<<

Language Sciences 34 (2012) 13–27

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Language Sciences

journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/langsci

Gender and mobile phones in cross-national context ⇑ Naomi S. Baron , Elise M. Campbell

American University, Washington, DC 20016-8045, USA article info abstract

Article history: The sociolinguistic literature has frequently noted differences in how males and females Received 6 August 2010 communicate face-to-face and in writing, and more recently, through information and Received in revised form 16 June 2011 communication technologies. This article reports on gender patterns identified in a Accepted 23 June 2011 cross-national study of use by university students in Sweden, the US, Italy, Available online 22 July 2011 Japan and Korea. Data were analyzed with respect to the purpose of communication, politeness issues, contact management and volume of use (along with user complaints Keywords: about dependency and reachability). Results indicated a number of gendered usage and Attitudes attitudinal patterns. However, in some cases, cultural variables may prove more explana- Cell phone Culture tory than gender. Gender Ó 2011 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. Mobile phone

Communicating online and via mobile devices has come of age. As of 2010, there were an estimated 2.1 billion users and almost 5.3 billion mobile phone subscriptions (ITU, 2010), out of a world population of roughly 6.9 billion. In much of the world, males were initially more likely than females to utilize information and communication technologies (ICTs) (Ono and Zavodny, 2005), though in a growing number of countries, the gap is closing or has been closed (Rainie et al., 2000; World Internet Project, 2010). Gender differences persist in the uses to which new media technologies are put. For example, more American females than males send email to strengthen ties with family and friends (Rainie et al., 2000); at least some American men use mobile phones to adjust personal schedules in their favor, leaving women to bear the brunt of household responsibilities (Chesley, 2005). A growing body of research documents differences in male versus female use of mobile in a range of national and/or cultural contexts (e.g., Baron and Hård af Segerstad, 2010; Fortunati, 2009a; Fortunati and Manganelli, 2002; Fujimoto, 2005; Hijazi-Omari and Ribak, 2008; Hjorth, 2005; Lee, 2005; Lemish and Cohen, 2005; Skog, 2002). The present study explores gender and by analyzing data from a cross-national investigation of mobile phone use by university students in Sweden, the US, Italy, Japan and Korea. Collecting data from multiple countries enables us to compare the role of gender versus culture (to the extent culture corresponds with nationhood) in use of and attitudes towards mobile telephony.

1. The gender question in language

The sociolinguistic literature has frequently reported that males and females tend to use language differently (e.g., Bergvall et al., 1996; Holmes, 1993, 1995; Holmes and Meyerhoff, 2003; James, 1996; Labov, 1991; Lakoff, 1975; Romaine, 2003; Tannen, 1994). Linguistic variation runs the gamut from who dominates the conversation to type of vocabulary, function of message, or use of politeness conventions. Many scholars (e.g., Aries, 1996; Dindia and Canary, 2006; James and Clarke, 1993; Tannen, 1993) rightly observe that differences in linguistic interaction sometimes have more to do with

⇑ Corresponding author. E-mail address: [email protected] (N.S. Baron).

0388-0001/$ - see front matter Ó 2011 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. doi:10.1016/j.langsci.2011.06.018 14 N.S. Baron, E.M. Campbell / Language Sciences 34 (2012) 13–27 the relationship between interlocutors (including how long they have known one another and their relative position of sta- tus and power) than with gender. In recent years, the field of language and gender research has developed in new post-struc- turalist directions that focus on discourse-levels of analysis and rethink the notion of gender itself (e.g., Bucholtz, 2003; Cameron and Kulick, 2003). Nonetheless, correlations observed using more traditional approaches to gender and language are too strong to ignore, even where findings are traceable to socialization and circumstance. Among the many linguistic domains we might consider, we will focus here on four: purpose of communication, politeness issues, contact management and volume of use. In some instances, previous studies of language and gender offer direct par- allels to issues involved in mobile telephony. In other cases, we use the existing literature as a conceptual springboard for suggesting areas of inquiry that are relevant to mobile phones.

1.1. Purpose of communication

The language and gender literature reports that while women frequently use language to facilitate social interaction, men more commonly employ language for conveying information. This finding has been documented for both face-to-face spoken language (e.g., Cameron, 1998; Coates, 1993; Eckert and McConnell-Ginet, 2003; Holmes, 1993; Romaine, 2003; Smith-Lovin and Robinson, 1992; Tannen, 1994) and written communication (e.g., Argamon et al., 2003; Biber, 1988; Biber et al., 1998; Mulac and Lundell, 1994; Palander-Collin, 1999). Studies of online communication reveal similar trends. Research in the US and the UK indicates that statistically, the con- tent of female email and (IM) is more social in nature, while male messages more often convey informa- tion (Boneva et al., 2001; Colley and Todd, 2002; Colley et al., 2004; Fox et al., 2007; Lee, 2003). Herring’s studies of US college students (2003) found that in online mixed-gender discussions, females showed more of an ‘aligned’ and supportive orientation towards their interlocutors than did males. In their analysis of blogs, Argamon et al. (2007) reported that male bloggers used more words referring to topics such as politics and business, while female topics commonly involved inter- personal conversation or relationships. A gender dichotomy is also evident in text messages sent on mobile phones. Drawing data from adolescent and/or college student populations, studies of mobile phone use in Japan (Igarashi et al., 2005; Okuyama, 2009; Schiano et al., 2007), Korea (Yoon, 2003), Hong Kong (Lin, 2005) and Taiwan (Wei and Lo, 2006) indicate that females are more likely to use phones for social purposes, while males more commonly engage in information-seeking or planning. In the US, Lenhart et al. (2010) re- port that while 59% of teenage girls age 12–17 text several times daily to ‘just say hello and chat’, only 42% of boys do so. Horstmanshof and Power (2005) found that the Australian males they studied tended to become disenchanted with texting because they were less willing (than females) to follow contemporary social texting conventions, such as immediately responding to messages or sending ‘good night’ messages to significant others. Yates (2006) reported that female messages in his corpus expressed more ‘support’ and ‘affection’ than did male messages. Gender differences also exist in the ways IM and text messages are structured. Baron (2004) reported that IM conversa- tional closings between female college students (FF) took twice as long (both in number of turns and time on the clock) as closings between males (MM). Similarly, in comparing FF and MM IM conversations, Lee (2003) found that female subjects used explicit openings and closing about 80% of the time, compared with males – who used them in less than 30% of messages. Females are generally more likely than males to use emoticons (or their equivalent, e.g., Japanese kaomoji, emoji or de-mo – Okuyama, 2009) and exclamation marks, both in computer-based communication (e.g., Baron, 2004; Colley and Todd, 2002; Colley et al., 2004; Herring, 2003; Lee, 2003; Waseleski, 2006; Witmer and Katzman, 1997) and in (e.g., Baron and Ling, in press; Miyake, 2010; Scott et al., 2009).

1.2. Politeness issues

Our second focus is on politeness. The language and gender literature includes discussion of such issues as paying com- pliments, interruptions, and use of profanity, off-color humor or insults. In studies of face-to-face speech, email, IM and tex- ting, males are generally found to be less polite than females by one or more of these measures (e.g., Brown and Levinson, 1987; Colley et al., 2004; Herring, 2003; Holmes, 1988, 1991, 1995; Lee, 2003; Selnow, 1985; Smith-Lovin and Robinson, 1992; Tannen, 1994; Yates, 2006; Yates et al., 2005). While the present study did not address any of the above specific issues, we used previous findings as conceptual grounding for creating measures of politeness more directly relevant to our data.

1.3. Contact management

A third issue is what we called contact management: How do interlocutors choreograph their interactions with others? Traditional forms of management range from dominating the conversational floor to ignoring an interlocutor or interrupting (also a politeness issue). Some of these behaviors are customarily associated with males (e.g., in face-to-face speech: Holmes, 1993; on listservs or newsgroups: Herring, 2010). However, the notion of contact management also encompasses other types of interaction between interlocutors, such as users delaying responding to a letter or choosing a written reply in lieu of a face-to-face meeting. New media offer further venues for contact management. Users of ICTs can choose not to reply (or delay replying) to mes- sages (though failure to respond promptly sometimes bears social consequences). Multitasking behavior (e.g., engaging in N.S. Baron, E.M. Campbell / Language Sciences 34 (2012) 13–27 15 several IM or texting conversations simultaneously) is yet another form of contact management. Our own study examined choice of medium (voice or text) for communicating on a mobile phone and use of mobile phones to avoid face-to-face com- munication with potential interlocutors.

1.4. Volume of use

Our last consideration is volume of communication. The literature suggests multiple factors may be at issue here. Coates (1993), Hearn (1992) and Mulac (1989) report men speaking more than women in mixed-gender and/or face-to-face con- texts, though James and Drakich (1993) argue that such results reflect unequal power relationships along with differential socialization. A French study of telephony (based on billing records and personal interviews) indicates that female subjects initiated more calls than males and spoke for nearly twice as long each month (Smoreda and Licoppe, 2000). ICT research suggests that volume of online communication also correlates with gender. In online conferences and dis- cussion forums, males have been shown to post longer and more frequent contributions, arguably in keeping with an un- equal power relationship (Herring, 2003; Self and Meyer, 1991). However, in one-to-one contexts such as email and IM, females generate greater volume (Baron, 2004; Colley et al., 2004), arguably reflecting the social function of communication. Mobile phone studies indicate that females send more and/or longer texts, or are more likely to use texting, than males. These findings are robust across national contexts (e.g., Australia: Littlefield, 2004; Finland: Oksman and Turtiainen, 2004; Hong Kong: Lin, 2005; Italy: Herring and Zelenkauskaite, 2008; Japan: Boase and Kobayashi, 2008; Miyake, 2007; Okuyama, 2009; Schiano et al., 2007; Scott et al., 2009; Norway: Ling, 2005; UK: Yates, 2006; US: Lenhart et al., 2010).

2. Cross-cultural/cross national study of ICTs

While gender is one relevant variable in considering patterns of mobile phone usage, culture is another. Culture can be viewed from multiple vantage points: surface manifestations (such as which hand you hold a fork in), behavioral patterns reflecting belief systems (e.g., how people bury their dead) or effects of culture on the way we think (e.g., Hall, 1976; Nisbett, 2003). Our interest here is to see if particular sorts of mobile phone use (or attitudes towards mobiles) correspond with dif- ferences in social praxis, thereby making culture a potential explanation of certain mobile usage patterns. Cross-cultural studies are, of course, fraught with challenges (Baron, 2010; Haddon, 2005; Livingstone, 2003; Thomas et al., 2005). While the motivation for doing cross-national studies is commonly to look for cultural differences, a single na- tion-state may have multiple cultures (e.g., northern versus southern Italy). Similarly, a culture may extend across national boundaries (e.g., the Sami, who live in Sweden, Norway, Finland and Russia). Other challenges are methodological: Subjects in one setting, for example, might be unfamiliar with doing online questionnaires. A further issue in doing cross-cultural analysis is finding objective depictions of individual cultures and measures of cross-cultural differences. Cultural profiles of individual groups (e.g., Daun, 1996) acknowledge the sensitivities involved in attempting to portray a national character. Hofstede’s work (e.g., 1980) comparing dimensions of culture (e.g., masculinity versus femininity, small versus large power distance) was an important step towards objective analysis, but is not without critics (e.g., McSweeney, 2002). When undertaking cross-national (and derivatively, cross-cultural) studies, it was rarely practical (or common practice) to create wide-ranging cultural profiles for each country in which data are collected. In the research presented below, the authors drew upon both relevant literature and discussions with social scientists in the countries studied to explore possible cultural dimensions of selected findings in the data. Cross-national comparisons involving ICTs bring additional challenges as well. The amount of experience a society has had with a particular technology may help explain differences in user attitudes towards that technology. Similarly, variation in cost (and income) may be the actual source of what, on the surface, might appear to be cultural distinctions.

3. Rationale for present study

New technologies potentially alter relationships between interlocutors in a conversation (Baym, 2010). When online media first became available for organizational and personal communication, it was hypothesized that interaction would become more democratic, since neither gender nor social status was necessarily revealed in messaging (e.g., Sproull and Kiesler, 1986). However, as Herring’s research has shown, gender differences persist in at least some types of online communication. Nearly all the gender-related studies to date regarding mobile phones were conducted in single countries, making it dif- ficult to compare results cross-nationally, since research methodologies varied. Moreover, most existing mobile phone re- search has focused on empirical usage issues rather than inquiring what users themselves say about the device. The present study addresses these lacuna by using a single data-collection method across multiple countries and by focusing on users’ attitudes towards mobile phones. There is already some literature on user attitudes regarding mobile tele- phony. For example, Love and Kewley (2005) studied whether personality affected subjects’ attitudes regarding the physical distance between themselves and another person using a mobile phone in public space. Lenhart et al. (2010) used a ques- tionnaire and focus groups to measure a variety of attitudes of American teenagers towards their phones (e.g., whether they 16 N.S. Baron, E.M. Campbell / Language Sciences 34 (2012) 13–27 were irritated when a call or text interrupted them). The present study centers on young adults and inquires about a broad range of attitudinal issues. We have structured our discussion here in terms of gender, analyzing the data both cumulatively and with respect to indi- vidual countries. At relevant junctures, we move from nation to culture, considering the role that particular cultural features of a country may play in explaining our findings.

4. Research hypotheses (RH) and research question (RQ)

We formulated four research hypotheses and one research question. Because our interest is primarily in user attitudes towards mobile phone use, a number of our measures of communication, politeness, contact management and volume of use do not directly correspond to those in the research we have summarized. However, in interpreting our findings, we will consider points at which our conclusions parallel or complement those in the prior literature.

4.1. Purpose of communication

The literature on speech, writing and ICTs suggests that females are more likely than males to use language for social interaction. The first research hypothesis was therefore

RH1. Female mobile phone users will evidence a stronger social orientation towards using their phones than will males.

4.2. Politeness issues

The gender literature suggests females are more linguistically polite than males. Our second research hypothesis was therefore

RH2. Female mobile phone users will evidence stronger concerns about politeness in mobile phone use than will males.

4.3. Contact management

Mobile phones offer novel ways of managing linguistic interaction, including choosing to talk or to text, or pretending to talk to avoid conversation with an acquaintance or a stranger. Lacking prior data on language use in these contexts, we for- mulated a research question (rather than a research hypothesis) regarding contact management:

RQ1. Are there gender differences in the ways males and females use mobile phones to manage contact?

4.4. Volume of use; dependency and reachability

The literature indicates that females (at least in France) have more frequent and longer landline conversations than males and that females are heavier users of text messaging than males. We therefore formulated our third research hypothesis:

RH3. Female mobile phone users will report a higher volume of both voice calls and text messages than males.

A high volume of communication via any medium may have consequences. Mobile phones have increasingly become ‘al- ways on’ technologies (Baron, 2008), through which we can reach others – and they can reach us. We refer to this issue (especially others being able to reach us) as ‘reachability’. Users may come to feel entrapped because they always need to be available to potential interlocutors. Moreover, they may become dependent upon the device and therefore at a loss if it is mislaid or does not function properly. If RH3 (that females will have a higher volume of phone usage) proves correct, we hypothesize that females will feel more dependent upon their phones than males, and will complain more about being reachable. Our fourth research hypothesis was therefore

RH4. Volume of communication will correlate with feelings of dependency on the mobile phone and complaints about reach- ability, and females will complain about dependency and reachability more than males.

5. Methodology

5.1. Research design

Data were collected using a convenience sample of 18–24 year-old university students in Sweden, the US, Italy, Japan and Korea between October 2007 and December 2008. Subjects were recruited through advertisements in student newspapers, campus posters, email distribution lists, class visits and invitations on course websites. Recruitment resulted in a preponder- N.S. Baron, E.M. Campbell / Language Sciences 34 (2012) 13–27 17 ance of female respondents, though there were a sufficient number of males in each country to permit statistical analysis. Participants were directed to an online survey mounted on the professional version of SurveyMonkey, an internet-based sur- vey tool. In each country, data were collected at universities in two cities. In each city, students were invited to participate in a drawing for a gift certificate to an online bookstore. The English questionnaire was translated into Swedish, Italian, Japanese and Korean by fluent bilinguals. The survey, which took approximately 10–12 min to complete, included quantitative and scalar questions, open-ended questions and a word association question. Verbal responses were translated into English by fluent bilinguals.

5.2. Subjects

Table 1 shows the gender and age distribution, by country, for the study participants. There were a total of 2001 subjects who completed the survey, of which 524 were male and 1477 were female. Mean age was 20.7 years.

5.3. Survey questions

RH1. Purpose of communication. One set of data for analyzing the purpose of communication was derived from the word asso- ciation question (‘What are the first three words you think of when you think of mobile phones?’). A second set of data was obtained by asking the open-ended question ‘What is the one thing you like most about having a mobile phone?’. RH2. Politeness issues. Data for analyzing politeness came from three sources. First, subjects were asked to judge the accept- ability of talking or texting on mobile phones while eating dinner at home with their family, sitting with people they knew in an informal café, paying at the cash register at a convenience store, walking in public, or riding a local bus, tram or subway. For each scenario, subjects could select ‘always’, ‘usually’, ‘occasionally’ or ‘never’.

The second data source was the word association question (‘first three words’). The last source was the question ‘What is the one thing you like least about having a mobile phone?’.

RQ1. Contact management. To gauge management of social interaction, we used two sets of questions. First, subjects evalu- ated how important it was (‘very important’, ‘somewhat important’, ‘not very important’ or ‘not important at all’) to talk or text for a particular reason:

choosing to call rather than sending a text message

 ‘I want to hear the voice of the person I’m communicating with.’

choosing to send a text message rather than calling

 ‘I want to make my message short, and talking takes too long.’  ‘Some communication is better done by sending a text message than talking.’

Another way of managing contact is pretending to use the phone (or playing with its functions – e.g., checking old text messages or playing games) to avoid potential conversation with an acquaintance or stranger. Subjects indicated how often (‘at least once a week’, ‘about once a month’, ‘occasionally’ or ‘never’) they

pretended to be talking on their mobile phone

 to avoid talking with someone they knew  to avoid having a stranger talk with them

played with other functions on their mobile phones

 to avoid talking with someone they knew  to avoid having a stranger talk with them

Table 1 Gender and age distribution of subjects, by country.

Sweden US Italy Japan Korea Total Gender Male 38.6% (N = 66) 26.8% (N = 140) 18.0% (N = 111) 29.1% (N = 154) 32.7% (N = 53) 524 Female 61.4% (N = 105) 73.2% (N = 383) 82.0% (N = 505) 70.9% (N = 375) 67.3% (N = 109) 1477 Total 100% (N = 171) 100% (N = 523) 100% (N = 616) 100% (N = 529) 100% (N = 162) 2001 Mean age (years) 21.5 19.8 21.3 19.8 21.3 20.7 18 N.S. Baron, E.M. Campbell / Language Sciences 34 (2012) 13–27

During pilot testing for the present study, American students of the first author suggested including questions regarding pretending to talk on mobile phones, reflecting their personal experience. When the present survey was being finalized, Swedish students of the first author indicated that while they did not pretend to talk on their mobiles, they used other device functions to avoid communication – and hence inclusion of the latter questions.

RH3 and RH4. Volume of use; dependency and reachability. To ascertain volume of mobile phone use, subjects were asked

 ‘Yesterday, what was the combined number of voice calls you made and received on your mobile phone [includ- ing voicemails]?’  ‘Yesterday, what was the combined number of text messages you sent and received on your mobile phone?’

Respondents were asked to select from a range of intervals (e.g., 1–2, 11–15, 21–30). Data regarding feelings of depen- dency, along with complaints about reachability, were drawn from the open-ended question ‘What is the one thing you like least about having a mobile phone?’.

5.4. Methodology for data analysis

5.4.1. Quantitative and scalar questions For the volume of use questions, some subcategories (e.g., 0, 1–2 and 3–4) were collapsed for purpose of analysis. All sca- lar questions used four-point Likert scales. Again, for purposes of analysis, some points on the scales were collapsed. Signif- icance levels for all scalar analyses were determined using the Two-Sample Test for Proportions.

5.4.2. Word association and open-ended questions For the word association task (‘first three words’), a coding scheme was developed on the basis of all codable responses. Major categories included Technology Issues, Physical Attributes and Functions, Communication Issues, Evaluation Issues, Cost Issues and Safety Issues. There were also many subcategories. (See Baron, 2011 regarding details on coding of the word association data.) For the open-ended questions (regarding what subjects liked most and liked least about having a mobile phone), a separate coding scheme was devised on the basis of all codable data. Major categories included Physical Attributes and Functions, Communication Issues, Evaluation Issues, Cost Issues, Safety Issues and No Comment. Again, there were many subcategories. Both coding systems evolved over an 18-month process, with refinements being made as additional responses were translated. Formal coding was done independently by two researchers, with differences resolved through consultation. Dur- ing the coding process, fluent bilinguals were often consulted to clarify the import of original responses for which proper coding was initially unclear. Because subjects were free to write whatever they wished, some ‘like most’ or ‘like least’ responses were found to address more than one issue, and therefore might be appropriate for multiple coding categories. For cases in which two categories were conjoined (e.g., ‘convenience and security’), the first category mentioned was chosen. In other instances, two concepts were intertwined. Consider one Swedish subject’s response to the ‘like least’ question: ‘I get a little stressed when it calls all the time. I feel forced to answer because the number gets shown as a missed call.’

This reply might be coded either as Dependency/Stress (a subcategory of Evaluation) or as Obligation to be Responsive (a subcategory under Communication). Coding decisions were made on the basis of what appeared to be the principle idea (here, the obligation to respond), even if the secondary idea (stress) appeared first in the sentence. Percentages calculated for the word association and open-ended questions reflect the ratio of coded responses within a category (e.g., word association responses from males relating to communication) to total number of codable responses (in this instance, all male word association responses). Significance levels for both the word association task and the open-ended questions were calculated using the Two-Sample Test for Proportions.

6. Results

6.1. RH1: purpose of communication

6.1.1. First three words Responses to the word association question (‘first three words’) yielded many replies involving communication. Subdivi- sions of this category included general terms (e.g., communicate), along with words relating to speech or writing (e.g., calling, text message), social community (e.g., parents, friends, boyfriend), etiquette (e.g., manners) or emotions (e.g., quarrels). Results are shown in Table 2. Overall, 46.9% of words provided by females related to communication, compared with 40.4% by males (p < .01). Con- sidering just responses involving social community, female responses were also higher (females: 5.9%; males: 4.6% – p < .03). N.S. Baron, E.M. Campbell / Language Sciences 34 (2012) 13–27 19

Table 2 Percent of ‘first three words’ responses involving communication.

Male Female Total words relating to communication 40.4% (N = 629) 46.9% (N = 2068) Words relating specifically to social community 4.6% (N = 71) 5.9% (N = 262)

Table 3 Percent of ‘like most’ responses involving communication.

Male Female Total comments relating to communication 57.9% (N = 303) 59.9% (N = 881) Comments relating specifically to social community 5.2% (N = 27) 8.5% (N = 125)

6.1.2. Like most Table 3 summarizes ‘like most’ responses relating to communication. There was no significant difference between genders for general responses involving communication (males: 57.9%; fe- males: 59.9%). However, as with the ‘first three words’ results, more females specifically mentioned social community (fe- males: 8.5%; males: 5.2% – p < .01). Examples of ‘like most’ responses involving social community included ‘talk to family’ and ‘planning social events’.

6.2. RH2: politeness issues

6.2.1. Acceptability of using phone in others’ presence Table 4 summarizes responses indicating it was ‘always’ or ‘usually’ acceptable to talk on mobile phones while in a variety of social contexts. In all personal situations (i.e., at dinner at home, in a café, at a cash register), males (collectively) were more likely than females to find talking on their mobiles acceptable (p < .01). This generalization held true for Swedish, American, Italian and Japanese subjects. In Korea, there was no gender difference. However, Korean acceptability rates were by far the highest (e.g., while 41% of Koreans found it ‘always’ or ‘usually’ acceptable to talk on phones during family dinner, 14% of Swedes – and only 3% of Italians – judged the behavior acceptable). Talking on mobiles in less personal contexts (i.e., walking in public, riding local transportation) generally revealed more balanced acceptability levels across genders. In Korea, however, more females (though not significantly) approved of talking on phones while riding public transportation (females: 67.9%; males: 56.6%). Finally, a cultural comment is in order about Japan, where passengers are reminded with prominent signs not to talk on a mobile phone while riding public transport (Okabe and Ito, 2005). In our data, overall Japanese acceptability for this behavior was only 4%. Removing Japanese data from the summation in Table 4 for riding a local bus, tram, or subway, acceptability levels for both genders rise about 20%. Table 5 summarizes responses from subjects who indicated it was ‘always’ or ‘usually’ acceptable to text on their mobile phones while in a variety of social contexts. As with talking on the mobile while face-to-face with others, males were more likely than females to find it acceptable to text when eating dinner at home, at a café, or at a convenience store cash register (p < .01).

Table 4 Percent acceptability of talking on mobile phone while in various social contexts.

Male Female Eating dinner at home with family 17.0% (N = 89) 7.4% (N = 109) Sitting with people you know in informal café 35.7% (N = 187) 21.4% (N = 316) Paying at cash register at convenience store 39.3% (N = 206) 28.0% (N = 414) Walking in public 88.5% (N = 464) 87.6% (N = 1294) Riding a local bus, tram or subway 55.7% (N = 292) 54.1% (N = 799)

Table 5 Percent acceptability of texting on mobile phone while in various social contexts.

Male Female Eating dinner at home with family 31.3% (N = 164) 23.0% (N = 340) Sitting with people you know in informal café 56.9% (N = 298) 44.4% (N = 656) Paying at cash register at convenience store 42.0% (N = 220) 35.0% (N = 517) Walking in public 84.2% (N = 441) 83.5% (N = 1234) Riding a local bus, tram or subway 90.8% (N = 476) 92.2% (N = 1362) 20 N.S. Baron, E.M. Campbell / Language Sciences 34 (2012) 13–27

Texting on a mobile while walking was equally acceptable to both genders in our samples from Sweden, the US, Italy and Japan, while in Korea, females were more likely to find the practice acceptable (females: 94.5%; males: 83.0% – p < .01). For texting while riding public transportation, genders were largely matched, except in Korea, where females were somewhat more likely to accept the behavior (females: 98.2%; males: 90.6% – p < .05).

6.2.2. First three words Politeness issues surfaced – slightly – in the word association task. Thirteen words (out of 5969 codable answers) explic- itly concerned ‘bad manners’. All 13 came from Japanese subjects, and they were proportionally balanced between males and females.

6.2.3. Like least Politeness concerns also appeared in responses to the ‘like least’ question. A cluster of responses referred to disruptions of ‘social order’, which took place through violations of etiquette (e.g., ‘it’s too noisy’; ‘when people talk on a cell phone when I am there’) or disturbance from the phone’s ringer (e.g., ‘it’s constantly ringing’). These results are shown in Table 6. There was little overall gender difference in the frequency with which disruption of the social order was mentioned (males: 6.0%; females: 5.8%), though gender difference was significant in the US (females: 8.6%; males: 3.6% – p < .03). More Americans (7.3%) and Japanese (8.6%) expressed concern about disruption of social order than subjects elsewhere.

6.3. RQ3: contact management

6.3.1. Choosing to talk or text Table 7 summarizes responses of ‘very important’ or ‘somewhat important’ to questions about the importance of calling (‘to hear the voice of the person I’m communicating with’) or texting (‘to make my message short [since] talking takes too long’ or because ‘some communication is better done by sending a text’). Females in the study were more likely to find it important to call to hear the other person’s voice (females: 77.5%; males: 69.1% – p < .01). Females were also more likely to judge texting important because talking takes too long (females: 64.0%; males: 57.3% – p < .01). By contrast, more males reported it was important to text because some communication was better done by texting (males: 57.4%; females: 51.0% – p < .01).

6.3.2. Pretending to talk/playing with phone to avoid conversation Table 8 summarizes the responses for pretending to talk at least once a month (i.e., ‘at least once a week’ plus ‘about once a month’) and at least occasionally (i.e., ‘at least once a week’ plus ‘about once a month’ plus ‘occasionally’). Males in the sample were somewhat more likely than females to report pretending to talk on their phones at least once a month to avoid conversation with an acquaintance (males: 8.6%; females: 6.1% – p < .05). Results were more similar across genders for avoiding conversation with a stranger at least once a month (males: 8.2%; females: 9.1%). Avoiding both acquaintances and strangers by pretending to talk was most prevalent in the US. Fifteen percent of Amer- ican males in the study and 11.7% of females reported pretending at least once a month to be talking on their phones to avoid people they knew – compared with 3.0% of Swedish males or 1.4% of Italian females. To avoid conversation with a stranger, 14.3% of American males and 15.4% of American females pretended to be talking (again, at least once a month), compared with only 1.7% of Swedish males and 3.5% of Swedish females. Looking at the broader response set of ‘at least occasionally’, genders were matched regarding avoiding acquaintances (males: 30.7%; females: 32.6%). However, females were more likely to pretend to talk to avoid strangers (females: 47.0%; males: 27.5% – p < .01).

Table 6 Percent ‘like least’ responses involving disruption of social order, by country.

Sweden US Italy Japan Korea Total Male 3.0% (N = 2) 3.6% (N = 5) 5.5% (N = 6) 11.7% (N = 18) 0.0% (N = 0) 6.0% (N = 31) Female 1.9% (N = 2) 8.6% (N = 33) 4.2% (N = 21) 7.3% (N = 27) 1.9% (N = 2) 5.8% (N = 85) Total 2.3% (N = 4) 7.3% (N = 38) 4.5% (N = 27) 8.6% (N = 45) 1.3% (N = 2) 5.8% (N = 116)

Table 7 Percent subjects judging it important to choose talk or text.

Male Female

Call to hear person’s voice 69.1% (N = 362) 77.5% (N = 1145) Text to keep message short 57.3% (N = 300) 64.0% (N = 946) Text is better for some communication 57.4% (N = 301) 51.0% (N = 753) N.S. Baron, E.M. Campbell / Language Sciences 34 (2012) 13–27 21

Table 8 Percent pretending to talk to avoid conversation with acquaintance or stranger.

Acquaintance Stranger Male Female Male Female At least once a month 8.6% (N = 44) 6.1% (N = 90) 8.2% (N = 43) 9.1% (N = 135) At least occasionally 30.7% (N = 161) 32.6% (N = 481) 27.5% (N = 144) 47.0% (N = 694)

Table 9 Percent playing with other functions to avoid conversation with acquaintance or stranger.

Acquaintance Stranger Male Female Male Female At least once a month 15.8% (N = 83) 18.3% (N = 271) 15.1% (N = 79) 22.9% (N = 338) At least occasionally 48.9% (N = 256) 60.7% (N = 897) 45.2% (N = 237) 70.9% (N = 1047)

Table 9 summarizes responses regarding playing with other functions on the phone to avoid conversation with an acquaintance or a stranger. Playing with phone functions at least once a month to avoid conversation – with either acquaintances or strangers – was roughly twice as common as pretending to talk for the same reason. Slightly (though not significantly) more females used other phone functions to avoid conversation with acquaintances (females: 18.3%; males: 15.8%), while the difference in use of this strategy to avoid strangers was wider (females: 22.9%; males: 15.1% – p < .01). Gender distinctions (for both conditions) increased when ‘occasionally’ data were included. For avoiding acquaintances, the results were females: 60.7%; males: 48.9% – p < .01. For avoiding strangers, the results were females: 70.9%; males: 45.2% – p < .01. There were fewer distinctions between countries for avoiding conversations by playing with other phone functions than by pretending to talk. Italians were least likely to avoid conversations by playing with other phone functions, with Italian females more likely than Italian males to use these functions to avoid having a stranger speak with them.

6.4. RH3 and RH4: volume of use; dependency and reachability

6.4.1. Volume of use Table 10 presents findings for volume of voice calls made and received. In each country, at least 80% of subjects (both male and female) made or received 610 voice calls per day, with essentially no difference between male and female totals across countries. Looking at subjects who engaged in >20 voice calls per day

Table 10 Frequency of mobile phone use reported for talking (‘yesterday’).a

Sweden N = 171 US N = 523 Italy N = 616 Japan N = 529 Korea N = 162 Total N = 2001 0–4 Male 60.6 (40) 52.9 (74) 81.1 (90) 86.4 (133) 41.5 (22) 68.5 (359) Female 62.9 (66) 47.0 (180) 78.2 (395) 85.1 (319) 51.4 (56) 68.8 (1016) 5–10 Male 33.3 (22) 33.6 (47) 17.1 (19) 10.4 (16) 45.3 (24) 24.4 (128) Female 30.5 (32) 40.2 (154) 19.4 (98) 13.6 (51) 29.4 (32) 24.8 (367) 11–20 Male 6.1 (4) 10.7 (15) 0.0 (0) 1.3 (2) 5.7 (3) 4.6 (24) Female 5.7 (6) 10.4 (40) 2.0 (10) 1.3 (5) 14.7 (16) 5.2 (77) 21–30 Male 0.0 (0) 2.1 (3) 0.9 (1) 0 (0) 3.8 (2) 1.1 (6) Female 0.9 (1) 1.0 (4) 0.4 (2) 0 (0) 3.7 (4) 0.7 (11) >30 Male 0 (0) 0.7 (1) 0.9 (1) 2.0 (3) 3.8 (2) 1.3 (7) Female 0 (0) 1.3 (5) 0 (0) 0 (0) 0.9 (1) 0.4 (6)

a Not all columns (by gender) sum to 100% due to rounding. 22 N.S. Baron, E.M. Campbell / Language Sciences 34 (2012) 13–27

Table 11 Frequency of mobile phone use reported for texting (‘yesterday’).a

Sweden N = 171 US N = 523 Italy N = 616 Japan N = 529 Korea N = 162 Total N = 2001 0–4 Male 66.7 (44) 45.0 (63) 36.9 (41) 22.7 (35) 5.7 (3) 35.5 (186) Female 49.5 (52) 38.6 (148) 32.5 (164) 16.5 (62) 2.7 (3) 29.1 (429) 5–10 Male 22.7 (15) 26.4 (37) 27.0 (30) 30.5 (47) 22.6 (12) 26.9 (141) Female 36.2 (38) 26.9 (103) 26.5 (134) 29.1 (109) 11.9 (13) 26.9 (397) 11–20 Male 7.6 (5) 10.7 (15) 13.5 (15) 26.0 (40) 30.2 (16) 17.4 (91) Female 11.4 (12) 16.7 (64) 17.0 (86) 25.1 (94) 24.8 (27) 19.2 (283) 21–30 Male 3.0 (2) 7.9 (11) 8.1 (9) 7.8 (12) 7.6 (4) 7.3 (38) Female 0.9 (1) 6.0 (23) 6.5 (33) 12.0 (45) 11.9 (13) 7.8 (115) >30 Male 0.0 (0) 10.0 (14) 14.4 (16) 13.0 (20) 34.0 (68) 13.0 (68) Female 1.9 (2) 11.7 (45) 17.4 (88) 17.3 (65) 48.6 (53) 17.1 (253)

a Not all columns (by gender) sum to 100% due to rounding.

(i.e., 21–30 plus >30), there was some difference between genders (males: 2.5%; females: 1.2% – p < .02), though the sample size is small. Again looking at >20 calls per day, Korean subjects were the heaviest users of voice functions (5.6%), followed by Americans (2.5%). Looking just at subjects who engaged in >30 voice calls per day (‘heavy callers’), the overall percent drops to 0.6%, with Korea still having the highest volume (1.9%), followed by the US (1.2%). Table 11 presents findings for volume of text messages sent and received. All countries except Sweden reported higher levels of texting than talking. Looking at subjects who indicated sending or receiving >30 texts the previous day (‘heavy texters’), the overall percent was 16.0% (compared with 0.6% for voice calls). More females than males reported >30 texting events per day (females: 17.1%; males: 13.0% – p < .02). Each country sample had a greater proportion of female heavy texters, though gender difference was only significant in Korea (p < .04).

6.4.2. Dependency Among the ‘like least’ responses were answers relating to dependency, e.g., ‘addicted’, ‘can’t resist the urge not to check it’, ‘feel restricted’, ‘feel insecure without it’ and ‘that it becomes an obsession’. Table 12 reports these results. Slightly (though not significantly) more females complained about dependency (females: 11.1%; males: 8.7%; – p < .06). Korean subjects voiced the most concern (30.2%). In every country, females complained more than males, though only sig- nificantly in Japan (p < .02).

6.4.3. Reachability ‘Like least’ responses involving reachability included specific complaints about other people reaching them (e.g., ‘people being able to track me down’, ‘just don’t want to be contacted’), comments in which the directionality of communication was not specified (e.g., ‘can’t be out of touch’, ‘I have to talk to people’), and, in one case, a complaint being able to reach other people (‘I have a hard time not calling the people I probably shouldn’t call’). Table 13 summarizes the reachability data, by gender. Females complained more often (though not significantly) about other people reaching them (females 34.9%; males: 31.0% – p < .06) and significantly more about reachability in general (females: 52.8%; males: 47.3% – p < .02). In Sweden, the US and Italy, between 52% (Italy) and 57% (Sweden and the US) of subjects mentioned reachability as what they ‘liked least’. In Japan, only 37% of subjects gave this response, while in Korea, 73% of subjects complained about reachability.

Table 12 Percent ‘like least’ responses relating to dependency.

Male Female Total Sweden 4.5% (N = 3) 8.6% (N = 9) 7.0% (N = 12) US 7.1% (N = 10) 7.3% (N = 28) 7.3% (N = 38) Italy 9.2% (N = 10) 9.9% (N = 49) 9.7% (N = 59) Japan 5.2% (N = 8) 11.6% (N = 43) 9.7% (N = 51) Korea 27.5% (N = 14) 31.5% (N = 34) 30.2% (N = 48) Total 8.7% (N = 45) 11.1% (N = 163) 10.5% (N = 208) N.S. Baron, E.M. Campbell / Language Sciences 34 (2012) 13–27 23

Table 13 Percent ‘like least’ responses relating to reachability.

Male Female Total Others reach me 31.0% (N = 161) 34.9% (N = 510) 33.8% (N = 671) Direction not specified 16.3% (N = 85) 17.9% (N = 262) 17.5% (N = 347) I reach others 0.0% (N = 0) 0.1% (N = 1) 0.0% (N =1) Total 47.3% (N = 246) 52.8% (N = 773) 51.4% (N = 1019)

7. Discussion and conclusions

This study has probed the extent to which gender plays a role in shaping mobile phone usage patterns and attitudes of university-aged students. We drew upon the existing literature on gender and language to identify general issues to explore (including purpose of communication, politeness, contact management and volume of use), although our specific research questions reflected particular affordances of mobile telephony (e.g., ability to pretend to talk on a mobile phone to avoid con- versation with acquaintances). By collecting cross-national data, we were able to begin exploring whether culture rather than gender was the more relevant variable in explaining certain findings. Like many empirical studies, this one had its methodological shortcomings. Before discussing our results, we consider some of these limitations.

7.1. Study limitations

The major methodological challenge was construction of the sample. Timing and resource constraints necessitated using convenience sampling rather than random sampling. One consequence was that the final subject pool had three times as many females as males. This response rate to solicitation for participation reflects a variety of factors, including gender balance in the particular university courses from which many of the subjects were recruited, gender balance in the universities themselves, and an apparent tendency for females to be more likely than males to volunteer as subjects in studies. Secondly, self-reporting of behaviors (or even attitudes) can be problematic. Consider self-reporting regarding volume of mobile phone use. Without records from carriers, we cannot ascertain if our subjects’ estimates were accurate. (Obtaining actual records for this study was not feasible.) Similarly, consider subjects’ judgments of the acceptabil- ity of a behavior such as talking on a mobile phone while eating dinner with their family. It is difficult to know the extent to which responses reflected actual personal opinion as opposed to mirroring normative expectations. In future research, obser- vational studies might help resolve this issue. A third limitation of the study is that university students in five countries are hardly representative of all mobile phone users. Future research should look at different age cohorts, socioeconomic classes and educational levels, as well as include data from other parts of the world. It would also be instructive to see if characteristics of dyadic pairs influence attitudes towards mobile telephony (e.g., children getting texts from parents versus peers, students communicating with friends as opposed to significant others; same-sex versus mixed-sex interlocutors; gay or lesbian versus heterosexual interlocutors). Another issue is financial. The cost of sending (or receiving) voice calls or text messages might affect users’ volume of usage. In the fuller study of which the present analysis is part, focus groups were conducted in all countries but Korea. When asked to estimate their monthly phone charges, participants were generally unable to do so. Moreover, data from other ques- tions on the survey revealed considerable variation regarding who paid the phone bill: In Sweden, almost 90% reported pay- ing the entire cost themselves; in Italy, 50%; in the US, Japan and Korea, only 15%. (The remaining costs were usually paid by parents.) Furthermore, each country had a wide range of pricing plans, making voice calls inexpensive on one plan but costly on another. In the end, it proved impossible to draw meaningful conclusions regarding the possible impact of cost upon vol- ume of mobile phone use or attitudes towards mobile telephony. Finally, there is the issue of when the data were collected. Usage patterns with digital technologies are moving targets. As the technologies improve, become more affordable, and become viewed as social necessities, volume of usage may grow exponentially. In the US, for example, text messaging was still in its relative infancy in the mid 2000s. Nielsenmobile (2008) reported that in the second quarter of 2006, the average US mobile subscriber sent or received 79 text messages a month. By the second quarter of 2008, that number has grown to 375. In the second quarter of 2008, 18–24 year-olds in the US who had mobile subscriptions were sending or receiving an average of 790 texts a month. By the second quarter of 2010, that number had reached 1630 (Nielsenmobile, 2010). Were we to administer our same survey in 2012, the data would likely have changed in several significant ways. First, usage levels would probably be much higher (at least for texting in the US). Second, given the proliferation of smart phones around the world, would be far more widespread and, presumably, commented upon by subjects everywhere it was in common use. More generally, we cannot assume that the data reported here would be reproduced in subsequent studies. However, we would predict that gender distinctions and some culturally-driven distinctions would likely maintain. 24 N.S. Baron, E.M. Campbell / Language Sciences 34 (2012) 13–27

With these caveats in mind, we turn to what we have learned regarding our research hypotheses and questions.

7.2. RH1: purpose of communication

Responses to both the ‘first three words’ and the ‘like most’ questions suggest that females in the study were more likely than males to use mobile phones to facilitate social interaction. This orientation was seen both in general responses relating to communication (‘first three words’) and in replies specifically concerning social community (‘first three words’ and ‘like most’). These findings are consonant with previous literature on gender differences in face-to-face speech, in writing, and in online and mobile communication, although our measures of social interaction differed from those used in previous studies.

7.3. RH2: politeness issues

While the literature suggests that females are more linguistically polite than males, none of the common measures of politeness (e.g., interruptions, profanity) was directly relevant to the present study. Our closest correlate was subjects’ judg- ments regarding the acceptability of using mobile phones in various social contexts. During personal face-to-face encounters (at dinner, in a café, at a cash register), males were significantly more likely than females to accept both talking and texting on mobiles. These findings suggest either that females are following stricter politeness norms or perhaps that females priv- ilege social interaction with physically present interlocutors over those reached by phone. There was considerable disparity across national samples. For example, 41.4% of Koreans (males: 45.3%; females: 39.4%) found it acceptable to talk on their mobiles during a family dinner, compared with only 3.1% of Italians (males: 8.1%; fe- males: 2.0%). Thus, while gender distinctions appear in Korea and Italy, cultural issues are relevant as well. Similarly, in the word association question, the only subjects offering terms dealing with ‘bad manners’ were from Japan – a culture known for its politeness conventions (Lebra, 1976; Wagatsuma and Rosett, 1986). Politeness judgments were less stringent when moving through public space (walking in public or riding local transpor- tation), where talking and texting were more acceptable to both genders. The only gender differences occurred in Korea, where females were more accepting of talking (while riding public transportation) and texting (while both walking and rid- ing public transport). Recall that Korean females were the heaviest texters.

7.4. RQ1: contact management

Our inquiries regarding contact management revealed several interesting results involving gender. More females than males chose talking (over texting) because they wanted to hear the voice of their interlocutor. This choice suggests the importance of social connection. Therefore, these data complement findings regarding RH1, i.e., that females are more likely than males to have a social orientation towards using mobile phones. More females than males reported choosing to text (rather than talk) because ‘talking takes too long’. In the focus groups, students in several countries noted how easy it is to become embroiled in a lengthy voice call. With texting, manage the interaction, circumventing potential obligation to hear the other person out. Deborah Tannen (2010) has suggested that in using this strategy, females also avoid being impolite (i.e., in making curt phone calls). Males in the study evidenced a different strategy for managing social interaction, being more likely than females to judge it important to text (rather than talk) because ‘some communication is better done by sending a text message than talking’. While desire for brevity might be one explanation, our finding that females, not males, were more likely to text because ‘talking takes too long’ suggests looking elsewhere. An alternative possibility is that at least some males felt awkward talking voice-to-voice about certain issues (e.g., breaking up with a girlfriend), making texting a more desirable option. The ‘pretending to talk’ strategy for avoiding acquaintances was particularly prevalent in the US, where males in the sam- ple were numerically more likely than females to use the strategy at least once a month. This male avoidance strategy is consonant with their choosing to text rather than talk to communicate about particular topics. The distinction between American and Italian behavior for avoiding acquaintances at least once a month by pretending to talk on their phones is stark: 12.6% of Americans in the sample versus 2.1% of Italians. The Italian pattern is consistent with the very small proportion of Italians who found it appropriate to talk on their phones at a family dinner (3.1%). These data support the notion of Italy as a culture that strongly values communication with friends or family members in one’s physical presence (Fortunati, 2009b). In the future, it would be useful to examine whether Italians privilege face-to-face social inter- action over use of mobile phones in contexts involving familiar strangers (Milgram, 1972), such as when transacting business in a local tabacchi or taking the traditional evening stroll (passeggiata) through the center of smaller towns. Similarly, re- search in the US might investigate whether pretending to talk on mobiles to avoid acquaintances correlates with other typ- ically American behavioral characteristics. Using the mobile phone to avoid strangers was generally a strategy employed more often by females than by males. Disparities between genders were particularly high for occasionally pretending to talk and for occasionally playing with other functions on the phone to avoid conversation. Given females’ greater physical vulnerability in potential encounters with strangers, these findings are not surprising. N.S. Baron, E.M. Campbell / Language Sciences 34 (2012) 13–27 25

7.5. RH3 and RH4: volume of use; dependency and reachability

Females in our study reported higher levels of texting than males, a finding consistent with previous literature on gender differences in volume of texting. The highest volume was by Koreans. A high volume of mobile telephony seems to take its toll. While 10.5% of all subjects ‘liked least’ something to do with dependency, the percentage was three times as high in Korea. For all countries combined, female subjects were slightly more troubled by dependency than males. Females were also more likely to complain about reachability, including that others could always reach them. Overall, 52.8% of females voiced this complaint as what they ‘liked least’ about mobile phones, compared with 74.1% of Korean females. Japanese subjects had a substantial proportion of high-volume texting (16.1% sent or received >30 texts per day). Yet un- like Koreans, the Japanese had relatively few complaints either about dependency (Korea: 30.2%; Japan: 9.7%) or about reach- ability (Korea: 73.0%; Japan: 36.8%). Explanation for the discrepancy may be cultural. In Japan, a positive notion of dependency (amae) is embedded in the social structure (Doi, 1971). Future research is needed to explore if low levels of Jap- anese complaints regarding mobile phone communication reflect a cultural expectation of being available to others.

7.6. Conclusions

New technologies enable people to alter existing patterns of communication. In their time, the telegraph and the (land- line) redefined assumptions about how social relationships should be conducted (Baron, 2000). More recently, it was initially assumed that the internet would equalize power relationships between genders (see Herring, 2003). Yet tech- nologies do not automatically redefine long-standing patterns of socialization. We have seen a number of ways in which tra- ditional gendered language usage is perpetuated on mobile phones, particularly with regard to the purpose of communication, politeness considerations in face-to-face interaction, and volume of texting. We also observed novel ways in which both genders take advantages of mobile phones’ affordances to manage social interaction with others. And we have seen the toll that heavy text messaging can take upon users. Gender is just one variable influencing how young adults use mobile phones. Sometimes culture may be the critical factor, as with American subjects avoiding strangers by pretending to talk on their phones, Italians not using phones while sitting at dinner with their families, or Japanese being least bothered by dependency or reachability. To confirm whether culture actu- ally is at play in each instance will require more research. The present research is a first step in probing the sources of commonality and difference in the ways male and female university students in several parts of the world use mobile phones. Given the centrality of mobile communication to con- temporary life, sociolinguists and new media scholars have much to contribute by pursuing questions of the sort raised in this study.

Acknowledgments

Support for this research was provided to the first author by the Swedish Fulbright Commission and by American Univer- sity. The following people provided invaluable assistance in designing and administering the survey, doing translations and analyzing results: Kumiko Akikawa, Sachiko Aoshima, Assen Assenov, Ann-Sofie Axelsson, Marina Bondi, Maria Bortoluzzi, Miriam Callahan, Ranieri Cavaceppi, colleagues at Ritsumeikan University, Jonathan Donner, Nicole Ellison, Leopoldina For- tunati, Solveig Granath, Ylva Hård af Segerstad, Kumi Iwasaki, Dong-Hoo Lee, Eunjeong Lee, Rich Ling, Misa Matsuda, Marieta Pehlivanova, students in the first author’s Autumn 2007 class ‘‘Language in an Online and Mobile World’’ at the University of Gothenburg, and Oscar Westlund. The authors are also grateful to anonymous reviews of an earlier version of this paper for their insightful suggestions.

References

Argamon, S., Koppel, M., Fine, J., Shimoni, A.R., 2003. Gender, genre, and writing style in formal written texts. Text 23, 321–346. Argamon, S., Koppel, M., Pennebaker, J.W., Schler, J., 2007. Mining the blogosphere: age, gender, and the varieties of self-expression. First Monday 12 (9). Aries, E., 1996. Men and Women in Interaction: Reconsidering the Differences. Oxford University Press, New York. Baron, N.S., 2000. Alphabet to Email: How Written English Evolved and Where It’s Heading. Routledge, London. Baron, N.S., 2004. ‘See you online’: Gender issues in college student use of instant messaging. Journal of Language and Social Psychology 23 (4), 397–423. Baron, N.S., 2008. Always On: Language in an Online and Mobile World. Oxford University Press, New York. Baron, N.S., 2010. Mobile phones in cross-cultural context: Sweden, Estonia, the USA and Japan. New Media & Society 12 (1), 3–11. Baron, N.S., 2011. Attitudes towards mobile phones: a cross-cultural comparison. In: Greif, H., Hjorth, L., Lasen, A., Lobet, C. (Eds.), Cultures of Participation. Peter Lang, Frankfurt, pp. 77–94. Baron, N.S., Hård af Segerstad, Y., 2010. Cross-cultural patterns in mobile-phone use: public space and reachability in Sweden, the USA and Japan. New Media & Society 12 (1), 13–34. Baron, N.S., Ling, R., in press. Necessary smileys and useless periods: redefining punctuation in electronically-mediated communication. Visible Language. Baym, N., 2010. Personal Connections in the Digital Age. Polity Press, Malden, MA. Bergvall, V., Bing, J., Freed, A. (Eds.), 1996. Rethinking Language and Gender Research. Longman, New York. Biber, D., 1988. Variation across Speech and Writing. Cambridge University Press, New York. Biber, D., Conrad, S., Reppen, R., 1998. Corpus Linguistics: Investigating Language Structure and Use. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge. Boase, J., Kobayashi, T., 2008. Kei-tying teens: using mobile phone e-mail to bond, bridge, and break with social ties – a study of Japanese adolescents. International Journal of Human–Computer Studies 66, 930–943. 26 N.S. Baron, E.M. Campbell / Language Sciences 34 (2012) 13–27

Boneva, B., Frolich, D., Kraut, R., 2001. Using e-mail for personal relationships: the difference gender makes. American Behavioral Scientist 45 (3), 530–549. Brown, P., Levinson, S., 1987. Politeness: Some Universals in Language Usage. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge. Bucholtz, M., 2003. Theories of discourse as theories of gender: discourse analysis in language and gender studies. In: Holmes, J., Meyerhoff, M. (Eds.), Handbook of Language and Gender. Blackwell, Malden, MA, pp. 43–68. Cameron, D., 1998. Gender, language, and discourse: a review essay. Signs: Journal of Women in Culture and Society 23 (4), 945–973. Cameron, D., Kulick, D., 2003. Language and Sexuality. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge. Chesley, N., 2005. Blurring boundaries? Linking technology use, spillover, individual distress, and family satisfaction. Journal of Marriage and Family 67, 1237–1248. Coates, J., 1993. Women, Men, and Language, second ed. Longman, New York. Colley, A., Todd, Z., 2002. Gender-linked differences in the style and content of e-mails to friends. Journal of Language and Social Psychology 21 (4), 380–392. Colley, A., Todd, Z., Bland, M., Holmes, M., Khanom, N., Pike, H., 2004. Style and content in e-mails and letters to male and female friends. Journal of Language and Social Psychology 23 (3), 369–378. Daun, A., 1996. Swedish Mentality. Pennsylvania State University Press, State College, PA. Dindia, K., Canary, D. (Eds.), 2006. Sex Differences and Similarities in Communication. Lawrence Erlbaum, Mahwah, NJ. Doi, T., 1971. The Anatomy of Dependence. Kodansha International, Tokyo. Eckert, P., McConnell-Ginet, S., 2003. Language and Gender. Cambridge University Press, New York. Fortunati, L., 2009a. Gender and the mobile phone. In: Goggin, G., Hjorth, L. (Eds.), Mobile Technologies: From Telecommunications to Media. Routledge, New York, pp. 23–34. Fortunati, L., 2000b. Personal communication. Fortunati, L., Manganelli, A.M., 2002. A review of the literature on gender and ICTs in Italy. In: Sørensen, K., Stewart, J., Eds. Digital Divides and Inclusion Measures. A Review of Literature and Statistical Trends on Gender and ICT. NTNU, Trondheim/Edinburgh, pp. 137–170. Fox, A.B., Bukatko, D., Hallahan, M., Crawford, M., 2007. The medium makes a difference: gender similarities and differences in instant messaging. Journal of Language and Social Psychology 26 (4), 389–397. Fujimoto, K., 2005. The third-stage paradigm: territory machines from the girls’ revolution to mobile aesthetics. In: Ito, M., Okabe, D., Matsuda, M. (Eds.), Personal, Portable, Pedestrian: Mobile Phones in Japanese Life. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA, pp. 77–102. Haddon, L. (Ed.), 2005. International Collaborative Research: Cross-Cultural Differences and Cultures of Research. COST Action 269. Hall, E., 1976. Beyond Culture. Doubleday, New York. Hearn, J. (Ed.), 1992. Men in the Public Eye: The Construction and Deconstruction of Public Men and Public Patriarchies. Routledge, London. Herring, S.C., 2003. Gender and power in on-line communication. In: Holmes, J., Meyerhoff, M. (Eds.), The Handbook of Language and Gender. Blackwell, Malden, MA, pp. 202–228. Herring, S.C., 2010. Who’s Got the Floor in Computer-Mediated Communication? Edelsky’s Gender Patterns Revisited. Language@Internet 7, Article 8. . Herring, S.C., Zelenkauskaite, A., 2008. Gendered typography: abbreviation and insertion in Italian iTV SMS. In: Siegel, J.F., Nagel, T.C., Laurente-Lapole, A., Auger, J. (Eds.), IUWPL7: Gender in Language: Classic Questions, New Contexts. IULC Publications, Bloomington, IN, pp. 73–92, . Hijazi-Omari, H., Ribak, R., 2008. Playing with fire: on the domestication of the mobile phone among Palestinian teenage girls in Israel. Information, Communication & Society 11 (2), 149–166. Hjorth, L., 2005. Postal presence: a study of mobile customization and gender in Melbourne. In: Glotz, P., Bertschi, S. (Eds.), Thumb Culture: Social Trends and Mobile Phone Use. Transcript Verlag, Bielefeld, pp. 53–66. Hofstede, G., 1980. Culture’s Consequences: International Differences in Work-Related Values. Sage, Beverly Hills, CA. Holmes, J., 1988. Paying compliments: a sex-preferential politeness strategy. Journal of Pragmatics 12, 445–465. Holmes, J., 1991. Language and gender. Language Teaching 24, 207–220. Holmes, J., 1993. Women’s talk: the question of sociolinguistic universals. Australian Journal of Communication 20, 125–149. Holmes, J., 1995. Women, Men, and Politeness. Longman, New York. Holmes, J., Meyerhoff, M. (Eds.), 2003. Handbook of Language and Gender. Blackwell, Malden, MA. Horstmanshof, L., Power, M., 2005. Mobile phones, SMS, and relationships. Australian Journal of Communication 32 (1), 33–61. Igarashi, T., Takai, J., Yoshida, T., 2005. Gender differences in social network development in mobile phone text messages: a longitudinal study. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships 22 (5), 691–713. ITU (International Union), 2010. Free Statistics, Key 2005–2010 ICT Data. . James, D., 1996. Women, men and prestige speech forms: a critical review. In: Bergvall, V., Bing, J., Freed, A. (Eds.), Rethinking Language and Gender Research: Theory and Practice. Longman, New York, pp. 98–125. James, D., Clarke, S., 1993. Women, men, and interruptions: a critical review. In: Tannen, D. (Ed.), Gender and Conversational Interaction. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp. 231–280. James, D., Drakich, J., 1993. Understanding gender differences in amount of talk: a critical review of the literature. In: Tannen, D. (Ed.), Gender and Conversational Interaction. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp. 281–312. Labov, W., 1991. The intersection of sex and social class in the course of linguistic change. Language Variation and Change 2, 205–254. Lakoff, R., 1975. Language and Woman’s Place. Harper and Row, New York. Lebra, T.S., 1976. Japanese Patterns of Behavior. University of Hawaii Press, Honolulu. Lee, C., 2003. How does instant messaging affect interaction between the genders? The Mercury Project of Instant Messaging Studies, Stanford University. . Lee, D.-H., 2005. Women’s creation of culture. Fibreculture 6, . Lemish, D., Cohen, A., 2005. On the gendered nature of mobile phone culture in Israel. Sex Roles 52 (7/8), 511–521. Lenhart, A., Ling, R., Campbell, S., Purcell, K., 2010. Teens and Mobile Phones. Pew Internet & American Life Project, April 20. . Lin, A., 2005. Gendered, bilingual communication practices: mobile text-messaging among Hong Kong college students. Fibreculture 6, . Ling, Rich, 2005. The socio-linguistics of SMS: an analysis of SMS use by a random sample of Norwegians. In: Ling, R., Pedersen, P. (Eds.), Mobile Communications: Renegotiation of the Social Sphere. Springer, London, pp. 335–349. Littlefield, L., 2004. Psychological aspects of mobile phone use among adolescents. The Australian Psychological Society 3, 1–7. Livingstone, S., 2003. On the challenges of cross-national comparative media research. European Journal of Communication 18 (4), 477–500. Love, S., Kewley, J., 2005. Does personality affect people’s attitude towards mobile phone use in public places? In: Ling, R., Pedersen, P. (Eds.), Mobile Communications: Renegotiation of the Social Sphere. Springer, London, pp. 273–284. McSweeney, B., 2002. Hofstede’s model of national cultural differences and their consequences: a triumph of faith—a failure of analysis. Human Relations 55 (1), 89–118. Milgram, S., 1972. The Familiar Stranger, Division 8, Newsletter. Miyake, K., 2007. How young Japanese express their emotions visually in mobile phone messages: a sociolinguistic analysis. Japanese Studies 27 (1), 53–72. Miyake, K., 2010. Personal communication. Mulac, A., 1989. Men’s and women’s talk in same-gender and mixed-gender dyads: power or polemic? Journal of Language and Social Psychology 8 (3–4), 249–270. N.S. Baron, E.M. Campbell / Language Sciences 34 (2012) 13–27 27

Mulac, A., Lundell, T.L., 1994. Effects of gender-linked language differences in adults’ written discourse: multivariate tests of language effects. Language and Communication 14, 299–309. Nielsenmobile, 2008. In US, SMS Text Messaging Tops Phone Calling, September 22. . Nielsenmobile, 2010. US Teen Mobile Report: Calling Yesterday, Texting Today, Using Apps Tomorrow, October 14. . Nisbett, R., 2003. The Geography of Thought: How Asians and Westerners Think Differently ... and Why. Free Press, New York. Okabe, D., Ito, M., 2005. Keitai in public transportation. In: Ito, M., Okabe, D., Matsuda, M. (Eds.), Personal, Portable, Pedestrian: Mobile Phones in Japanese Life. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA, pp. 205–218. Oksman, V., Turtiainen, 2004. Mobile communication as a social stage. New Media & Society 6 (3), 319–339. Okuyama, Y., 2009. Keetai Meeru: younger people’s mobile written communication in Japan. Electronic Journal of Contemporary Japanese Studies. . Ono, H., Zavodny, M., 2005. Gender differences in information technology usage: a US–Japan comparison. Sociological Perspectives 48 (1), 105–133. Palander-Collin, M., 1999. Male and female styles in seventeenth century correspondence. Language Variation and Change 11, 123–141. Rainie, L., Lenhart, A., Fox, S., Spooner, T., Horrigan, J., 2000. Tracking Online Life. Pew Internet & American Life Project, May 10. . Romaine, S., 2003. Variation in language and gender. In: Holmes, J., Meyerhoff, M. (Eds.), Handbook of Language and Gender. Blackwell, Malden, MA, pp. 98– 118. Schiano, D., Elliott, A., Bellotti, V., 2007. Tokyo youth at leisure: online support of leisure outings. In: Joinson, A., McKenna, K., Postmes, T., Reips, U.-D. (Eds.), The Oxford Handbook of Internet Psychology. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp. 361–370. Scott, D.J., Kato, Y., Kato, S., 2009. Comparing cultural and gender differences in the informal mobile telephone text messages of Japanese and American college students. Waseda Journal of Human Sciences 22 (2), 71–86. Self, C., Meyer, P., 1991. Testing claims for on-line conferences. Written Communication 8 (2), 163–192. Selnow, G., 1985. Sex differences in uses and perceptions of profanity. Sex Roles 12 (3–4), 303–312. Skog, B., 2002. Mobiles and the Norwegian teen: identity, gender and class. In: Katz, J., Aakhus, M. (Eds.), Perpetual Contact: Mobile Communication, Private Talk, Public Performance. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp. 255–273. Smith-Lovin, M., Robinson, D.T., 1992. Gender and conversational dynamics. In: Ridgeway, C. (Ed.), Gender, Interaction, and Inequality. Springer, New York, pp. 120–146. Smoreda, Z., Licoppe, C., 2000. Gender-specific use of the domestic telephone. Social Psychology Quarterly 63 (3), 238–252. Sproull, L., Kiesler, S., 1986. Reducing social context cues: electronic mail in organizational communication. Management Science 32, 1492–1512. Tannen, D., 1993. The relativity of linguistic strategies: rethinking power and solidarity in gender and dominance. In: Tannen, D. (Ed.), Gender and Conversational Interaction. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp. 165–188. Tannen, D., 1994. Gender and Discourse. Oxford University Press, New York. Tannen, D., 2010. Personal communication. Thomas, F., Haddon, L., Gilligan, R., Heinzman, P., de Gournay, C., 2005. Cultural factors shaping the experience of ICTs: an exploratory review. In: Haddon, L. (Ed.), International Collaborative Research: Cross-Cultural Differences and Cultures of Research, COST Action, 269. EU Publications Office, Brussels, pp. 13–49. Wagatsuma, H., Rosett, A., 1986. The implications of apology: law and culture in Japan and the United States. Law and Society Review 20 (4), 461–498. Waseleski, C., 2006. Gender and the use of exclamation points in computer-mediated communication: an analysis of exclamations posted to two electronic discussion lists. Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication 11 (4), . Wei, R., Lo, V.-H., 2006. Staying connected while on the move: cell phone use and social connectedness. New Media & Society 8 (1), 53–72. Witmer, D., Katzman, S., 1997. On-line smiles: does gender make a difference in the use of graphic accents? Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication 2 (4), . World Internet Project, 2010. . Yates, S., 2006. A device for doing culture: gender, design, and use of the mobile phone. Lecture Presented to BayCHI, June 13. Reported on Rashmi’s Blog, June 14. . Yates, S., Mills, S., Lockley, E., Doherty, K., 2005. Gender, ‘‘face’’ management, and mediated interaction. In: Paper Presented at the 9th International Pragmatics Conference, Riva del Garda, Italy, July 10–15. Yoon, K., 2003. Retraditionalizing the mobile phone. European Journal of Cultural Studies 6 (3), 327–343.