Comparative Acoustical and Psychoacoustical Analyses of Gamelan Instrument Tones

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Comparative Acoustical and Psychoacoustical Analyses of Gamelan Instrument Tones J. Acoust. Soc. Jpn.(E) 14, 6 (1993) Comparative acoustical and psychoacoustical analyses of gamelan instrument tones Edward C.Carterette,*,** Roger A.Kendall,** and Sue Carole DeVale** * Departments of Psychology, Brain Research Institute, and **Music Perception, Cognition and Acoustics Laboratory, Department of Ethnomusicology and Systematic Musicology, University of California, Los Angeles [UCLA], 405 Hilgard Avenue, Los Angeles, California 90024, USA (Received 1 March 1993) The comparative acoustical and psychoacoustical elements of the Javanese gamelan were studied with a focus on tuning, vibrational characteristics and associated per ceptual effects. In addition to relating acoustical, perceptual, and musical frameworks- to each other, we seek to clarify some common misconceptions about tuning modes, scales, and the assignment of pitch to metallophones which have inharmonic spectra. We found that the inharmonic modes of the spectra of the bars of the gamelan instru ments saron demung, saron barung, and saron peking are described well by the standard- physical theory of rectangular bars. The makers have tuned the bars by shaping so that harmonic partials also appear. The frequencies of the scale of the seven measured bars of each instrument (Low 6, Middle 1, 2, 3, 5, 6, High 1) are fit perfectly by an ex ponential function. Results of perceptual experiments show that, although many octave- mismatches occur, both Indonesian and Western subjects generally matched a pitch chroma. In judging small-cents deviations from equi-pentatonic tuning, we found one professional Indonesian musician of four who could reliably discern them. We argue that the paradox of three different pathets (modes) for equi-pentatonic tunings can be explained by differing distributions of chroma based on Markof chains of order 0, 1, 2 or higher and other imposed formulaic and hierarchical structures, which we call chromals. Key words:Musical acoustics, Pitch, Gamelan, Tuning systems, Psychoacoustics PACS number:43.75.Bc, 43.75.Cd, Kk linked. 1. INTRODUCTION For us, there is a continuing concern about all There has been considerable controversy in the musicological research in its tendency to focus on interpretation of data, variously obtained, regarding single frames of reference in isolation. For example, both the gamelan metallophone tuning and spectral music theorists working in the notational frame of and timbral characteristics (e.g. Hood, 1954; Kunst, reference impute perceptual meaning to their anal 1973; Beurmann and Schneider, 1989; Surjodinin yses, when in fact they have not explicitly or - em grat, Sudarjana and Susanto, 1972). These- acous pirically linked the notational to the perceptual- tical and psychoacoustical facets of organological- frame of reference. This failing is often found in and musicological research have spawned additional the work of cognitive musicologists, and is an im controversies regarding the classification of pathets portant weakness in contemporary trends in cogni- or modes. Many of these problems evaporate when tive science. Recently a paradigm has been outlined- the various frames of reference are appropriately for psychomusicological studies wherein converging 383 J. Acoust. Soc. Jpn.(E) 14, 6 (1993) methods link various frames of reference (Kendall those of the gamelan, and to what extent the pitch and Carterette, 1990; Carterette and Kendall, 1993); is ambiguous. for example, the ability of the performer to com Such inharmonic spectra do have periodic pat municate musical expression to the listener was- in terns, and cognitive rules apparently exist to make- vestigated, which involved relationships among the- sense out of such patterns. Several models have perceptual, acoustical, and notational frames of been proposed, including numerous attempts to reference. acount for perceptual resolution of missing funda The present study is designed to investigate the mentals (Schouten, 1940; Goldstein, -1973; comparative psychomusicology of elements of the Wightman, 1973; Terhardt, 1974; Patterson, 1989). Javanese gamelan. Our initial explorations focus The fact is, people do hear bell pitches, even on the tuning, vibrational characteristics, and as though bell spectra may not contain energy at the sociated perceptual effects. We seek here also -to frequency to which pitch is assigned. Bell foundries clarify some common misconceptions. routinely strive to tune bells by traditional artisan The acoustical characteristics of the glockenspiel rules, even casting the intended pitch into the metal and related rectangular bar instruments which are itself, as in the case of the two-pitched bronze chime free at both ends have been described well and bells of the Shang-Zhou dynasty manufactured modeled by Rossing (Rossing, 1990; Fletcher and about 500 BC (Ma Chengyuan, 1981). The tedious Rossing, 1991). Although such bars have spectra process of tuning bells after casting by filing and with partials which are not integer multiples of the shaping nearly has been eliminated by successful fundamental, in contrast to most aerophones and computer modeling in recent experimental studies chordophones, the pitch class often can be assigned (Shoofs, van Asperen, Maas, and Lehr, 1987; reliably. There nevertheless appears to be a mis Houtsma and Tholen, 1987; Lehr, 1987). conception in musicology that inharmonic spectra,- Rao, Carterette and Wu (1987) show that both especially those produced by metallophones or bells, Chinese and Americans readily make pitch matches cannot be assigned a pitch easily (Beurmann and to the sound of a subset of the Zhou bells. Further Schneider, 1989; Schneider and Stoltz, 1988; more, both Westerners and non-Westerners- per Schneider, 1991). [At the other pole, some musi ceived the same pitch to a given bell. It is easy- to cologists apparently believe that all music emanates- induce from their study three simple rules for the from the harmonic series (e.g. Hindemith, 1937).] assignment of pitch to Chinese bell spectra Rule 1: For example, Schneider and Stoltz (1988) cite the case The frequency associated with the pitch is equal to of ancient Chinese bronze bells (see discussion be the frequency of the lowest partial if it also has low), in which none of the observed partials (N=4)- maximal amplitude. Rule 2:If the partial having fit into a single harmonic series. They exhibit highest amplitude is not lowest in frequency, the signal analysis of gamelan tones (saron and bonang) frequency associated with the pitch is associated stating that their unstable vibration patterns make it with a sub-partial (f/2) of that partial. Rule 3 impossible to assign pitch to these instruments: If neither Rule 1 nor Rule 2 applies, then the fre … the fact remains that most bell sounds (as well quency of the partial associated with the pitch is -the as those obtained from many gongs and other ratio of the mean of the low frequency local maxima metallophones) show several non-harmonic com to the mean of the two strongest high-frequency ponents or spectra [sic] which tend to confuse - the local maxima. Of course, additional experimenta perception of 'pitch'(Schneider and Stoltz, 1988, tion and connection to hearing models - needs to be p.273). done, but these rules cover all thirteen of the Zhou Earlier in the same article they remark that bells, and clearly exemplify auditory contrast and In any case, there is a more or less marked grouping mechanisms. ambiguity•cfor "Western" bell sounds as well As we will see in the present study, the gamelan as for stimuli having similar spectral character bars have inharmonic components, the upper ones istics (e.g.instruments of the Javanese- and of which die out extremely quickly, leaving a strong, Balinese gamelan)•c(p.267). salient first partial. This is also true of the Western Of course, it is an experimental question to deter glockenspiel (Rossing, 1990). We turn now to the mine to what extent the spectra of bells are similar - to gamelan, in which metal bars are the principal 384 E. C. CARTERETTE et al.: GAMELAN INSTRUMENT TONES generators. namics, and duration of the work. The function of the saron barung metallophone in 2. THE GAMELAN the classical gamelan orchestra is to play the nuclear Gamelan is the term for a specific type of classical melody in its unembellished form, thereby function- orchestra in Java and Bali. Generally, it consists ing in manner analogous to a cantus firmus. The of seven to seventy-five percussion instruments, metal bars of the saron barung are positioned over a mainly individual gongs, gong chimes and metal- trough resonator (Fig. 1), and usually rest on woven lophones [xylophones with metal keys]. The sound- rattan squares through nails at the nodes of the first ing parts are usually bronze. mode of vibration, which minimizes damping, There are two tuning systems in Javanese gamelan, allowing the struck bar to ring. It is this important one of five tones, called slendro, the other of seven melodic instrument that we studied. tones, called pelog. The pelog system has seven unequally-spaced intervals and slendro have five 3. ACOUSTICAL ANALYSES OF SARON BARUNG ' WILAH' (BARS) (relatively) equally-spaced intervals. Each of the pelog and slendro tunings yields three modes. In the 3.1 Characteristics of the Bar pelog system, the three modes contain five subsets In order to assess our contention that the gamelan of the original seven; the remaining two tones are wilah (bars) are well-modeled by standard acoustical used for embellishment. theory dealing with rectangular bars, we measured Only the slendro system is involved in the present the wilah or bar of the first scale tone saron barung paper. Each of the three slendro modes or pathets slendro of the UCLA Javanese gamelan Kyai contains the same five tones. One way a pathet Mendung [The Venerable Dark Cloud] (Hood and (= "to constrain") differs from another is in the Susilo, 1967) (Figs. 2(a); 2(b)). Thickness measure- prominence accorded to certain of the five tones, in ments were made to the nearest 0.0001 inch with a other words by a (statistical) limiting of the choices Mitutoyo micrometer.
Recommended publications
  • An Adaptive Tuning System for MIDI Pianos
    David Løberg Code Groven.Max: School of Music Western Michigan University Kalamazoo, MI 49008 USA An Adaptive Tuning [email protected] System for MIDI Pianos Groven.Max is a real-time program for mapping a renstemningsautomat, an electronic interface be- performance on a standard keyboard instrument to tween the manual and the pipes with a kind of arti- a nonstandard dynamic tuning system. It was origi- ficial intelligence that automatically adjusts the nally conceived for use with acoustic MIDI pianos, tuning dynamically during performance. This fea- but it is applicable to any tunable instrument that ture overcomes the historic limitation of the stan- accepts MIDI input. Written as a patch in the MIDI dard piano keyboard by allowing free modulation programming environment Max (available from while still preserving just-tuned intervals in www.cycling74.com), the adaptive tuning logic is all keys. modeled after a system developed by Norwegian Keyboard tunings are compromises arising from composer Eivind Groven as part of a series of just the intersection of multiple—sometimes oppos- intonation keyboard instruments begun in the ing—influences: acoustic ideals, harmonic flexibil- 1930s (Groven 1968). The patch was first used as ity, and physical constraints (to name but three). part of the Groven Piano, a digital network of Ya- Using a standard twelve-key piano keyboard, the maha Disklavier pianos, which premiered in Oslo, historical problem has been that any fixed tuning Norway, as part of the Groven Centennial in 2001 in just intonation (i.e., with acoustically pure tri- (see Figure 1). The present version of Groven.Max ads) will be limited to essentially one key.
    [Show full text]
  • 3 Manual Microtonal Organ Ruben Sverre Gjertsen 2013
    3 Manual Microtonal Organ http://www.bek.no/~ruben/Research/Downloads/software.html Ruben Sverre Gjertsen 2013 An interface to existing software A motivation for creating this instrument has been an interest for gaining experience with a large range of intonation systems. This software instrument is built with Max 61, as an interface to the Fluidsynth object2. Fluidsynth offers possibilities for retuning soundfont banks (Sf2 format) to 12-tone or full-register tunings. Max 6 introduced the dictionary format, which has been useful for creating a tuning database in text format, as well as storing presets. This tuning database can naturally be expanded by users, if tunings are written in the syntax read by this instrument. The freely available Jeux organ soundfont3 has been used as a default soundfont, while any instrument in the sf2 format can be loaded. The organ interface The organ window 3 MIDI Keyboards This instrument contains 3 separate fluidsynth modules, named Manual 1-3. 3 keysliders can be played staccato by the mouse for testing, while the most musically sufficient option is performing from connected MIDI keyboards. Available inputs will be automatically recognized and can be selected from the menus. To keep some of the manuals silent, select the bottom alternative "to 2ManualMicroORGANircamSpat 1", which will not receive MIDI signal, unless another program (for instance Sibelius) is sending them. A separate menu can be used to select a foot trigger. The red toggle must be pressed for this to be active. This has been tested with Behringer FCB1010 triggers. Other devices could possibly require adjustments to the patch.
    [Show full text]
  • The Group-Theoretic Description of Musical Pitch Systems
    The Group-theoretic Description of Musical Pitch Systems Marcus Pearce [email protected] 1 Introduction Balzano (1980, 1982, 1986a,b) addresses the question of finding an appropriate level for describing the resources of a pitch system (e.g., the 12-fold or some microtonal division of the octave). His motivation for doing so is twofold: “On the one hand, I am interested as a psychologist who is not overly impressed with the progress we have made since Helmholtz in understanding music perception. On the other hand, I am interested as a computer musician who is trying to find ways of using our pow- erful computational tools to extend the musical [domain] of pitch . ” (Balzano, 1986b, p. 297) Since the resources of a pitch system ultimately depend on the pairwise relations between pitches, the question is one of how to conceive of pitch intervals. In contrast to the prevailing approach which de- scribes intervals in terms of frequency ratios, Balzano presents a description of pitch sets as mathematical groups and describes how the resources of any pitch system may be assessed using this description. Thus he is concerned with presenting an algebraic description of pitch systems as a competitive alternative to the existing acoustic description. In these notes, I shall first give a brief description of the ratio based approach (§2) followed by an equally brief exposition of some necessary concepts from the theory of groups (§3). The following three sections concern the description of the 12-fold division of the octave as a group: §4 presents the nature of the group C12; §5 describes three perceptually relevant properties of pitch-sets in C12; and §6 describes three musically relevant isomorphic representations of C12.
    [Show full text]
  • The Hungarian Rhapsodies and the 15 Hungarian Peasant Songs: Historical and Ideological Parallels Between Liszt and Bartók David Hill
    James Madison University JMU Scholarly Commons Dissertations The Graduate School Spring 2015 The unH garian Rhapsodies and the 15 Hungarian Peasant Songs: Historical and ideological parallels between Liszt and Bartók David B. Hill James Madison University Follow this and additional works at: https://commons.lib.jmu.edu/diss201019 Part of the Musicology Commons Recommended Citation Hill, David B., "The unH garian Rhapsodies and the 15 Hungarian Peasant Songs: Historical and ideological parallels between Liszt and Bartók" (2015). Dissertations. 38. https://commons.lib.jmu.edu/diss201019/38 This Dissertation is brought to you for free and open access by the The Graduate School at JMU Scholarly Commons. It has been accepted for inclusion in Dissertations by an authorized administrator of JMU Scholarly Commons. For more information, please contact [email protected]. The Hungarian Rhapsodies and the 15 Hungarian Peasant Songs: Historical and Ideological Parallels Between Liszt and Bartók David Hill A document submitted to the graduate faculty of JAMES MADISON UNIVERSITY In Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the degree of Doctor of Musical Arts School of Music May 2015 ! TABLE!OF!CONTENTS! ! Figures…………………………………………………………………………………………………………….…iii! ! Abstract……………………………………………………………………………………………………………...iv! ! Introduction………………………………………………………………………………………………………...1! ! PART!I:!SIMILARITIES!SHARED!BY!THE!TWO!NATIONLISTIC!COMPOSERS! ! A.!Origins…………………………………………………………………………………………………………….4! ! B.!Ties!to!Hungary…………………………………………………………………………………………...…..9!
    [Show full text]
  • Unified Music Theories for General Equal-Temperament Systems
    Unified Music Theories for General Equal-Temperament Systems Brandon Tingyeh Wu Research Assistant, Research Center for Information Technology Innovation, Academia Sinica, Taipei, Taiwan ABSTRACT Why are white and black piano keys in an octave arranged as they are today? This article examines the relations between abstract algebra and key signature, scales, degrees, and keyboard configurations in general equal-temperament systems. Without confining the study to the twelve-tone equal-temperament (12-TET) system, we propose a set of basic axioms based on musical observations. The axioms may lead to scales that are reasonable both mathematically and musically in any equal- temperament system. We reexamine the mathematical understandings and interpretations of ideas in classical music theory, such as the circle of fifths, enharmonic equivalent, degrees such as the dominant and the subdominant, and the leading tone, and endow them with meaning outside of the 12-TET system. In the process of deriving scales, we create various kinds of sequences to describe facts in music theory, and we name these sequences systematically and unambiguously with the aim to facilitate future research. - 1 - 1. INTRODUCTION Keyboard configuration and combinatorics The concept of key signatures is based on keyboard-like instruments, such as the piano. If all twelve keys in an octave were white, accidentals and key signatures would be meaningless. Therefore, the arrangement of black and white keys is of crucial importance, and keyboard configuration directly affects scales, degrees, key signatures, and even music theory. To debate the key configuration of the twelve- tone equal-temperament (12-TET) system is of little value because the piano keyboard arrangement is considered the foundation of almost all classical music theories.
    [Show full text]
  • Models of Octatonic and Whole-Tone Interaction: George Crumb and His Predecessors
    Models of Octatonic and Whole-Tone Interaction: George Crumb and His Predecessors Richard Bass Journal of Music Theory, Vol. 38, No. 2. (Autumn, 1994), pp. 155-186. Stable URL: http://links.jstor.org/sici?sici=0022-2909%28199423%2938%3A2%3C155%3AMOOAWI%3E2.0.CO%3B2-X Journal of Music Theory is currently published by Yale University Department of Music. Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of JSTOR's Terms and Conditions of Use, available at http://www.jstor.org/about/terms.html. JSTOR's Terms and Conditions of Use provides, in part, that unless you have obtained prior permission, you may not download an entire issue of a journal or multiple copies of articles, and you may use content in the JSTOR archive only for your personal, non-commercial use. Please contact the publisher regarding any further use of this work. Publisher contact information may be obtained at http://www.jstor.org/journals/yudm.html. Each copy of any part of a JSTOR transmission must contain the same copyright notice that appears on the screen or printed page of such transmission. The JSTOR Archive is a trusted digital repository providing for long-term preservation and access to leading academic journals and scholarly literature from around the world. The Archive is supported by libraries, scholarly societies, publishers, and foundations. It is an initiative of JSTOR, a not-for-profit organization with a mission to help the scholarly community take advantage of advances in technology. For more information regarding JSTOR, please contact [email protected]. http://www.jstor.org Mon Jul 30 09:19:06 2007 MODELS OF OCTATONIC AND WHOLE-TONE INTERACTION: GEORGE CRUMB AND HIS PREDECESSORS Richard Bass A bifurcated view of pitch structure in early twentieth-century music has become more explicit in recent analytic writings.
    [Show full text]
  • Harmonic Resources in 1980S Hard Rock and Heavy Metal Music
    HARMONIC RESOURCES IN 1980S HARD ROCK AND HEAVY METAL MUSIC A thesis submitted to the College of the Arts of Kent State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Master of Arts in Music Theory by Erin M. Vaughn December, 2015 Thesis written by Erin M. Vaughn B.M., The University of Akron, 2003 M.A., Kent State University, 2015 Approved by ____________________________________________ Richard O. Devore, Thesis Advisor ____________________________________________ Ralph Lorenz, Director, School of Music _____________________________________________ John R. Crawford-Spinelli, Dean, College of the Arts ii Table of Contents LIST OF FIGURES ............................................................................................................................... v CHAPTER I........................................................................................................................................ 1 INTRODUCTION ........................................................................................................................... 1 GOALS AND METHODS ................................................................................................................ 3 REVIEW OF RELATED LITERATURE............................................................................................... 5 CHAPTER II..................................................................................................................................... 36 ANALYSIS OF “MASTER OF PUPPETS” ......................................................................................
    [Show full text]
  • Pitch-Class Set Theory: an Overture
    Chapter One Pitch-Class Set Theory: An Overture A Tale of Two Continents In the late afternoon of October 24, 1999, about one hundred people were gathered in a large rehearsal room of the Rotterdam Conservatory. They were listening to a discussion between representatives of nine European countries about the teaching of music theory and music analysis. It was the third day of the Fourth European Music Analysis Conference.1 Most participants in the conference (which included a number of music theorists from Canada and the United States) had been looking forward to this session: meetings about the various analytical traditions and pedagogical practices in Europe were rare, and a broad survey of teaching methods was lacking. Most felt a need for information from beyond their country’s borders. This need was reinforced by the mobility of music students and the resulting hodgepodge of nationalities at renowned conservatories and music schools. Moreover, the European systems of higher education were on the threshold of a harmoni- zation operation. Earlier that year, on June 19, the governments of 29 coun- tries had ratifi ed the “Bologna Declaration,” a document that envisaged a unifi ed European area for higher education. Its enforcement added to the urgency of the meeting in Rotterdam. However, this meeting would not be remembered for the unusually broad rep- resentation of nationalities or for its political timeliness. What would be remem- bered was an incident which took place shortly after the audience had been invited to join in the discussion. Somebody had raised a question about classroom analysis of twentieth-century music, a recurring topic among music theory teach- ers: whereas the music of the eighteenth and nineteenth centuries lent itself to general analytical methodologies, the extremely diverse repertoire of the twen- tieth century seemed only to invite ad hoc approaches; how could the analysis of 1.
    [Show full text]
  • Helmholtz's Dissonance Curve
    Tuning and Timbre: A Perceptual Synthesis Bill Sethares IDEA: Exploit psychoacoustic studies on the perception of consonance and dissonance. The talk begins by showing how to build a device that can measure the “sensory” consonance and/or dissonance of a sound in its musical context. Such a “dissonance meter” has implications in music theory, in synthesizer design, in the con- struction of musical scales and tunings, and in the design of musical instruments. ...the legacy of Helmholtz continues... 1 Some Observations. Why do we tune our instruments the way we do? Some tunings are easier to play in than others. Some timbres work well in certain scales, but not in others. What makes a sound easy in 19-tet but hard in 10-tet? “The timbre of an instrument strongly affects what tuning and scale sound best on that instrument.” – W. Carlos 2 What are Tuning and Timbre? 196 384 589 amplitude 787 magnitude sample: 0 10000 20000 30000 0 1000 2000 3000 4000 time: 0 0.23 0.45 0.68 frequency in Hz Tuning = pitch of the fundamental (in this case 196 Hz) Timbre involves (a) pattern of overtones (Helmholtz) (b) temporal features 3 Some intervals “harmonious” and others “discordant.” Why? X X X X 1.06:1 2:1 X X X X 1.89:1 3:2 X X X X 1.414:1 4:3 4 Theory #1:(Pythagoras ) Humans naturally like the sound of intervals de- fined by small integer ratios. small ratios imply short period of repetition short = simple = sweet Theory #2:(Helmholtz ) Partials of a sound that are close in frequency cause beats that are perceived as “roughness” or dissonance.
    [Show full text]
  • Memory and Production of Standard Frequencies in College-Level Musicians Sarah E
    University of Massachusetts Amherst ScholarWorks@UMass Amherst Masters Theses 1911 - February 2014 2013 Memory and Production of Standard Frequencies in College-Level Musicians Sarah E. Weber University of Massachusetts Amherst Follow this and additional works at: https://scholarworks.umass.edu/theses Part of the Cognition and Perception Commons, Fine Arts Commons, Music Education Commons, and the Music Theory Commons Weber, Sarah E., "Memory and Production of Standard Frequencies in College-Level Musicians" (2013). Masters Theses 1911 - February 2014. 1162. Retrieved from https://scholarworks.umass.edu/theses/1162 This thesis is brought to you for free and open access by ScholarWorks@UMass Amherst. It has been accepted for inclusion in Masters Theses 1911 - February 2014 by an authorized administrator of ScholarWorks@UMass Amherst. For more information, please contact [email protected]. Memory and Production of Standard Frequencies in College-Level Musicians A Thesis Presented by SARAH WEBER Submitted to the Graduate School of the University of Massachusetts Amherst in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of MASTER OF MUSIC September 2013 Music Theory © Copyright by Sarah E. Weber 2013 All Rights Reserved Memory and Production of Standard Frequencies in College-Level Musicians A Thesis Presented by SARAH WEBER _____________________________ Gary S. Karpinski, Chair _____________________________ Andrew Cohen, Member _____________________________ Brent Auerbach, Member _____________________________ Jeff Cox, Department Head Department of Music and Dance DEDICATION For my parents and Grandma. ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS I would like to thank Kristen Wallentinsen for her help with experimental logistics, Renée Morgan for giving me her speakers, and Nathaniel Liberty for his unwavering support, problem-solving skills, and voice-over help.
    [Show full text]
  • Course Name : Indonesian Cultural Arts – Karawitan (Seni Budaya
    Course Name : Indonesian Cultural Arts – Karawitan (Seni Budaya Indonesia – Karawitan) Course Code / Credits : BDU 2303/ 3 SKS Teaching Period : January-June Semester Language Instruction : Indonesian Department : Sastra Nusantara Faculty : Faculty of Arts and Humanities (FIB) Course Description The course of Indonesian Cultural Arts (Karawitan) is a compulsory course for (regular) students of Faculty of Cultural Sciences Universitas Gadjah Mada, especially for the first and second semesters. The course is held every semester and is offered and can be taken by every student from semester 1 to 2. There are no prerequisites for Karawitan courses. The position of Indonesian Culture Arts (Karawitan) as the compulsory course serves to introduce the students to one aspect of Indonesian (or Javanese) art and culture and the practical knowledge related to the performance of traditional Javanese musical instruments, namely gamelan. This course also aims to provide both introduction and theoretical and practical understanding for the students of the Faculty of Cultural Science on gamelan instrument techniques, namely gendhing technique, that is found in Karawitan. Topics in this course include identification of Javanese gamelan instruments, exploration of tones in Javanese gamelan, gendhing instrument method and practice, as well as observation of traditional art performances. Proportionally, 30% of these courses contains briefing theoretical insights, 40% contains gamelan practice, and 30% contains provision of experience in a form of group collaboration and interaction Course Objectives The course of Indonesian Culture Arts (Karawitan) in general aims to provide theoretical and practical supplies through skill, application, and carefulness to recognize various instruments of Gamelan. Through this course, students are observant in identifying the various instruments of the gamelan and its application as instrumental and vocal art in karawitan.
    [Show full text]
  • 11-Music Scales
    Scales in Music Gary Hardegree Department of Philosophy University of Massachusetts Amherst, MA 01003 1. Introduction......................................................................................................................................1 2. The Fundamental Unit – The Octave...............................................................................................2 1. An Aside on Logarithmic Scales .........................................................................................5 2. Cents and Sensibility............................................................................................................6 3. The Pythagorean Construction of the Major Scale..........................................................................7 4. Ptolemaic Tuning...........................................................................................................................10 5. Mean-Tone Temperament (Tuning by Major Thirds) ...................................................................11 6. Problems with Perfect Tuning........................................................................................................12 1. An Aside on The Circle of Fifths and the Circle of Thirds................................................14 2. Back to the Problem of Tuning by Fifths...........................................................................14 3. Ptolemaic Tuning Makes Matters Even Worse! ................................................................15 4. Mean-Tone Temperament’s Wolves..................................................................................17
    [Show full text]