New Perspectives on the Genetic Classification of Manda (Bantu N.11)

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

New Perspectives on the Genetic Classification of Manda (Bantu N.11) DigitalResources Electronic Working Paper 2016-001 New perspectives on the genetic classification of Manda (Bantu N.11) Hazel Gray and Tim Roth New perspectives on the genetic classification of Manda (Bantu N.11) Hazel Gray and Tim Roth SIL International® 2016 SIL Electronic Working Paper 2016-001 ©2016 SIL International® ISSN 1087-9250 Fair Use Policy Documents published in the Electronic Working Paper series are intended for scholarly research and educational use. You may make copies of these publications for research or instructional purposes (under fair use guidelines) free of charge and without further permission. Republication or commercial use of an Electronic Working Paper or the documents contained therein is expressly prohibited without the written consent of the copyright holder(s). Managing Editor Eric Kindberg Series Editor Lana Martens Content/Copy Editor Mary Huttar Compositor Bonnie Waswick Abstract Manda (N.11) is an under-documented Bantu language in southwestern Tanzania, with most mention of its closest genetic affiliation in the work of Nurse (1988). Nurse concludes that Manda belongs with the Southern Tanzania Highlands (SH) subgroup based primarily on phonological evidence. This paper uses new data from workshops and surveys conducted by SIL International to show that Nurse’s conclusion does not take into account the necessary dialectal information, namely that the Matumba dialect (which Nurse uses) is phonologically distinct from the remaining Manda dialects. Lexicostatistical, phonological, and sociohistorical evidence is taken into account. Further, in light of this new evidence, we propose two likely historical scenarios, both of which posit that Manda is most closely genetically affiliated with the Rufiji subgroup. Contents 1 Introduction and background 2 Dialectometry and lexicostatistical evidence 3 Phonological evidence 3.1 Dahl’s Law 3.2 Spirant weakening 3.3 *NC>̥ NC or N 4 Sociohistorical evidence 5 Synthesis and conclusion Appendix A: Lexicostatistics Appendix B: 296-item wordlist for corpus languages References iv 1 Introduction and background Manda [ISO 639-3 code: mgs] is a Bantu language (N.11) spoken by the Manda and Matumba language communities located in the area between Lake Nyasa and the Livingstone Mountains (Maho 2009). The language area straddles two administrative regions in Tanzania: Njombe Region, north of the Ruhuhu River, and Ruvuma Region, south of the river. The area is bordered to the north by Kisi (G.67),1 to the east by Pangwa (G.64) and Ngoni (N.12), and to the south by Matengo (N.13) and Mpoto (N.14) (Maho 2009). Lewis et al. (2015) report the Manda (and Matumba) population at 22,000. A previous SIL survey puts the estimate even higher, between 25,000 and 40,000 (Anderson et al. 2003a). This paper uses survey data from four other locations in addition to the Matumba variety in Luilo: Iwela, Lituhi, Litumba Kuhamba and Nsungu (see map below). We examine three streams of evidence (lexicostatistical, phonological, and sociohistorical) in this study to determine the closest genetic relatives of Manda (and Matumba). New data is put forward from survey work conducted by SIL personnel in 2013 and subsequent additional linguistic research (Gray, forthcoming; Gray and Mitterhoffer 2016). 1 The Guthrie codes for Bantu are referential, reflecting geography and not genetic relationship (see Schadeberg 2003: 146). 1 2 3 The Matumba consider themselves a separate ethnic group from the Manda, but still regard their language to be essentially the same as Manda. The Matumba themselves claim that they were once Manda who moved from the shores of Lake Malawi up into the mountains (Anderson et al. 2003a). The prestige dialect (even according to the Matumba) of Manda is spoken in the area near Ilela and Nsungu, villages on the lakeshore (Gray and Mitterhoffer 2016). Various comparative linguistic studies have included Manda wordlists (Guthrie 1971, Nurse and Philippson 1975) and mention of Manda is made in diachronic studies by Nurse and Philippson (1980) and Nurse (1988, 1999). None of these studies mention the Matumba, so it is quite likely that the Matumba were unintentionally grouped with the Manda in previous studies. As we will see in section 3, this lack of previous dialect research has given rise to some misconceptions about the innovations that Manda shares with its neighbors. Of those who have studied the Manda language with the aim of positing its closest genetic affiliation, Nurse and Philippson (1980) and Nurse (1988, 1999) are in conflict with Ehret (1999, 2009). Ehret (2009:17) puts Manda alongside Ngoni in the Rufiji-Ruvuma (RR) subgroup, whereas Nurse classifies Manda within his Southern (Tanzania) Highlands (SH) subgroup. These are the main hypotheses we evaluate in light of the new data which reflects a better understanding of Manda dialectology. RR consists of two sub-branches. We are concerned primarily with Rufiji, which includes Manda’s immediate neighbors: Ngoni, Matengo, and Mpoto. The SH subgroup includes the G.60 languages, of which we are primarily concerned with Kisi and Pangwa for the same reason. This paper mainly focuses on the arguments put forward by Nurse (1988), since he deals with the question of Manda’s genetic affiliation in the most depth. Ehret primarily relies on stem-morpheme innovations (lexical/semantic evidence) in his (1999) work, and does not offer the same depth of interaction with the corpus most like an SH language that Nurse does. From the Manda data Nurse uses, Manda appears to behave phonologically mostly like an SH language. However, we argue that the Manda data Nurse uses appears to be the Matumba dialect, which phonologically is quite different from the other Manda dialects. In section 3 we see how the features of Dahl’s Law, spirant weakening, and NC̥>NC or N cast doubt on Nurse’s argument for Manda as SH. We offer three scenarios regarding the genetic history of Manda and Matumba in section 5. In section 2 we present initial lexicostatistical evidence using dialectometry. Manda and Matumba varieties are compared to the other corpus languages: Kisi, Matengo, Mpoto, Ngoni, and Pangwa. Section 3 examines the phonological evidence for the corpus languages, while section 4 briefly examines the sociohistorical evidence. Section 5 concludes the paper with a synthesis, preliminary conclusions, and possibilities for future research. 2 Dialectometry and lexicostatistical evidence Dialectometry is essentially quantitative dialectology. Distance-based networks are examples of such quantitative data explorations. A distance-based network analysis offers ‘‘an introductory visual means of data exploration’’ (Pelkey 2011:279). Subsumed under the rising field of dialectometry are several distance-based algorithmic applications that aim to help researchers explore language variation and/or change, while “making it possible to show more than one evolutionary pathway on a single graph” (Holden and Gray 2006:24). To create a distance-based network the opposite values of regular lexicostatistical percentages are used within a standard matrix (e.g., the Kisi and Pangwa languages in figure 2 are 0.59 similar, but 0.41 dissimilar; see Appendix A). The resulting distance matrix is then subjected to the Neighbor-Net algorithm, as developed by Bryant and Moulton (2004) and implemented within the Splits Tree 4 (4.11.3) software program (see Huson and Bryant 2010). If the lexical relationships are ambiguous, the length of each branch indicates the confidence level of the split. Figure 1 shows such a network using four Bantu lects from southwest Tanzania (Roth 2011:43). 4 Nyika Nyiha Safwa Malila Figure 1. Sample network demonstrating ambiguity (adapted from Roth 2011:43). In figure 1, the middle box indicates the ambiguous relationship: Split A—Nyika and Nyiha/ Safwa and Malila versus Split B—Nyika and Safwa/ Nyiha and Malila. In figure 1, Split A is more likely. Holden and Gray discuss other patterns and their meaning within the network diagram: Rapid radiation may be inferred from a lack of phylogenetic signal, i.e. a rake- or star-shaped phylogeny, whereas reticulation would indicate possible borrowing. Reticulations can also pinpoint those languages which may have been involved in borrowing. Complex chains of conflicting relationships involving numerous languages may indicate that borrowing occurred in the context of dialect chains. (2006:24) If the lexical relationships are more clear, the splits-graph looks more like a regular tree diagram (see Holden and Gray (2006) for further explanation of the Neighbor-Net algorithm, and the unique historical relationships between other Bantu languages). Figure 2 below is based on the lexicostatistical data in Appendix A. The data includes lexical percentages from 313-item wordlists elicited in five locations (Iwela, Lituhi, Litumba Kuhamba, Luilo and Nsungu) during the 2013 dialect survey (Gray and Mitterhoffer 2016). This 313-item wordlist was based on the 100-item Leipzig-Jakarta wordlist (Tadmor 2009). Two hundred ninety-six of the lexical items2 in the 313-item wordlist were compared to data from SIL Fieldworks Language Explorer (FLEx) 2 Some lexical items were omitted due to duplications in the data. 5 databases for Kisi and Pangwa, and CBOLD data for Mpoto, Matengo and Ngoni (supplemented by data from Ngonyani 2003 and Yoneda 2006) using the comparative method as the basis for determining cognacy. The lexical data (the 296-item wordlist) can be found in Appendix B. Consider the distance- based network below in Figure 2. Matengo Pangwa Kisi RR SH Mpoto Ngoni Lituhi Litumba Kuhamba Nsungu Luilo Iwela (Matumba) Manda Figure 2. Distance-based network for SH, Rufiji and Manda corpus languages. From the diagram in figure 2, we can clearly see the split between the branch containing the SH languages (Pangwa and Kisi) on the left and two of the Rufiji languages (Matengo and Mpoto) on the right. We can also see that the Manda dialects, while lexically related to SH and Rufiji, are at the same time lexically distinct, i.e., the Manda dialects seem to have a split-lexicon.
Recommended publications
  • Here Referred to As Class 18A (See Hyman 1980:187)
    WS1 Remarks on the nasal classes in Mungbam and Naki Mungbam and Naki are two non-Grassfields Bantoid languages spoken along the northwest frontier of the Grassfields area to the north of the Ring languages. Until recently, they were poorly described, but new data reveals them to show significant nasal noun class patterns, some of which do not appear to have been previously noted for Bantoid. The key patterns are: 1. Like many other languages of their region (see Good et al. 2011), they make productive use of a mysterious diminutive plural prefix with a form like mu-, with associated concords in m, here referred to as Class 18a (see Hyman 1980:187). 2. The five dialects of Mungbam show a level of variation in their nasal classes that one might normally expect of distinct languages. a. Two dialects show no evidence for nasals in Class 6. Two other dialects, Munken and Ngun, show a Class 6 prefix on nouns of form a- but nasal concords. In Munken Class 6, this nasal is n, clearly distinct from an m associated with 6a; in Ngun, both 6 and 6a are associated with m concords. The Abar dialect shows a different pattern, with Class 6 nasal concords in m and nasal prefixes on some Class 6 nouns. b. The Abar, Biya, and Ngun dialects show a Class 18a prefix with form mN-, rather than the more regionally common mu-. This reduction is presumably connected to perseveratory nasalization attested throughout the languages of the region with a diachronic pathway along the lines of mu- > mũ- > mN- perhaps providing a partial example for the development of Bantu Class 9/10.
    [Show full text]
  • Closed Adjective Classes and Primary Adjectives in African Languages Guillaume Segerer
    Closed adjective classes and primary adjectives in African Languages Guillaume Segerer To cite this version: Guillaume Segerer. Closed adjective classes and primary adjectives in African Languages. 2008. halshs-00255943 HAL Id: halshs-00255943 https://halshs.archives-ouvertes.fr/halshs-00255943 Preprint submitted on 14 Feb 2008 HAL is a multi-disciplinary open access L’archive ouverte pluridisciplinaire HAL, est archive for the deposit and dissemination of sci- destinée au dépôt et à la diffusion de documents entific research documents, whether they are pub- scientifiques de niveau recherche, publiés ou non, lished or not. The documents may come from émanant des établissements d’enseignement et de teaching and research institutions in France or recherche français ou étrangers, des laboratoires abroad, or from public or private research centers. publics ou privés. Closed adjective classes and primary adjectives in African Languages Guillaume Segerer – LLACAN (INALCO, CNRS) Closed adjective classes and primary adjectives in African Languages INTRODUCTION The existence of closed adjective classes (henceforth CAC) has long been recognized for African languages. Although I probably haven’t found the earliest mention of this property1, Welmers’ statement in African Language Structures is often quoted: “It is important to note, however, that in almost all Niger-Congo languages which have a class of adjectives, the class is rather small (...).” (Welmers 1973:250). Maurice Houis provides further precision: “Il y a lieu de noter que de nombreuses langues possèdent des lexèmes adjectivaux. Leur usage en discours est courant, toutefois l’inventaire est toujours limité (autour de 30 à 40 unités).” (Houis 1977:35). These two statements are not further developped by their respective authors.
    [Show full text]
  • Bernander Et Al AAM NEC in Bantu
    The negative existential cycle in Bantu1 Bernander, Rasmus, Maud Devos and Hannah Gibson Abstract Renewal of negation has received ample study in Bantu languages. Still, the relevant literature does not mention a cross-linguistically recurrent source of standard negation, i.e., the existential negator. The present paper aims to find out whether this gap in the literature is indicative of the absence of the Negative Existential Cycle (NEC) in Bantu languages. It presents a first account of the expression of negative existence in a geographically diverse sample of 93 Bantu languages. Bantu negative existential constructions are shown to display a high degree of formal variation both within dedicated and non-dedicated constructions. Although such variation is indicative of change, existential negators do not tend to induce changes at the same level as standard negation. The only clear cases of the spread of an existential negator to the domain of standard negation in this study appear to be prompted by sustained language contact. Keywords: Bantu languages, negation, language change, morphology 1 Introduction The Bantu language family comprises some 350-500 languages spoken across much of Central, Eastern and Southern Africa. According to Grollemund et al. (2015), these languages originate from a proto-variety of Bantu, estimated to have been spoken roughly 5000 years ago in the eastern parts of present-day northwest Cameroon. Many Bantu languages exhibit a dominant SVO word order. They are primarily head-marking, have a highly agglutinative morphology and a rich verbal complex in which inflectional and derivational affixes join to an obligatory verb stem. The Bantu languages are also characterised by a system of noun classes – a form of grammatical gender.
    [Show full text]
  • Music of Ghana and Tanzania
    MUSIC OF GHANA AND TANZANIA: A BRIEF COMPARISON AND DESCRIPTION OF VARIOUS AFRICAN MUSIC SCHOOLS Heather Bergseth A Thesis Submitted to the Graduate College of Bowling Green State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of MASTERDecember OF 2011MUSIC Committee: David Harnish, Advisor Kara Attrep © 2011 Heather Bergseth All Rights Reserved iii ABSTRACT David Harnish, Advisor This thesis is based on my engagement and observations of various music schools in Ghana, West Africa, and Tanzania, East Africa. I spent the last three summers learning traditional dance- drumming in Ghana, West Africa. I focus primarily on two schools that I have significant recent experience with: the Dagbe Arts Centre in Kopeyia and the Dagara Music and Arts Center in Medie. While at Dagbe, I studied the music and dance of the Anlo-Ewe ethnic group, a people who live primarily in the Volta region of South-eastern Ghana, but who also inhabit neighboring countries as far as Togo and Benin. I took classes and lessons with the staff as well as with the director of Dagbe, Emmanuel Agbeli, a teacher and performer of Ewe dance-drumming. His father, Godwin Agbeli, founded the Dagbe Arts Centre in order to teach others, including foreigners, the musical styles, dances, and diverse artistic cultures of the Ewe people. The Dagara Music and Arts Center was founded by Bernard Woma, a master drummer and gyil (xylophone) player. The DMC or Dagara Music Center is situated in the town of Medie just outside of Accra. Mr. Woma hosts primarily international students at his compound, focusing on various musical styles, including his own culture, the Dagara, in addition music and dance of the Dagbamba, Ewe, and Ga ethnic groups.
    [Show full text]
  • Cover Page the Handle Holds Various Files of This Leiden University Dissertation. Author: Lima
    Cover Page The handle http://hdl.handle.net/1887/85723 holds various files of this Leiden University dissertation. Author: Lima Santiago J. de Title: Zoonímia Histórico-comparativa: Denominações dos antílopes em bantu Issue Date: 2020-02-26 729 ANEXO 1: TABELA RECAPITULATIVA DAS PROTOFORMAS Nas protoformas provenientes do BLR (2003) e nas reconstruções de outros autores (majoritariamente, Mouguiama & Hombert, 2006), as classes nominais em negrito e sublinhadas, são sugestões da autora da tese. Significados Reconstruções Propostas Propostas do BLR e de de correções (De Lima outros autores Santiago) *-bʊ́dʊ́kʊ́ °-bʊ́dʊ́gʊ́ (cl. 9/10, 12/13) °-cénda (cl. 12/13) Philantomba °-cótɩ́ monticola (cl. 12/13) *-kùengà > °-kùèngà (cl. 11/5, 7/8) °°-cécɩ/ °°-cétɩ (cl. 9/10, 12/13) *-pàmbı ́ °-pàmbɩ́ (cl. 9/10) °-dòbò Cephalophus (cl. 3+9/4, nigrifrons 5/6) *-pùmbɩ̀dɩ̀ °-pùmbèèdɩ̀ (cl. 9/10, 9/6) 730 Significados Reconstruções Propostas Propostas do BLR e de de correções (De Lima outros autores Santiago) *-jʊ́mbɩ̀ (cl. 9/10, 3/4) °°-cʊ́mbɩ (cl. 9/10, 5/6, 7/8, 11/10) *-jìbʊ̀ °-tʊ́ndʊ́ Cephalophus (cl. 9/10) (cl. 9/10) silvicultor °°-bɩ́mbà °-bɩ̀mbà (cl. 9/10) °-kʊtɩ (cl. 9, 3) *-kʊ́dʊ̀pà/ °-bɩ́ndɩ́ *-kúdùpà (cl. 9/10, 7/8, (cl. 9/10) 3, 12/13) Cephalophus dorsalis °°-cíbʊ̀ °-pòmbɩ̀ (cl. 7/8) (cl. 9/10) °°-cʊmɩ >°-cʊmɩ́ °-gindà (cl. 9) Cephalophus (cl. 3/4) callipygus °°-cábè >°-cábà (cl. 9/10, 7/8) °°-bɩ̀jɩ̀ (cl. 9) 731 Significados Reconstruções Propostas Propostas do BLR e de de correções (De Lima outros autores Santiago) *-bengeda >°-bèngédè °-cégé (cl.9/10) (cl. 9/10) °°-àngàdà >°-jàngàdà Cephalophus (cl.
    [Show full text]
  • Some Principles of the Use of Macro-Areas Language Dynamics &A
    Online Appendix for Harald Hammarstr¨om& Mark Donohue (2014) Some Principles of the Use of Macro-Areas Language Dynamics & Change Harald Hammarstr¨om& Mark Donohue The following document lists the languages of the world and their as- signment to the macro-areas described in the main body of the paper as well as the WALS macro-area for languages featured in the WALS 2005 edi- tion. 7160 languages are included, which represent all languages for which we had coordinates available1. Every language is given with its ISO-639-3 code (if it has one) for proper identification. The mapping between WALS languages and ISO-codes was done by using the mapping downloadable from the 2011 online WALS edition2 (because a number of errors in the mapping were corrected for the 2011 edition). 38 WALS languages are not given an ISO-code in the 2011 mapping, 36 of these have been assigned their appropri- ate iso-code based on the sources the WALS lists for the respective language. This was not possible for Tasmanian (WALS-code: tsm) because the WALS mixes data from very different Tasmanian languages and for Kualan (WALS- code: kua) because no source is given. 17 WALS-languages were assigned ISO-codes which have subsequently been retired { these have been assigned their appropriate updated ISO-code. In many cases, a WALS-language is mapped to several ISO-codes. As this has no bearing for the assignment to macro-areas, multiple mappings have been retained. 1There are another couple of hundred languages which are attested but for which our database currently lacks coordinates.
    [Show full text]
  • 1 Parameters of Morpho-Syntactic Variation
    This paper appeared in: Transactions of the Philological Society Volume 105:3 (2007) 253–338. Please always use the published version for citation. PARAMETERS OF MORPHO-SYNTACTIC VARIATION IN BANTU* a b c By LUTZ MARTEN , NANCY C. KULA AND NHLANHLA THWALA a School of Oriental and African Studies, b University of Leiden and School of Oriental and African Studies, c University of the Witwatersrand and School of Oriental and African Studies ABSTRACT Bantu languages are fairly uniform in terms of broad typological parameters. However, they have been noted to display a high degree or more fine-grained morpho-syntactic micro-variation. In this paper we develop a systematic approach to the study of morpho-syntactic variation in Bantu by developing 19 parameters which serve as the basis for cross-linguistic comparison and which we use for comparing ten south-eastern Bantu languages. We address conceptual issues involved in studying morpho-syntax along parametric lines and show how the data we have can be used for the quantitative study of language comparison. Although the work reported is a case study in need of expansion, we will show that it nevertheless produces relevant results. 1. INTRODUCTION Early studies of morphological and syntactic linguistic variation were mostly aimed at providing broad parameters according to which the languages of the world differ. The classification of languages into ‘inflectional’, ‘agglutinating’, and ‘isolating’ morphological types, originating from the work of Humboldt (1836), is a well-known example of this approach. Subsequent studies in linguistic typology, e.g. work following Greenberg (1963), similarly tried to formulate variables which could be applied to any language and which would classify languages into a number of different types.
    [Show full text]
  • Marriage Laws of Malawi
    MARRIAGE LAWS OF MALAWI - THE EVOLUTION OF AFRICAN MARRIAGE LAWS UNDER COLONIAL RULE BY TCHUPA N. CHIBAMBO A thesis submitted to the University of London in fulfilment of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy in Law July, 1987 School of Oriental and African Studies ProQuest Number: 11010656 All rights reserved INFORMATION TO ALL USERS The quality of this reproduction is dependent upon the quality of the copy submitted. In the unlikely event that the author did not send a com plete manuscript and there are missing pages, these will be noted. Also, if material had to be removed, a note will indicate the deletion. uest ProQuest 11010656 Published by ProQuest LLC(2018). Copyright of the Dissertation is held by the Author. All rights reserved. This work is protected against unauthorized copying under Title 17, United States C ode Microform Edition © ProQuest LLC. ProQuest LLC. 789 East Eisenhower Parkway P.O. Box 1346 Ann Arbor, Ml 48106- 1346 DEDICATION This dissertation is dedicated to: my grandfather, the late Edward Zachariah Mhango, and my grandmother, Edith Tembo. ABSTRACT This thesis examines the evolution of the law regulating African marriages and divorce in Malawi. It offers a re­ view of the principal rules of customary law governing marriage and describes the introduction of relevant legis­ lative provisions by the colonial administration, including the provisions for the registration of customary marriages enacted by African authorities. Special attention is given to the position of African Christians and to the policies and attitudes of Christian missions as determinants of colonial legislative policy. The analyses focus on the interaction of African custom, Christianity and secular statutory regulation of marriage and divorce.
    [Show full text]
  • Reglas De Congo: Palo Monte Mayombe) a Book by Lydia Cabrera an English Translation from the Spanish
    THE KONGO RULE: THE PALO MONTE MAYOMBE WISDOM SOCIETY (REGLAS DE CONGO: PALO MONTE MAYOMBE) A BOOK BY LYDIA CABRERA AN ENGLISH TRANSLATION FROM THE SPANISH Donato Fhunsu A dissertation submitted to the faculty of the University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy in the Department of English and Comparative Literature (Comparative Literature). Chapel Hill 2016 Approved by: Inger S. B. Brodey Todd Ramón Ochoa Marsha S. Collins Tanya L. Shields Madeline G. Levine © 2016 Donato Fhunsu ALL RIGHTS RESERVED ii ABSTRACT Donato Fhunsu: The Kongo Rule: The Palo Monte Mayombe Wisdom Society (Reglas de Congo: Palo Monte Mayombe) A Book by Lydia Cabrera An English Translation from the Spanish (Under the direction of Inger S. B. Brodey and Todd Ramón Ochoa) This dissertation is a critical analysis and annotated translation, from Spanish into English, of the book Reglas de Congo: Palo Monte Mayombe, by the Cuban anthropologist, artist, and writer Lydia Cabrera (1899-1991). Cabrera’s text is a hybrid ethnographic book of religion, slave narratives (oral history), and folklore (songs, poetry) that she devoted to a group of Afro-Cubans known as “los Congos de Cuba,” descendants of the Africans who were brought to the Caribbean island of Cuba during the trans-Atlantic Ocean African slave trade from the former Kongo Kingdom, which occupied the present-day southwestern part of Congo-Kinshasa, Congo-Brazzaville, Cabinda, and northern Angola. The Kongo Kingdom had formal contact with Christianity through the Kingdom of Portugal as early as the 1490s.
    [Show full text]
  • The Use of the Augment in Nguni Languages with Special Reference to the Referentiality of the Noun Eva-Marie Bloom Ström & Matti Miestamo
    [Draft, August 2020; to appear in Lutz Marten, Rozenn Guérois, Hannah Gibson & Eva-Marie Bloom-Ström (eds), Morphosyntactic Variation in Bantu. Oxford: Oxford University Press.] The use of the augment in Nguni languages with special reference to the referentiality of the noun Eva-Marie Bloom Ström & Matti Miestamo Abstract This chapter examines the use of the augment, a prefix preceding the noun class prefix, in a number of language varieties in the Nguni subgroup of Bantu languages. The study of these closely related varieties, which show striking similarities as well as differences in the use of the augment, gives new insights into developmental tendencies of the augment. All contexts in which the augment can be omitted are non-fact contexts. Contrary to what has previously been argued for some varieties, however, we find that the presence vs. absence of the augment does not mark a referentiality distinction. It is argued that referentiality constitutes a semantic and pragmatic explanation to the absence and presence of the augment in different contexts in a diachronic perspective, but that this function is eroded in present-day Nguni. What remains is a limited referentiality distinction for some speakers in some varieties. The loss of function explains why the augment is included in the noun in nearly all contexts in some varieties, and omitted everywhere in others. Due to its loss of function, the augment has become free to participate in sociolinguistic and stylistic variation in some Bantu languages. Key-words: negation, non-fact, referentiality, augment, morphosyntax, Nguni 1. Introduction The aim of this paper is to explore the connection between the use of a nominal prefix in Bantu referred to as the augment1 and (non-)referentiality, such as has been claimed to exist in Swati: 2 (1) a.
    [Show full text]
  • Deliberation As an Epistemic Endeavor: Umunthu and Social Change In
    Deliberation as an Epistemic Endeavor: UMunthu and Social Change in Malawi’s Political Ecology A dissertation presented to the faculty of the Scripps College of Communication of Ohio University In partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree Doctor of Philosophy Fletcher O. M. Ziwoya December 2012 © 2012 Fletcher O. M. Ziwoya All Rights Reserved. This dissertation titled Deliberation as an Epistemic Endeavor: UMunthu and Social Change in Malawi’s Political Ecology by FLETCHER O. M. ZIWOYA has been approved for the School of Communication Studies and the Scripps College of Communication by Claudia L. Hale Professor of Communication Studies Scott Titsworth Interim Dean, Scripps College of Communication ii ABSTRACT ZIWOYA, FLETCHER O. M., Ph.D. December 2012, Communication Studies Deliberation as an Epistemic Endeavor: UMunthu and Social Change in Malawi’s Political Ecology Director of Dissertation: Claudia Hale This dissertation examines the epistemic role of democratic processes in Malawi. In this study, I challenge the view that Malawi’s Local Government model of public participation is representative and open to all forms of knowledge production. Through a case study analysis of the political economy of knowledge production of selected District Councils in Malawi, I argue that the consultative approach adopted by the Councils is flawed. The Habermasian approach adopted by the Councils assumes that development processes should be free, fair, and accommodative of open forms of deliberation, consultation, and dissent. The Habermasian ideals stipulate that no single form of reasoning or knowledge dominates others. By advocating for “the power of the better argument” Habermas (1984, 1998a, 1998b, 2001) provided room for adversarial debate which is not encouraged in the Malawi local governance system.
    [Show full text]
  • African Languages and Linguistics Nicolas Quint, Paulette Roulon-Doko, Loïc-Michel Perrin
    African Languages and Linguistics Nicolas Quint, Paulette Roulon-Doko, Loïc-Michel Perrin To cite this version: Nicolas Quint, Paulette Roulon-Doko, Loïc-Michel Perrin. African Languages and Linguistics. Quint Nicolas; Perrin Loïc Michel; Roulon-Doko Paulette. France. Lambert Lucas, 130pp, 2015, 978-2- 35935-146-0. halshs-01482156 HAL Id: halshs-01482156 https://halshs.archives-ouvertes.fr/halshs-01482156 Submitted on 8 Mar 2017 HAL is a multi-disciplinary open access L’archive ouverte pluridisciplinaire HAL, est archive for the deposit and dissemination of sci- destinée au dépôt et à la diffusion de documents entific research documents, whether they are pub- scientifiques de niveau recherche, publiés ou non, lished or not. The documents may come from émanant des établissements d’enseignement et de teaching and research institutions in France or recherche français ou étrangers, des laboratoires abroad, or from public or private research centers. publics ou privés. L Linguistique et Langues Africaines : L LLA une nouvelle voix pour les langues et les cultures d’Afrique (Paulette Roulon-Doko, A Une nouvelle voix Linguistique Nicolas Quint & Loïc-Michel Perrin) 11 et Langues pour les langues Africaines #01 Juin 2015 et les cultures d’Afrique 1. Sur l’origine du suffixe du pluriel dans le groupe Nyun-Buy (Sylvie Voisin) 13 2. L’indexation du sujet et de l’objet dans les langues atlantiques nord (Alain-Christian Bassène) 43 3. Serial Verb Constructions in Papiamentu: Historical, Synchronic and Comparative Observations #01 (Bart Jacobs) 59 Juin 2015 4. A Note on the (Non-Existing) Passive in Matengo (Jenneke van der Wal) 81 Notes et documents - Bure Verbs (Gian Claudio Batic) 101 Comptes-rendus de lecture Sandra Bornand et Cécile Leguy, Anthropologie des pratiques langagières.
    [Show full text]