<<

Behavioral Medicine

ISSN: 0896-4289 (Print) 1940-4026 (Online) Journal homepage: http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/vbmd20

Optimism and the Experience of : A Systematic Review

Johanna Basten-Günther, Madelon Peters & Stefan Lautenbacher

To cite this article: Johanna Basten-Günther, Madelon Peters & Stefan Lautenbacher (2018): Optimism and the Experience of Pain: A Systematic Review, Behavioral Medicine To link to this article: https://doi.org/10.1080/08964289.2018.1517242

Published online: 20 Dec 2018.

Submit your article to this journal

View Crossmark data

Full Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at http://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journalCode=vbmd20 BEHAVIORAL MEDICINE https://doi.org/10.1080/08964289.2018.1517242

REVIEW Optimism and the Experience of Pain: A Systematic Review

Johanna Basten-Gunther€ a , Madelon Petersb, and Stefan Lautenbachera aPhysiological , University of Bamberg, Bamberg, Germany; bDepartment of Clinical Psychological Science, Maastricht University, Maastricht, The Netherlands

ABSTRACT KEYWORDS A growing body of literature provides evidence of the -promoting effects of optimism, chronic pain; optimism; including its protective role in acute and chronic pain. Optimists are characterized by posi- pain; ; tive expectations concerning the future. These positive outcome expectancies lead to more resilience and longer goal-directed efforts and the use of approach coping strategies. No systematic review on the effects of optimism on the experience of pain has so far been conducted. A search in the databases PubMed, Web of Science and PsycInfo, and the scanning of refer- ence lists identified 69 eligible studies. These were categorized according to sample size, participants’ age and sex, design, optimism-pain relation as primary vs. secondary study objective, and level of study/publication quality. Overall percentages of positive, zero, and negative associations between optimism and pain as well as relative frequencies of these associations in the different categories were analyzed. About 70% of the studies showed a positive, i.e., beneficial association between optimism and at least one pain outcome. A larger percentage of beneficial associations was found in studies with experimental designs, in studies with the optimism-pain relation as primary objective, in high-quality studies/publi- cations, and in studies including participants with a higher average age. The review sug- gests that optimism is associated with less acute and chronic pain, especially since a higher percentage of beneficial associations was found with high study/publication quality and with the primary focus on this relationship. For the moderating role of age, different explan- ations are proposed. Further research on causal relationships and on optimism-fostering clinical interventions is needed.

Introduction and hypervigilance, disuse and disability, and in turn For decades, pain research has been dominated by the increased pain. Optimism seems to protect against the examination of risk factors. Only recently, in the wake development of this vicious circle leading to chronic of Antonovsky’s salutogenic model1 and Martin pain in that it stops catastrophizing and hypervigi- Seligman’s work in positive psychology,2 this trad- lance to negative , as will be described. itional focus has been extended to the search for Originally defined by Scheier and Carver as resilience factors, i.e. factors that can dampen acute “generalized positive outcome expectancies”6 in the pain experience, promote adaptation to chronic pain, context of their theory of self-regulation,7 optimism or protect against developing chronic pain. Self-effi- describes the tendency of individuals to expect posi- cacy, , and positive are among these char- tive things to happen to them in the future. As opti- acteristics that have been shown to promote pain mists subjectively evaluate the probability of success resilience.3,4 higher, they are more likely to engage and persist in As stated in the -avoidance model of musculo- goal-directed efforts (as opposed to the “why-bother”- skeletal pain,5 chronic pain can be a result of a dys- of pessimists),8 which in turn increases their functional psychological reaction to an acute pain chance to effectively cope with stressors.9 This is in experience. People who tend to engage in catastroph- line with traditional expectancy x value-theories of izing thoughts concerning their pain are likely to get motivation,10 which emphasize the role of expecta- caught in a vicious circle of fear of pain, avoidance tions in motivation and motivated behavior.

CONTACT Johanna Basten-Gunther€ [email protected] Physiologische Psychologie, Markusplatz 3 (02.01), 96047 Bamberg, Germany. ß 2018 Taylor & Francis Group, LLC 2 J. BASTEN-GUNTHER€ ET AL.

There is evidence of a wide range of health-pro- age effects in the prediction of self-rated health by moting effects of optimism (for a comprehensive over- several psychological variables such as positive affect view, see Carver et al.;11 for a recent mini review or depressive symptoms (for example, Spuling et al.,17 concerning chronic diseases, see Avvenuti et al.12). Benyamini et al.,18 French et al.19). In these studies, Although the association of optimism with pain has the influence of psychological as compared to physio- been examined in numerous studies, no systematic logical variables augmented with increasing age. It can review or meta-analysis has to our knowledge been therefore be speculated that likewise optimism’s asso- published on this topic. ciation with pain might be bigger in older individuals. Apart from several overviews covering related For this reason, age will be analyzed as another pos- topics such as the relationship between pain and posi- sible moderating variable. The probability that an tive traits3 or between optimism and coping,13 there existing association between optimism and pain is have been two publications on the topic of optimism statistically detected also depends on parameters of and the experience of pain: Garofalo’s review on per- study design. Studies with large sample sizes have ceived optimism and chronic pain, covering the litera- more statistical power. In experimental studies, con- ture until 2000,8 and Goodin and Bulls’ review on founding variables can more easily be ruled out. optimism and the experience of pain incorporating Studies focusing on optimism and pain as their pri- research from 2000 to 2013.9 Although being valuable mary aim can be supposed to be tailored to more sources of information, the two publications lack the accurately measure the two variables of . methodic and scrupulous approach of system- Therefore, these technical variables as well as an over- atic reviews. all measure of study/publication quality are included Garofalo,8 limiting his review to chronic pain con- in the of variables possibly influencing the opti- ditions, concluded that the body of literature available mism-pain relation. at that time was too scant for definite conclusions, So far, optimism has largely been conceptualized as but tentatively suggested positive effects of optimism a personality disposition measured by trait question- on chronic pain. Goodin and Bulls,9 who were able to naires, especially the Life Orientation Test (LOT)6 and make inferences from a significantly larger number of its revised version (LOT-R),20 which offer both a com- studies, confirmed these findings, extending them to posite optimism score and separate scores on opti- experimental pain conditions. They also listed several mism and subscales. cognitive and behavioral mediators that are associated Only recently, attempts have been made to experi- with less severe pain reports and therefore might mentally induce an optimistic state for a short while. explain the underlying mechanisms of the optimism- Fosnaugh and colleagues described significant positive pain relationship: optimists tend to show lower pain changes in a dispositional (LOT-R) and a comparative catastrophizing (see Pulvers and Hood,3 for an over- optimism measure both after a future thinking view), higher hopefulness and pain and manipulation and after a semantic optimism-priming more effective coping strategies. task.21 Peters and associates22 as well as Hanssen and The present overview adds to and extends this pre- researchers23 reported positive evidence obtained by a vious work in that it uses a research algorithm in different manipulation, the Best-Possible-Self-task order to systematically retrieve all relevant studies on (BPS).24 During this exercise, participants imagine the optimism-pain relationship, covering experimental and write about themselves in the future, when they and clinical pain as well as dispositional and situ- envision to have reached all their goals, and when all ational optimism. It is thus the first to give a compre- their dreams have become true (for the exact instruc- hensive account of the current state of research. tions, see supplement 2).25 The BPS was shown to be Additionally, in order to propose explanations for successful in bringing about an increase in partic- divergent results, we seek to identify variables which ipants’ situational optimism, which was recorded by influence whether a study finds a significant associ- the Questionnaire for Future Expectations (FEX),an ation; that is, moderating factors of the optimism-pain adaptation of the Subjective Probability Task.26 relation. In the pain context, sex and age have been In the present work, both studies using experimen- shown to be among the most important moderating tal pain and studies of clinical pain are reviewed. As variables.14,15 Accordingly, for men and women differ- the theoretical framework on optimism claims that ential relations between optimism and various health- optimism has a trait (dispositional) as well as a state related variables like, for example, stress symptoms (situational) component,27 we will consider trait as have been reported.6,16 Similarly, there is evidence of well as state measures of optimism, which are known BEHAVIORAL MEDICINE 3 to be highly correlated,28 and include induced (experi- differences of pain perception between adults and mentally or through clinical interventions) as well as children31 we decided to exclude pain in children and spontaneous (non-induced) optimism. As implied by focus on adult humans. Therefore, “children” as well Carver and Scheier’s definition,6 optimism concerns as “animals” were applied as NOT-terms (for the generalized outcome expectations, which is why spe- exact syntax used for the search, see supplement 1). cific expectations (e.g., health-related beliefs) are not There were no restrictions concerning years of publi- considered here as a measure of optimism. cation. Five additional relevant studies from other Since there are many different pain-related varia- sources, for example, scanning of reference lists, were bles, it seems reasonable to focus first on pain experi- added (see Figure 1). Titles and abstracts of the 675 ence in the narrow sense, which comprises reports of studies retrieved via this systematic search were pain intensity, frequency or unpleasantness, the meas- screened. Five hundred forty records had to be urement of pain thresholds, pain tolerance thresholds, excluded because they were either not related to the and psycho-physiological parameters like evoked topic of pain and optimism or they were records in a potentials or heart rate responses. Also considered are language other than English, reviews or no peer- parameters, which can be derived from the first group reviewed journal articles. Of the 135 articles assessed of parameters such as habituation, temporal summa- in full-text for eligibility by two reviewers, only those tion, conditioned pain modulation, or placebo/nocebo were included in our synthesis which fulfilled the fol- effects. Due to the growing number of methodological lowing three conditions: (1) involving the exact varia- approaches to assess pain and its various dimensions, bles of interests—pain and optimism rather than it is not possible to incorporate all different pain out- related constructs such as hope; (2) providing distinct come variables into one review. Thus, it seemed rea- measures of both optimism and pain (within the same sonable to focus only on the basic variables of pain person, thus precluding studies treating concepts like experience in this first overview. Secondary pain out- “caregiver optimism”) in order extract the specific val- comes such as fear of pain, pain-related disability, ues of these two constructs; (3) reporting some form functional impairment, coping with pain, or adjust- of statistical measure of the relation between both ment to pain conditions are not included here, but constructs (e.g., correlation, effect size in ANOVA or may be considered in further reviews. regression; regressions of optimism on pain were It is, in summary, our aim to provide an overview excluded on the basis of our research question). of a topic of high clinical relevance, the association of For a methodological study/publication description, optimism with pain experience. We set out for the the 69 studies meeting all inclusion criteria (see Table 1) first time to systematically review research on the were assessed using an index of quality, inspired by optimism-pain-relation in order to propose answers to assessment tools for studies in meta-analyses such as the following questions: Is optimism associated with the Newcastle-Ottawa scale,32 which provide an opera- less pain and, if yes, under which conditions is this tionalized score for each study. Lower levels of quality association observed? intherespectivecategories(e.g.,nocleardescriptionof measures, missing documentation of comorbidity) are Methods awarded fewer points (see Table 2). The quality of each The present article is based on the recommendations study/publication could range from a minimum of 1.5 of the PRISMA guidelines for the creation of system- points to a maximum of 11 points. Based on our atic reviews and meta-analyses.29 The studies included theoretical assumptions on potential moderating varia- in this review were identified through a computerized bles described in the introduction, we classified all stud- 33 search in the databases PubMed, Web of Science and ies according to age of subjects (following Erikson’s PsycInfo, which cover a large part of research articles categorization), sex of subjects, sample size, design in the field of psychology and medicine and have (clinical-cross-sectional vs. clinical-longitudinal vs. been used in previous reviews on similar or related experimental), importance of the optimism-pain relation topics.3,30 A search algorithm combining keywords within the study (among the primary objectives of the referring to pain (such as “pain,”“clinical pain,” study or not), and the previously mentioned quality “experimental pain,”“pain intensity,”“pain thresh- index. The included pain measures reflect different old,” etc.) with keywords referring to optimism (such dimensions (e.g., pain intensity, unpleasantness, thresh- as “optimism,”“dispositional optimism,”“situational old, tolerance, duration) and can therefore not sensibly optimism”) was employed. Due to the known be aggregated. For this reason, it was not possible to 4 J. BASTEN-GUNTHER€ ET AL.

Figure 1. chart illustrating the selection of studies for the review according to the PRISMA guidelines. perform a meta-analysis incorporating the whole sample Clinical Studies: Most studies concerning clinical of studies. conditions had participants with either musculoskel- We analyzed for each moderating variable the per- etal pain (k ¼ 10)[2], arthritis (k ¼ 10) or post-opera- centage of studies with positive (i.e., significant benefi- tive pain (k ¼ 17). Another seven articles dealt with cial association between optimism and pain)[1] versus different forms of pain, the remaining eleven not positive results in the different classes of this vari- studies with various other clinical conditions, which able (e.g., different age groups). Due to the numerous are listed in detail in Table 1. To assess pain, the clin- — cell frequencies of n < 5 in the resulting multi-field ical studies used questionnaires mostly the SF-36 ¼ ¼ panels, we did not perform chi2 tests, but instead (k 7), the MPQ and its short form (k 5), the ¼ tested for significant deviations from an equal distri- BPI(-SF) (k 7) or illness-specific questionnaires ¼ — bution with binominal tests. (k 12) as, for example, the RADAR and/or rating scales (k¼ 24). Results Experimental Studies: In experimental settings, the majority of the 14 studies used the cold pressor task Descriptive statistics (k ¼ 8). Three studies applied laser (k ¼ 1) or ther- Of the 69 studies included in the review, 55 dealt with mode-induced (k ¼ 3) heat pain. The remaining three – – clinical pain,34 88 12 with experimental pain,23,89 99 studies used thermode-induced cold pain (k ¼ 1), and two100,101 with both of them (cf. Tables 1 and 3). chemical (k ¼ 1) and ischemic (k ¼ 1) pain. Pain Table 1. Studies comprised in the review. Type of Pain (Pain-Causing Method/Event/ Result (Type of Among Sample Age Medical Condition) Optimism Design Association) Quality Primary Authors (Year) Size Measure Points Objectives Ø Ø Ø Cross- <30 30-60 >60 Experimental Clinical sectional Longitudinal Experimental Beneficial Zero Non-beneficial 34 Achat et al. (2000) 659 ? ? ? – x LOT x ––x –– 6.5 x 35 Airila et al. (2014) 360 x – multiside musculoskeletal single item – x ––x – 7? pain 36 Allison et al. (2000) 88 x – head and neck cancer LOT – x – x –– 8x 37 Bediako et al. (2011) 83 x – sickle-cell disease LOT-R x ––x –– 8 – 38 Bentsen et al. (2008) 101 x – chronic low back pain single item x ––x –– 7.5 x after spinal fusion 39 Benyamini (2005) 120 x – osteoarthritis LOT – x – x –– 9 – 40 Booth-Kewley et al. (2014) 134 x – musculoskeletal injuries LOT-R – x ––x – 9x 89 Boselie et al. (2014) 74 x cold pressor task – LOT-R; FEX –– x – x – 8 – 41 Brewer et al. (2007) 91 x – anterior cruciate LOT-R – x ––x – 8x ligament reconstruction 42 Bruce et al. (2012) 338 x – breast cancer surgery LOT SF – x – x –– 8x 43 Bruce et al. (2014) 338 x – breast cancer surgery LOT SF – x – xx – 8x 44 Callahan (2000) 163 x – temporomandibular LOT x ––x –– 7.5 x disorders 45 Chamberlain et al. (1992) 57 x – surgery for joint LOT – x – xx – 8x replacement 46 Chrisler et al. (2006) 92 x – menstrual pain LOT x ––x –– 7 – 47 Coronado (2017) 63 x – shoulder pain LOT-R – x ––x – 8.5 x 90 Corsi et al. (2017) 46 x heat – LOT-R –– x – x – 9.5 x 100 Costello et al. (2002) 48 x ischemic pain temporomandibular LOT –– xxx– 9.5 x disorders 91 Dimova et al. (2015) 110 ? ? ? capsaicin – LOT-R –– xx––10.5 x 48 Ferreira et al. (2007) 72 x – osteoarthritis LOT-R x ––x –– 7.5 x 49 Fishbain et al. (2001) 637 ? ? ? – myofascial pain PAS x ––xx – 7 – syndrome 50 Fitzgerald et al. (1993) 49 x – coronary artery LOT – x – xx – 7x bypass surgery 92 Geers et al. (2010) 116 x cold pressor task – LOT-R –– xx–– 9x 93 Geers et al. (2008) 72 x cold pressor task – LOT-R –– xxx– 9x 101 Goodin et al. (2013a) 100 x heat knee osteoarthritis LOT-R –– xxx– 10 x 94 Goodin et al. (2013b) 149 x cold pressor task – LOT-R –– xxx– 11 x 51 Gramke et al. (2009) 648 ? ? ? – day-case surgery LOT – x ––x – 9x 23 Hanssen et al. (2013) 79 x cold pressor task – LOT-R; FEX –– xx–– 8x 95 Hanssen et al. (2014) 60 x cold pressor task – LOT-R –– xxx– 8.5 x

52 Hetmann et al. (2015) 106 x – thoracotomy LOT-R – x – x –– 9.5 x MEDICINE BEHAVIORAL 96 Hood et al. (2012) 114 x cold pressor task – LOT-R –– xx–– 9.5 x 53 Hoofwijk et al. (2015) 908 x – outpatient surgery 4 items of LOT – x – x –– 9.5 x 54 Katz et al. (2016) 164 x – inflammatory bowel LOT-R x –––x – 9 – disease 55 Kurtz et al. (2008) 214 x – during chemotherapy LOT – x – x –– 8x 56 Lam et al. (2012) 253 x – nasopharynegeal cancer single item – x ––x – 7 – 57 Langbach et al. (2016) 265 x – hernia and hernia repair LOT-R – x – xx – 9.5 x 58 Lau et al. (2008) 5163 x – x 1 item of LOT – x – x –– 7.5 x 59 Long et al. (1993) 200 x – / osteoarthritis LOT x –––x – 8.5 – (Continued) 5 Table 1. Continued. 6 Type of Pain (Pain-Causing Method/Event/ Result (Type of Among Sample Age Medical Condition) Optimism Design Association) Quality Primary BASTEN-G J. Authors (Year) Size Measure Points Objectives Ø Ø Ø Cross- <30 30-60 >60 Experimental Clinical sectional Longitudinal Experimental Beneficial Zero Non-beneficial NHRE AL. ET UNTHER 60 Mahler et al. (2000) 215 x – coronary bypass surgery LOT – x – x ––10 x € 97 Morton et al. (2009) 62 x laser heat – LOT-R –– xxx– 8.5 x 61 Mueller et al. (2003) 148 x – 4 items x –––x – 7 – 62 Pence et al. (2007) 27 x – sickle-cell disease LOT-R – x ––x – 8.5 – 63 Peters et al. (2007) 625 x – surgery LOT x –––x – 8x 64 Peters et al. (2010) 401 x – elective surgery LOT – x ––x – 9x 65 Pinto et al. (2015) 252 x – hysterectomy/ joint arthroplasty LOT-R – x – xx – 7x 66 Pinto et al. (2017) 124 x – hip and knee arthroplasty LOT-R – x – xx – 9.5 x 67 Pinto et al. (2014) 110 x – major joint arthroplasty LOT-R – x – x ––10 – 68 Pinto et al. (2013) 124 x – primary total hip/ knee arthroplasty LOT-R – x – x –– 9x 69 Powell et al. (2012) 135 x – inguinal hernia repair surgery 2 items of LOT – x – x –– 7.5 x 70 Pulgar et al. (2015) 69 ? ? ? – hematological cancer LOT x –––x – 7x 71 Ramırez-Maestre et al. (2012) 98 x – chronic pain LOT x –––x – 7.5 x 72 Ronaldson et al. (2014) 197 x – coronary artery bypass graft surgery LOT-R – x – xx – 9x 73 Rosenberger et al. (2009) 180 x – arthroscopic knee surgery LOT-R – x – xx – 9.5 x 74 Saariaho et al. (2011) 602 x – chronic pain EMS x ––x –– 8x 75 Sherman et al. (2013) 160 x – osteoarthritis LOT-R x ––xx – 8 – 76 Singh et al. (2010) 1449 x – knee replacement MMPI – x – x –– 8x 77 Sipil€a et al. (2009) 5696 ? ? ? – temporomandibular disorders LOT-R x ––x –– 8x 78 Smith et al. (2008) 170 x – rheumatoid arthritis/ osteoarthritis LOT-R – x ––x – 8 – 98 Smith et al. (2009) 47 x heat and cold – LOT –– xxx– 8.5 x 99 Snyder et al. (2005) 116 x cold pressor task – LOT-R –– x – x – 8.5 x 79 Soderlund€ et al. (1999) 104 x – whiplash associated disorders LOT x –––x – 7.5 x 80 Sorbi et al. (2006) 80 x – chronic pain single item – x – x –– 8.5 x 81 Stessel et al. (2017) 1118 x – day surgery 4 items of LOT-R – x – x –– 9.5 – 82 Su et al. (2017) 320 x – temporomandibular disorders LOT-R x ––xx – 7.5 x 83 Tennen et al. (1992) 54 x – rheumatoid arthritis LOT – x – xx – 7.5 – 84 Treharne et al. (2005) 154 x – rheumatoid arthritis LOT – x – x x x 8.5 x 85 Tsakogia et al. (2011) 96 x – chronic musculoskeletal pain LOT-R x ––x –– 8x 86 Wiesmann et al. (2014) 387 x – x LOT x ––xx – 8x 87 Wong et al. (2007) 334 x – lung cancer single item – x – xx – 7x 88 Wright et al. (2011) 89 x – chronic musculoskeletal pain LOT-R x –––x – 8x numbers are identical to those by which the studies are designated in the text. experimental optimism induction. BEHAVIORAL MEDICINE 7

Table 2. Quality index applied for the selected studies. Category Points Sample size small (n < 50) 0.5 medium (n¼ 50-100) 1 large (n > 100) 1.5 Sex not reported 0 one sex only 0.5 both sexes, not well-balanced 1 well-balanced ratio (max. 40:60) 1.5 Age not well documented 0 moderately well documented (e.g., range OR average) 0.5 well documented 1 Type of pain unclear description 0 sufficiently clear description 1 Description of pain measure unclear description 0 sufficiently clear description 1 Type of pain measure health measure including a pain-item 0.5 specific pain measure 1 Measure of optimism single item 0 more than one item out of validated questionnaire 0.5 validated questionnaire 1 Medication not reported 0 moderately well specified 0.5 precisely described 1 Comorbidity not reported 0 moderately well specified 0.5 precisely described 1 Design cross-sectional 0.5 longitudinal/experimental 1

in one case was taken from the LOT. One study took Table 3. Summary of types of pain and pain measures in the two items from the LOT, the remaining three studies 69 reviewed studies. used other scales (PAS, EMS and MMPI). All of the Clinical pain K Experimental pain k Type of pain musculoskeletal pain 10 cold pressor task 8 measures concerned dispositional optimism. The two arthritis pain 10 laser heat pain 1 studies which conducted an experimental induction of post-operative pain 17 thermode heat pain 3 23,89 cancer pain 7 chemical pain 1 optimism additionally recorded situational opti- other 11 ischemic pain 1 mism (measured by the FEX). Sample Size: Sample sizes ranged from 27 to 5,696. Pain measure questionnaires rating of intensity 11 SF-36 7 rating of unpleasantness 4 Sex: The majority of the studies (k ¼ 62) included MPQ(-SF) 5 pain threshold 4 both sexes; five studies had only female and two stud- BPI(-SF) 7 tolerance threshold 6 illness-specific 12 CPM & TS 2 ies only male participants. (f.ex. RADAR) Age: As several reports (k ¼ 5) did not specify par- rating scales 24 habituation 1 ’ placebo analgesia 2 ticipants age, no exact indication of the average or range of age can be made. Of the studies where age was reported, about half (k ¼ 33) had an average experience was determined by recording reports of between 30 and 60 years, 22% (k ¼ 14) younger than intensity (k ¼ 11) and unpleasantness (k ¼ 4), pain 30 years, and 27% (k ¼ 17) older than 60 years. In thresholds (k ¼ 4) or pain tolerance thresholds (k ¼ 6). experimental studies, the average age seems to be From these, some studies computed markers of younger than 30 years (27.9 in studies that reported inhibitory or facilitatory processes (conditioned pain average age); in clinical studies, the mean of specified modulation or temporal summation, k ¼ 2), habitu- age averages was 53.6 years. ation (k ¼ 1) and placebo analgesia (k ¼ 2). As only Study/Publication Quality: Quality according to three articles included psycho-physiological and stress the index we applied (see Table 2) ranged from 6 to parameters (blood pressure and heart rate92,93 and the 11 points in the present studies. Only one article94 pro-inflammatory cytokine interleukin-6100), we did scored the maximum of 11 quality points. not perform any separate analyses with these out- (Co)morbidity: In more than half of the studies come measures. (k ¼ 36), morbidity or comorbidity was not reported. Optimism Measure: All but nine studies (k ¼ 60) Most experimental designs excluded illnesses that are fully or partly employed the same optimism measure, known to influence pain perception. In at least 10 namely the LOT or LOT-R. In five studies, measure- clinical studies, a part of the participants was affected ment of optimism was limited to a single item, which by major illnesses such as or diabetes. 8 J. BASTEN-GUNTHER€ ET AL.

Figure 2. Possible moderating variables of the optimism-pain relationship: (a) age, (b) type of design, (c) relevance of the relation within the study, (d) level of quality.

Optimism-pain relation negative association for one subgroup of the sample (patients with established rheumatoid arthritis), bene- In total, 48 of the 69 studies (69.6%) found a signifi- ficial associations for the other two subgroups (early cant beneficial association between optimism and at and intermediate rheumatoid arthritis) and no associ- least one pain outcome. Of those, 25 studies (36.2% of all 69 studies) revealed an exclusively beneficial—no ation between optimism and pain in the overall additional zero or negative association. The remaining correlation. 23 studies (33.3% of all 69 studies) showed mixed results which means that they report two or more out- Moderating variables come measures with diverging results (see Table 1). These different results partly stem from different stat- Sample Size: 72.3% (k ¼ 47 of 65) of studies with istical analyses of the same data: in some cases, for medium (n¼ 50–100) or large (n > 100) sample sizes example, simple correlations or univariate regressions report a beneficial association between optimism and were significantly positive, while the association disap- at least one pain outcome. Since there were only a few peared in more complex models such as hierarchical studies with small sample sizes (k ¼ 4), we are not regressions or multivariate models.82,83,86 Apart from able to draw any general conclusions about the impact that, “mixed results” also refers to diverging results of sample size on the likelihood of positive findings. for subgroups of the sample (e.g., men vs. women,49 Age: As shown in Figure 2, the proportion of stud- different experimental conditions,93 clinical population ies showing beneficial associations increases with vs. healthy controls),100 for different optimism param- higher age. Thus, 88.2% (k ¼ 15 of 17) of studies with eters (e.g., subscales of the LOT),75 for different pain an average age above 60 years revealed significant outcomes (e.g., pain intensity vs. pain tolerance or beneficial associations, compared to 65.6% (k ¼ 21 of pain threshold;78,95 clinical vs. experimental pain;101 32) and 57.1% (k ¼ 8 of 14) in the age groups of different types of clinical pain)57,65,66 or for different 30–60 years and under 30 years, respectively. times of measurement (first vs. second experimental Binominal tests showed that there were significantly session;97 baseline vs. follow-up).50,73,87 more beneficial than zero associations in the two older Twenty-one studies (30.4% of all 69 studies) did age groups (30–60 years: p ¼ 0.03; above 60: not detect any association; one study84 reports a p < 0.005), but not in the youngest one (p ¼ 0.18). BEHAVIORAL MEDICINE 9

Sex: Due to the too small number of studies generalized expectations concerning the future, includ- including either exclusively women or men or expli- ing trait and state measures. Pain experience accord- citly testing for sex differences (k ¼ 8), no conclusions ing to our definition included reports of pain can be derived concerning the moderating role of sex. intensity, frequency or unpleasantness, the measure- Design: While both experimental and clinical stud- ment of pain thresholds, pain tolerance thresholds ies show a clear majority of beneficial over zero asso- and psycho-physiological parameters as well as ciations, this tendency becomes more apparent in higher-ordered pain processes like habituation, tem- experimental designs, where beneficial associations are poral summation, conditioned pain modulation or reported in 78.6% (k ¼ 11 of 14) of studies, compared placebo/nocebo effects. to 70.6% (k ¼ 24 of 34) in clinical-longitudinal and Of the 69 eligible articles comprising experimental 61.9% (k ¼ 13 of 21) in clinical-cross-sectional studies and clinical studies with a variety of different types (see Figure 2). The proportion of beneficial associa- and measures of pain, about 70% (k ¼ 48) showed a tions was significantly larger than an equal distribu- beneficial association regarding at least one pain out- tion in the former two (experimental: p ¼ 0.02; come measure. A significantly bigger proportion of clinical-longitudinal: p ¼ 0.01), but not in clinical- beneficial associations than expected under an equal cross-sectional designs (p ¼ 0.10). distribution of was found in experimental and clin- Optimism as Primary Objective: As there were ical-longitudinal studies, studies with the major focus studies whose major focus was set on examining the on the optimism-pain relation, studies with high relationship between optimism and pain experience as study/publication quality as well as studies with a opposed to others in which optimism was one of a higher average age of the participants. All in all, the multitude of psychological variables measured and present state of research suggests that optimism can pain one of health-related outcomes, we analyzed sep- indeed be considered a psychological factor which is arately those studies that treated the optimism-pain associated with a diminished experience of pain one relation as primary vs. secondary objective. While in of several health-related outcomes. the latter group, only half of the studies (k ¼ 9 of 18; p ¼ 0.19) found a positive optimism-pain association, Moderating variables studies focusing on optimism and pain yielded benefi- cial associations at 76.5% (k ¼ 39 of 51; p < 0.005) and In order to determine why some studies found signifi- thus significantly more often than expected under an cant beneficial associations while others did not, we equal distribution (see Figure 2). examined several moderating factors of the optimism- Study/Publication Quality: Eventually, regarding pain relation. quality of study and publication as a possible moder- Experimental and clinical-longitudinal designs, ating factor, our analysis showed a markedly higher studies with the optimism-pain relation as primary percentage of beneficial associations with high study/ objective and studies with a higher study/publication publication quality (quality index 9.5): as shown in quality were shown to produce a significantly larger Figure 2, 92.9% of the studies in this group (k ¼ 13 of percentage of beneficial associations than expected 14) report a beneficial association between optimism under an equal distribution. This seems to further and at least one pain outcome, compared to 60% corroborate our assumption of a beneficial optimism- (k ¼ 6 of 10) of records with low study/publication pain relationship, since these are studies which are quality ( 7 points) and 64.4% (k ¼ 29 of 45) of more likely to detect an association if it does exist: records with medium study/publication quality (7.5–9 studies with high study/publication quality presumably points). The proportion of beneficial associations was yield more valid data than studies with low study/ significantly bigger than expected under an equal dis- publication quality. Studies primarily focusing on tribution in the medium (p ¼ 0.02) and high-quality optimism and pain can be supposed to more accur- group (p < 0.005), but not in the low quality ately measure the two variables of interest. In experi- group (p ¼ 0.20). mental and clinical-longitudinal designs, confounding variables can be better controlled by context manipu- lation or recording temporal relationships. Discussion The higher percentage of beneficial associations The present systematic review on studies investigating with increasing age of participants may be explained the relation between optimism and the experience of by a model proposed by Jylh€a and colleagues.102 The pain is the first of its kind. Optimism was defined as authors assume that self-rated health results from an 10 J. BASTEN-GUNTHER€ ET AL. evaluation process, incorporating both physical health optimism on health-related variables was found for factors and additional factors such as chronological men in general (e.g., in mortality, see Giltay et al.,104 age or health expectations. Furthermore, according to Peterson et al.105), only one of the 69 studies included the model, the relative importance of these evaluation in the present review49 explicitly investigated sex dif- criteria changes with age. Self-rated health in older ferences of the optimism-pain relation: they detected a people could reflect to a higher degree psychological beneficial association for women and no association adaptation to decreasing health than in younger peo- for men. Future studies should test for sex as a mod- ple.103 Similar processes might be at work in pain erator variable. reports. In higher age, the relative importance of psy- Geers and colleagues93 proposed the explanation chological processes such as appraisal or social com- that optimists are not generally less reactive to pain parison (for example, “It is normal to have pain at stimuli, which indeed could be highly dysfunctional in this age,”“Given my high age, my pain is relatively certain situations when detecting and monitoring pain low,” etc.), which are in turn influenced by optimism, is crucial. They assumed that instead optimists are could become bigger compared to that of actual phys- more flexible in coping with pain than pessimists: ical symptoms in predicting pain reports. These they might be generally inclined to focus their atten- assumptions gain great plausibility because very simi- tion on the positive aspects of a situation. Whenever lar phenomena have been repeatedly found for the it becomes apparent that certain stimuli (e.g., pain – prediction of self-rated health.17 19 cues) are of relevance for their well-being and require Sixty-five of the 69 studies of this review were pub- their action, however, they could switch to an lished after 2000, i.e. within a relatively short time “approach mode” of problem-focused coping and face span. Therefore, we cannot rule out that the age effect the pain, as described in Garofalo’s8 model for we found is in fact a cohort effect; that is, not caused chronic pain. by the age of participants, but by differences between It is thus conceivable that two different mecha- earlier and later-born cohorts, for example as regards nisms are at work in experimental vs. clinical studies: , environmental conditions, values, or health/ healthy optimists who are confronted with an experi- disease definitions.17 Apart from that, the age effect mental pain induction are aware that the noxious we found could, as far as clinical studies are con- stimuli are not harmful and will be over soon. It is cerned, simply result from either a different pain dur- likely that they therefore divert their from ation or from different clinical conditions represented these negative features of the situation and subse- in the respective age groups: in our analysis, signifi- quently report less pain. Corroborating this assump- cant relations were most likely found with post-opera- tion, Peters and associates106 found in an eye-tracker tive pain (beneficial associations in 80% of the study that optimists tended to turn away from angry respective studies) and rheumatoid diseases (75%, faces and gazed longer at joyful faces. Facing a serious compared to 71% in cancer pain and 60% in musculo- threat to their well-being, on the contrary, such as an skeletal pain); all but two studies with an average age operation or cancer, the same optimists focus on pain of above 60 years (k ¼ 15 of 17) belong to either one and its context instead of withdrawing from it. They of these two categories. Another possible explanation take steps to tackle the problem (cf. Luo et al.107 for a is that if individuals experience little pain from early study on optimism and skin cancer information) in on, this could in turn diminish their expectations of the sense of the approach-style coping as described.9 future harm and thus increase their optimism, which Unlike pessimists they still expect there are things again would lead to even less pain experience. Over they can do to improve their condition. decades, these reciprocally intensifying effects could However, this problem-focused coping, based on cumulate and stabilize the benefits of optimism at a the optimistic expectations that by trying hard pain high level. However, the fact that experimental studies will decrease, might not be unconditionally functional revealed a higher percentage of significant beneficial in people confronted with a chronic or malignant ill- associations even though their participants were ness that holds little or no improvement over the younger than those in clinical studies (76.9% of years. When there is not much one can do about laboratory studies had an age average younger than one’s condition, strategies like acceptance and distrac- 30 years, compared to only 8% of clinical studies) casts tion are more suitable to maintain a high quality of some on this assumption. life.108 Indeed, in another study by Saariaho and asso- While previous research supports the moderating ciates71 optimism was associated with active coping, role of sex in that a stronger protective effect of which in turn had positive effects on chronic pain, BEHAVIORAL MEDICINE 11 impairment and functioning. Active coping is concep- these possibilities and each result was weighted tually different from the previously mentioned equally, independent of the statistical approach which approach or problem-focused coping in that it does was employed. not aim at eliminating the problem (in this case, the One must also take into account that our descrip- pain or pain-related illness) but instead—much like tive analysis was based on the percentage of signifi- acceptance strategies—aims at staying active and cant effects. As we do not have detailed information maintaining activities and well-being despite the pain. on the power and robustness of the statistical tests Furthermore, optimists have been reported to have employed in most studies, it is likely that both alpha the highly adaptive flexibility to switch to - and beta errors are contained in our sample of stud- focused coping (including acceptance, seeking emo- ies. We therefore recommend to especially consider tional support or positive reinterpretation) as soon as effect sizes in future quantitative reviews. it becomes clear that the situation cannot be Similarly, the percentage of significant associations changed.13 Thus, in chronic pain conditions, between optimism and pain might be over-estimated optimism may not be helpful anymore to lower pain, if—due to publication bias—non-significant results but it can still be of benefit for functioning and were less likely to be published. well-being. Even if optimism is measured at an earlier time than the pain experience (e.g., pre-operatively), and even in studies with adequate control groups, one Limitations cannot be entirely sure about the causality of the opti- There are some limitations as to the generalizability of mism-pain relation: it is also conceivable that a gen- our conclusions and thus to the informative value of eral preparedness for experiencing and coping with this review. pain—be it due to biological or psychological predis- As mentioned, comorbidity and pain medication— positions or due to a sufficient “immunization” by two factors that strongly influence pain experience—have gradual exposure and subsequent adaptation to pain not been sufficiently documented and accounted for in a in the past—has, over the years, resulted in a high large part of the studies on patients. If they differ between level of dispositional optimism, which in turn will optimists and pessimists, results could be distorted. dampen future pain experiences. Thus, a reversed Besides, despite the high percentage of beneficial causality from little pain to high optimism cannot be associations, one must consider that within the same ruled out. In consequence, lower levels of optimism in study, the significance of these associations often clinical pain cohorts8,109 could either arise from the tended to disappear as soon as more complex statis- fact that less optimistic individuals are more likely to tical models such as multivariate or hierarchical linear develop pain or from the tendency of patients’ opti- regression analyses were computed. This might partly mism to be dampened as a result of their increased be due to the reduced statistical power of complex pain vulnerability, existing already before the develop- models integrating several different variables. It is also ment of the clinical pain condition. Only some studies conceivable, however, that optimism accounts for less preclude this latter possibility by controlling for base- incremental variance as soon as correlated variables line symptoms or, as far as experimental designs are are added to the model, i.e., there is no significant concerned, by manipulating optimism. Lastly, there unique contribution of optimism in predicting pain. could be third variables which substantially influence While there is evidence that the association of opti- both pain and optimism reports, as for example mism with pain is independent of affect39 and social response biases like a tendency towards positive state- desirability,93 several other factors are possibly corre- ments. In consequence, we cannot derive definitive lated with optimism. In some studies,50,52,83 optimism conclusions regarding causality from this review. The reached significance when entered alone or early in findings should cautiously be interpreted in terms of the model, but did not explain significant additional correlation. variance as soon as other variables (e.g., control and While the focus of this review was on pain experi- benefit appraisals, self-efficacy, social support) were ence, one must keep in mind that especially in chronic entered simultaneously or even before. It remains pain this is not the only relevant pain outcome factor. unclear whether any of these variables are mediators Even when optimism does not positively affect pain of the optimism-pain relation, i.e., whether optimism intensity in itself, optimists could still benefit in other works through these mechanisms, or only moderators. respects, as for example in adjustment to pain,110 In our descriptive analysis, we could not account for or goal-directed efforts.111 12 J. BASTEN-GUNTHER€ ET AL.

Given the heterogeneity of the retrieved studies in samples. Effect sizes were bigger when applying the terms of design and measures (especially pain meas- BPS compared to other optimism interventions (for ures112), it was not possible to perform quantitative example cognitive-behavioral techniques) and when analyses including all of them. The present work may, interventions were provided in person instead of however, be useful as a basis for future meta-analytic online. While therapeutic short-term effects have been evaluations as it provides an overview of the variety of shown to be very likely, evidence for long-term effects approaches and variables, which might be used to is still scarce. Therefore, clinical research should focus derive and answer more specific research questions. on how to preserve and stabilize the short-term opti- Lastly, while we were obliged to focus on those mism effect for longer action. Meevissen and research- moderating variables for which sufficient data were ers114 recently succeeded in creating longer-term provided, it would be interesting to examine the role changes in optimism in healthy individuals through of further demographic and clinical variables such as, an intensive optimism-fostering intervention. Another – for example, ethnic background or pain duration, as three studies115 117 that trained optimism by combin- soon as a critical number of studies will have ing the previously mentioned Best-Possible-Self become available. imagery and writing technique with other positive The review provides suggestions for plausible mecha- psychology-exercises, found increased optimism for nisms of the optimism-pain relation and likely moderat- up to six months and promising results on well-being ing variables. These require explicit testing in in chronic pain patients. This gives reason to hope future studies. that similar interventions may in future be used as The manipulation of optimism—by means of future part of the treatment of pain, possibly selectively in thinking exercises or semantic priming as, for individuals “at risk”, i.e., low in disposi- example, in Fosnaugh and colleagues21—is a chance tional optimism. to explain causality and to develop clinical interven- tions. So far, the Best-Possible-Self-technique seems to be the only one to have been applied in the Conclusions pain context. The present analysis gives reason for assuming a Although in some subdomains studies are still beneficial association of optimism with pain experi- missing (e.g., clinical studies applying an optimism ence. Studies with a presumed higher validity pro- manipulation), given the retrieved material and the vided a higher percentage of beneficial associations. number of studies with sufficiently homogenous out- Significant associations between optimism and pain come measures, we propose that the time is ripe for a were more frequently found in older participants. meta-analysis. It seems reasonable that if a significant association of optimism with pain exists, this associ- Further research is needed to illuminate causal rela- ation is more likely to be detected in studies with tions and to suggest evidence-based clinical applica- high study/publication quality, which is why future tions of optimism-fostering interventions. research should be especially concerned with the men- tioned quality criteria. Notes Furthermore, while our review was necessarily lim- ited on pain experience in the narrow sense, we rec- 1. In order to avoid confusions of the term “positive ommend enlarging future research to the previously result” which could be interpreted as either a positive mentioned other pain-related outcomes such as cogni- correlation between optimism and pain or as a protective association (i.e., a negative correlation) tive or emotional adjustment to pain or functional between optimism and pain, the term “beneficial disability. These might reveal differential relations association of optimism with pain” (referring to a with optimism and thus provide further interesting negative statistical correlation) will be used throughout insights into the optimism-pain research. the text. As shown in this review, optimism might be a 2. In the following, numbers of studies are referred to by powerful resilience factor against pain. Therefore, the k common in meta-analyses while numbers of enhancing optimism could help in reducing acute persons are designated by n. pain experience as well as in preventing the transition to chronic pain. A recent meta-analysis113 indicates that optimism can indeed be increased by psycho- Disclosure statement logical interventions in both clinical and healthy The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest. BEHAVIORAL MEDICINE 13

Funding Rev. 2006;10(3):235–251. doi: 10.1207/s15327957pspr 1003_3. This study was funded by Evangelisches Studienwerk Villigst. 14. Bartley E, Fillingim R. Sex differences in pain: a brief review of clinical and experimental findings. Br J Anaesth. 2013; 111(1):52–58. doi: 10.1093/bja/aet127. 15. Lautenbacher S, Peters J, Heesen M. Age changes in ORCID pain perception: a systematic-review and meta-ana- Johanna Basten-Gunther€ http://orcid.org/0000-0002- lysis of age effects on pain and tolerance thresholds. 5759-8563 Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2017;75:104–113. doi 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.01.039. 16. Sherman A, Walls J. Gender differences in the rela- tionship of moderator variables to stress and symp- References toms. Psychol Health. 1995;10(4):321–331. doi: 10.1080/08870449508400246. 1. Antonovsky A. Unraveling the Mystery of Health: 17. Spuling S, Wurm S, Tesch-Romer€ C, Huxhold O. How People Manage Stress and Stay Well. San Changing predictors of self-rated health: disentan- Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass; 1987. gling age and cohort effects. Psychol Aging. 2015; 2. Seligman M, Peterson C. Positive clinical psychology. 30(2):462. doi: 10.1037/a0039111. In: Aspinwall G, Staudinger U, eds. A Psychology of 18. Benyamini Y, Idler E, Leventhal H, Leventhal E. Human Strengths: Fundamental Questions and Positive affect and function as influences on self- Future Directions for a Positive Psychology. assessments of health: expanding our view beyond Washington, DC: American Psychological illness and disability. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. Association; 2003: 305–317. doi:10.1037/10566-021. 2000;55(2):P107–P116. doi: 10.1093/geronb/ 3. Pulvers K, Hood A. The role of positive traits and 55.2.P107. pain catastrophizing in pain perception. Curr Pain 19. French D, Sargent-Cox K, Luszcz M. Correlates of Headache Rep. 2013;17(5):330. doi:10.1007/s11916- subjective health across the aging lifespan: under- 013-0330-2l. standing self-rated health in the oldest old. J Aging 4. Finan P, Garland E. The role of positive affect in Health. 2012;24(8):1449–1469. doi:10.1177/ pain and its treatment. Clin J Pain. 2015;31(2): 177–187. doi: 10.1097/AJP.0000000000000092l. 0898264312461151. 5. Leeuw M, Goossens M, Linton S, Crombez G, 20. Scheier M, Carver C, Bridges M. Distinguishing opti- Boersma K, Vlaeyen J. The fear-avoidance model of mism from (and trait , self-mas- musculoskeletal pain: current state of scientific evi- tery, and self-esteem): a reevaluation of the life dence. J Behav Med. 2007;30(1):77–94. doi: 10.1007/ orientation test. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1994;67(6): – s10865-006-9085-0. 1063 1078. 6. Scheier M, Carver C. Optimism, coping, and health: 21. Fosnaugh J, Geers A, Wellman J. Giving off a rosy assessment and implications of generalized outcome glow: the manipulation of an optimistic orientation. – expectancies. Health Psychol. 1985;4(3):219–247. doi: J Soc Psychol. 2009;149(3):349 364. doi:10.3200/ 10.1037/0278-6133.4.3.219. SOCP.149.3.349-364. 7. Carver C, Scheier M. Attention and Self-Regulation: 22. Peters M, Flink I, Boersma K, Linton S. A Control-Theory Approach to Human Behavior. Manipulating optimism: can imagining a best pos- New York, NY: Springer Publishing Company; 1981. sible self be used to increase positive future expect- – 8. Garofalo J. Perceived optimism and chronic pain. In: ancies? J Posit Psychol. 2010;5(3):204 211. doi: Gatchel R, Weisberg J, eds. Personality 10.1080/17439761003790963. Characteristics of Patients with Pain. Washington, 23. Hanssen M, Peters M, Vlaeyen J, Meevissen Y, DC: American Psychological Association; 2000: Vancleef L. Optimism lowers pain: evidence of the 203–217. doi:10.1037/10376-009. causal status and underlying mechanisms. Pain. 9. Goodin B, Bulls H. Optimism and the experience of 2013;154(1):53–58. doi:10.1016/j.pain.2012.08.006. pain: benefits of seeing the glass as half full. Curr 24. King L. The health benefits of writing about life Pain Headache Rep. 2013;17(5):329. doi:10.1007/ goals. Pers Soc Psychol Bull. 2001;27(7):798–807. doi: s11916-013-0329-8. 10.1177/0146167201277003. 10. Atkinson JW. Motivational determinants of risk-tak- 25. Sheldon K, Lyubomirsky S. How to increase and sus- ing behavior. Psychol Rev. 1957;64(6, Pt.1):359–372. tain positive emotion: the effects of expressing grati- doi:10.1037/h0043445. tude and visualizing best possible selves. J Posit 11. Carver C, Scheier M, Segerstrom S. Optimism. Clin Psychol. 2006;1(2):73–82. doi: 10.1080/ Psychol Rev. 2010;30(7):879–889. doi:10.1016/j.cpr. 17439760500510676. 2010.01.006. 26. MacLeod A. Affect, emotional disorder, and future- 12. Avvenuti G, Baiardini I, Giardini A. Optimism’s directed thinking. Cogn Emot. 1996;10(1):69–86. doi: explicative role for chronic diseases. Front Psychol. 10.1080/026999396380394. 2016;7:1–8. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00295. 27. Luthans F. The need for and meaning of positive 13. Solberg Nes L, Segerstrom S. Dispositional optimism organizational behavior. J Organiz Behav. 2002;23(6): and coping: a meta-analytic review. Pers Soc Psychol 695–706. 2002; doi: 10.1002/job.165. 14 J. BASTEN-GUNTHER€ ET AL.

28. Kluemper D, Little L, DeGroot T. State or trait: 061120070736008–061120070736529. doi:10.1111/ effects of state optimism on job-related outcomes. j.1600-0838.2006.00601.x. J Organiz Behav. 2009;30(2):209–231. doi:10.1002/ 42. Bruce J, Thornton A, Scott N, et al. Chronic pre- job.591. operative pain and psychological robustness predict 29. Moher D, Liberati A, Tetzlaff J, Altman D. Prisma acute postoperative pain outcomes after surgery for Group. Preferred reporting items for systematic breast cancer. Br J Cancer. 2012;107(6):937–946. doi: reviews and meta-analyses: the PRISMA statement. 10.1038/bjc.2012.341. PLoS Med. 2009;6(7):e1000097. doi:10.1371/ 43. Bruce J, Thornton A, Powell R, et al. Psychological, journal.pmed.1000097. surgical, and sociodemographic predictors of pain 30. Stewart D, Yuen T. A systematic review of resilience outcomes after breast cancer surgery: a population- in the physically ill. Psychosomatics 2011;52(3): based cohort study. Pain. 2014;155(2):232–243. doi: 199–209. doi:10.1016/j.psym.2011.01.036. 10.1016/j.pain.2013.09.028. 31. Ghio D, Thomson W, Calam R, et al. The prioritiza- 44. Callahan C. Stress, coping, and personality hardiness tion of symptom beliefs over illness beliefs: the in patients with temporomandibular disorders. development and validation of the Pain Perception Rehabil Psychol. 2000;45(1):38–48. doi:10.1037/0090- Questionnaire for Young People. Br J Health Psychol. 5550.45.1.38. – 2018;23(1):68 87. doi:10.1111/bjhp.12275. 45. Chamberlain K, Petrie K, Azariah R. The role of ’ 32. Wells G, Shea B, O Connell D, et al. The Newcastle- optimism and sense of coherence in predicting Ottawa Scale (NOS) for assessing the quality of non- recovery following surgery. Psychol Health. 1992; randomized studies in meta-analyses. Available from: 7(4):301–310. doi:10.1080/08870449208403159. http://www.ohri.ca/programs/clinical_epidemiology/ 46. Chrisler J, Rose J, Dutch S, Sklarsky K, Grant M. oxford.asp. Accessed May 12, 2017. The PMS illusion: social cognition maintains social 33. Erikson E. (1968). Identity: Youth and Crisis. New construction. Sex Roles. 2006;54(5-6):371–376. doi: York, NY: Norton. 10.1007/s11199-006-9005-3. 34. Achat H, Kawachi I, Spiro A, DeMolles D, Sparrow 47. Coronado R, Simon C, Lentz T, Gay C, Mackie L, D. Optimism and depression as predictors of phys- George S. Optimism moderates the influence of pain ical and mental health functioning: the normative catastrophizing on shoulder pain outcome: a longitu- aging study. Ann Behav Med. 2000;22(2):127–130. dinal analysis. J Orthop Sports Phys Ther. 2017;47(1): doi:10.1007/BF02895776. 21–30. doi 10.2519/jospt.2017.7068. 35. Airila A, Hakanen J, Luukkonen R, Lusa S, 48. Ferreira V, Sherman A. The relationship of opti- Punakallio A, Leino-Arjas P. Developmental trajecto- mism, pain and social support to well-being in older ries of multisite musculoskeletal pain and depressive adults with osteoarthritis. Aging Ment Health. 2007; symptoms: the effects of job demands and resources 11(1):89–98. doi:10.1080/13607860600736166. and individual factors. Psychol Health. 2014;29(12): 49. Fishbain D, Turner D, Rosomoff H, Rosomoff R. 1421–1441. doi:10.1080/08870446.2014.945929. Millon behavioral health inventory scores of patients 36. Allison P, Guichard C, Gilain L. A prospective inves- with chronic pain associated with myofascial pain tigation of dispositional optimism as a predictor of – health-related quality of life in head and neck cancer syndrome. Pain Med. 2001;2(4):328 335. doi: patients. Qual Life Res. 2000;9(8):951–960. doi: 10.1046/j.1526-4637.2001.01040.x. 10.1023/A:1008931906253. 50. Fitzgerald T, Tennen H, Affleck G, Pransky G. The 37. Bediako S, Neblett E. Optimism and perceived stress relative importance of dispositional optimism and in sickle-cell disease: the role of an afrocultural social control appraisals in quality of life after coronary ethos. J Black Psychol. 2011;37(2):234–253. doi: artery bypass surgery. J Behav Med. 1993;16(1): – 10.1177/0095798410385681. 25 43. doi:10.1007/BF00844753. 38. Bentsen S, Rustøen T, Wahl A, Miaskowski C. The 51. Gramke H, Rijke J, van Kleef M, et al. Predictive fac- pain experience and future expectations of chronic tors of postoperative pain after day-case surgery. – low back pain patients following spinal fusion. J Clin Clin J Pain. 2009;25(6):455 460. doi:10.1097/ Nurs. 2008;17(7B):153–159. doi:10.1111/j.1365- AJP.0b013e31819a6e34. 2702.2007.02234.x. 52. Hetmann F, Kongsgaard U, Sandvik L, Schou-Bredal 39. Benyamini Y. Can high optimism and high pessim- I. Prevalence and predictors of persistent post-surgi- ism co-exist?: findings from arthritis patients coping cal pain 12 months after thoracotomy. Acta with pain. Pers Individ Dif. 2005;38(6):1463–1473. Anaesthesiol Scand. 2015; 59(6):740–748. doi: doi:10.1016/j.paid.2004.09.020. 10.1111/aas.12532. 40. Booth-Kewley S, Schmied E, Highfill-McRoy R, 53. Hoofwijk D, Fiddelers A, Peters M, et al. Prevalence Sander T, Blivin S, Garland C. A prospective study and predictive factors of chronic postsurgical pain of factors affecting recovery from musculoskeletal and poor global recovery 1 year after outpatient sur- injuries. J Occup Rehabil. 2014;24(2):287–296. doi: gery. Clin J Pain. 2015;31(12):1017–1025. doi: 10.1007/s10926-013-9456-7. 10.1097/AJP.0000000000000207. 41. Brewer B, Cornelius A, Sklar J, et al. Pain and 54. Katz L, Tripp D, Ropeleski M, et al. Mechanisms of negative mood during rehabilitation after anterior quality of life and social support in inflammatory cruciate ligament reconstruction: a daily process bowel disease. J Clin Psychol Med Settings. 2016; analysis. Scand J Med Sci Sports. 2006;0(0): 23(1):88–98. doi:10.1007/s10880-015-9431-x. BEHAVIORAL MEDICINE 15

55. Kurtz M, Kurtz J, Given C, Given B. Patient opti- anxiety following primary total hip and knee arthro- mism and mastery—do they play a role in cancer plasty. J Pain. 2013;14(5):502–515. doi:10.1016/ patients’ management of pain and fatigue? J Pain j.jpain.2012.12.020. Symptom Manage. 2008;36(1):1–10. doi:10.1016/ 69. Powell R, Johnston M, Smith W, et al. Psychological j.jpainsymman.2007.08.010. risk factors for chronic post-surgical pain after 56. Lam W, Ye M, Fielding R. Trajectories of quality of inguinal hernia repair surgery: A prospective cohort life among Chinese patients diagnosed with naso- study. EJP. 2012; 16(4):600–610. doi:10.1016/ pharynegeal cancer. PloS One. 2012;7(9):e44022. doi: j.ejpain.2011.08.010. 10.1371/journal.pone.0044022. 70. Pulgar A, Alcala A, Reyes Del Paso G. Psychosocial 57. Langbach O, Bukholm I, Benth J, Røkke O. Long- predictors of quality of life in hematological cancer. term quality of life and functionality after ventral Behav Med. 2015;41(1):1–8. doi:10.1080/ hernia mesh repair. Surg Endosc. 2016;30(11): 08964289.2013.833083. 5023–5033. doi:10.1007/s00464-016-4850-9. 71. Ramırez-Maestre C, Esteve R, Lopez A. The role of 58. Lau B, Knardahl S. Perceived job insecurity, job pre- optimism and pessimism in chronic pain, patients dictability, personality, and health. J Occup Environ adjustment. Span J Psychol. 2012;15(01):286–294. Med. 2008;50(2):172–181. doi:10.1097/ doi:10.5209/rev_SJOP.2012.v15.n1.37335. JOM.0b013e31815c89a1. 72. Ronaldson A, Poole L, Kidd T, Leigh E, Jahangiri M, 59. Long B, Sangster J. Dispositional optimism/pessim- Steptoe A. Optimism measured pre-operatively is ism and coping strategies: predictors of psychosocial associated with reduced pain intensity and physical adjustment of rheumatoid and osteoarthritis patients. symptom reporting after coronary artery bypass graft J Appl Social Pyschol. 1993;23(13):1069–1091. doi: surgery. J Psychosom Res. 2014;77(4):278–282. doi: 10.1111/j.1559-1816.1993.tb01022.x. 10.1016/j.jpsychores.2014.07.018. 60. Mahler H, Kulik J. Optimism, pessimism and recov- 73. Rosenberger P, Kerns R, Jokl P, Ickovics J. Mood ery from coronary bypass surgery: prediction of and attitude predict pain outcomes following arthro- affect, pain and functional status. Psychol Health scopic knee surgery. Ann Behav Med. 2009;37(1): Med. 2000;5(4):347–358. doi:10.1080/713690216. 70–76. doi:10.1007/s12160-008-9078-z. 61. Mueller A, Hartmann M, Mueller K, Eich W. 74. Saariaho T, Saariaho A, Karila I, Joukamaa M. Early Validation of the arthritis self-efficacy short-form maladaptive schemas in Finnish adult chronic pain scale in German fibromyalgia patients. Eur J Pain. patients and a control sample. Scand J Psychol. 2011; 2003;7(2):163–171. doi:10.1016/S1090-3801(02)000 52(2):146–153. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9450.2010.00849.x. 97-6. 75. Sherman A, Cotter K. Well-being among older 62. Pence L, Valrie C, Gil K, Redding-Lallinger R, adults with OA: direct and mediated patterns of con- Daeschner C. Optimism predicting daily pain medi- trol beliefs, optimism and pessimism. Aging Ment cation use in adolescents with sickle cell disease. J Health. 2013;17(5):595–608. doi:10.1080/ Pain Symptom Manage. 2007;33(3):302–309. doi: 13607863.2013.765831. 10.1016/j.jpainsymman.2006.08.010. 76. Singh JA, O’Byrne MM, Colligan RC, Lewallen DG. 63. Peters M, Sommer M, Rijke J, et al. Somatic and Pessimistic : a psychological risk psychologic predictors of long-term unfavorable out- factor for poor pain and functional outcomes two come after surgical intervention. Ann Surg. 2007; years after knee replacement. J Bone Joint Surg Br. 245(3):487–494. doi:10.1097/01.sla.0000245495.79781. 2010; 92-B(6):799–806. doi:10.1302/0301- 65. 620X.92B6.23114. 64. Peters M, Sommer M, van Kleef M, Marcus M. 77. Sipil€aK,Ylostalo€ P, Ek E, Zitting P, Knuuttila M. Predictors of physical and emotional recovery 6 and Association between optimism and self-reported 12 months after surgery. Br J Surg. 2010;97(10): facial pain. Acta Odontol Scand. 2006; 64(3):177–182. 1518–1527. doi:10.1002/bjs.7152. doi:10.1080/00016350500514816. 65. Pinto P, McIntyre T, Araujo-Soares V, Costa P, 78. Smith B, Zautra A. Vulnerability and resilience in Almeida A. Differential predictors of acute post-sur- women with arthritis: test of a two-factor model. J gical pain intensity after abdominal hysterectomy Consult Clin Psychol. 2008;76(5):799–810. doi: and major joint arthroplasty. Ann Behav Med. 2015; 10.1037/0022-006X.76.5.799. 49(3):384–397. doi:10.1007/s12160-014-9662-3. 79. Soderlund€ A, Lindberg P. Long-term functional and 66. Pinto P, McIntyre T, Araujo-Soares V, Costa P, psychological problems in whiplash associated disor- Ferrero R, Almeida A. A comparison of predictors ders. Int J Rehabil Res. 1999;22(2):77–84. https:// and intensity of acute postsurgical pain in patients www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/10448618. Accessed: undergoing total hip and knee arthroplasty. Jpr. July 10, 2017. 2017;10:1087. doi:10.2147/JPR.S126467. 80. Sorbi MJ, Peters ML, Kruise DA, et al. Electronic 67. Pinto P, McIntyre T, Araujo-Soares V, Costa P, momentary assessment in chronic pain i: psycho- Ferrero R, Almeida A. The role of pain catastrophiz- logical pain responses as predictors of pain intensity. ing in the provision of rescue analgesia by health Clin J Pain. 2006; 22(1):55–66. doi:10.1097/ care providers following major joint arthroplasty. 01.ajp.0000148624.46756.fa. Pain Physician 2014;17(6):515–524. 81. Stessel B, Fiddelers A, Marcus M, et al. External val- 68. Pinto P, McIntyre T, Ferrero R, Almeida A, Araujo- idation and modification of a predictive model for Soares V. Predictors of acute postsurgical pain and acute postsurgical pain at home after day surgery. 16 J. BASTEN-GUNTHER€ ET AL.

Clin J Pain. 2017;33(5):405–413. doi:10.1097/ 95. Hanssen M, Vancleef L, Vlaeyen J, Peters M. More AJP.0000000000000413. optimism, less pain! The influence of generalized 82. Su N, Lobbezoo F, Wijk A, Heijden G, Visscher C. and pain-specific expectations on experienced cold- Associations of pain intensity and pain-related dis- pressor pain. J Behav Med. 2014;37(1):47–58. doi: ability with psychological and socio-demographic 10.1007/s10865-012-9463-8. factors in patients with temporomandibular disor- 96. Hood A, Pulvers K, Carrillo J, Merchant G, Thomas ders: a cross-sectional study at a specialised dental M. Positive traits linked to less pain through lower clinic. J Oral Rehabil. 2017;44(3):187–196. doi: pain catastrophizing. Pers Individ Dif. 2012;52(3): 10.1111/joor.12479. 401–405. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2011.10.040. 83. Tennen H, Affleck G, Urrows S, Higgins P, Mendola 97. Morton D, Watson A, El-Deredy W, Jones A. R. Perceiving control, construing benefits, and daily Reproducibility of placebo analgesia: effect of dispo- processes in rheumatoid arthritis. Can J Behav Sci. sitional optimism. Pain. 2009;146(1):194–198. doi: 1992;24(2):186–203. doi:10.1037/h0078709. 10.1016/j.pain.2009.07.026. 84. Treharne G, Kitas G, Lyons A, Booth D. Well-being 98. Smith B, Tooley E, Montague E, Robinson A, in rheumatoid arthritis: the effects of disease dur- Cosper C, Mullins P. The role of resilience and pur- ation and psychosocial factors. J Health Psychol. pose in life in habituation to heat and cold pain. J 2005;10(3):457–474. doi:10.1177/1359105305051416. Pain. 2009; 10(5):493–500. doi:10.1016/ 85. Tsakogia Z, Lyrakos G, Damigos D, Mayreas V, j.jpain.2008.11.007. Dimoliatis I. The effect of dispositional optimism in 99. Snyder C, Berg C, Woodward J, et al. Hope against HRQOL in patients with chronic musculoskeletal the cold: individual differences in trait hope and pain conditions in Greece. Appl Res Q Life. 2011; acute pain tolerance on the cold pressor task. J 6(1):53–70. Doi:10.1007/s11482-010-9113-7. Personality. 2005;73(2):287–312. doi:10.1111/j.1467- 86. Wiesmann U, Dezutter J, Hannich H. Sense of 6494.2005.00318.x. coherence and pain experience in older age. Int 100. Costello N, Bragdon E, Light K, et al. Psychogeriatr. 2014;26(1):123–133. doi:10.1017/ Temporomandibular disorder and optimism: rela- S1041610213001695. tionships to ischemic pain sensitivity and interleu- 87. Wong W, Fielding R. Quality of life and pain in kin-6. Pain. 2002;100(1):99–110. doi:10.1016/S0304- Chinese lung cancer patients: is optimism a moder- 3959(02)00263-4. ator or mediator? Qual Life Res. 2007;16(1):53–63. 101. Goodin B, Glover T, Sotolongo A, et al. The associ- doi:10.1007/s11136-006-9106-z. ation of greater dispositional optimism with less 88. Wright M, Wren A, Somers T, et al. Pain accept- endogenous pain facilitation is indirectly transmitted ance, hope, and optimism: relationships to pain and through lower levels of pain catastrophizing. J Pain. adjustment in patients with chronic musculoskeletal 2013;14(2):126–135. doi:10.1016/j.jpain.2012.10.007. pain. J Pain. 2011;12(11):1155–1162. doi:10.1016/ 102. Jylh€a M, Leskinen E, Alanen E, Leskinen A, j.jpain.2011.06.002. Heikkinen E. Self-rated health and associated factors 89. Boselie J, Vancleef L, Smeets T, Peters M. Increasing among men of different ages. J Gerontol. 1986;41(6): optimism abolishes pain-induced impairments in 710–717. doi:10.1093/geronj/41.6.710. executive task performance. Pain. 2014;155(2): 103. Idler E, Benyamini Y. Self-rated health and mortal- 334–340. doi:10.1016/j.pain.2013.10.014. ity: a review of twenty-seven community studies. J 90. Corsi N, Colloca L. Placebo and nocebo effects: the Health Soc Behav. 1997;38(1):21–37. advantage of measuring expectations and psycho- 104. Giltay E, Geleijnse J, Zitman F, Hoekstra T, logical factors. Front Psychol. 2017;8:308. doi: Schouten E. Dispositional optimism and all-cause 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00308. and cardiovascular mortality in a prospective cohort 91. Dimova V, Oertel B, Kabakci G, et al. A more pes- of elderly Dutch men and women. Arch Gen simistic life orientation is associated with experimen- Psychiatry. 2004;61(11):1126–1135. doi:10.1001/ tal inducibility of a neuropathy-like pain pattern in archpsyc.61.11.1126. healthy individuals. J Pain. 2015;16(8):791–800. doi: 105. Peterson C, Seligman M, Yurko K, Martin L, 10.1016/j.jpain.2015.05.004. Friedman H. Catastrophizing and untimely death. 92. Geers A, Wellman J, Fowler S, Helfer S, France C. Psychol Sci. 1998;9(2):127–130. doi:10.1111/1467- Dispositional optimism predicts placebo analgesia. J 9280.00023. Pain. 2010; 11(11):1165–1171. doi:10.1016/ 106. Peters M, Vieler J, Lautenbacher S. Dispositional and j.jpain.2010.02.014. induced optimism lead to attentional preference for 93. Geers A, Wellman J, Helfer S, Fowler S, France C. faces displaying positive : an eye-tracker Dispositional optimism and thoughts of well-being study. J Posit Psychol. 2016;11(3):258–269. doi: determine sensitivity to an experimental pain task. 10.1080/17439760.2015.1048816. Ann Behav Med. 2008;36(3):304–313. doi:10.1007/ 107. Luo J, Isaacowitz D. How optimists face skin cancer s12160-008-9073-4. information: risk assessment, attention, memory, and 94. Goodin B, Kronfli T, King C, Glover T, Sibille K, behavior. Psychol Health. 2007;22(8):963–984. doi: Fillingim R. Testing the relation between disposi- 10.1080/14768320601070951. tional optimism and conditioned pain modulation: 108. Esteve R, Ramırez-Maestre C, Lopez-Mart ınez A. does ethnicity matter? J Behav Med. 2013;36(2): Adjustment to chronic pain: the role of pain accept- 165–174. doi:10.1007/s10865-012-9411-7. ance, coping strategies, and pain-related cognitions. BEHAVIORAL MEDICINE 17

Ann Behav Med. 2007;33(2):179–188. doi:10.1007/ 113. Malouff J, Schutte N. Can psychological interven- BF02879899. tions increase optimism? a meta-analysis. J Posit 109. Turk D, Holzman A. Commonalities among psycho- Psychol. 2017;12(6):594–604. logical approaches in the treatment of chronic pain: 114. Meevissen Y, Peters M, Alberts H. Become more specifying the meta-constructs. In: Holzman A, Turk optimistic by imagining a best possible self: effects of D, eds. Pain Management: A Handbook of a two week intervention. J Behav Ther Exp Psychological Treatment Approaches. Elmsford, NY: Psychiatry. 2011;42(3):371–378. doi:10.1016/ Pergamon; 1986:257–267. j.jbtep.2011.02.012. 110. Carver C, Pozo-Kaderman C, Harris S, et al. 115. Flink I, Smeets E, Bergbom S, Peters M. Happy des- Optimism versus pessimism predicts the quality of pite pain: pilot study of a positive psychology inter- women’s adjustment to early stage breast cancer. vention for patients with chronic pain. Scand J Pain. Cancer. 1994;73(4):1213–1220. doi:10.1002/1097- 2015;7(1):71–79. doi:10.1016/j.sjpain.2015.01.005. 0142(19940215)73:4 < 1213::AID- 116. Peters M, Smeets E, Feijge M, et al. Happy despite CNCR2820730415 > 3.0.CO;2-Q. pain: a randomized controlled trial of an 8-week 111. Affleck G, Tennen H, Zautra A, Urrows S, Abeles internet-delivered positive psychology intervention M, Karoly P. Women’s pursuit of personal goals in for enhancing well-being in patients with chronic daily life with fibromyalgia: a value-expectancy ana- pain. Clin J Pain. 2017; 33(11):962–975. doi:10.1097/ lysis. J Consult Clin Psychol. 2001;69(4):587. doi: AJP.0000000000000494. 10.1037/0022-006X.69.4.587. 117. Boselie J, Vancleef L, Peters M. Filling the glass: 112. Deyo R, Battie M, Beurskens A, et al. Outcome effects of a positive psychology intervention on measures for low back pain research: a proposal for executive task performance in chronic pain patients. standardized use. Spine 1998;23(18):2003–2013. doi: Eur J Pain. 2018;22(7):1268–1280. doi:10.1002/ 10.1097/00007632-199809150-00018. ejp.1214.