Seizure-Induced Neglect

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Seizure-Induced Neglect J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry: first published as 10.1136/jnnp.43.11.1035 on 1 November 1980. Downloaded from Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 1980, 43. 1035-1040 Seizure-induced neglect KENNETH M HEILMAN AND GREGORY J HOWELL From the Department of Neurology, College of Medicine, University of Florida, and the Veterans Administration Medical Center, Gainesville, Florida, USA SUMMARY A man with intermittent right parieto-occipital seizures was monitored by electro- encephalography while he received 60 trials of being touched on the right, left, or both hands. Half of the trials were given during a focal seizure, and half were given interictally. While the patient was having seizures, he appropriately responded to all 10 stimuli delivered to the right hand, but four of 10 responses were incorrect (allaesthetic) when he was stimulated on the left. With bilateral simultaneous stimulation he neglected the left hand in all 10 trials. His interictal performance was flawless. When given a line-bisection task on two occasions during a seizure, the patient attempted to make a mark to the left of the entire sheet of paper. Immediately postictally he made a mark at the right end of the line. The case illustrates that focal seizures may induce elements of the neglect syndrome and that attention (to contralateral stimuli) and intention to perform (in the contralateral guest. Protected by copyright. hemispatial field) may be dissociable phenomena. Under a variety of stimulus conditions, patients seen in the neglect syndrome, such as hemi- who can detect stimuli fail to report, respond, or spatial neglect, may be induced by an intentional orient to stimuli presented on the side contra- defect.4 6 Recently, we observed a patient with lateral to a cerebral lesion. The failure to report right parietal lobe seizures. The patient showed or respond appropriately to unilateral stimuli has that elements of the neglect syndrome may be been termed "unilateral neglect" or "inatten- associated with focal seizures. He also provided tion."' Sometimes patients with unilateral lesions evidence for the postulate that attentional and respond appropriately to unilateral stimuli but intentional defects are dissociable. fail to report the contralateral stimulus when stimulated on both sides simultaneously. Critch- Case report ley' thought that extinction to simultaneous stimulation is another instance of unilateral A 63 year old man was in good health until two neglect made manifest at a particular moment days before admission when he developed "impair- by the technique of simultaneous stimulation. ment of vision in the left eye." He had had difficulty When touched on the side seeing the chain on a night light. He attributed the opposite to their problem to impaired vision in the left eye because lesion, patients with unilateral neglect often re- the vision in his right eye was already poor secondary ported that they were touched on the side ipsi- to a cataract. He had no other specific complaints http://jnnp.bmj.com/ lateral to their lesion. This has been termed until the next morning when he began to have inter- "allaesthesia."2 Critchley' thought that unilateral mittent episodes of "double vision." During these neglect also was responsible for allaesthesia. episodes, everything seemed to move to the right Because inattention can be induced by a variety and appeared to have constantly fluctuating colours. of cortical and subcortical lesions, we have The patient did not complain of dizziness but did say postulated that many of the behavioural signs that during the two days before admission he had reflect an attention-arousal disorder induced by difficulty in walking, which was most severe during the episodes of "double vision." disruption of a corticolimbic reticular loop.3 4 We Past medical and family histories were non- on October 1, 2021 by have also postulated that some of the behaviour contributory. Medication included spironolactone with hydrochlorothiazide and digoxin. Physical exam- Address for reprint requests: Dr KM Heilman, Box J-236, Depart- ination showed the patient's temperature to be ment of Neurology, College of Medicine, University of Florida, Gainesville, FL 36i7'C; pulse rate, 80 beats per min and regular; 32610, USA. respirations, 16 per min and unlaboured; and blood Accepted 10 July 1980 pressure, 135/90 mmHg without postural change. 1035 J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry: first published as 10.1136/jnnp.43.11.1035 on 1 November 1980. Downloaded from 1036 Kenneth M Heilman and Gregory J Howell Neurological examination showed an alert but half of the state. He would write his name in the indifferent right-handed man who was oriented in right upper quadrant of a sheet of paper and would time, place and person. He accurately named some incorrectly bisect a line too far to the right even of the preceding presidents and recalled major when he was able to identify correctly the left side current events. Despite distraction, he was able to re- of the line by searching for a letter at that end.5 call three objects five minutes after presentation, and His mood was generally eufphoric, but when told he he could recall seven numbers forward and four in was having seizures, he adamantly denied he was reverse order. There was no aphasia, acalculia, finger having anything but brief spells of "double vision." agnosia, or right-left confusion. Reading and writing When we tested for auditory extinction, the patient also were normal. The patient had difficulty drawing was asked to close his eyes. In the unilateral con- a cube, a square, and a triangle. He also appeared ditions (unilateral left, unilateral right), the examiner to have an indifferent affect. Several other tests simultaneously snapped the fingers of both the right of right-hemisphere function were performed. The and left hand approximately 5 cm from the patient's ability to judge mood from pictures of faces patient's right or left ear. In the bilateral condition, was incorrect6 in nine of 12 trials; he correctly the fingers of both hands were simultaneously identified only happy faces. In judging verbal mood, snapped; however, one hand was placed behind each he was incorrect in five of 12 trials; he would judge of the patient's ears. We randomised the order in what was said rather than how it was said. His which these three conditions were given. Tests for ability to create a mood with his own voice was tactile extinction were performed in a similar manner. impaired except for happy moods. Song recognition In the unilateral conditions, the back of the right or and spontaneous singing were normal. When asked left hand was touched. In the bilateral condition both to tell whether two pictures of unfamiliar faces were hands were simultaneously touched on the back. The the same or different, he made four errors in 12 order in which these conditions were given was trials. randomised. guest. Protected by copyright. The pupils were equal in size, round, and reacted Interictally, the patient was given 18 auditory to light and accommodation. Visual acuity in the trials, six in each condition, and 24 tactile trials, left eye was 20/70 with glasses; the patient could eight each condition. He flawlessly performed all of see only hand movements with the right eye because these trials. While he was having a seizure, he was of a dense cataract. There was a dense left homony- also given 18 auditory trials, six in each condition. mous visual field defect. The remainder of the cranial He flawlessly detected and lateralised all unilateral nerves were normal. Motor examination, including stimuli; however, with bilateral simultaneous auditory muscle strength, tone, and coordination, was normal stimuli, he neglected the left-side stimulus in all six except for a tendency for the outstretched left arm trials. During a seizure he was also given 14 uni- to rise. Cerebellar testing, including finger-to-nose, lateral tactile stimuli (seven to each hand) and six heel-to-shin, and rapid alternating movements, was bilateral stimultaneous tactile stimuli. With unilateral normal. The patient walked cautiously and appeared right-hand tactile stimuli, his performance was flaw- to have slight bradykinesia of the left limbs. Deep less. With unilateral left-hand tactile stimuli, he tendon reflexes were normal except for absent ankle detected all seven stimuli, but on two of these trials jerks. The right plantar response was flexor, and the he thought he was being touched on the right hand left was equivocal. There were no other pathologic (allaesthetic response). With bilateral stimuli he reflexes. Sensory examination, including pain, pos- neglected left-hand stimulation in all six trials. ition, vibration, and graphesthesia, was normal except An EEG performed on the day of admission dis- for mild impairment of vibration in the distal portion closed occasional spikes and an occasional slow wave of both lower extremities. focus in the right parieto-occipital region. With the During the neurological examination the patient onset of the patient's sensation of diplopia, the complained that an episode of "double vision" was record revealed polyspikes from the right parieto- http://jnnp.bmj.com/ beginning. His head and eyes then began to deviate occipital area that gradually increased in amplitude to the left. On command he could momentarily and rhythm with a decrease in frequency to approxi- return his head and eyes to the right; however, they mately two to three Hz sharp and slow wave dis- again promptly deviated to the left. The sensation charge that became generalised before returning to of diplopia was present even with his eyes closed. baseline rhythms (see figure). During this seizure the previously noted tendency While being monitored with the EEG, the patient for the outstretched left arm to rise increased, and received 60 trials of being touched on the right, the patient was slow to respond to questions but left, or both hands.
Recommended publications
  • Dissociation Between Awareness and Spatial Coding: Evidence from Unilateral Neglect
    Dissociation between Awareness and Spatial Coding: Evidence from Unilateral Neglect Barbara Treccani1, Roberto Cubelli1, Roberta Sellaro1, Carlo Umiltà2, and Sergio Della Sala3 Downloaded from http://mitprc.silverchair.com/jocn/article-pdf/24/4/854/1777452/jocn_a_00185.pdf by MIT Libraries user on 17 May 2021 Abstract ■ Prevalent theories about consciousness propose a causal re- dissociate. A patient with left neglect, who was not aware of lation between lack of spatial coding and absence of conscious contralesional stimuli, was able to process their color and po- experience: The failure to code the position of an object is sition. However, in contrast to (ipsilesional) consciously per- assumed to prevent this object from entering consciousness. ceived stimuli, color and position of neglected stimuli were This is consistent with influential theories of unilateral neglect processed separately. We propose that individual object fea- following brain damage, according to which spatial coding of tures, including position, can be processed without attention neglected stimuli is defective, and this would keep their process- and consciousness and that conscious perception of an ob- ing at the nonconscious level. Contrary to this view, we report ject depends on the binding of its features into an integrated evidence showing that spatial coding and consciousness can percept. ■ INTRODUCTION the effects of processing of neglected stimuli on responses Awareness of a stimulus can be defined as the explicit to stimuli presented in the intact hemispace. Properties knowledge of its physical and semantic properties or sim- that can be processed implicitly include color and shape, ply of its existence. This knowledge can be demonstrated identity of alphanumerical symbols, and even the mean- by direct reports of the perception of the stimulus; for ing of words and pictures (Della Sala, van der Meulen, example, by naming it, categorizing it, or just by signaling Bestelmeyer, & Logie, 2010; Làdavas, Paladini, & Cubelli, its presence.
    [Show full text]
  • Spatial Stroop with Directional Cues
    Manuscript accepted for publication in “Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology” Increased attentional load moves the left to the right Mario Bonato1,2 * and Simone Cutini3,4 1 Department of Experimental Psychology, Ghent University 2 Department of General Psychology, University of Padova 3 Department of Developmental Psychology, University of Padova 4 Center for Cognitive Neurosciences, University of Padova Running head: Load-dependent contralesional mislocalizations * Corresponding author: Dr. Mario Bonato, Department of Experimental Psychology, Ghent University, Henri Dunantlaan 2, B-9000 Ghent, Belgium e-mail: [email protected] Abstract Introduction Unilateral brain damage can heterogeneously alter spatial processing. Very often brain-lesioned patients fail to report (neglect) items appearing within the contralesional space. Much less often patients mislocalize items’ spatial position. We investigated whether a top-down attentional load manipulation (dual-tasking), known to result in contralesional omissions even in apparently unimpaired cases, might also induce spatial mislocalizations. Method Nine right-hemisphere damaged patients performed three computer-based tasks encompassing different levels of attentional load. The side of appearance of visual targets had to be reported either in isolation or while processing additional information (visual or auditory dual-task). Spatial mislocalizations (from the contralesional hemispace towards the ipsilesional -unaffected- one) were then contrasted with omissions both within and across tasks, at individual as well as at group level. Results The representation of ipsilesional targets was accurate and not affected by dual-tasking requirements. Contralesional targets were instead often omitted and, under dual-task conditions, also mislocalized by four patients. Three cases reported a significant number of left targets as appearing on the right (alloesthesia).
    [Show full text]
  • Abadie's Sign Abadie's Sign Is the Absence Or Diminution of Pain Sensation When Exerting Deep Pressure on the Achilles Tendo
    A.qxd 9/29/05 04:02 PM Page 1 A Abadie’s Sign Abadie’s sign is the absence or diminution of pain sensation when exerting deep pressure on the Achilles tendon by squeezing. This is a frequent finding in the tabes dorsalis variant of neurosyphilis (i.e., with dorsal column disease). Cross References Argyll Robertson pupil Abdominal Paradox - see PARADOXICAL BREATHING Abdominal Reflexes Both superficial and deep abdominal reflexes are described, of which the superficial (cutaneous) reflexes are the more commonly tested in clinical practice. A wooden stick or pin is used to scratch the abdomi- nal wall, from the flank to the midline, parallel to the line of the der- matomal strips, in upper (supraumbilical), middle (umbilical), and lower (infraumbilical) areas. The maneuver is best performed at the end of expiration when the abdominal muscles are relaxed, since the reflexes may be lost with muscle tensing; to avoid this, patients should lie supine with their arms by their sides. Superficial abdominal reflexes are lost in a number of circum- stances: normal old age obesity after abdominal surgery after multiple pregnancies in acute abdominal disorders (Rosenbach’s sign). However, absence of all superficial abdominal reflexes may be of localizing value for corticospinal pathway damage (upper motor neu- rone lesions) above T6. Lesions at or below T10 lead to selective loss of the lower reflexes with the upper and middle reflexes intact, in which case Beevor’s sign may also be present. All abdominal reflexes are preserved with lesions below T12. Abdominal reflexes are said to be lost early in multiple sclerosis, but late in motor neurone disease, an observation of possible clinical use, particularly when differentiating the primary lateral sclerosis vari- ant of motor neurone disease from multiple sclerosis.
    [Show full text]
  • Deficits in Response Space Following Unilateral Striatal Dopamine Depletion in the Rat
    The Journal of Neuroscience, March 1989, g(3): 983-989 Deficits in Response Space Following Unilateral Striatal Dopamine Depletion in the Rat Verity J. Brown and Trevor W. Robbins Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Cambridge, Cambridge CB2 3EB, United Kingdom Hungry rats were trained to report the occurrence and lo- location of stimuli on one side of spaceonly, before depleting cation of brief, unpredictable visual stimuli presented to the DA in the striatum contralateral to that side. In another group left of their heads in 1 of 2 response locations. After training, of rats, DA was depleted from the striatum on the sameside as they received unilateral infusions of 6-hydroxydopamine, de- the discrimination, as a control procedure for nonspecific post- pleting dopamine throughout the head of the caudate pu- operative effects. tamen, either on the left or the right side, that is, either A secondquestion of importance concerning striatal neglect ipsilateral or contralateral to the side on which they were is whether a possible hemispatial deficit induced by unilateral required to respond. striatal lesionsis restricted to one side of the body or, altema- Following an ipsilateral lesion there were no impairments tively, is relative, or allocentric, in nature. Kinsboume and War- in localization of the visual discriminanda and there was no rington (1962) and Bisiach and Luzzatti (1978) found parietal lengthening of reaction time. The contralaterally lesioned neglect not only for left retinotopic, but also for left conceptual rats, however, showed considerably lengthened reaction space.In a human reaction time task, Ladavas (1987) described times to both stimuli and a profound bias to the nearer of 2 components of the neglect; there was not only an overall the 2 response locations.
    [Show full text]
  • Reference of Sensation at the Spinal Level by P
    J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry: first published as 10.1136/jnnp.19.2.88 on 1 May 1956. Downloaded from J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiat., 1956, 19, 88. REFERENCE OF SENSATION AT THE SPINAL LEVEL BY P. W. NATHAN From the Neurological Research Unit of the Medical Research Council, National Hospital, Queen Square, London After the spino-thalamic tract has been cut in The purpose of this paper is to describe the man, loss of sensation of pain and temperature features of such reference of sensation and to record caudal to the lesion is to be expected. In a small it in conditions other than in those previously proportion of patients, although analgesia-in the reported. Certain related phenomena will also be usual sense of the term-is present, painful or described. An investigation of the various forms thermal stimuli applied to parts of the body caudal of the reference of sensation will be reported; and to the lesion arouse a sensation, which is felt, not the possible underlying mechanism and the ana- at the place actually stimulated, but in a normally tomical implications will be considered. innervated part of the body. Thus, a pin applied to the analgesic left leg, for instance, may cause a Observations sensation referred to the normally innervated right has been observed in This reference of sensation Protected by copyright. leg. one patient having an amputation of an upper This form of reference of sensation following the limb, in one patient with a thrombosis of the cutting of the spino-thalamic tract on one side of anterior spinal artery, and in 13 patients following the cord was described by Ray and Wolff in 1945.
    [Show full text]
  • Body Awareness Disorders: Dissociations Between Body-Related Visual and Somatosensory Information Laure Pisella, Laurence Havé, Yves Rossetti
    Body awareness disorders: dissociations between body-related visual and somatosensory information Laure Pisella, Laurence Havé, Yves Rossetti To cite this version: Laure Pisella, Laurence Havé, Yves Rossetti. Body awareness disorders: dissociations between body- related visual and somatosensory information. Brain - A Journal of Neurology , Oxford University Press (OUP), 2019, 142 (8), pp.2170-2173. 10.1093/brain/awz187. hal-02346581 HAL Id: hal-02346581 https://hal.archives-ouvertes.fr/hal-02346581 Submitted on 7 Nov 2019 HAL is a multi-disciplinary open access L’archive ouverte pluridisciplinaire HAL, est archive for the deposit and dissemination of sci- destinée au dépôt et à la diffusion de documents entific research documents, whether they are pub- scientifiques de niveau recherche, publiés ou non, lished or not. The documents may come from émanant des établissements d’enseignement et de teaching and research institutions in France or recherche français ou étrangers, des laboratoires abroad, or from public or private research centers. publics ou privés. BADs: dissociations between body-related visual and somatosensory information L. Pisella1, L. Havé1,3 & Y. Rossetti1,2 1 ImpAct Team, Lyon Neuroscience Research Center CRNL, INSERM U1028, CNRS UMR5292 and University Claude Bernard Lyon I, Villeurbanne, France 2 Plate-forme Mouvement et Handicap, Hospices Civils de Lyon, Centre de Recherche en Neurosciences de Lyon, 69500 Bron, France 3 Hôpital d'instruction des armées Desgenettes, 69275 Lyon, France Glossary : Precuneus: medial part of the posterior parietal cortex, between the occipital (cuneus) and the anterior parietal (paracentral lobule) cortices, well located for visual- somatosensory integration. Body image typically depicts mental representation of one’s own body, arising from all sources of sensory and cognitive information, whereas body schema is used to depict the unconscious use of sensory information required by our motor system to maintain body posture and produce accurate movements.
    [Show full text]
  • Functional Sensory Symptoms
    Handbook of Clinical Neurology, Vol. 139 (3rd series) Functional Neurologic Disorders M. Hallett, J. Stone, and A. Carson, Editors http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-801772-2.00024-2 © 2016 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved Chapter 24 Functional sensory symptoms J. STONE1* AND M. VERMEULEN2 1Department of Clinical Neurosciences, Centre for Clinical Brain Sciences, University of Edinburgh, Edinburgh, UK 2Department of Neurology, Academic Medical Center, Amsterdam, The Netherlands Abstract Functional (psychogenic) sensory symptoms are those in which the patient genuinely experiences alter- ation or absence of normal sensation in the absence of neurologic disease. The hallmark of functional sen- sory symptoms is the presence of internal inconsistency revealing a pattern of symptoms governed by abnormally focused attention. In this chapter we review the history of this area, different clinical presentations, diagnosis (including sensitivity of diagnostic tests), treatment, experimental studies, and prognosis. Altered sensation has been a feature of “hysteria” since descriptions of witchcraft in the middle ages. In the 19th century hysteric sensory stigmata were considered a hallmark of the condition. Despite this long history, relatively little attention has been paid to the topic of functional sensory disturbance, compared to functional limb weakness or functional movement disorders, with which it commonly coexists. There are recognizable clinical patterns, such as hemisensory disturbance and sensory disturbance fin- ishing at the groin or shoulder, but in keeping with the literature on reliability of sensory signs in neurology in general, the evidence suggests that physical signs designed to make a positive diagnosis of functional sensory disorder may not be that reliable.
    [Show full text]
  • Coding of Spatial Lnformation in the with Neglect Following Parietal Lobe
    Coding of Spatial Lnformation in the Somatosensory System: Evidence Erom Patients with Neglect following Parietal Lobe Damage Morris Moscovitch and Marlene Behrmann University of Toronto and Rotnian Research Institute of Baycrest Centre, Toronto Downloaded from http://mitprc.silverchair.com/jocn/article-pdf/6/2/151/1755120/jocn.1994.6.2.151.pdf by guest on 18 May 2021 Abstract Unilateral parietal lobe darnage, particularly in the right both the palm up and the palm down position. Patients ne- cerehral hemisphere, leads to neglect of stimuli on the contra- glected the stimuli on the side of space contralateral to the 1ater;rl side. To determine the reference frame within which lesion regardless of hand position. These results indicate that neglect operates in the somatosensory system, 11 patients with point-localization in the somatosensory system is accomplished unilateral neglect were touched simultaneously on the left and with respect to a spatially defined frame-of-referenceand not right side of the wrist of one hand. The hand was tested in strictly with respect to somatotopically defined coordinates. INTRODUCTION stimulation of the sensory surface projecting to the le- sioned hemisphere. Recent work in vision has refuted Individuals with damage to the right parietal lobes often the notion that this view can account for all aspects of suffer from hemineglect, a dramatic attentional disorder neglect. A number of investigators have shown that what in which information on the left side is ignored (Bisiach is neglected is not necessarily the stimulus in the left & Vattar, 1988; Critchley, 1953; Heilman, Watson, & Val- visual field, but rather the left-most item of a set of enstcin, 1985; Mesulam, 1981).
    [Show full text]
  • A Dictionary of Neurological Signs.Pdf
    A DICTIONARY OF NEUROLOGICAL SIGNS THIRD EDITION A DICTIONARY OF NEUROLOGICAL SIGNS THIRD EDITION A.J. LARNER MA, MD, MRCP (UK), DHMSA Consultant Neurologist Walton Centre for Neurology and Neurosurgery, Liverpool Honorary Lecturer in Neuroscience, University of Liverpool Society of Apothecaries’ Honorary Lecturer in the History of Medicine, University of Liverpool Liverpool, U.K. 123 Andrew J. Larner MA MD MRCP (UK) DHMSA Walton Centre for Neurology & Neurosurgery Lower Lane L9 7LJ Liverpool, UK ISBN 978-1-4419-7094-7 e-ISBN 978-1-4419-7095-4 DOI 10.1007/978-1-4419-7095-4 Springer New York Dordrecht Heidelberg London Library of Congress Control Number: 2010937226 © Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2001, 2006, 2011 All rights reserved. This work may not be translated or copied in whole or in part without the written permission of the publisher (Springer Science+Business Media, LLC, 233 Spring Street, New York, NY 10013, USA), except for brief excerpts in connection with reviews or scholarly analysis. Use in connection with any form of information storage and retrieval, electronic adaptation, computer software, or by similar or dissimilar methodology now known or hereafter developed is forbidden. The use in this publication of trade names, trademarks, service marks, and similar terms, even if they are not identified as such, is not to be taken as an expression of opinion as to whether or not they are subject to proprietary rights. While the advice and information in this book are believed to be true and accurate at the date of going to press, neither the authors nor the editors nor the publisher can accept any legal responsibility for any errors or omissions that may be made.
    [Show full text]
  • Peripersonal Space: a Multisensory Interface for Body-Objects Interactions
    Peripersonal space : a multisensory interface for body-objects interactions Claudio Brozzoli To cite this version: Claudio Brozzoli. Peripersonal space : a multisensory interface for body-objects interactions. Human health and pathology. Université Claude Bernard - Lyon I, 2009. English. NNT : 2009LYO10233. tel-00675247 HAL Id: tel-00675247 https://tel.archives-ouvertes.fr/tel-00675247 Submitted on 29 Feb 2012 HAL is a multi-disciplinary open access L’archive ouverte pluridisciplinaire HAL, est archive for the deposit and dissemination of sci- destinée au dépôt et à la diffusion de documents entific research documents, whether they are pub- scientifiques de niveau recherche, publiés ou non, lished or not. The documents may come from émanant des établissements d’enseignement et de teaching and research institutions in France or recherche français ou étrangers, des laboratoires abroad, or from public or private research centers. publics ou privés. N° d’ordre 233-2009 Année 2009 THESE DE L‘UNIVERSITE DE LYON Délivrée par L’UNIVERSITE CLAUDE BERNARD LYON 1 ECOLE DOCTORALE Neurosciences et Cognition DIPLOME DE DOCTORAT (arrêté du 7 août 2006) soutenue publiquement le 20/11/2009 par M. Claudio BROZZOLI PERIPERSONAL SPACE : A MULTISENSORY INTERFACE FOR BODY-OBJECTS INTERACTIONS Directeur de thèse : Dr. FARNÈ Alessandro, Ph.D. JURY: Prof. Y. Rossetti M.D., Ph.D. Dr. A. Farnè Ph.D., D.R. Prof. S. Soto-Faraco Ph.D. Prof. C. Spence Ph.D. Prof. O. Blanke M.D., Ph.D. Prof. F. Pavani, Ph.D. Dr. J.-R. Duhamel Ph.D., D.R. A Fede, perché questo spazio mi ha preso nel tempo che a volte era suo AKNOWLEDGEMENTS It has been amazingly exciting.
    [Show full text]
  • Synesthesia: Phenomenology and Neuropsychology a Review of Current Knowledge
    Synesthesia: Phenomenology And Neuropsychology A Review of Current Knowledge Richard E. Cytowic 4720 Blagden Terrace, NW Washington DC 20011-3720 USA [email protected] Copyright (c) Richard E. Cytowic 1995 PSYCHE, 2(10), July 1995 http://psyche.cs.monash.edu.au/v2/psyche-2-10-cytowic.html KEYWORDS: consciousness, emotion, perception, subjectivity, synesthesia, neurology. ABSTRACT: Synesthesia (Greek, syn = together + aisthesis = perception) is the involuntary physical experience of a cross-modal association. That is, the stimulation of one sensory modality reliably causes a perception in one or more different senses. Its phenomenology clearly distinguishes it from metaphor, literary tropes, sound symbolism, and deliberate artistic contrivances that sometimes employ the term "synesthesia" to describe their multisensory joinings. An unexpected demographic and cognitive constellation co-occurs with synesthesia: females and non-right-handers predominate, the trait is familial, and memory is superior while math and spatial navigation suffer. Synesthesia appears to be a left-hemisphere function that is not cortical in the conventional sense. The hippocampus is critical for its experience. Five clinical features comprise its diagnosis. Synesthesia is "abnormal" only in being statistically rare. It is, in fact, a normal brain process that is prematurely displayed to consciousness in a minority of individuals. 1. Introduction 1.1 Although medicine has known about synesthesia for three centuries, it keeps forgetting that it knows. After decades of neglect, a revival of inquiry is under way. As in earlier times, today's interest is multidisciplinary. Neuroscience is particularly curious this time - or at least it should be - because of what synesthesia might tell us about consciousness, the nature of reality, and the relationship between reason and emotion.
    [Show full text]
  • Reflex Neurogenic Inflammation I
    0270.6474/85/0505-1380$02.00/O The Journal of Neuroscience CopyrIght 0 Society for Neuroscience Vol. 5, No. 5, pp. 1380-1386 Printed In U.S.A. May 1985 Reflex Neurogenic Inflammation I. Contribution of the Peripheral Nervous System to Spatially Remote Inflammatory Responses That Follow Injury’ JON D. LEVINE,*s2 SAMUEL J. DARDICK,* ALLAN I. BASBAUM,* AND EBENEZER SCIPIO§ Departments of *Medicine, *Anatomy, and $&‘tomatology, University of California, San Francisco, San Francisco, California 94143 Abstract and hyperalgesia produced by unilateral hindlimb injury. We also provide evidence that this spread of acute inflammation involves a Recent studies of the mechanism of neurogenic inflam- spinal cord-mediated connection between the injured and remote mation have focused on the contribution of neuropeptides sites. released from peripheral terminals of primary afferent sen- sory neurons. In this study we addressed the contribution of Materials and Methods humoral and neural factors to the hyperalgesia and swelling The experiments were performed on 250. to 300.gm male Sprague-Dawley that are produced contralateral to an injured hindpaw, a rats (Bantin and Kingman, Fremont CA). The standard injury stimulus con- phenomenon which we refer to as reflex neurogenic inflam- sisted of a subcutaneous injection of 0.15 ml of normal saline, through a 30 mation. The contralateral inflammatory response develops gauge hypodermic needle, into the footpad on the plantar surface of the gradually, over a period of hours, and shows no tachyphylaxis midfoot. In one group of rats, capsaicin (Sigma Chemical Co., St. Louis, MO) with repeated application of the same stimulus. Denervation in a vehicle of 50% by volume dimethylsulfoxide (DMSO) in normal saline of either limb significantly attenuated the contralateral re- was injected in place of the standard injury stimulus.
    [Show full text]