioned in the same breath and in which neither psychologists nor biologists felt that animal emotions were off limits. One of the tenets supporting a renewed interest in this topic is to avoid unanswerable questions and to view emotions as mental and bodily states that potentiate behavior appropriate to environmental challenges. Understanding the emotionally deep structure of behavior will be the next frontier in the study of animal behavior. Keywords: evolution; animal cognition; empathy; facial expressions; instinct Emotions used to be an uncontroversial part of any ior to attribute it to an emotion if our only ev- description of animal behavior, as uncontroversial idence of the emotion is the very behaviour the as the instincts with which they were compared emotion is supposed to explain.” Since the 1970s, and equated.
    [Show full text]