The Bored Brain: Insular Cortex and the Default Mode Network

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

The Bored Brain: Insular Cortex and the Default Mode Network The bored brain: insular cortex and the default mode network. James Danckert and Julia Isacescu Department of Psychology University of Waterloo 200 University Avenue West Waterloo, Ontario, CANADA, N2L 3G1 Corresponding author: J. Danckert Email: [email protected] Abstract We recently showed that posterior components of the default mode network (DMN) are prominently active when people are bored. In addition, the anterior insular is anti-correlated with the DMN during a boredom mood induction, suggestive of a failure to activate executive network regions necessary for engaging with the information at hand. Importantly, while the DMN was also active during rest, there was no evidence of anticorrelated insula activity. Here we replicate these findings in another sample of healthy individuals. In this sample, we did find some anticorrelated activity in the insular both at rest and when bored. In line with our original study, the region of insula that was anticorrelated with the DMN was substantially larger in the boredom mood induction. Limitations of the work are outlined within the context of the neural networks associated with state boredom and distinct modes of self-regulation. Key Words: boredom, fMRI, insula cortex Introduction Boredom is not a trivial cognitive-affective state. Negatively valenced and conceived of as a state of disengagement from one’s environment, boredom is associated with a raft of negative cognitive, affective, and behavioural consequences [1]. Those high in boredom proneness have an increased propensity to take risks, engage in substance abuse and problem gambling, and tend to have poor outcomes in achievement settings [2-5]. From the point of view of cognitive mechanisms, boredom has consistently been associated with failures of attention both in terms of everyday activities (e.g., pouring orange juice on your cereal)[6, 7] and in terms of experimental evaluations of sustained attention [8, 9]. In terms of affect regulation, boredom proneness has been associated with a broad range of negatively valenced affective states and syndromes, most notably, depression [10, 11]. In addition, research has shown strong associations between boredom and difficulties dealing with and expressing anger [12], and exhibiting aggressive tendencies [13, 14]. The cognitive and affective dysregulation that is characteristic of boredom begs the question of a common underlying mechanism. The capacity for effective self-regulation in the pursuit of goals 1 represents one possible mechanism underlying the cognitive and affective correlates of boredom proneness. That is, boredom represents a kind of failed motivational state. The bored individual wants to be engaged with their environment in some meaningful and satisfying way [15], but all attempts to do so fail [1]. That failure, which in turn leads to the cognitive and affective consequences of boredom, may result from impoverished self-regulatory skills [13, 16, 17]. Indeed, recent research from our lab demonstrates a strong negative relationship between boredom proneness and individual levels of self-control [17]. In other words, the more adept one is at controlling one’s own thoughts, emotions, and actions, the more effective they will be in goal pursuit and the less likely they are to be boredom prone. Interestingly, age has been shown to be a consistent negative predictor of boredom proneness levels [18]. That is, as we get older we are less prone to boredom. Key for our purposes is the age range over which this has been demonstrated – in our case age operated as a negative predictor of boredom even when the age range was restricted to 17 to 22 year olds [18]. This age range reflects the developmental period in which late stage maturation of frontal cortex occurs [19]. These results suggest a prominent role for frontal cortical regions in behavioural regulation that may be operating suboptimally in those highly prone to experiencing boredom. As already suggested, boredom is consistently associated with failures of attentional control and more recently with off-task thinking (i.e., spontaneous mind-wandering) [18]. Functional neuroimaging research exploring off-task processing and spontaneous mind-wandering, have shown neural activation within a network of brain regions known collectively as the default mode network (DMN) [20-26]. A highly-interconnected set of brain regions, the DMN supports internally-focused thinking (i.e., thinking to oneself, imagining past events, thinking of the future) and is activated when there is no external task or stimulus for an individual to engage with [22, 25]. Structurally, the main hubs of the DMN include the posterior cingulate cortex and precuneus, ventromedial prefrontal cortex and medial, lateral, and inferior parietal cortices [22]. Research exploring the neural underpinnings of boredom is in its infancy. While imaging studies have assessed boredom levels in passing, few have made it the primary focus. For instance, Ulrich and colleagues [27] assessed the neural networks of an experimentally induced state of “flow” – a state one might consider to be the opposite of boredom, in which the individual is deeply engaged in an activity to the point that all else fades away [28, 29]. In their experiment, participants performed mental arithmetic tasks at varying levels of difficulty: the task with the lowest degree of difficulty was considered “boring” and was used as a control relative to an adaptive “flow” condition in which difficulty levels were adjusted to match participants’ performance [27]. Relative to the boring condition, the state of flow was associated with a decrease in DMN activation, specifically in medial prefrontal cortex and the amygdala. In addition, posterior portions of the DMN were more active during the boring condition [27]. We examined the state of boredom more directly via a video mood induction [30, see 31 for the video mood induction]. This boring mood induction was contrasted with a resting state, an induction of interest/engagement, and a sustained attention task [30]. Results showed that posterior components of the DMN were active during the boredom mood induction and resting state scans. Interestingly, anticorrelated activity was observed in the anterior insula during the boredom mood induction and was not evident during the resting state [30]. In a more recent study, people were asked to do a very simple task (decide if the frame around a picture was blurry or not) that was presumably also very boring, prior to making decisions about how much they would be willing to pay to download music [32]. These purchasing decisions were also made after having done a more challenging visual search task or a task that simply asked them which images they liked more. When bored, people were willing to pay more for music – as though desperate for any stimulation to escape the boredom. Activity in the caudate nucleus – a 2 subcortical structure known to be important for representing reward value [33] – was associated with decisions to avoid the boring task. Activity in the insula cortex was associated with just how willing people were to pay to avoid boredom – the higher your willingness to pay to avoid boredom the more activity was seen in the insula [32]. So on the one hand, failures to engage are reflected in the downregulation of the insula [30], while an increased desire to engage (by paying more to avoid boredom) is associated with upregulation of the insular [32]. Here we present a replication of our initial work that we deemed important for a number of reasons. First, the original sample consisted of individuals very low in boredom proneness. Here, our sample had more typical responses on this trait measure. Second, the initial sample was rather small given current standards in imaging (n=10). The current sample was slightly larger, but we saw potential strength in replicating our initial results in a new sample. Finally, replication in psychology has increasingly been viewed as a vital part of the scientific endeavor with a spotlight on failures to replicate [34]. This is particularly concerning for imaging studies in which replication attempts are typically lower than in experimental psychology. Results Trait self-report measures Boredom Proneness Participants’ mean score on the SBPS was 22.33 (SD=7.34, range=10-33), which is on par with the SBPS scores obtained in larger pools of participants in our prior work [18]. Mind-Wandering Participants’ mean score on the deliberate mind-wandering scale was 21.00 (SD=1.95, range=7-17), while spontaneous mind-wandering scores were somewhat lower (M= 15.08, SD = 4.81, range 8-26). These scores are comparable to the larger sample size tested in our prior work [18]. State self-report measures Boredom Probes Participants reported feeling most bored in the boredom (M = 6.85, SD = 1.82; Figure 1) and resting state conditions (M = 7.54, SD = 1.71), which did not differ significantly from each other (t = -1.74, p = .108). Both conditions were significantly more boring than the interest condition (M = 5.15, SD = 2.34; t = 2.48, p = .029 and t = 3.48, p = .005, respectively). Mind-Wandering Probes A similar pattern emerged with respect to the mind-wandering probes: participants mind- wandered the most during the boredom (M = 6.23, SD = 2.35; Figure 2) and resting state (M = 6.92, SD = 1.66) scans. Again, these two conditions did not differ significantly from each other (t = -.987, p = .343). Participants mind-wandered significantly less in the Interest condition relative to both the boredom and resting state conditions (t = 3.811, p = .002; t = 4.251, p = .001 respectively). 3 Figure 1. Self-reported levels of state boredom (white bars) and mind-wandering (black bars) ratings for each scanning condition. Error bars represent SE +/-1. fMRI Results largely replicated our earlier work. For the boredom mood induction posterior regions of the DMN showed correlated activity prominently in the posterior cingulate cortex (PCC; Figure 2 and Table 1).
Recommended publications
  • Anatomy of the Temporal Lobe
    Hindawi Publishing Corporation Epilepsy Research and Treatment Volume 2012, Article ID 176157, 12 pages doi:10.1155/2012/176157 Review Article AnatomyoftheTemporalLobe J. A. Kiernan Department of Anatomy and Cell Biology, The University of Western Ontario, London, ON, Canada N6A 5C1 Correspondence should be addressed to J. A. Kiernan, [email protected] Received 6 October 2011; Accepted 3 December 2011 Academic Editor: Seyed M. Mirsattari Copyright © 2012 J. A. Kiernan. This is an open access article distributed under the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited. Only primates have temporal lobes, which are largest in man, accommodating 17% of the cerebral cortex and including areas with auditory, olfactory, vestibular, visual and linguistic functions. The hippocampal formation, on the medial side of the lobe, includes the parahippocampal gyrus, subiculum, hippocampus, dentate gyrus, and associated white matter, notably the fimbria, whose fibres continue into the fornix. The hippocampus is an inrolled gyrus that bulges into the temporal horn of the lateral ventricle. Association fibres connect all parts of the cerebral cortex with the parahippocampal gyrus and subiculum, which in turn project to the dentate gyrus. The largest efferent projection of the subiculum and hippocampus is through the fornix to the hypothalamus. The choroid fissure, alongside the fimbria, separates the temporal lobe from the optic tract, hypothalamus and midbrain. The amygdala comprises several nuclei on the medial aspect of the temporal lobe, mostly anterior the hippocampus and indenting the tip of the temporal horn. The amygdala receives input from the olfactory bulb and from association cortex for other modalities of sensation.
    [Show full text]
  • Revista Brasileira De Psiquiatria Official Journal of the Brazilian Psychiatric Association Psychiatry Volume 34 • Number 1 • March/2012
    Rev Bras Psiquiatr. 2012;34:101-111 Revista Brasileira de Psiquiatria Official Journal of the Brazilian Psychiatric Association Psychiatry Volume 34 • Number 1 • March/2012 REVIEW ARTICLE Neuroimaging in specific phobia disorder: a systematic review of the literature Ila M.P. Linares,1 Clarissa Trzesniak,1 Marcos Hortes N. Chagas,1 Jaime E. C. Hallak,1 Antonio E. Nardi,2 José Alexandre S. Crippa1 ¹ Department of Neuroscience and Behavior of the Ribeirão Preto Medical School, Universidade de São Paulo (FMRP-USP). INCT Translational Medicine (CNPq). São Paulo, Brazil 2 Panic & Respiration Laboratory. Institute of Psychiatry, Universidade Federal do Rio de Janeiro (UFRJ). INCT Translational Medicine (CNPq). Rio de Janeiro, Brazil Received on August 03, 2011; accepted on October 12, 2011 DESCRIPTORS Abstract Neuroimaging; Objective: Specific phobia (SP) is characterized by irrational fear associated with avoidance of Specific Phobia; specific stimuli. In recent years, neuroimaging techniques have been used in an attempt to better Review; understand the neurobiology of anxiety disorders. The objective of this study was to perform a Anxiety Disorder; systematic review of articles that used neuroimaging techniques to study SP. Method: A literature Phobia. search was conducted through electronic databases, using the keywords: imaging, neuroimaging, PET, spectroscopy, functional magnetic resonance, structural magnetic resonance, SPECT, MRI, DTI, and tractography, combined with simple phobia and specific phobia. One-hundred fifteen articles were found, of which 38 were selected for the present review. From these, 24 used fMRI, 11 used PET, 1 used SPECT, 2 used structural MRI, and none used spectroscopy. Result: The search showed that studies in this area were published recently and that the neuroanatomic substrate of SP has not yet been consolidated.
    [Show full text]
  • Microsurgical and Tractographic Anatomical Study of Insular and Transsylvian Transinsular Approach
    Neurol Sci (2011) 32:865–874 DOI 10.1007/s10072-011-0721-2 ORIGINAL ARTICLE Microsurgical and tractographic anatomical study of insular and transsylvian transinsular approach Feng Wang • Tao Sun • XinGang Li • HeChun Xia • ZongZheng Li Received: 29 September 2008 / Accepted: 16 July 2011 / Published online: 24 August 2011 Ó The Author(s) 2011. This article is published with open access at Springerlink.com Abstract This study is to define the operative anatomy of Introduction the insula with emphasis on the transsylvian transinsular approach. The anatomy was studied in 15 brain specimens, In humans, the insula is a highly developed structure, among five were dissected by use of fiber dissection totally encased within the brain. In many clinical and technique; diffusion tensor imaging of 10 healthy volun- experimental studies, a variety of functions have been teers was obtained with a 1.5-T MR system. The temporal attributed to the insula, however, the full and comprehen- stem consists mainly of the uncinate fasciculus, inferior sive role that it plays continues to remain obscure. Oper- occipitofrontal fasciculus, Meyer’s loop of the optic radi- ation of neurosurgery, specifically of epilepsy surgery, is a ation and anterior commissure. The transinsular approach window onto function and dysfunction of the human brain requires an incision of the inferior limiting sulcus. In this [1]. The insula, as part of the paralimbic system, has both procedure, the fibers of the temporal stem can be inter- invasive anatomical and functional connections with the rupted to various degrees. The fiber dissection technique is temporal lobe through white matter fibers [2–6].
    [Show full text]
  • Altered Resting State Connectivity of the Insular Cortex in Individuals with Fibromyalgia
    The Journal of Pain, Vol 15, No 8 (August), 2014: pp 815-826 Available online at www.jpain.org and www.sciencedirect.com Original Reports Altered Resting State Connectivity of the Insular Cortex in Individuals With Fibromyalgia Eric Ichesco,* Tobias Schmidt-Wilcke,*,** Rupal Bhavsar,*,y Daniel J. Clauw,* Scott J. Peltier,z Jieun Kim,x Vitaly Napadow,x,{,k Johnson P. Hampson,* Anson E. Kairys,*,yy David A. Williams,* and Richard E. Harris* *Department of Anesthesiology, Chronic Pain and Fatigue Research Center, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, Michigan. yNeurology Department, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, Pennsylvania. z Functional MRI Laboratory, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, Michigan. xMGH/MIT/HMS Athinoula A. Martinos Center for Biomedical Imaging, Charlestown, Massachusetts. {Department of Anesthesiology, Perioperative and Pain Medicine, Brigham and Women’s Hospital, Harvard Medical School, Boston, Massachusetts. kDepartment of Radiology, Logan College of Chiropractic, Chesterfield, Missouri. **Department of Neurology, Bergmannsheil, Ruhr University Bochum, Bochum, Germany. yyDepartment of Psychology, University of Colorado Denver, Denver, Colorado. Abstract: The insular cortex (IC) and cingulate cortex (CC) are critically involved in pain perception. Previously we demonstrated that fibromyalgia (FM) patients have greater connectivity between the insula and default mode network at rest, and that changes in the degree of this connectivity were associated with changes in the intensity of ongoing clinical pain. In this study we more thoroughly evaluated the degree of resting-state connectivity to multiple regions of the IC in individuals with FM and healthy controls. We also investigated the relationship between connectivity, experimental pain, and current clinical chronic pain. Functional connectivity was assessed using resting-state functional magnetic resonance imaging in 18 FM patients and 18 age- and sex-matched healthy controls using predefined seed regions in the anterior, middle, and posterior IC.
    [Show full text]
  • Insular Cortex Corticotropin-Releasing Factor Integrates Stress Signaling with Social Decision Making
    bioRxiv preprint doi: https://doi.org/10.1101/2021.03.23.436680; this version posted March 23, 2021. The copyright holder for this preprint (which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder. All rights reserved. No reuse allowed without permission. Insular cortex corticotropin releasing factor 1 Title: Insular cortex corticotropin-releasing factor integrates stress signaling with social decision making. 1 1 1 2 1 Authors: Nathaniel S. Rieger , Juan A. Varela , Alexandra Ng , Lauren Granata , Anthony Djerdjaj , Heather C. Brenhouse2, John P. Christianson1* 1Department of Psychology & Neuroscience, Boston College, 140 Commonwealth Ave, Chestnut Hill, MA 02467 USA. 2Psychology Department, Northeastern University, 360 Huntington Avenue, 115 Richards Hall, Boston, MA, 02115 *Corresponding Author: John Christianson, [email protected], 617-552-3970 Abstract Impairments in social cognition manifest in a variety of psychiatric disorders, making the neurobiological mechanisms underlying social decision making of particular translational importance. The insular cortex is consistently implicated in stress-related social and anxiety disorders, which are associated with diminished ability to make and use inferences about the emotions of others to guide behavior. We investigated how corticotropin releasing factor (CRF), a neuromodulator evoked by both self and social stressors, influenced the insula. In acute slices from male and female rats, CRF depolarized insular pyramidal neurons. In males, but not females, CRF suppressed presynaptic
    [Show full text]
  • Multiple Areas of the Cerebral Cortex Influence the Stomach
    Multiple areas of the cerebral cortex influence the stomach David J. Levinthala,b and Peter L. Stricka,c,d,1 aUniversity of Pittsburgh Brain Institute, University of Pittsburgh School of Medicine, Pittsburgh, PA 15261; bNeurogastroenterology & Motility Center, Division of Gastroenterology, Hepatology, and Nutrition, Department of Medicine, University of Pittsburgh School of Medicine, Pittsburgh, PA 15261; cSystems Neuroscience Center, University of Pittsburgh School of Medicine, Pittsburgh, PA 15261; and dDepartment of Neurobiology, University of Pittsburgh School of Medicine, Pittsburgh, PA 15261 Contributed by Peter L. Strick, April 5, 2020 (sent for review February 13, 2020; reviewed by Atsushi Iriki and Peter Sterling) The central nervous system both influences and is influenced by the cortical neurons that most directly influence this organ. We gastrointestinal system. Most research on this gut–brain connection confined our analysis to cases in which cortical neurons infected has focused on how ascending signals from the gut and its micro- with RV were restricted to Layer V, the origin of subcortical biome alter brain function. Less attention has focused on how outputs from the cerebral cortex. To isolate cortical labeling to descending signals from the central nervous system alter gut func- parasympathetic or sympathetic circuits, we carefully adjusted tion. Here, we used retrograde transneuronal transport of rabies survival times and employed bilateral subdiaphragmatic vagot- virus to identify the cortical areas that most directly influence para- omy in some animals (Fig. 1). sympathetic and sympathetic control of the rat stomach. Cortical neurons that influence parasympathetic output to the stomach orig- Parasympathetic Network. Transport of RV in parasympathetic inated from the rostral insula and portions of medial prefrontal circuits labeled first-order neurons in the dorsal motor nucleus of cortex, regions that are associated with interoception and emo- the vagus, second-order neurons in the nucleus of the solitary tional control.
    [Show full text]
  • Insula Functional Connectivity in Schizophrenia
    bioRxiv preprint doi: https://doi.org/10.1101/2019.12.16.878827; this version posted December 18, 2019. The copyright holder for this preprint (which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder, who has granted bioRxiv a license to display the preprint in perpetuity. It is made available under aCC-BY-NC-ND 4.0 International license. Insula Functional Connectivity in Schizophrenia Julia M. Sheffield, PhD1, Baxter P. Rogers, PhD2, Jennifer Urbano Blackford, PhD1,3, Stephan Heckers, MD1, Neil D. Woodward, PhD1 1 Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Vanderbilt University School of Medicine, Nashville, TN, USA 2 Vanderbilt University Institute of Imaging Sciences, Nashville, TN, USA 3Tennessee Valley Health Service, Department of Veterans Affairs Medical Center, Nashville, TN, USA Corresponding Author: Julia M. Sheffield, PhD Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences Vanderbilt Psychiatric Hospital Suite 3057, 1601 23rd Ave. S., Nashville, TN 37212 E-Mail: [email protected] Phone: 615-343-3839; Fax: 615-936-3563 bioRxiv preprint doi: https://doi.org/10.1101/2019.12.16.878827; this version posted December 18, 2019. The copyright holder for this preprint (which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder, who has granted bioRxiv a license to display the preprint in perpetuity. It is made available under aCC-BY-NC-ND 4.0 International license. Abstract The insula is structurally abnormal in schizophrenia, demonstrating robust reductions in gray matter volume, cortical thickness, and altered gyrification during prodromal, early and chronic stages of the illness. Despite compelling structural alterations, less is known about its functional connectivity, limited by studies considering the insula as a whole or only within the context of resting-state networks.
    [Show full text]
  • Functional Connectivity of the Insula in the Resting Brain
    NeuroImage 55 (2011) 8–23 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect NeuroImage journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/ynimg Functional connectivity of the insula in the resting brain Franco Cauda a,b,⁎, Federico D'Agata a,b,c, Katiuscia Sacco a,b, Sergio Duca a, Giuliano Geminiani a,b, Alessandro Vercelli d a CCS fMRI, Koelliker Hospital, Turin, Italy b Department of Psychology, University of Turin, Turin, Italy c Department of Neuroscience, AOU San Giovanni Battista, Turin, Italy d Department of Anatomy, Pharmacology and Forensic Medicine, National Institute of Neuroscience, Turin, Italy article info abstract Article history: The human insula is hidden in the depth of the cerebral hemisphere by the overlying frontal and temporal Received 4 June 2010 opercula, and consists of three cytoarchitectonically distinct regions: the anterior agranular area, posterior Revised 10 November 2010 granular area, and the transitional dysgranular zone; each has distinct histochemical staining patterns and Accepted 15 November 2010 specific connectivity. Even though there are several studies reporting the functional connectivity of the insula Available online 24 November 2010 with the cingulated cortex, its relationships with other brain areas remain elusive in humans. Therefore, we Keywords: decided to use resting state functional connectivity to elucidate in details its connectivity, in terms of cortical fl Default networks and subcortical areas, and also of lateralization. We investigated correlations in BOLD uctuations between Cingulate cortex specific
    [Show full text]
  • Current Understanding of the Involvement of the Insular Cortex in Neuropathic Pain: a Narrative Review
    International Journal of Molecular Sciences Review Current Understanding of the Involvement of the Insular Cortex in Neuropathic Pain: A Narrative Review Ning Wang 1,2 , Yu-Han Zhang 1,2, Jin-Yan Wang 1,2,* and Fei Luo 1,2 1 CAS Key Laboratory of Mental Health, Institute of Psychology, Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing 100101, China; [email protected] (N.W.); [email protected] (Y.-H.Z.); [email protected] (F.L.) 2 Department of Psychology, University of Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing 100049, China * Correspondence: [email protected] Abstract: Neuropathic pain is difficult to cure and is often accompanied by emotional and psycho- logical changes. Exploring the mechanisms underlying neuropathic pain will help to identify a better treatment for this condition. The insular cortex is an important information integration center. Numerous imaging studies have documented increased activity of the insular cortex in the presence of neuropathic pain; however, the specific role of this region remains controversial. Early studies suggested that the insular lobe is mainly involved in the processing of the emotional motivation dimension of pain. However, increasing evidence suggests that the role of the insular cortex is more complex and may even be related to the neural plasticity, cognitive evaluation, and psychosocial aspects of neuropathic pain. These effects contribute not only to the development of neuropathic pain, but also to its comorbidity with neuropsychiatric diseases. In this review, we summarize the changes that occur in the insular cortex in the presence of neuropathic pain and analgesia, as well as the molecular mechanisms that may underlie these conditions.
    [Show full text]
  • Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation for Neuromodulation of the Operculo-Insular Cortex in Humans
    This is an electronic reprint of the original article. This reprint may differ from the original in pagination and typographic detail. Zugaib, João; Souza, Victor Hugo Transcranial magnetic stimulation for neuromodulation of the operculo-insular cortex in humans Published in: Journal of Physiology DOI: 10.1113/JP277415 Published: 01/02/2019 Document Version Peer reviewed version Please cite the original version: Zugaib, J., & Souza, V. H. (2019). Transcranial magnetic stimulation for neuromodulation of the operculo-insular cortex in humans. Journal of Physiology, 597(3), 677-678. https://doi.org/10.1113/JP277415 This material is protected by copyright and other intellectual property rights, and duplication or sale of all or part of any of the repository collections is not permitted, except that material may be duplicated by you for your research use or educational purposes in electronic or print form. You must obtain permission for any other use. Electronic or print copies may not be offered, whether for sale or otherwise to anyone who is not an authorised user. Powered by TCPDF (www.tcpdf.org) DOI: 10.1113/JP277415 Transcranial magnetic stimulation for neuromodulation of the operculo-insular cortex in humans João Zugaib1 and Victor Hugo Souza2 1 Research Group on the Dynamics of the Neuromusculoskeletal System, Bahiana School of Medicine and Public Health, Avenida Dom João VI – 275, Postal code 40290-000, Salvador, BA, Brazil. E-mail: [email protected] 2 Department of Neuroscience and Biomedical Engineering, Aalto University School of Science, P.O. Box 12200, FI-00076 AALTO, Espoo, Finland. E-mail: [email protected] The authors contributed equally to this work.
    [Show full text]
  • Anterior Insular Cortex and Emotional Awareness
    REVIEW Anterior Insular Cortex and Emotional Awareness Xiaosi Gu,1,2* Patrick R. Hof,3,4 Karl J. Friston,1 and Jin Fan3,4,5,6 1Wellcome Trust Centre for Neuroimaging, University College London, London, United Kingdom WC1N 3BG 2Virginia Tech Carilion Research Institute, Roanoke, Virginia 24011 3Fishberg Department of Neuroscience, Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai, New York, New York 10029 4Friedman Brain Institute, Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai, New York, New York 10029 5Department of Psychiatry, Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai, New York, New York 10029 6Department of Psychology, Queens College, The City University of New York, Flushing, New York 11367 ABSTRACT dissociable from ACC, 3) AIC integrates stimulus-driven This paper reviews the foundation for a role of the and top-down information, and 4) AIC is necessary for human anterior insular cortex (AIC) in emotional aware- emotional awareness. We propose a model in which ness, defined as the conscious experience of emotions. AIC serves two major functions: integrating bottom-up We first introduce the neuroanatomical features of AIC interoceptive signals with top-down predictions to gen- and existing findings on emotional awareness. Using erate a current awareness state and providing descend- empathy, the awareness and understanding of other ing predictions to visceral systems that provide a point people’s emotional states, as a test case, we then pres- of reference for autonomic reflexes. We argue that AIC ent evidence to demonstrate: 1) AIC and anterior cingu- is critical and necessary for emotional awareness. J. late cortex (ACC) are commonly coactivated as Comp.
    [Show full text]
  • Neurosurgical Anatomy of the Insular Cortex T Diogo Cunha-Cabrala, Susana M
    Clinical Neurology and Neurosurgery 186 (2019) 105530 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Clinical Neurology and Neurosurgery journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/clineuro Neurosurgical anatomy of the insular cortex T Diogo Cunha-Cabrala, Susana M. Silvaa,b, Hélio Alvesa, Ricardo P. Vaza,b,c, Pedro A. Pereiraa,b, ⁎ José P. Andradea,b, a Department of Biomedicine, Unit of Anatomy, Faculty of Medicine, University of Porto, Alameda Professor Hernâni Monteiro, 4200-319, Porto, Portugal b Center for Health Technology and Services Research (CINTESIS), Rua Dr. Plácido da Costa, s/n, 4200-450, Porto, Portugal c Otorhinolaryngology Department, Centro Hospitalar Universitário S. João, EPE, Alameda Professor Hernâni Monteiro, 4200-319, Porto, Portugal ARTICLE INFO ABSTRACT Keywords: Objective: The purpose of this study was to clarify the morphology of the insular cortex focusing not only on the Island of Reil shape of the insula, but also on sulcal and gyral organization. Insular cortex Patients and methods: Sixty formalin-fixed adult brain hemispheres had their insula exposed and photographed. Insular gyri The dimensions of each gyrus and sulcus were measured using an image analysis software. The morphometric Insular sulci data obtained was statistically analysed. Sylvian fissure Results: The insular cortex shape alternates between triangular and trapezoid, being the triangular shape the Neuroanatomy most common (75%). The angle between the posterior and inferior peri-insular sulcus in the trapezoid insulae had a mean range of 131.17° (SD = 12.277). A minimum of 3 and a maximum of 6 insular gyri were observed, being 5 the most common total number of gyri observed. The accessory gyrus was present in 66% of the insulae and well-developed in 38% of the cases.
    [Show full text]