Body Awareness Disorders: Dissociations Between Body-Related Visual and Somatosensory Information Laure Pisella, Laurence Havé, Yves Rossetti
Total Page:16
File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb
Load more
Recommended publications
-
Cerebral Cortex Structure, Function, Dysfunction Reading Ch 10 Waxman Dental Neuroanatomy Lecture Suzanne Stensaas, Ph.D
Cerebral Cortex Structure, Function, Dysfunction Reading Ch 10 Waxman Dental Neuroanatomy Lecture Suzanne Stensaas, Ph.D. March 15, 2011 Anatomy Review • Lobes and layers • Brodmann’s areas • Vascular Supply • Major Neurological Findings – Frontal, Parietal, Temporal, Occipital, Limbic • Quiz Questions? Types of Cortex • Sensory • Motor • Unimodal association • Multimodal association necessary for language, reason, plan, imagine, create Structure of neocortex (6 layers) The general pattern of primary, association and mulimodal association cortex (Mesulam) Brodmann, Lateral Left Hemisphere MCA left hemisphere from D.Haines ACA and PCA -Haines Issues of Functional Localization • Earliest studies -Signs, symptoms and note location • Electrical discharge (epilepsy) suggested function • Ablation - deficit suggest function • Reappearance of infant functions suggest loss of inhibition (disinhibition), i.e. grasp, suck, Babinski • Variabilities in case reports • Linked networks of afferent and efferent neurons in several regions working to accomplish a task • Functional imaging does not always equate with abnormal function associated with location of lesion • fMRI activation of several cortical regions • Same sign from lesions in different areas – i.e.paraphasias • Notion of the right hemisphere as "emotional" in contrast to the left one as "logical" has no basis in fact. Limbic System (not a true lobe) involves with cingulate gyrus and the • Hippocampus- short term memory • Amygdala- fear, agression, mating • Fornix pathway to hypothalamus • -
Lateralization of Function Human Brain
Lateralization of Function Dr. Coulson Cognitive Science Department UCSD Human Brain • An extension of the spinal cord 1 Cerebral Hemispheres Corpus Callosum 2 Cartoon View of Brain Cerebral Lobes 3 Neurons • Brain composed of neurons – 100 billion • Neurons both send and receive signals to other cells in form of pulses • Important parts – Cell body –Axon – Synapse Connectivity • Each neuron connected to 10,000 other neurons • Point of contact is the synapse • Computing power of brain comes from connections 4 Cortex • Two millimeters thick and has area of 1.5 square meters Cartoon View: Frontal Lobe • In front of central sulcus • Decisions, judgments, emotions 5 Cartoon View: Parietal Lobe • Behind central sulcus • Perception of stimuli related to touch, pressure, temperature, pain Cartoon View: Temporal Lobe • Below lateral fissure • Perception, recognition, auditory processing 6 Cartoon View: Occipital Lobe • Located at back of brain, behind the parietal lobe and temporal lobe • Vision Lateralization of Function • One side of the brain is more crucial for a given function and/or more efficient at the underlying computational tasks • Typically a matter of degree – Strongly vs. Weakly Lateralized • Motor control a good example of a lateralized function 7 Sensorimotor Cortex 8 Motor Control What about language? • Language is a paradigmatic example of a lateralized cognitive phenomenon 9 Wada Test Lateralization of Function • Most evidence of lateralized brain function comes from observing how brain damage affects behavior on various sorts -
Toward a Common Terminology for the Gyri and Sulci of the Human Cerebral Cortex Hans Ten Donkelaar, Nathalie Tzourio-Mazoyer, Jürgen Mai
Toward a Common Terminology for the Gyri and Sulci of the Human Cerebral Cortex Hans ten Donkelaar, Nathalie Tzourio-Mazoyer, Jürgen Mai To cite this version: Hans ten Donkelaar, Nathalie Tzourio-Mazoyer, Jürgen Mai. Toward a Common Terminology for the Gyri and Sulci of the Human Cerebral Cortex. Frontiers in Neuroanatomy, Frontiers, 2018, 12, pp.93. 10.3389/fnana.2018.00093. hal-01929541 HAL Id: hal-01929541 https://hal.archives-ouvertes.fr/hal-01929541 Submitted on 21 Nov 2018 HAL is a multi-disciplinary open access L’archive ouverte pluridisciplinaire HAL, est archive for the deposit and dissemination of sci- destinée au dépôt et à la diffusion de documents entific research documents, whether they are pub- scientifiques de niveau recherche, publiés ou non, lished or not. The documents may come from émanant des établissements d’enseignement et de teaching and research institutions in France or recherche français ou étrangers, des laboratoires abroad, or from public or private research centers. publics ou privés. REVIEW published: 19 November 2018 doi: 10.3389/fnana.2018.00093 Toward a Common Terminology for the Gyri and Sulci of the Human Cerebral Cortex Hans J. ten Donkelaar 1*†, Nathalie Tzourio-Mazoyer 2† and Jürgen K. Mai 3† 1 Department of Neurology, Donders Center for Medical Neuroscience, Radboud University Medical Center, Nijmegen, Netherlands, 2 IMN Institut des Maladies Neurodégénératives UMR 5293, Université de Bordeaux, Bordeaux, France, 3 Institute for Anatomy, Heinrich Heine University, Düsseldorf, Germany The gyri and sulci of the human brain were defined by pioneers such as Louis-Pierre Gratiolet and Alexander Ecker, and extensified by, among others, Dejerine (1895) and von Economo and Koskinas (1925). -
Dissociation Between Awareness and Spatial Coding: Evidence from Unilateral Neglect
Dissociation between Awareness and Spatial Coding: Evidence from Unilateral Neglect Barbara Treccani1, Roberto Cubelli1, Roberta Sellaro1, Carlo Umiltà2, and Sergio Della Sala3 Downloaded from http://mitprc.silverchair.com/jocn/article-pdf/24/4/854/1777452/jocn_a_00185.pdf by MIT Libraries user on 17 May 2021 Abstract ■ Prevalent theories about consciousness propose a causal re- dissociate. A patient with left neglect, who was not aware of lation between lack of spatial coding and absence of conscious contralesional stimuli, was able to process their color and po- experience: The failure to code the position of an object is sition. However, in contrast to (ipsilesional) consciously per- assumed to prevent this object from entering consciousness. ceived stimuli, color and position of neglected stimuli were This is consistent with influential theories of unilateral neglect processed separately. We propose that individual object fea- following brain damage, according to which spatial coding of tures, including position, can be processed without attention neglected stimuli is defective, and this would keep their process- and consciousness and that conscious perception of an ob- ing at the nonconscious level. Contrary to this view, we report ject depends on the binding of its features into an integrated evidence showing that spatial coding and consciousness can percept. ■ INTRODUCTION the effects of processing of neglected stimuli on responses Awareness of a stimulus can be defined as the explicit to stimuli presented in the intact hemispace. Properties knowledge of its physical and semantic properties or sim- that can be processed implicitly include color and shape, ply of its existence. This knowledge can be demonstrated identity of alphanumerical symbols, and even the mean- by direct reports of the perception of the stimulus; for ing of words and pictures (Della Sala, van der Meulen, example, by naming it, categorizing it, or just by signaling Bestelmeyer, & Logie, 2010; Làdavas, Paladini, & Cubelli, its presence. -
Spatial Stroop with Directional Cues
Manuscript accepted for publication in “Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology” Increased attentional load moves the left to the right Mario Bonato1,2 * and Simone Cutini3,4 1 Department of Experimental Psychology, Ghent University 2 Department of General Psychology, University of Padova 3 Department of Developmental Psychology, University of Padova 4 Center for Cognitive Neurosciences, University of Padova Running head: Load-dependent contralesional mislocalizations * Corresponding author: Dr. Mario Bonato, Department of Experimental Psychology, Ghent University, Henri Dunantlaan 2, B-9000 Ghent, Belgium e-mail: [email protected] Abstract Introduction Unilateral brain damage can heterogeneously alter spatial processing. Very often brain-lesioned patients fail to report (neglect) items appearing within the contralesional space. Much less often patients mislocalize items’ spatial position. We investigated whether a top-down attentional load manipulation (dual-tasking), known to result in contralesional omissions even in apparently unimpaired cases, might also induce spatial mislocalizations. Method Nine right-hemisphere damaged patients performed three computer-based tasks encompassing different levels of attentional load. The side of appearance of visual targets had to be reported either in isolation or while processing additional information (visual or auditory dual-task). Spatial mislocalizations (from the contralesional hemispace towards the ipsilesional -unaffected- one) were then contrasted with omissions both within and across tasks, at individual as well as at group level. Results The representation of ipsilesional targets was accurate and not affected by dual-tasking requirements. Contralesional targets were instead often omitted and, under dual-task conditions, also mislocalized by four patients. Three cases reported a significant number of left targets as appearing on the right (alloesthesia). -
Function of Cerebral Cortex
FUNCTION OF CEREBRAL CORTEX Course: Neuropsychology CC-6 (M.A PSYCHOLOGY SEM II); Unit I By Dr. Priyanka Kumari Assistant Professor Institute of Psychological Research and Service Patna University Contact No.7654991023; E-mail- [email protected] The cerebral cortex—the thin outer covering of the brain-is the part of the brain responsible for our ability to reason, plan, remember, and imagine. Cerebral Cortex accounts for our impressive capacity to process and transform information. The cerebral cortex is only about one-eighth of an inch thick, but it contains billions of neurons, each connected to thousands of others. The predominance of cell bodies gives the cortex a brownish gray colour. Because of its appearance, the cortex is often referred to as gray matter. Beneath the cortex are myelin-sheathed axons connecting the neurons of the cortex with those of other parts of the brain. The large concentrations of myelin make this tissue look whitish and opaque, and hence it is often referred to as white matter. The cortex is divided into two nearly symmetrical halves, the cerebral hemispheres . Thus, many of the structures of the cerebral cortex appear in both the left and right cerebral hemispheres. The two hemispheres appear to be somewhat specialized in the functions they perform. The cerebral hemispheres are folded into many ridges and grooves, which greatly increase their surface area. Each hemisphere is usually described, on the basis of the largest of these grooves or fissures, as being divided into four distinct regions or lobes. The four lobes are: • Frontal, • Parietal, • Occipital, and • Temporal. -
Abadie's Sign Abadie's Sign Is the Absence Or Diminution of Pain Sensation When Exerting Deep Pressure on the Achilles Tendo
A.qxd 9/29/05 04:02 PM Page 1 A Abadie’s Sign Abadie’s sign is the absence or diminution of pain sensation when exerting deep pressure on the Achilles tendon by squeezing. This is a frequent finding in the tabes dorsalis variant of neurosyphilis (i.e., with dorsal column disease). Cross References Argyll Robertson pupil Abdominal Paradox - see PARADOXICAL BREATHING Abdominal Reflexes Both superficial and deep abdominal reflexes are described, of which the superficial (cutaneous) reflexes are the more commonly tested in clinical practice. A wooden stick or pin is used to scratch the abdomi- nal wall, from the flank to the midline, parallel to the line of the der- matomal strips, in upper (supraumbilical), middle (umbilical), and lower (infraumbilical) areas. The maneuver is best performed at the end of expiration when the abdominal muscles are relaxed, since the reflexes may be lost with muscle tensing; to avoid this, patients should lie supine with their arms by their sides. Superficial abdominal reflexes are lost in a number of circum- stances: normal old age obesity after abdominal surgery after multiple pregnancies in acute abdominal disorders (Rosenbach’s sign). However, absence of all superficial abdominal reflexes may be of localizing value for corticospinal pathway damage (upper motor neu- rone lesions) above T6. Lesions at or below T10 lead to selective loss of the lower reflexes with the upper and middle reflexes intact, in which case Beevor’s sign may also be present. All abdominal reflexes are preserved with lesions below T12. Abdominal reflexes are said to be lost early in multiple sclerosis, but late in motor neurone disease, an observation of possible clinical use, particularly when differentiating the primary lateral sclerosis vari- ant of motor neurone disease from multiple sclerosis. -
Deficits in Response Space Following Unilateral Striatal Dopamine Depletion in the Rat
The Journal of Neuroscience, March 1989, g(3): 983-989 Deficits in Response Space Following Unilateral Striatal Dopamine Depletion in the Rat Verity J. Brown and Trevor W. Robbins Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Cambridge, Cambridge CB2 3EB, United Kingdom Hungry rats were trained to report the occurrence and lo- location of stimuli on one side of spaceonly, before depleting cation of brief, unpredictable visual stimuli presented to the DA in the striatum contralateral to that side. In another group left of their heads in 1 of 2 response locations. After training, of rats, DA was depleted from the striatum on the sameside as they received unilateral infusions of 6-hydroxydopamine, de- the discrimination, as a control procedure for nonspecific post- pleting dopamine throughout the head of the caudate pu- operative effects. tamen, either on the left or the right side, that is, either A secondquestion of importance concerning striatal neglect ipsilateral or contralateral to the side on which they were is whether a possible hemispatial deficit induced by unilateral required to respond. striatal lesionsis restricted to one side of the body or, altema- Following an ipsilateral lesion there were no impairments tively, is relative, or allocentric, in nature. Kinsboume and War- in localization of the visual discriminanda and there was no rington (1962) and Bisiach and Luzzatti (1978) found parietal lengthening of reaction time. The contralaterally lesioned neglect not only for left retinotopic, but also for left conceptual rats, however, showed considerably lengthened reaction space.In a human reaction time task, Ladavas (1987) described times to both stimuli and a profound bias to the nearer of 2 components of the neglect; there was not only an overall the 2 response locations. -
Reference of Sensation at the Spinal Level by P
J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry: first published as 10.1136/jnnp.19.2.88 on 1 May 1956. Downloaded from J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiat., 1956, 19, 88. REFERENCE OF SENSATION AT THE SPINAL LEVEL BY P. W. NATHAN From the Neurological Research Unit of the Medical Research Council, National Hospital, Queen Square, London After the spino-thalamic tract has been cut in The purpose of this paper is to describe the man, loss of sensation of pain and temperature features of such reference of sensation and to record caudal to the lesion is to be expected. In a small it in conditions other than in those previously proportion of patients, although analgesia-in the reported. Certain related phenomena will also be usual sense of the term-is present, painful or described. An investigation of the various forms thermal stimuli applied to parts of the body caudal of the reference of sensation will be reported; and to the lesion arouse a sensation, which is felt, not the possible underlying mechanism and the ana- at the place actually stimulated, but in a normally tomical implications will be considered. innervated part of the body. Thus, a pin applied to the analgesic left leg, for instance, may cause a Observations sensation referred to the normally innervated right has been observed in This reference of sensation Protected by copyright. leg. one patient having an amputation of an upper This form of reference of sensation following the limb, in one patient with a thrombosis of the cutting of the spino-thalamic tract on one side of anterior spinal artery, and in 13 patients following the cord was described by Ray and Wolff in 1945. -
A Practical Review of Functional MRI Anatomy of the Language and Motor Systems
REVIEW ARTICLE FUNCTIONAL A Practical Review of Functional MRI Anatomy of the Language and Motor Systems X V.B. Hill, X C.Z. Cankurtaran, X B.P. Liu, X T.A. Hijaz, X M. Naidich, X A.J. Nemeth, X J. Gastala, X C. Krumpelman, X E.N. McComb, and X A.W. Korutz ABSTRACT SUMMARY: Functional MR imaging is being performed with increasing frequency in the typical neuroradiology practice; however, many readers of these studies have only a limited knowledge of the functional anatomy of the brain. This text will delineate the locations, anatomic boundaries, and functions of the cortical regions of the brain most commonly encountered in clinical practice—specifically, the regions involved in movement and language. ABBREVIATIONS: FFA ϭ fusiform face area; IPL ϭ inferior parietal lobule; PPC ϭ posterior parietal cortex; SMA ϭ supplementary motor area; VOTC ϭ ventral occipitotemporal cortex his article serves as a review of the functional areas of the brain serving to analyze spatial position and the ventral stream working Tmost commonly mapped during presurgical fMRI studies, to identify what an object is. Influenced by the dorsal and ventral specifically targeting movement and language. We have compiled stream model of vision, Hickok and Poeppel2 hypothesized a sim- what we hope is a useful, easily portable, and concise resource that ilar framework for language. In this model, the ventral stream, or can be accessible to radiologists everywhere. We begin with a re- lexical-semantic system, is involved in sound-to-meaning map- view of the language-processing system. Then we describe the pings associated with language comprehension and semantic ac- gross anatomic boundaries, organization, and function of each cess. -
Seed MNI Coordinates Lobe
MNI Coordinates Seed Lobe (Hemisphere) Region BAa X Y Z FP1 -18 62 0 Frontal Lobe (L) Medial Frontal Gyrus 10 FPz 4 62 0 Frontal Lobe (R) Medial Frontal Gyrus 10 FP2 24 60 0 Frontal Lobe (R) Superior Frontal Gyrus 10 AF7 -38 50 0 Frontal Lobe (L) Middle Frontal Gyrus 10 AF3 -30 50 24 Frontal Lobe (L) Superior Frontal Gyrus 9 AFz 4 58 30 Frontal Lobe (R) Medial Frontal Gyrus 9 AF4 36 48 20 Frontal Lobe (R) Middle Frontal Gyrus 10 AF8 42 46 -4 Frontal Lobe (R) Inferior Frontal Gyrus 10 F7 -48 26 -4 Frontal Lobe (L) Inferior Frontal Gyrus 47 F5 -48 28 18 Frontal Lobe (L) Inferior Frontal Gyrus 45 F3 -38 28 38 Frontal Lobe (L) Precentral Gyrus 9 F1 -20 30 50 Frontal Lobe (L) Superior Frontal Gyrus 8 Fz 2 32 54 Frontal Lobe (L) Superior Frontal Gyrus 8 F2 26 32 48 Frontal Lobe (R) Superior Frontal Gyrus 8 F4 42 30 34 Frontal Lobe (R) Precentral Gyrus 9 F6 50 28 14 Frontal Lobe (R) Middle Frontal Gyrus 46 F8 48 24 -8 Frontal Lobe (R) Inferior Frontal Gyrus 47 FT9 -50 -6 -36 Temporal Lobe (L) Inferior Temporal Gyrus 20 FT7 -54 2 -8 Temporal Lobe (L) Superior Temporal Gyrus 22 FC5 -56 4 22 Frontal Lobe (L) Precentral Gyrus 6 FC3 -44 6 48 Frontal Lobe (L) Middle Frontal Gyrus 6 FC1 -22 6 64 Frontal Lobe (L) Middle Frontal Gyrus 6 FCz 4 6 66 Frontal Lobe (R) Medial Frontal Gyrus 6 FC2 28 8 60 Frontal Lobe (R) Sub-Gyral 6 FC4 48 8 42 Frontal Lobe (R) Middle Frontal Gyrus 6 FC6 58 6 16 Frontal Lobe (R) Inferior Frontal Gyrus 44 FT8 54 2 -12 Temporal Lobe (R) Superior Temporal Gyrus 38 FT10 50 -6 -38 Temporal Lobe (R) Inferior Temporal Gyrus 20 T7/T3 -
Functional Sensory Symptoms
Handbook of Clinical Neurology, Vol. 139 (3rd series) Functional Neurologic Disorders M. Hallett, J. Stone, and A. Carson, Editors http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-801772-2.00024-2 © 2016 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved Chapter 24 Functional sensory symptoms J. STONE1* AND M. VERMEULEN2 1Department of Clinical Neurosciences, Centre for Clinical Brain Sciences, University of Edinburgh, Edinburgh, UK 2Department of Neurology, Academic Medical Center, Amsterdam, The Netherlands Abstract Functional (psychogenic) sensory symptoms are those in which the patient genuinely experiences alter- ation or absence of normal sensation in the absence of neurologic disease. The hallmark of functional sen- sory symptoms is the presence of internal inconsistency revealing a pattern of symptoms governed by abnormally focused attention. In this chapter we review the history of this area, different clinical presentations, diagnosis (including sensitivity of diagnostic tests), treatment, experimental studies, and prognosis. Altered sensation has been a feature of “hysteria” since descriptions of witchcraft in the middle ages. In the 19th century hysteric sensory stigmata were considered a hallmark of the condition. Despite this long history, relatively little attention has been paid to the topic of functional sensory disturbance, compared to functional limb weakness or functional movement disorders, with which it commonly coexists. There are recognizable clinical patterns, such as hemisensory disturbance and sensory disturbance fin- ishing at the groin or shoulder, but in keeping with the literature on reliability of sensory signs in neurology in general, the evidence suggests that physical signs designed to make a positive diagnosis of functional sensory disorder may not be that reliable.