Latent Emotion Memory for Multi-Label Emotion Classification

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Latent Emotion Memory for Multi-Label Emotion Classification The Thirty-Fourth AAAI Conference on Artificial Intelligence (AAAI-20) Latent Emotion Memory for Multi-Label Emotion Classification Hao Fei,1 Yue Zhang,2 Yafeng Ren,3∗ Donghong Ji1∗ 1Key Laboratory of Aerospace Information Security and Trusted Computing, Ministry of Education, School of Cyber Science and Engineering, Wuhan University, Wuhan, China 2School of Engineering, Westlake University, Hangzhou, China 3Guangdong Collaborative Innovation Center for Language Research & Services, Guangdong University of Foreign Studies, Guangzhou, China {hao.fei, renyafeng, dhji}@whu.edu.cn [email protected] Abstract Identifying multiple emotions in a sentence is an important research topic. Existing methods usually model the problem as multi-label classification task. However, previous methods have two issues, limiting the performance of the task. First, these models do not consider prior emotion distribution in a sentence. Second, they fail to effectively capture the context information closely related to the corresponding emotion. In this paper, we propose a Latent Emotion Memory network (LEM) for multi-label emotion classification. The proposed model can learn the latent emotion distribution without exter- nal knowledge, and can effectively leverage it into the clas- sification network. Experimental results on two benchmark datasets show that the proposed model outperforms strong baselines, achieving the state-of-the-art performance. Figure 1: Multiple emotions with different intensities. Introduction Emotion classification is an important task in natural lan- and fail to consider prior emotion distribution in a sentence. guage processing (NLP). Automatically inferencing the Intuitively, different emotion in a sentence has different in- emotions is the initial step for downstream applications such tensity. Figure 1 illustrates the emotion distribution of sen- as emotional chatbots (Zhou et al. 2018), stock market pre- tence S1, where there are four different emotions with differ- diction (Nguyen, Shirai, and Velcin 2015), policy studies ent intensities. In particular, the emotions anticipation and (Bermingham and Smeaton 2011), etc. However, it is com- pessimism receive higher intensities than disgust and love. mon that there exist more than one emotion in a piece of text. Emotion labels with higher intensity should deserve higher Intuitively, people tend to express multiple emotions in one probabilities at the final prediction for the model. 2) Previ- piece of text. Taking the following sentences as example: ous work does not effectively capture the context informa- (S1) How’s the new Batman Telltale Series? Looks good tion closely related to the corresponding emotion, which is but I’m growing weary of this gaming style. crucial for the prediction. In sentence S2, the clues indicat- (S2) It really is amazing in the worst ways. It was very ing the sadness emotion, ‘worst’, are scattered broadly, and hard to stifle my laughter after I overheard this comment. surrounded by the words ‘laughter’ and ‘amazing’ that sup- In sentence S1, multiple emotions are conveyed, includ- port the joy emotion. Correct predictions can be made only ing anticipation, disgust, love and pessimism. Sentence S2 when these features can be sufficiently mined and properly contains two emotions: joy and sadness. How to identify the selected. If we can sufficiently capture effective features for multiple co-existing emotions in a sentence remains a chal- each emotion, the final prediction will be relatively easy. lenging task. This requires a strong ability of the model on features ex- There has been work considering multi-label emotion traction. classification (He and Xia 2018; Almeida et al. 2018; Yu et To address these issues, we propose a Latent Emotion al. 2018). However, there still are two limitations. 1) They Memory network (LEM) for multi-label emotion classifica- assume each emotion occurs with equal prior probability, tion. LEM consists of two main components: a latent emo- ∗Corresponding author tion module and a memory module, which are shown in Copyright c 2020, Association for the Advancement of Artificial Figure 2. First, the latent emotion module learns emotions Intelligence (www.aaai.org). All rights reserved. distribution by reconstructing the input via variational au- 7692 improve the performance of multi-label emotion classifica- tion. However, these methods do not consider the prior emo- tion distribution information in a sentence. Our work is related to the work proposed by Zhou et al. (2016). They proposed an emotion distribution learning (EDL) method, which first learned the relations between emotions based on the theory of Plutchik’s wheel of emo- tions (Plutchik 1980), and then conducted multi-label emo- tion classification by incorporating these label relations into the cost function (Zhou et al. 2016a). Nevertheless, our method differs from theirs in three aspects: 1) our model ef- fectively learns the emotion distribution, which is free from the restraint of any theory. 2) the emotion intensities distri- bution is automatically captured during the reconstruction of inputs in VAE model. 3) multi-hop memory module ensures that each emotion makes full use of context information for the corresponding emotion. Figure 2: The basic unit of LEM, including the latent emo- Variational Models Our proposed method is also related tion module and the memory module with one hop. Wled is to work on variational models in NLP applications. Bow- the latent emotion distribution embedding. Wef is the emo- tion feature embedding, also used as memory representation man et al. (2015) introduced a RNN-based VAE model for memory module. for generating diverse and coherent sentences. Miao et al. (2016) proposed a neural variational framework incorporat- ing multilayer perceptrons (MLP), CNN and RNN for gen- toencoder. Second, the memory module captures emotion- erative models of text. Bahuleyan et al. (2017) proposed an related features for the corresponding emotion. Finally, the attention-based variational seq2seq model for alleviating the feature representation from the memory module concate- attention bypassing effect. Different from the above meth- nated with emotion distribution representation from the la- ods, we first employ the VAE model to make reconstruction tent emotion module is fed into a bi-directional Gated Re- for original input, and then make use of the intermediate la- current Unit (BiGRU) to make prediction. tent representation as a prior emotion distribution informa- All the components are trained jointly in a supervised end- tion for facilitating downstream prediction. to-end learning: the latent variable representation from the latent emotion module guides the prediction of the mem- Method ory module, and the emotion memory module in return en- The proposed model consists of two main components: a la- courages the latent emotion module to better learn the emo- tent emotion module and a memory module. The mechanism tion distribution through back-propagation. Our model can of the basic unit of LEM is shown in Figure 2. learn latent emotion distribution information without exter- nal knowledge, effectively leveraging it into the classifica- Latent Emotion Module tion network. Since we cannot measure the emotion distribution explic- We conduct experiments on the SemEval 2018 task 1C itly, we model it as a set of latent variables. We employ English dataset and the Ren-CECps Chinese dataset. Experi- variational autoencoder (VAE) to learn the latent multino- mental results show that our model outperforms strong base- mial distribution representation Z during the reconstruction lines, achieving the state-of-the-art performance. of the input. Related Work Encoding The input of latent emotion module is emotion- Multi-label Emotion Classification Emotion detection BoW (eBoW) features of the sentence. Before being fed has been extensively researched in recent years (Ren et al. into VAE, the BoW features are preprocessed so that stop- 2017; Tang et al. 2019). Existing work mainly includes words or meaningless words are excluded from the vocabu- lexicon-based methods (Wang and Pal 2015), graphical lary. The reasons are two fold: 1) our target is to capture the model-based methods (Li et al. 2015) and linear classifier- emotion distribution, and the latent representation should be based methods (Quan et al. 2015). More recently, various emotion-rich rather than general semantic meaning. 2) re- neural networks models have been proposed for this task, ducing the size of BoW vocabulary is beneficial to the train- achieving highly competitive results on several benchmark ing of VAE. The encoder fe(·) consists of multiple non-linear hidden datasets (Ren et al. 2016; Felbo et al. 2017; Baziotis et al. L layers, transforming the input XeBoW ∈ R (L is the max 2018; He and Xia 2018). For example, Wang et al. (2016) μ σ employed the TDNN framework by constructing a convo- length of feature sequence) into prior parameters and : lutional neural network (CNN) for multiclass classification. μ = fe,μ(XeBoW ), (1) Yu et al. (2018) proposed a transfer learning architecture to logσ = fe,σ(XeBoW ). 7693 We define (Bowman et al. 2015) an emotion latent variable Z = μ + σ · , where is Gaussian noise variable sampled from N (0, 1), Z ∈ RK (K denotes the number of emotion labels). Then, the variable is normalized: Z = softmax(Z ). (2) Correspondingly, the latent emotion distribution p(ek|,k = 1, ··· ,K) is reflected in Z. Decoding Variational inference is used to approximate a posterior distribution over Z. We first use a linear hidden layer fled(·) to transform Z into embeddings: led R = fled(Z; Wled), (3) K×E where Wled ∈ R led (Eled is the corresponding embed- ding dimension) is the learned embedding of the latent emo- Figure 3: The overall framework of the LEM model. The tion distribution. This embedding will later be used to guide memory module are private for each k-th emotion. The la- the feature learning of memory module, and to control the tent emotion module is shared globally. overall prediction of emotions. A good decoder can learn a bunch of high level represen- tation of rich emotion features during the process of recon- Emotion Memory Module structing data.
Recommended publications
  • Bad Is Stronger Than Good
    Review of General Psychology Copyright 2001 by the Educational Publishing Foundation 2001. Vol. 5. No. 4. 323-370 1089-2680/O1/S5.O0 DOI: 10.1037//1089-2680.5.4.323 Bad Is Stronger Than Good Roy F. Baumeister and Ellen Bratslavsky Catrin Finkenauer Case Western Reserve University Free University of Amsterdam Kathleen D. Vohs Case Western Reserve University The greater power of bad events over good ones is found in everyday events, major life events (e.g., trauma), close relationship outcomes, social network patterns, interper- sonal interactions, and learning processes. Bad emotions, bad parents, and bad feedback have more impact than good ones, and bad information is processed more thoroughly than good. The self is more motivated to avoid bad self-definitions than to pursue good ones. Bad impressions and bad stereotypes are quicker to form and more resistant to disconfirmation than good ones. Various explanations such as diagnosticity and sa- lience help explain some findings, but the greater power of bad events is still found when such variables are controlled. Hardly any exceptions (indicating greater power of good) can be found. Taken together, these findings suggest that bad is stronger than good, as a general principle across a broad range of psychological phenomena. Centuries of literary efforts and religious pothesis that bad is stronger than good (see also thought have depicted human life in terms of a Rozin & Royzman, in press). That is, events struggle between good and bad forces. At the that are negatively valenced (e.g., losing metaphysical level, evil gods or devils are the money, being abandoned by friends, and receiv- opponents of the divine forces of creation and ing criticism) will have a greater impact on the harmony.
    [Show full text]
  • Emotion Detection in Twitter Posts: a Rule-Based Algorithm for Annotated Data Acquisition
    2020 International Conference on Computational Science and Computational Intelligence (CSCI) Emotion detection in Twitter posts: a rule-based algorithm for annotated data acquisition Maria Krommyda Anastatios Rigos Institute of Communication and Computer Systems Institute of Communication and Computer Systems Athens, Greece Athens, Greece [email protected] [email protected] Kostas Bouklas Angelos Amditis Institute of Communication and Computer Systems Institute of Communication and Computer Systems Athens, Greece Athens, Greece [email protected] [email protected] Abstract—Social media analysis plays a key role to the person that provided the text and it is categorized as positive, understanding of the public’s opinion regarding recent events negative, or neutral. Such analysis is mainly used for movies, and decisions, to the design and management of advertising products and persons, when measuring their appeal to the campaigns as well as to the planning of next steps and mitigation actions for public relationship initiatives. Significant effort has public is of interest [4] and require extended quantities of text been dedicated recently to the development of data analysis collected over a long period of time from different sources to algorithms that will perform, in an automated way, sentiment ensure an unbiased and objective output. analysis over publicly available text. Most of the available While this technique is very popular and well established, research work, focuses on binary categorizing text as positive or with many important use cases and applications, there are negative without further investigating the emotions leading to that categorization. The current needs, however, for in-depth analysis other equally important use cases where such analysis fail to of the available content combined with the complexity and multi- provided the needed information.
    [Show full text]
  • QUESTION 20 Despair We Next Have to Consider the Vices Opposed to Hope
    QUESTION 20 Despair We next have to consider the vices opposed to hope: first, despair (desperatio) (question 20) and, second, presumption (praesumptio) (question 21). On the first topic there are four questions: (1) Is despair a sin? (2) Can despair exist without unbelief? (3) Is despair the greatest of sins? (4) Does despair arise from listlessness (acedia)? Article 1 Is despair a sin? It seems that despair is not a sin (desperatio not sit peccatum): Objection 1: As is clear from Augustine in De Libero Arbitrio, every sin involves turning toward some changeable good, combined with turning away from the unchangeable good. But despair does not involve turning toward any changeable good. Therefore, it is not a sin. Objection 2: What arises from a good root does not seem to be a sin, since, as Matthew 7:18 says, “A good tree cannot bring produce bad fruits.” But despair seems to proceed from a good root, viz., fear of God, or horror at the magnitude of one’s sins. Therefore, despair is not a sin. Objection 3: If despair were a sin, then, in the case of the damned, their despairing would be a sin. But this is not imputed to them as a sin (non imputatur eis ad culpam); instead, it is imputed to their being damned. Therefore, it is not imputed as a sin to those who are in this life, either (neque viatoribus imputatur ad culpam). And so despair is not a sin. But contrary to this: That through which men are induced to sin seems to be not only a sin, but a principle of sins.
    [Show full text]
  • University Basic Needs Insecurity: a National #Realcollege Survey Report
    APRIL 2019 College and University Basic Needs Insecurity: A National #RealCollege Survey Report AUTHORS: Sara Goldrick-Rab, Christine Baker-Smith, Vanessa Coca, Elizabeth Looker and Tiffani Williams Executive Summary NEARLY 86,000 STUDENTS PARTICIPATED. THE RESULTS The #RealCollege survey is the nation’s largest annual INDICATE: assessment of basic needs security among college students. The survey, created by the Hope Center • 45% of respondents were food for College, Community, and Justice (Hope Center), insecure in the prior 30 days specifically evaluates access to affordable food and housing. This report describes the results of the • 56% of respondents were #RealCollege survey administered in the fall of 2018 at housing insecure in the previous year 123 two- and four-year institutions across the United States. • 17% of respondents were homeless in the previous year Rates of basic needs insecurity are higher for students attending two-year colleges compared to those attending four-year colleges. Rates of basic needs insecurity are higher for marginalized students, including African Americans, students identifying as LGBTQ, and students who are independent from The Hope Center thanks the their parents or guardians for financial aid purposes. Lumina Foundation, the Jewish Students who have served in the military, former foster Foundation for Education of youth, and students who were formerly convicted of a crime are all at greater risk of basic needs insecurity. Women, the City University Working during college is not associated with a lower of New York, the Chicago risk of basic needs insecurity, and neither is receiving City Colleges, the Institute for the federal Pell Grant; the latter is in fact associated with higher rates of basic needs insecurity.
    [Show full text]
  • Machine Learning Based Emotion Classification Using the Covid-19 Real World Worry Dataset
    Anatolian Journal of Computer Sciences Volume:6 No:1 2021 © Anatolian Science pp:24-31 ISSN:2548-1304 Machine Learning Based Emotion Classification Using The Covid-19 Real World Worry Dataset Hakan ÇAKAR1*, Abdulkadir ŞENGÜR2 *1Fırat Üniversitesi/Teknoloji Fak., Elektrik-Elektronik Müh. Bölümü, Elazığ, Türkiye ([email protected]) 2Fırat Üniversitesi/Teknoloji Fak., Elektrik-Elektronik Müh. Bölümü, Elazığ, Türkiye ([email protected]) Received Date : Sep. 27, 2020 Acceptance Date : Jan. 1, 2021 Published Date : Mar. 1, 2021 Özetçe— COVID-19 pandemic has a dramatic impact on economies and communities all around the world. With social distancing in place and various measures of lockdowns, it becomes significant to understand emotional responses on a great scale. In this paper, a study is presented that determines human emotions during COVID-19 using various machine learning (ML) approaches. To this end, various techniques such as Decision Trees (DT), Support Vector Machines (SVM), k-nearest neighbor (k-NN), Neural Networks (NN) and Naïve Bayes (NB) methods are used in determination of the human emotions. The mentioned techniques are used on a dataset namely Real World Worry dataset (RWWD) that was collected during COVID-19. The dataset, which covers eight emotions on a 9-point scale, grading their anxiety levels about the COVID-19 situation, was collected by using 2500 participants. The performance evaluation of the ML techniques on emotion prediction is carried out by using the accuracy score. Five-fold cross validation technique is also adopted in experiments. The experiment works show that the ML approaches are promising in determining the emotion in COVID- 19 RWWD.
    [Show full text]
  • The Effects of an Emotion Strengthening Training Program On
    BALCI ÇELİK / Duyguları Güçlendirme Eğitimi Programı’nın Hemşirelerin ... • 793 Th e Eff ects of an Emotion Strengthening Training Program on the Optimism Level of Nurses Seher BALCI ÇELİK* Abstract Th e aim of this study is to investigate the eff ects of emotion strengthening as a training programme for optimistic for nurses. Th e experimental and control group of this rese- arch has totally composed of 20 nurses. A pre-test post-test research model with control group was been used in this research. Nurses’ optimistic levels have been measured by ‘Optimistic Scale’ which was developed by Balcı and Yılmaz (2002). In order to test that, meaningful diff erences between the scores of pre-test and post-test within both control and experimental group Mann Whitey U and Wilcoxon Signed Rank test were applied for statisical analysis. It was found that this emotional strengthening training had positive eff ects on levels of optimistic for nurses. Th e findings have been discussed in the light of literature, and some suggestions have been made. Key Words Emotion Strengthening Training Program, Nurse, Optimistic, Optimistic Scale. * Correspondence: Ondokuz Mayıs University, Faculty of Education, Department of Education Sciences, Kurupelit 55139 Samsun-Turkey / E-mail: [email protected] Kuram ve Uygulamada Eğitim Bilimleri / Educational Sciences: Th eory & Practice 8 (3) • September 2008 • 793-804 © 2008 Eğitim Danışmanlığı ve Araştırmaları İletişim Hizmetleri Tic. Ltd. Şti. 794 • EDUCATIONAL SCIENCES: THEORY & PRACTICE As internal mechanisms emotions are psychological signs of how an individual feels. Th ey develop in situations which call for an individual’s life style or behavioral pattern.
    [Show full text]
  • Using an Ambiguous Cue Paradigm to Assess Cognitive Bias in Gorillas (Gorilla Gorilla Gorilla) During a Forage Manipulation
    ABC 2017, 4(1):70-83 Animal Behavior and Cognition DOI: 10.12966/abc.06.02.2017 ©Attribution 3.0 Unported (CC BY 3.0) Using an Ambiguous Cue Paradigm to Assess Cognitive Bias in Gorillas (Gorilla gorilla gorilla) during a Forage Manipulation Molly C. McGuire1, Jennifer Vonk1*, Grace Fuller2, & Stephanie Allard2 1Oakland University, Department of Psychology, Rochester, MI, USA 2Detroit Zoological Society, Royal Oak, MI USA *Corresponding author (Email: [email protected]) Citation – McGuire, M. C., Vonk, J., Fuller, G., & Allard, S. (2017). Using an ambiguous cue paradigm to assess cognitive bias in gorillas (Gorilla gorilla gorilla) during a forage manipulation. Animal Behavior and Cognition, 4(1), 70–83. doi: 10.12966/abc.06.02.2017 Abstract - In nonhumans, ‘optimism’ is often defined as responding to an ambiguous item in the same manner as to items previously associated with reward (or lack of punishment), and “pessimism” is defined as responding to an ambiguous item in the same manner as to items previously associated with a lack of reward (or with punishment). We measured the degree of “optimism” and “pessimism” in three captive male western lowland gorillas (Gorilla gorilla gorilla) during four consecutive two-week periods in which the amount of available forage material (mulberry, Moraceae or alfalfa, Medicago sativa) was manipulated. We assessed cognitive bias using an ambiguous cue paradigm for the first time. Pairs of two-dimensional shapes were presented on a touch-screen computer in a forced choice task in which one shape was always reinforced (P), one was never reinforced (N), and one was reinforced half the time, making it ambiguous (A).
    [Show full text]
  • Joy, Distress, Hope, and Fear in Reinforcement Learning (Extended Abstract)
    Joy, Distress, Hope, and Fear in Reinforcement Learning (Extended Abstract) Elmer Jacobs Joost Broekens Catholijn Jonker Interactive Intelligence, Interactive Intelligence, Interactive Intelligence, TU Delft TU Delft TU Delft Delft, The Netherlands Delft, The Netherlands Delft, The Netherlands [email protected] [email protected] [email protected] ABSTRACT orbitofrontal cortex, reward representation, and (subjective) In this paper we present a mapping between joy, distress, affective value (see [14]). hope and fear, and Reinforcement Learning primitives. Joy While most research on computational modeling of emo- / distress is a signal that is derived from the RL update sig- tion is based on cognitive appraisal theory [9], our work is nal, while hope/fear is derived from the utility of the current different in that we aim to show a direct mapping between state. Agent-based simulation experiments replicate psycho- RL primitives and emotions, and assess the validity by repli- logical and behavioral dynamics of emotion including: joy cating psychological findings on emotion dynamics, the lat- and distress reactions that develop prior to hope and fear; ter being an essential difference with [5]. We believe that fear extinction; habituation of joy; and, task randomness before affectively labelling a particular RL-based signal, it that increases the intensity of joy and distress. This work is essential to investigate if that signal behaves according to distinguishes itself by assessing the dynamics of emotion in what is known in psychology and behavioral science. The an adaptive agent framework - coupling it to the literature extent to which a signal replicates emotion-related dynamics on habituation, development, and extinction.
    [Show full text]
  • Emotion Classification Based on Biophysical Signals and Machine Learning Techniques
    S S symmetry Article Emotion Classification Based on Biophysical Signals and Machine Learning Techniques Oana Bălan 1,* , Gabriela Moise 2 , Livia Petrescu 3 , Alin Moldoveanu 1 , Marius Leordeanu 1 and Florica Moldoveanu 1 1 Faculty of Automatic Control and Computers, University POLITEHNICA of Bucharest, Bucharest 060042, Romania; [email protected] (A.M.); [email protected] (M.L.); fl[email protected] (F.M.) 2 Department of Computer Science, Information Technology, Mathematics and Physics (ITIMF), Petroleum-Gas University of Ploiesti, Ploiesti 100680, Romania; [email protected] 3 Faculty of Biology, University of Bucharest, Bucharest 030014, Romania; [email protected] * Correspondence: [email protected]; Tel.: +40722276571 Received: 12 November 2019; Accepted: 18 December 2019; Published: 20 December 2019 Abstract: Emotions constitute an indispensable component of our everyday life. They consist of conscious mental reactions towards objects or situations and are associated with various physiological, behavioral, and cognitive changes. In this paper, we propose a comparative analysis between different machine learning and deep learning techniques, with and without feature selection, for binarily classifying the six basic emotions, namely anger, disgust, fear, joy, sadness, and surprise, into two symmetrical categorical classes (emotion and no emotion), using the physiological recordings and subjective ratings of valence, arousal, and dominance from the DEAP (Dataset for Emotion Analysis using EEG, Physiological and Video Signals) database. The results showed that the maximum classification accuracies for each emotion were: anger: 98.02%, joy:100%, surprise: 96%, disgust: 95%, fear: 90.75%, and sadness: 90.08%. In the case of four emotions (anger, disgust, fear, and sadness), the classification accuracies were higher without feature selection.
    [Show full text]
  • A Guide for Nurturing Hope, Resilience and Happiness Pasiàka Style
    I AM A guide for nurturing hope, resilience and happiness Pasi\ ka style. By Philip Siataga Acknowledgements Fakatu’amelie innovation fund Before anything else is said, sincere thanks are due to Manase Lua and Monique Faleafa and the awesome Le Va team, for inspiration, encouragement and support. Without which the I AM approach would not have developed to this point. I would like to thank my friends and colleagues Wiremu Gray Ngai Tahu, Ngati Porou (Mana Facilitation Ltd) and Paddy Pawson (Adventure Therapy) for invaluable support, insightful commentary and precious time spent sharing your hearts and minds. Special acknowledgement is due to Maui Paraki (Humble Enterprises) and to Andrew Smith (About Life ltd) whose lives are inspirational. To Alan and Alice Va’a, who continue to also serve so many in the true spirit of grace and humility – much respect. To my family, with mountains of love and gratitude, I can not thank you enough. And to, Sophia and Talia, wonderful daughters, thank you for making my journey one of enduring happiness. I am blessed with the joy of a fathers pride. Many thanks are due to Fuimaono Karl Pulotu-Endemann and Dr Francis Agnew, who inspire innovation and integrity from those of us privileged to have journeyed with them. I must also acknowledge the Occasional paper on negotiated space and thank Karlo Mila Schaaf and Maui Hudson for their insightful theorising which has boosted my own desire to plant this work in the va. To the fantastic crew at Waipuna Trust, my warmest thanks for your support and your many kindnesses along the way.
    [Show full text]
  • Hope and Anger As Mediators Between Collective Action Frames and Participation in Collective Mobilization: the Case of 15-M
    Journal of Social and Political Psychology jspp.psychopen.eu | 2195-3325 Original Research Reports Hope and Anger as Mediators Between Collective Action Frames and Participation in Collective Mobilization: The Case of 15-M Anna Wlodarczyk* ab, Nekane Basabe c, Darío Páez b, Larraitz Zumeta b [a] Escuela de Psicología, Universidad Católica del Norte, Antofagasta, Chile. [b] Department of Social Psychology and Methodology of Behaviour Sciences, University of the Basque Country, San Sebastián, Spain. [c] Department of Social Psychology and Methodology of Behaviour Sciences, University of the Basque Country, Vitoria, Spain. Abstract The study set out to integrate collective action models and emphasize the role of emotions. Whereas the importance of anger is indisputable, relatively little attention has been paid to the role of positive emotions, such as hope, in collective action research. Hence, the aim of the study was to explore the role of hope and anger as drivers of participation and involvement in collective mobilizations. A cross-sectional field study (N = 638) conducted right after the emergence of the 15-M socio-political protest movement in Spain assessed the emotions and beliefs of both demonstrators and those who took no part in the active mobilization. We hypothesized that anger and hope would sequentially mediate the relationship between collective action frames and participation in collective action. Furthermore, to test this premise, we ran two alternative sequential mediation models based on the social identity model of collective action (SIMCA) and the encapsulated model of social identity in collective action (EMSICA), but with emotions as mediators between collective action frames and intensity of participation.
    [Show full text]
  • Research-Based Practice with Women Who Have Had Miscarriages
    CMcal Scholarship Research- based Practice with Women Who Have Had Miscarriages Kristen M. Swanson Purpose: To summarize a research-based description of what it is like to miscarry and to recommend an empirically tested theory of caring for women who have experienced miscarriage. Design: The research program included three phases: interpretive theory generation, descriptive survey and instrument development, and experimental testing of a theory-based intervention. Methods: Research methods included interpretive phenomenologE factor analysis, and ANCOVA. Findings: A theory of caring and a model of what it is like to miscarry were generated, refined, and tested. A case study shows one woman’s response to miscarrying and illustrates clinical application of the caring theory. Conclusions: The Miscarriage Model is a useful framework for anticipating the variety of responses women have to miscarrying. The caring theory is an effective and sensitive guide to clinical practice with women who miscarry. IMAGE:JOURNAL OF NURSINGSCHOLARSHIP, 1999; 31 :4,339-345.01999 SIGMA THETATAU INTERNATIONAL. [Key words: caring, miscarriage, theory construction, counseling] t least one in five pregnancies ends in miscarriage-the program of inquiry about miscarriage and its aftermath. An unplanned, unexpected ending of pregnancy before the important contribution of the pilot study was the conclusion that time of expected fetal viability (Hall, Beresford, & a woman’s feelings about miscarriage could be understood only Quinones, 1987). Women’s responses range from relief in the context of what being pregnant and having a miscarriage to devastation with much variability in the time required meant to her. For example, if being pregnant was perceived as a Ato achieve resolution.
    [Show full text]