Voiceless Stop Contrasts in Alemannic German

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Voiceless Stop Contrasts in Alemannic German Voiceless Stop Contrasts Annika Ronay . Ling 201 . 5/15/14 1 Voiceless Stop Contrasts in Alemannic German Introduction Voiceless stop contrasts that are not based on place of articulation can be among the hardest sounds to identify, especially phrase initially. Alemannic High German lost its voicing contrast in Pre-Old High German (OHG) during the second consonant shift, yet (obviously), the spoken language today is fully functional and contrastive. This paper will explore how the voiceless stops in Alemannic German established and retained contrasts, and whether the process involved is one of fortis-lenis, or one of consonant duration. Background The Modern Swiss German dialect of Thurgovian, an Alemannic German language, has phonological stops in only three places of articulation, labial, coronal, and velar; and the language makes six distinct sounds from them. Since the language does not possess a voicing contrast for stops, the contrast has to come from a process of either strengthening (fortitian), or lengthening (gemination). Researchers Aditi Lahiri and Astrid Kraehenmann (2004) looked to Old Alemannic, spoken about a thousand years earlier, to see if how the sound system evolved could offer an explanation. They studied a manuscript written by a Benedictine monk in the tenth century, in which the writer, Notker der Deutsche, had managed to capture the Voiceless Stop Contrasts Annika Ronay . Ling 201 . 5/15/14 2 allophonic alternation of the time in his orthography. They described his rule of alternation as Notker’s Anlautgesetz, or “law of initial consonants”: he alternated the letters he used for phrase-medial consonants at the beginnings of words, depending on whether the preceding word ended in a voiced or a voiceless sound. Specifically, he used the Latin letters ‘b’ and ‘g’ if the preceding word ending was voiced, and the letters ‘p’ and ‘k’ if the preceding word ending was voiceless. Since it has been historically postulated that Alemannic German had already lost its voicing contrast by Notker’s time, Lahiri and Kraehenmann argue that he was actually attempting to capture, using the writing system available to him at the time, a contrast of consonant quantity or duration. Nature of the Contrasts in Old Alemannic Notker’s Anlautgesetz was supposed to account for all stops in word-initial position; they were simply in complementary distribution based on the sonority of the word- final sound immediately preceding them. Based on this alone, it would seem that Old Alemannic lacked contrasts of any kind and that the change was merely one of allophonic alternation. However, when Lahiri and Kraehenmann charted all the stops in word initial and word final position (as written), they found that while <p> and <b>, and <k/c> and <g> were indeed in complementary distribution, <t> and <d> contrasted word finally, showing a phonemic, rather than an allophonic distinction between the coronal stops. (Lahiri and Kraehenmann, 2004) Voiceless Stop Contrasts Annika Ronay . Ling 201 . 5/15/14 3 Nature of the Contrasts in Modern Alemannic- Thurgovian In comparing the vocabulary with initial stops in Notker’s German with their Modern Alemannic equivalents, Lahiri and Kraehenmann found that most of the words survived into Thurgovian, and where the older language had an allophonic distinction between initial stops, the modern equivalents all began with a single voiceless stop. Where it had phonemic distinctions between <d> and <t>, Thurgovian words contrasted between a single [t] and a geminate [tt], respectively. However, the vocabulary of words in modern Thurgovian contain word-initial phonemic distinctions not just between the coronal stops [t] and [tt], but also the labials and dorsals [p], [pp], and [k], [kk], respectively. The question then, is where Thurgovian acquired its system of consonant duration stops for the other places of articulation. A closer look at the vocabulary quickly reveals that “words with initial geminates are essentially all borrowed, from the middle ages onwards: /ppaːʀ/ <Old French/Latin ‘pair’, /ppatə / n i h ‘pa e’, / pp/ French ‘c p ice-crea ’, / a tʃ/ n i h ‘c ch’.” Since speakers are limited by their system of phonology when introducing borrowed words into their lexicon, they have to use the resources available to them to maintain contrasts and avoid neutralization. From Old Alemannic they already had a singleton/geminate distinction for coronal stops, and when loan words were introduced with both voiced and voiceless word initial stops, they simply extended their current system of contrast to include labials and dorsals and effectively integrate new contrastive sounds into their language while maintaining their phonological inventory. Voiceless Stop Contrasts Annika Ronay . Ling 201 . 5/15/14 4 So, What is the Nature of the Contrasts Themselves? Researchers looking at Alemannic German have debated over what the contrast between voiceless stops actually is. Some (Penzl, 1971; Weinberg, 1911) have argued that the distinction is one of fortis-lenis, where what could be perceived as a duration distinction among consonants is in fact a phenomenon of stops being articulated more strongly to create a contrast. Others, like Lahiri and Kraehenmann, argue that the contrast is one of consonant quantity, where stops that were originally voiced before the Pre-OHG consonant shift surface as voiceless singleton stops, and stops that were originally voiceless surface as voiceless geminate stops, with a longer closure duration. They maintain that the voicing contrast was already lost in Notker’s time and that he merely used the orthography of voiced stops to show a distinction in the Latin writing system available to him. Fortis-Lenis Distinction According to Ladefoged and Maddieson (1996), fortis consonants have greater respiratory energy and/or greater articulatory energy. Since all the voiceless stops in Alemannic represent a ‘fortis’ distinction by these terms, the distinction would be found in the degree of fortitian between contrasting consonants, and lenis would be unspecified. DiCanio (2012) explained fortis in somewhat simpler terms, that it is “primarily realized by changes in consonant duration, while for obstruents, preaspiration is a significant correlate of the fortis series.” He goes on to say that “No acoustic correlate of increased articulatory strength… is shown to be a significant correlate of the contrast.” Voiceless Stop Contrasts Annika Ronay . Ling 201 . 5/15/14 5 He argues instead that fortis is best specified phonologically by duration and a spread glottis. However, fortis-lenis distinctions are often used not just to distinguish how strongly a sound is articulated (or for how long), but also to account for secondary characteristics such as spirantization. Since such a phenomenon is not a contrastive distinction in Alemannic German, and the glottis is always spread for the stop consonants, the fortis account for the contrast does not seem to be a necessary and sufficient description for the stop distinctions observed in Thurgovian. Quantity-Duration Distinction Lahiri and Kraehenmann have established since the beginning of their research that the contrast in Thurgovian is one of consonant duration, or as they say “quantity”, since it references the distinction between singleton and geminate stops. They give the following examples to show the distinctions: (Lahiri and Kraehenmann, 2004) Voiceless Stop Contrasts Annika Ronay . Ling 201 . 5/15/14 6 Originally, they had established that acoustically, [W]ord-initial geminate CDs were about twice the length of singletons when in an intersonorant context. However, when a preceding word ended in an obstruent consonant, the contrast was neutralized by geminates having become shorter and singletons longer. The quote was from their more recent, 2008 paper referencing their 2004 ‘Anlautgesetz’ work. In this, more current research, they add articulatory electropalatography (EPG) measures to compare with their previous closure duration (CD) acoustic data they had previously collected, in order to see if their findings were correct and if geminates did in fact neutralize after voiceless obstruents. What they found is that the contrast was indeed maintained, however minimally, in a phrase-medial context following another obstruent by an average difference of about 16 ms. They could not claim for sure if the contrast was detected by the listener, but speakers always articulated it in some way. Their EPG diagrams are included on the next page to show how long the stops are articulated in each context. They analyzed singleton and geminate stops in four specific contexts: namely: in isolation, consonantal, vocalic, and word medial context. They found that the difference between [ttoː ə] (roar) and [t ː ə] (can) word initially was about 150 ms., the difference between the two in a consonantal context- [nɔx tt ː ə] (after roar) vs. [nɔx t ː ə] (after can) was zero; however, when they re-did their CD data, they found a difference of 16 ms., as mentioned above. In a vocalic context, EPG readings showed a i erence 60 . between [ ni tt ː ə] (without roar) an [ ni t ː ə] (without can), and in a intervocalic word medial context, the difference between [lattə] (crossbar) and [latə] Voiceless Stop Contrasts Annika Ronay . Ling 201 . 5/15/14 7 (shop) was huge, at 160 ms. Their conclusion is that the singleton/geminate contrast was maintained in all positions in Thurgovian for stop consonants, even (at least articulatoraly) where it was previously considered neutralized in a consonantal context. Furthermore, since the distinction does not rely on aspiration or specific amounts of articulatory force, the contrast of duration seems to be necessary and sufficient. (Kraehenmann and Lahiri, 2008) Voiceless Stop Contrasts Annika Ronay . Ling 201 . 5/15/14 8 Conclusion Researchers looking at Alemannic German have tried to explain in the simplest way how the contrast between voiceless stops is maintained. As early as Heusler (1888), many have contended that the distinction is based on a level of strength, either in aspiration or articulation.
Recommended publications
  • A Bavarian-Speaking Exception in Alemannic-Speaking Switzerland: the Case of Samnaun 48 Located
    47 A Bavarian -speaking 1. Introduction1 data2 is missing, as the study by Gröger is the Exception in Alemannic- only detailed linguistic description of the speaking Switzerland: he municipality of Samnaun, situated German dialect in Samnaun. in the extreme east of Switzerland, In this paper, I present a new research The Case of Samnaun project which seeks to fill this gap. The T stands out from the rest of German- speaking Switzerland: Samnaun is usually project is dedicated to the current linguistic A Project Presentation described as the only place where not an situation in Samnaun. Its working title is Alemannic, but a Bavarian dialect is spoken Bairisch-alemannischer Sprachkontakt. Das Journal Article (cf., e.g., Sonderegger 2003: 2839; Wiesinger Spektrum der sprachlichen Variation in Susanne Oberholzer 1983: 817). This claim is based on a study by Samnaun (‘Bavarian-Alemannic Language Gröger (1924) and has found its way into Contact. The Range of Linguistic Variation in Samnaun has been described as the only Samnaun’). This paper describes Samnaun Bavarian-speaking municipality in Ale- many linguistic descriptions of (German- mannic-speaking Switzerland on the basis speaking) Switzerland. In the literature, this and summarises the available descriptions of of a study done in 1924. Hints in the viewpoint has been almost unchallenged for its language situation as well as the aims, literature about the presence of other over 90 years. Hints about the presence of research questions, and methods of the varieties for everyday communication – an other varieties (an Alemannic dialect as well planned project. intermediate variety on the dialect- In section 2, I sketch the municipality of standard -axis as well as an Alemannic as an intermediate variety on the dialect- dialect – have not resulted in more recent standard-axis) from the second half of the Samnaun.
    [Show full text]
  • Hatt Or Si? Neuter and Feminine Gender Assignment in Reference to Female Persons in Luxembourgish
    Hatt or si? Neuter and feminine gender assignment in reference to female persons in Luxembourgish Abstract: In Luxembourgish, feminine as well as neuter gender can be assigned to female persons. Here, female first names are morphologically treated as neuter and therefore trigger neuter gender on their targets (e.g. definite article, personal pronoun). Last names referring to women, however, are feminine and take feminine targets respectively. While the use of neuter and feminine in prototypical and invariable reference contexts are well-known, morphological conflicts often arise regarding more complex name types (e.g. female first name + last name) leading to different degrees of variation between both genders. Building especially upon previous findings by Döhmer (2016), the present contribution offers a first extensive empirical analysis on the use of neuter and feminine personal pronouns considering different female referents as well as familiarity, the referent’s and the speaker’s as decisive (socio-pragmatical) factors for gender assignment. The results are based on elicited data retrieved from an online survey and audio recordings collected by means of the the Luxembourgish language app Schnëssen and allow a quantification of the phenomenon going beyond previous contributions and descriptions in reference grammars. The apparent-time analysis, carried out in order to identify potential tendencies in language change, suggests a preference for neuter pronominalization for younger speakers of Luxembourgish in variable reference contexts. keywords: gender assignment, variation, pronominalization, neuter, Luxembourgish 1 Introduction While a correlation between gender and sex usually applies to appellatives and anthroponyms, one can use feminine as well as neuter when referring to female persons in Luxembourgish.
    [Show full text]
  • The Wili Benchmark Dataset for Written Natural Language Identification
    1 The WiLI benchmark dataset for written II. WRITTEN LANGUAGE BASICS language identification A language is a system of communication. This could be spoken language, written language or sign language. A spoken language Martin Thoma can have several forms of written language. For example, the E-Mail: [email protected] spoken Chinese language has at least three writing systems: Traditional Chinese characters, Simplified Chinese characters and Pinyin — the official romanization system for Standard Chinese. Abstract—This paper describes the WiLI-2018 benchmark dataset for monolingual written natural language identification. Languages evolve. New words like googling, television and WiLI-2018 is a publicly available,1 free of charge dataset of Internet get added, but written languages are also refactored. short text extracts from Wikipedia. It contains 1000 paragraphs For example, the German orthography reform of 1996 aimed at of 235 languages, totaling in 235 000 paragraphs. WiLI is a classification dataset: Given an unknown paragraph written in making the written language simpler. This means any system one dominant language, it has to be decided which language it which recognizes language and any benchmark needs to be is. adapted over time. Hence WiLI is versioned by year. Languages do not necessarily have only one name. According to Wikipedia, the Sranan language is also known as Sranan Tongo, Sranantongo, Surinaams, Surinamese, Surinamese Creole and I. INTRODUCTION Taki Taki. This makes ISO 369-3 valuable, but not all languages are represented in ISO 369-3. As ISO 369-3 uses combinations The identification of written natural language is a task which of 3 Latin letters and has 547 reserved combinations, it can appears often in web applications.
    [Show full text]
  • Spatial Boundaries and Transitions in Language and Interaction
    URPP Language and Space www.spur.uzh.ch International Conference SPATIAL BOUNDARIES AND TRANSITIONS IN LANGUAGE AND INTERACTION Perspectives from Linguistics and Geography April 23 – 28, 2017 Monte Verità, Ascona http://www.spur.uzh.ch/boundaries PROGRAM MONDAY, 24. APRIL TUESDAY, 25. APRIL 08:00 Breakfast Breakfast 09:00 Welcome 09:15 1 KEYNOTE TALK: Tom Güldemann 9 KEYNOTE TALK: Setha Low Linguistic macro-areas in Africa: when Language, Discourse and Space: boundaries are areas themselves p. 01 A Conceptual Framework for the Ethnography of Space and Place p. 09 10:15 Break Break 10:45 2 INPUT TALK SESSION 1: Peter Auer 10 INPUT TALK SESSION 2: Alfred Lameli Walking and talking: how speakers jointly Intangible Borders – Linguistic Areas and manoeuver in space p. 02 Socio-Cultural Practices p. 10 11:30 3 Paul Luff, Christian Heath, Menisha Patel 11 Stefanie Siebenhuetter Boundaries in interaction spaces: embodied Conceptual transfer of spatial reference due interaction within a large working to language contact? A semantic approach environment p. 03 to cultural conceptualization in the linguistic area Mainland Southeast Asia p. 11 12:00 Lunch Lunch 14:00 4 Martin de Heaver, Paul Luff, 12 Grossenbacher, Britain, Leemann, Christian Heath Kolly, Blaxter, Wanitsch Crossing Boundaries: interactions through Smartphone app methodologies for regional locations within a moving environment p. 04 dialectology: the English North-South divide in data from the English Dialects App p. 12 14:30 5 Albert Acedo and Marco Painho: 13 Daan Hovens “You should participate” or “I want What is the language of the euregio to participate” – engaging spatial rhine-meuse-north? Euroregional integration boundaries p.
    [Show full text]
  • How Much Does Geography Influence Language Variation? John Nerbonne (University of Groningen)
    How much does geography influence language variation? John Nerbonne (University of Groningen) Abstract This paper proceeds from a quantitative perspective and applies measurements of linguistic difference to obtain characterizations of the aggregate relations among varieties in entire language areas. From there it is a small step in quantitative methodology to ask how much geographic distance influences (aggregate) linguistic differences, applying a regression design. The paper goes on to contrast the degree to which a very bare concept of geography, just distance, competes with a more complicated one, involving areas or regions, in explaining the aggregate linguistic differences among language varieties. The data is taken from dialect atlases and various other collections, and the linguistic distance is measured using a variant of Levenshtein distance, which has been demonstrated to be valid with respect to dialect speakers’ judgments of linguistic difference. Pure geographic distance models account for between 14% and 38% of the variation found in the data in straightforward regression designs. The alternative to pure distance models involves dialect areas, which we examine on the basis of the independently established dialect areas in Wrede’s famous map of German dialect areas, adding dialect divisions as categorical independent variables to assess their explanatory value. This increases the explained variation from 32% to 45% in the German dialect dataset, indicating that geography is indeed structured more complexly than simple distances, and also that geography influences linguistic variation deeply. 1. Introduction Two insights about language variation are standard and uncontroversial, first that languages may vary in many ways and second, that nearby language varieties are generally – but not always – more similar than distant ones.
    [Show full text]
  • Differences in the Pronunciation of English in Switzerland
    MATURAPAPER Differences in the Pronunciation of English in Switzerland The differences in the pronunciation of English by non-native speakers in different cantons and its possible connection to their native dialect Submitted by: Lukas Tribelhorn, 4bW Supervised by: Michael Bühler 06.12.2013 Kantonsschule Wil Table of Contents Preface 1. Introduction ..................................................................................................................................... 1 2. Aims of this Paper ........................................................................................................................... 1 3. Phonetics and Phonology ................................................................................................................ 2 3.1. The International Phonetic Alphabet .......................................................................................... 2 4. Dialectology ..................................................................................................................................... 3 4.1. What is a language, what is a dialect and what is an accent ...................................................... 3 4.2. Received Pronunciation .............................................................................................................. 4 4.2.1. Vowels ............................................................................................................................ 5 4.2.2. Consonants ....................................................................................................................
    [Show full text]
  • The Wrong Side of the Tracks Claire Alexander-Joly
    bioStories sharing the extraordinary in ordinary lives The Wrong Side of the Tracks Claire Alexander-Joly As far as I knew, the world of my childhood was defined and divided by class, and as far as I was concerned, I did not live on the right side of the tracks. From 1963, when I was born, to 1986 when I left for the United States at the age of twenty-three, I lived in a big rectangular block of concrete in the northeastern suburbs of Paris, right outside the Périphérique, the freeway that contours the city and separates it from the banlieues. The building where I and my family lived sat on a hilltop, and from there overlooked not the beautiful sights for which Paris is known but its far less attractive working-class neighborhoods. Paris and its environs are divided by the Seine. The river runs through the city from the southeast to the southwest curving upward toward the middle. L’Île de la Cité— the site of the Notre Dame Cathedral—is the official center of the capital. South of the Seine lies the so-called “Left Bank” or “Rive Gauche,” historically the place of artists and intellectuals; to the north lies the “Right Bank” or “Rive Droite.” The terms are a bit misleading, suggesting an east/west division rather than a north/south one. When I was growing up, only the Rive Gauche seemed to matter. That’s where the rich lived. In 1971, Yves Saint Laurent launched his famous women’s perfume— Rive Gauche—named after his boutique in Saint-Germain-des-Prés on the south side of the river.
    [Show full text]
  • University of Groningen Phonological Grammar and Frequency Sloos
    University of Groningen Phonological grammar and frequency Sloos, Marjoleine IMPORTANT NOTE: You are advised to consult the publisher's version (publisher's PDF) if you wish to cite from it. Please check the document version below. Document Version Publisher's PDF, also known as Version of record Publication date: 2013 Link to publication in University of Groningen/UMCG research database Citation for published version (APA): Sloos, M. (2013). Phonological grammar and frequency: an integrated approach. s.n. Copyright Other than for strictly personal use, it is not permitted to download or to forward/distribute the text or part of it without the consent of the author(s) and/or copyright holder(s), unless the work is under an open content license (like Creative Commons). The publication may also be distributed here under the terms of Article 25fa of the Dutch Copyright Act, indicated by the “Taverne” license. More information can be found on the University of Groningen website: https://www.rug.nl/library/open-access/self-archiving-pure/taverne- amendment. Take-down policy If you believe that this document breaches copyright please contact us providing details, and we will remove access to the work immediately and investigate your claim. Downloaded from the University of Groningen/UMCG research database (Pure): http://www.rug.nl/research/portal. For technical reasons the number of authors shown on this cover page is limited to 10 maximum. Download date: 30-09-2021 Chapter 5 The Alemannic variety of Standard German long <ä>: opaque lexical structure1 Abstract This chapter reports on an experiment on the pronunciation of the long vowel <ä> in the Standard German variety of subjects who live in the Alemannic area.
    [Show full text]
  • The Language Situation in Switzerland
    Lingua 80 (1990) 109-148. North-Holland 109 THE LANGUAGE SITUATION IN SWITZERLAND: An Updated Survey* Leo PAP SUNY College, New PaIt-_. NY 12561, USA Received April 1989 The history of Switzerland is outlined briefly with special reference to evolution from unilingual to quadrilingual polity. Official modern language policy in the four language territories is described, with data on language boundaries and relative number of speakers. Extent of individual (as against societal) multilingualism in Switzerland is described, in conjunction with language attitudes and names attaching to the different language groups. The role of ‘dialects‘ vs. ‘standard language’ in the four territories is compared in some detail, Special developments since World War II are focused on. The survey concludes with pointing up the role of religious, economic, and/or political cleavages in interlingual relationships. 1. Preamble Switzerland is often called a ‘miracle of unity in diversity’. In saying this one is thinking primarily of the peaceful symbiosis of four different language groups, i.e. the absence (until recently at least) of interlingual tensions. This lingual’ diversity is superimposed on, or intertwined with, the diversity deriving from the limited autonomy of about two dozen separete ‘cantons’ which make up the Swiss Confederation. The Swiss are proud of this diversity, particularly the lingual one, considering it an asset which in some way makes up for the smallness of their country. Thus we find the largest Swiss language group, German, which comprises almost three fourths of the * This article is a greatly expanded version of a talk given before the American Society of Geolinguistics on November 21, 1987.
    [Show full text]
  • Collecting and Annotating Corpora for Three Under-Resourced Languages of France: Methodological Issues
    Vol. 15 (2021), pp. 316–357 http://nflrc.hawaii.edu/ldc http://hdl.handle.net/10125/74645 Revised Version Received: 13 Apr 2021 Collecting and annotating corpora for three under-resourced languages of France: Methodological issues Delphine Bernhard Pascale Erhart Université de Strasbourg, Université de Strasbourg, LiLPa, UR 1339 LiLPa, UR 1339 Anne-Laure Ligozat Jean Sibille LISN, ENSIIE CLLE, UMR 5263, Université Toulouse Jean Jaurès, CNRS Myriam Bras Amalia Todirascu CLLE, UMR 5263, Université de Strasbourg, Université Toulouse Jean Jaurès, CNRS LiLPa, UR 1339 Fanny Martin Philippe Boula de Mareüil Habiter Le Monde, EA 4287, LISN, CNRS Université de Picardie Jules Verne Marianne Vergez-Couret Dominique Huck FoReLLIS, EA 3816, Université de Strasbourg, Université de Poitiers LiLPa, UR 1339 In contrast to French, the vast majority of regional languages of France can be considered as under-resourced. In this article, we present the results of a research project aiming to produce annotated resources for three regional languages of France: Alsatian, Occitan, and Picard. These languages cover three different lan- guage families (Germanic and two subfamilies of Romance, Oïl and Oc languages) and different sociolinguistic situations. Yet, they all face issues common to many under-resourced languages: lack of human and financial resources and presence of geolinguistic variation. The originality of this project is that it brought together researchers from different fields (sociolinguistics, descriptive linguistics, dialectol- ogy, natural language processing, digital humanities) to work together towards the common goal of developing annotated corpora for Alsatian, Occitan, and Pi- card. This created a favorable and stimulating working environment which could not have been achieved had different research groups worked independently, each on a single language.
    [Show full text]
  • A SOCIOLINGUISTIC STUDY of BITBURGER PLATT GERMAN By
    A SOCIOLINGUISTIC STUDY OF BITBURGER PLATT GERMAN by Zebulon Aaron Pischnotte A dissertation submitted to the faculty of The University of Utah in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy Department of Linguistics The University of Utah May 2015 Copyright © Zebulon Aaron Pischnotte 2015 All Rights Reserved The University of Utah Graduate School STATEMENT OF DISSERTATION APPROVAL The dissertation of Zebulon Aaron Pischnotte has been approved by the following supervisory committee members: Johanna Watzinger-Tharp Chair 10/29/2014 Date Approved Edward Rubin Member 10/29/2014 Date Approved Rachel Hayes-Harb Member 10/29/2014 Date Approved Joseph Salmons Member 10/30/2014 Date Approved Lyle Campbell Member 10/30/2014 Date Approved and by Edward Rubin Chair/Dean of the Department/College/School o f __________________ Linguistics and by David B. Kieda, Dean of The Graduate School. ABSTRACT Bitburger Platt, spoken in the Eifel region of western Germany, exhibits a merger of the Standard German (d) and (t) sounds, the reflexes of West Germanic *6 and *d, respectively. A chain shift yielded the modern Standard German variants. Biburger Platt, however, did not follow through with the first phase of this shift; rather, the two sounds were merged into [d] in the dialect (Veith, 1999). As an example, the Standard German phrase du tust ‘you do (cognate to English thou doest)’ is realized in Bitburger Platt as [dou dej s]. Bitburg is a town where many (if not most) residents are undergoing or have recently undergone a transition from a home-based, agrarian lifestyle to one requiring a commute to an urban center and more contact with nonlocals.
    [Show full text]
  • Language As a Tool for Integration
    Language as a tool for integration When adults move abroad, getting an education in the language of their new home country is often a necessary first step in the process of personal and professional integration. This was also the case for me, and the four people interviewed for this article. We are from around the world, with different backgrounds and hopes for the future. But we all entered a language class at the Academia School in Bern with the same goal in mind: to learn advanced German. Helen is a 28-year-old woman from Eritrea who has many irons in the fire: she is working and also learning German and mathematics. Helen came to Bern as an asylum-seeker in January 2017. She has a diploma in Computer Sciences from Eritrea and used to work as a maths teacher there. When she arrived in Bern, she was first enrolled in an intensive language course from level A1 to B1. Through a friend that she met there, she heard about “Power Coders” – a three-month, full-time programme in impact coding. This enabled her to look for opportunities suited to her educational background and she then found an Informatics programme at Fachhochschule Nordwestschweiz (FHNW). A language is not only a language, but more importantly it’s a way into the inner worlds of a people and their culture. To enter the programme there were a few conditions: she needed to have level C1 in German and to take a course in maths. Helen started an internship with the programme, taking a course in B2 level German on the side.
    [Show full text]